Abstract
Methods for predicting circadian phase have been developed for healthy individuals. It is unknown whether these methods generalize to clinical populations, such as delayed sleep–wake phase disorder (DSWPD), where circadian timing is associated with functional outcomes. This study evaluated two methods for predicting dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) in 154 DSWPD patients using ~ 7 days of sleep–wake and light data: a dynamic model and a statistical model. The dynamic model has been validated in healthy individuals under both laboratory and field conditions. The statistical model was developed for this dataset and used a multiple linear regression of light exposure during phase delay/advance portions of the phase response curve, as well as sleep timing and demographic variables. Both models performed comparably well in predicting DLMO. The dynamic model predicted DLMO with root mean square error of 68 min, with predictions accurate to within ± 1 h in 58% of participants and ± 2 h in 95%. The statistical model predicted DLMO with root mean square error of 57 min, with predictions accurate to within ± 1 h in 75% of participants and ± 2 h in 96%. We conclude that circadian phase prediction from light data is a viable technique for improving screening, diagnosis, and treatment of DSWPD.
Subject terms: Circadian rhythms and sleep, Diagnostic markers
Introduction
Humans possess an endogenous circadian clock that is responsible for synchronization of many physiological and behavioral processes1. Quantitative techniques, such as mathematical models and machine-learning approaches, have been developed to predict the timing of circadian rhythms from non-invasive ambulatory signals2. Commonly used ambulatory signals include activity and light3–6, skin temperature7,8, and heart rate or heart rate variability9. Mathematical models, such as the Jewett-Kronauer model3, quantify the characteristics of the circadian clock and its response to light, in particular the phase-dependent sensitivity of the clock to light. These models have been developed and tested in healthy individuals under laboratory and field conditions to predict circadian phase markers such as core body temperature minimum (CBTmin)3,10 and dim light melatonin onset (DLMO)4. It is currently unknown, however, whether these models can accurately predict circadian phase in clinical populations, such as those with circadian rhythm sleep disorders.
One such disorder, delayed sleep–wake phase disorder (DSWPD), is thought to be driven by an underlying delay in the timing of the circadian clock relative to the required sleep–wake schedule11,12. Despite this etiology, current diagnostic criteria do not mandate an objective measure of circadian phase. Rather, diagnosis is based on measures of sleep timing13 with delayed timing of sleep onset and wake times considered indicative of a delay in the biological clock12,13. This is problematic, as sleep and/or wake times can be delayed despite no underlying circadian delay14. For example, we have shown previously that 43% of DSWPD patients, using current diagnostic criteria, do not have a circadian phase delay relative to the desired sleep–wake schedule14, with others reporting similar findings15. This apparent discrepancy can occur because the relationship between the onset of the evening rise in melatonin (a gold-standard circadian phase marker16) and sleep is highly variable, with an inter-individual range of up to 5 h between melatonin onset and sleep onset in healthy populations17–19. This range can be even greater in sleep disordered populations, with a difference of up to 8 h seen in patients with insomnia20.
Currently, circadian phase assessments using salivary DLMO are not widely applied in clinical practice due to the cost, lack of insurance reimbursement, perceived inconvenience of the procedures, and complicated analytical interpretation of results21. While simpler biochemical measures are being developed22–24, this clinical gap could be addressed by less invasive techniques that attempt to predict circadian timing using ambulatory monitoring of activity and light, given that light is the primary synchronizing agent for the circadian clock25.
Here we evaluated the utility of a statistical regression model for predicting circadian phase in a sample of patients clinically diagnosed with DSWPD. To evaluate the performance of the model, we compared its performance against a dynamic model3 that has been shown to accurately predict circadian phase in healthy individuals4,10.
Results
Participant characteristics
Participants (N = 154) were 30.1 ± 10.7 years of age (range 16–64 years) with body mass index (BMI) of 24.6 ± 4.0 kg/m2. Average DLMO time was 22:07 (range 18:42–2:24), average bedtime was 0:40, and average wake time was 8:44 (see Table 1). There were no significant differences between the test and training datasets for sex, age, DLMO time, desired bedtime-DLMO phase angle, bed and wake times, composite morningness–eveningness questionnaire (cMEQ), or clinical global impression (CGI) scale. BMI differed modestly between the training and test datasets (23.8 vs. 25.3 kg/m2), although both groups were still within the healthy range.
Table 1.
Overall data set | Training set | Test set | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N | 154 | 77 | 77 | |
Sex n (%) | 70 M (45.5), 84 F (54.5) | 31 M (40.3), 46 F (59.7) | 39 M (50.6), 38 F (49.4) | 0.20 |
Age (y) M ± SD | 30.1 ± 10.7 | 29.6 ± 11.4 | 30.5 ± 10.1 | 0.61 |
BMI (kg/m2) M ± SD | 24.5 ± 4.0 | 23.8 ± 3.6 | 25.3 ± 4.2 | 0.02 |
DLMO time (hh:mm), M ± SD | 22:07 ± 1:26 | 22:11 ± 1:22 | 22:04 ± 1:31 | 0.61 |
DBT-DLMO PAD (h), M ± SD | − 0:19 ± 1:17 | − 0:11 ± 1:15 | − 0:25 ± 1:18 | 0.28 |
Bedtime (hh:mm), M ± SD* | 0:41 ± 1:19 | 0:41 ± 1:13 | 0:40 ± 1:24 | 0.94 |
Wake time (h:mm), M ± SD* | 8:46 ± 1:21 | 8:49 ± 1:17 | 8:42 ± 1:26 | 0.71 |
CGI˄ | ||||
Mildly ill | 74 (48.7) | 31 (40.8) | 43 (56.6) | 0.05 |
Moderately-severely ill | 78 (51.3) | 45 (59.2) | 33 (43.4) | |
cMEQ | ||||
Moderately evening | 23 (14.9) | 8 (10.4) | 15 (19.5) | 0.11 |
Definitely evening | 131 (85.1) | 69 (89.6) | 62 (80.5) |
BMI = Body Mass Index, DLMO = Dim Light Melatonin Onset, DBT-DLMO PAD = Desired Bedtime –Dim Light Melatonin Onset Phase Angle Difference, CGI = Clinical Global Impressions scale, cMEQ = Composite Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; Data is represented as mean ± SD.
*Average objective bed and wake times over 5–7 days.
˄Reduced n.
Phase predictions
Previously, others have used a cutoff of ± 1.5 h error between actual and predicted DLMO (3 h range) to determine accuracy of phase predictions26. Permitting such a wide range to be considered as a successful prediction (3 h from a total population range of 7.7 h, N = 154) may not be optimal for practical use. We therefore based our assessment of prediction accuracy on a 2 h range, i.e., within ± 1 h of actual DLMO.
Performance of the default dynamic model (i.e., the model previously validated against healthy participants) had root mean square error (RMSE) of 83 min using the default 60-min epochs for light data. Performance of the dynamic model was improved by training on the DSWPD dataset, which resulted in selection of the following optimal parameters: τ = 24.4 h for intrinsic circadian period; k = 0.45 to determine shape of the PRC; G = 37 to determine amplitude of the PRC; maximum allowed missing data interval of 2 h, using mean of previous 2 h for filling missing intervals; and binning light in 60-min windows to the maximum value within the bin.
Performances of the statistical and dynamic models on training and test datasets, respectively, are summarized in Table 2. On the test dataset, the mean absolute errors for the statistical model and dynamic model were 44 and 57 min, respectively. The RMSE for the statistical model and dynamic model were 57 and 68 min, respectively. The statistical model predicted 39% within ± 30 min, 75% within ± 1 h, and 96% within ± 2 h of actual DLMO, while the dynamic model predicted 25% within ± 30 min, 58% within ± 1 h, and 94% within ± 2 h. Actual DLMO was significantly correlated with both the statistical model test predictions (R2 = 0.61, p < 0.001) and the dynamic model test predictions (R2 = 0.48, p < 0.001). The addition of advance and delay light regions, based on average DLMO timing, significantly increased the amount of variance explained compared to when only habitual bed or wake time were included in the regression model (R2 = 0.49 vs. R2 = 0.61).
Table 2.
Statistical model | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Training dataset (n = 77) | Test dataset (n = 77) | |||||||
Mean error ± SD (min) | 0.02 ± 52.51 | − 4.39 ± 56.87 | ||||||
Mean absolute error ± SD (min) | 39.62 ± 34.15 | 44.58 ± 35.21 | ||||||
Root mean square error (min) | 52.17 | 56.67 | ||||||
R | 0.766 | 0.778 | ||||||
R2 | 0.587 | 0.605 |
n | n Cumulative | % | %Cumulative | n | n Cumulative | % | %Cumulative | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictions within ± 0.5 h | 37 | 37 | 48.05 | 48.05 | 30 | 30 | 38.96 | 38.96 |
Predictions within ± 1 h | 25 | 62 | 32.47 | 80.52 | 28 | 58 | 36.36 | 75.33 |
Predictions within ± 1.5 h | 9 | 71 | 11.69 | 92.21 | 12 | 70 | 15.58 | 90.91 |
Predictions within ± 2 h | 4 | 75 | 5.2 | 97.4 | 4 | 74 | 5.2 | 96.1 |
Predictions > 2 h | 2 | 77 | 2.6 | 100 | 3 | 77 | 3.9 | 100 |
Dynamic model | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Training Dataset (n = 76) | Test Dataset (n = 77) | |||||||
Mean Error ± SD (min) | − 1.22 ± 63.15 | − 4.11 ± 67.91 | ||||||
Mean Absolute Error ± SD (min) | 47.53 ± 40.20 | 48.40 ± 40.20 | ||||||
Root Mean Square Error (min) | 62.74 | 67.60 | ||||||
R | 0.655 | 0.692 | ||||||
R2 | 0.430 | 0.479 |
n | n Cumulative | % | %Cumulative | n | n Cumulative | % | %Cumulative | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictions within ± 0.5 h | 29 | 29 | 38.16 | 38.16 | 19 | 19 | 24.68 | 24.68 |
Predictions within ± 1 h | 24 | 53 | 31.58 | 69.74 | 26 | 45 | 33.76 | 58.44 |
Predictions within ± 1.5 h | 13 | 66 | 17.11 | 86.84 | 18 | 63 | 23.38 | 81.82 |
Predictions within ± 2 h | 5 | 71 | 6.58 | 93.42 | 10 | 73 | 12.99 | 94.81 |
Predictions > 2 h | 5 | 76 | 6.58 | 100 | 4 | 77 | 5.19 | 100 |
Examination of the correlations between actual and predicted DLMO between the training and test datasets within each approach showed that the training and test predictions did not differ significantly from one another for either approach (statistical model, Z = 0.18, p = 0.86; dynamic model, Z = 0.41, p = 0.68). A comparison of the correlations between the two models for test predictions also showed no significant difference (statistical model vs. dynamic model, Z = − 1.15. p = 0.25). Similarly, the slopes of the regression lines for the two models did not significantly differ (slope for statistical model = 0.60 vs. slope for dynamic model = 0.62, p = 0.85).
As a comparative method, DLMO was also predicted by subtracting 2 h from actigraphically derived bedtime (averaged over 5–7 days) for each participant, which is reported to be the average phase angle between DLMO and habitual bedtime in a healthy population18–20,27. DLMO predicted in this way significantly correlated with actual DLMO (R2 = 0.40, p < 0.001), with RMSE of 129 min. When this correlation was compared with the final models, there was a significant difference to the statistical model (Z = − 0.02, p < 0.05), but not the dynamic model (Z = − 0.64, p = 0.52).
Figure 1 shows predicted and actual DLMO for each participant for each method, with actual DLMO ranked from earliest to latest onset time. Both methods illustrate a tendency to underestimate population variability, regressing very early or very late individuals towards the population mean.
A comparison was made to determine whether the same participants were being predicted within ± 1 h between the two models (Table 3). Combining the training and test datasets, there were 93 (60%) participants who were predicted within ± 1 h in both models, 32 (21%) participants whose predictions fell in different categories between the dynamic and statistical models, and 28 (18%) participants who were inaccurately predicted (> 1 h error) by both models. There were no significant differences in functional outcomes (mood and daytime function, illness severity) or circadian phase (DLMO time, DBT-DLMO phase angle) between the 28 participants who were inaccurately predicted by both models and the 93 participants who were accurately predicted by both models within ± 1 h.
Table 3.
Dynamic model | |||
---|---|---|---|
Within ± 0.5 h | > ± 0.5 h | ||
Statistical model | Within ± 0.5 h | 32 | 35 |
> ± 0.5 h | 16 | 68 | |
Within ± 1 h | > ± 1 h | ||
Within ± 1 h | 93 | 27 | |
> ± 1 h | 5 | 28 | |
Within ± 1.5 h | > ± 1.5 h | ||
Within ± 1.5 h | 126 | 14 | |
> ± 1.5 h | 3 | 10 | |
Within ± 2 h | > ± 2 h | ||
Within ± 2 h | 141 | 7 | |
> ± 2 h | 3 | 2 |
As a test of the potential clinical utility of these methods, predicted DLMO was used to classify participants as circadian or non-circadian DSWPD using criteria previously described14. Briefly, a circadian classification was given if DLMO occurred 30 min before, or any time after, desired bedtime, while a non-circadian classification was given if DLMO occurred more than 30 min prior to desired bedtime. Performance of the two models in the test dataset was comparable, with similar numbers of true positives, true negatives, false positives, and false negatives (see Table 4). The sensitivity for predicting group classification was 74% for the statistical model and 64% for the dynamic model; specificity was 63% for the statistical model and 66% for the dynamic model. The F1 scores were 0.71 and 0.65 for the statistical and dynamic models, respectively.
Table 4.
Statistical model | Dynamic model | |
---|---|---|
Sensitivity | 74.4% | 64.1% |
Specificity | 63.2% | 65.8% |
F1 Score | 0.71 | 0.65 |
Accuracy | 0.69 | 0.65 |
Discussion
State-of-the-art methods for predicting circadian timing have yet to be tested in clinical populations. We evaluated and compared a novel statistical regression model and a previously validated dynamic model for predicting DLMO time in a clinically diagnosed DSWPD patient population. Both methods for performed similarly. The statistical model accounted for a slightly greater proportion of the variance in actual DLMO time than the dynamic model (60% vs. 48%), but the statistical model also required knowledge of the average DLMO time for the sample to define its PRC regions. The two models were similarly accurate for classifying circadian vs. non-circadian DSWPD (69% vs. 65%). Both models were moderately reliable in predicting circadian phase to within ± 1 h (75% and 58%).
Our study showed that a regression model using mean light exposure during the delay and advance portions of the human PRC, in combination with sex, age, bed/wake times, and chronotype, could predict DLMO significantly more accurately than using bed/wake times alone. Self-reported sleep timing information has previously been used in regression analyses to predict circadian phase in DSWPD, whereby the combination of variables accounted for 77% of the variance in DLMO26. The previous study, however, achieved only 79% of predictions within ± 1.5 h, whereas 91% of our regression model estimates were within ± 1.5 h. Additionally, a recent study in which the temporal stability of the melatonin profile over time was examined in 8 DSWPD patients and 5 healthy controls found that DLMO time could be accurately predicted using wake time and the time of morning light onset28 Specifically, average actigraphically derived wake time and light onset (averaged across the 3 days prior to DLMO) explained 89% of the variance in DLMO timing. Similar findings were reported by Crowley and colleagues29, who estimated DLMO in an adolescent population with healthy sleep. Three separate regression analyses were conducted in which the independent variables were either bed, wake, or mid-sleep times. DLMOs were predicted to within ± 1 h for 78–82% of participants using bedtime, 82–86% using mid-sleep time, and 80–81% using wake time. Our application of a similar statistical model for estimating DLMO in an adult, patient population with a larger sample extends these previous studies, demonstrating the generalizability of such an approach to a clinical setting. Additionally, the current study supports the potential use of objective measures of light exposure, in addition to bed and wake times, for estimating circadian phase timing. The combination of these findings indicates not only that simpler, less invasive methods for estimating circadian phase are a viable alternative, but also that the addition of light is valuable for improving prediction accuracy.
While the statistical model we present here appears promising, it requires further validation before it is to be generalized to clinical settings. The current implementation of the statistical model requires knowledge of the group average DLMO time, which may not be known. Furthermore, the average DLMO time in our sample of DSWPD patients was relatively early compared with other published studies in DSWPD patients. Applying this average DLMO time to other samples could therefore lead to less accurate predictions with the statistical model. In future work, the statistical model should be tested in other DSWPD samples to determine its accuracy, and to determine whether the new sample’s average DLMO time must first be determined. In practice, this DLMO testing could be performed in a subsample to obtain an estimate of the average timing, rather than needing to test the entire sample.
The dynamic model of the circadian pacemaker was originally developed and validated under lab conditions in healthy participants3,4. It has since been demonstrated to also perform accurately in healthy participants in the field under a range of conditions30,31, making it the best validated model of the human circadian clock2. To date, however, it has not been tested in any patient populations. Individuals with DSWPD appear, on average, to have physiological predispositions to phase delay, including a longer intrinsic circadian period32 and increased sensitivity to phase-delaying light33. Consistent with this, we found that the default model (i.e., using parameters fit against healthy individuals) predicted DLMO time systematically too early. When we allowed parameters to be refit against the Training dataset, we found that a significantly better fit was obtained for DSWPD patients by lengthening the intrinsic circadian period and by slightly increasing the size of the phase-delay region of the phase response curve. With these modifications, the accuracy of the model (RMSE = 68 min) was similar to that reported in the field in healthy populations2.
There are many proponents for circadian phase assessments to occur in a clinical setting for diagnosis of circadian rhythm sleep disorders16,34–36. It is acknowledged, however, that there are practical barriers to this occurring, including the costs and logistical difficulties involved in conducting DLMO assessments21. The results of the current study show that, in addition to predicting actual DLMO timing for the majority of patients within ± 1 h, both models perform equally well at classifying patients with circadian DSWPD14. Currently there is no simple method for clinicians to determine if and when a DLMO assessment should be conducted for diagnosis of DSWPD. The models presented here could be valuable clinical screening tools to classify patients as high or low risk for circadian misalignment, enabling efficient identification of those who need further formal phase assessment. Moreover, they could be used to help optimize the timing of interventions, such as light37 or melatonin38.
For both of the models that we tested, we observed that accuracy tended to be highest for individuals with DLMO times close to the group average, and less accurate for individuals with more extreme DLMO times. This same phenomenon has been reported in other applications of phase prediction models2. This tendency for models to regress towards the mean and underestimate population variability is a natural consequence of them being fit at the group, rather than the individual, level. Using an average DLMO time for the entire sample to calculate an average CBTmin time (for the statistical model), as well as the assumption that this population has normal phase relationships between DLMO and sleep or CBTmin (for both models), would both tend to increase accuracy for individuals who are closer to the average. In defining the light bins relative to average DLMO, participants whose actual DLMO occurred closer to the average likely had the most accurately defined lights bins. Similarly, the dynamic model has physiological parameters (e.g., tau) that are fit at the group level. Individuals whose physiology is closer to the average would therefore be likely to have their circadian timing predicted more accurately than for individuals with more extreme physiological parameters. Individuals who are more extreme in their physiological parameters may also experience great intra-individual differences in circadian timing39, which could contribute to less accurate predictions. Improved individual-level estimates of model parameters are likely to yield more accurate predictions for individuals who deviate further from the population mean.
There are a number of ways to potentially improve the prediction accuracy of the dynamic and statistical models. These include consideration of inter-individual differences in light sensitivity40,41, which have been theoretically demonstrated as a means of capturing inter-individual differences in DLMO time42. In defining the light inputs for the statistical regression model, we assumed that the DSWPD population has a similar light phase response curve to the healthy population. There is evidence that patients with DSWPD may have a hypersensitivity to evening light33,41. Establishing the light phase response curve in a DSWPD population could therefore help to improve the models. Using an average DLMO time for the entire sample to calculate an average CBTmin time (for the statistical model) and the assumption that this population has normal phase relationships between DLMO and sleep or CBTmin (for both models) are also potential areas of improvement. Although there is some suggestion that phase angle is not altered between sleep and circadian phase markers in DSWPD patients when allowed to sleep at their habitual times compared to healthy individuals11, it has not been well characterized, particularly when bed and wake times are shifted to an earlier desired time. Finally, both models were based on photopic illuminance measured at wrist level, which is known not to accurately represent the effect of light on the circadian pacemaker under all conditions43,44. Presently, wrist-based actigraphy remains the standard method for collecting patterns of light exposure, but an alternative method that measures light closer to the eye and measure impacts on all photoreceptors in the circadian phototransduction pathway could help improve prediction accuracy45.
A measure of circadian timing is important for the diagnosis and appropriate treatment of DSWPD in a clinical setting. Here we have shown that modeling approaches that rely on light–dark and sleep–wake information produce reasonably accurate estimates of circadian phase in up to 75% of participants. Both the dynamic and statistical models show good utility as screening tools in DSWPD and use information that is already routinely collected in diagnostic approaches for DSWPD and other circadian rhythm sleep disorders.
Methods
This study was conducted as part of the delayed sleep on melatonin (DelSoM) Study (ACTRN12612000425897)14,46, a randomized controlled trial testing the efficacy of exogenous melatonin for DSWPD. As the study was a multi-center trial, approval was given by the following human research ethics committees: Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee, The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee, Southern Adelaide Clinical Human Research Ethics Committee, and The University of Adelaide Human Research Ethics Committee. All research was performed in accordance with the ethical regulations and practices stipulated by each ethics committee. Written informed consent was provided by participants prior to study engagement and they were reimbursed for study-related expenses14,46.
Participants
Data from this study have previously been reported14,46,47. Briefly, data were included from 182 (89 M, 93 F) participants with clinically diagnosed DSWPD. Participants were recruited at three study sites (Melbourne, Sydney, Adelaide) from the community via radio, newspaper, and poster advertising, and from clinics through referrals from sleep physicians and psychologists.
Participants were aged 16–65 years, with a body mass index > 18 and < 35 kg/m2. Additionally, participants were required to work or study ≥ 5 consecutive days each week. Participants who reported any of the following were excluded: comorbid sleep disorder (except insomnia); drugs of abuse or concurrent medication likely to affect sleep; history of psychiatric disorder in the past 12 months, other than depression; caffeine consumption > 300 mg per day; alcohol consumption > 14 standard drinks per week; history of substance abuse in past 12 months; investigational drug use in past 60 days; pregnancy or lactation; night shift work in past 6 months; transmeridian travel in the past 2 months; allergies to any medicines, foods, preservatives, or dyes; and liver, kidney, or autoimmune disease. More detailed inclusion and exclusion criteria have been reported elsewhere14.
Screening
Potential participants completed a preliminary online eligibility questionnaire to assess risk of DSWPD. Those deemed as high risk completed further screening after providing consent. Participants were assessed by a sleep physician to confirm a diagnosis of DSWPD, according to current International Classification of Sleep Disorders diagnostic criteria at the time of study48, and also completed questionnaires relating to health and lifestyle, sleep habits, mood, and daytime function. Participants also completed the Composite Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (cMEQ)49,50, a 13-item questionnaire measuring an individual’s preferences for the timing of mental and physical activity.
Sleep/wake and light assessment
Participants recorded sleep and wake times at home for 7 days using a sleep diary and wrist actigraphy (Actiwatch-L, Actiwatch-2, or Actiwatch Spectrum; Philips Respironics, Bend, OR, USA). Participants were instructed to maintain their normal sleep–wake schedules and record bed and wake times, time to fall asleep, and any awakenings after sleep onset each morning upon waking in the sleep diary provided.
Actigraphy devices, worn on the non-dominant wrist, were used to obtain light (white light, lux) and activity measures in 1-min epochs. Software sensitivity was set to medium (40 activity counts/minute) to determine each 1-min epoch as sleep or wake (Actiware 5 software, Philips Respironics Inc, Bend, OR, USA). To limit loss of light data, participants were instructed to keep the device uncovered (e.g., by sleeves) at all times. Bed and wake times in actigraphy were identified using the times reported in sleep diaries. In the case of discrepancies between sleep diaries and actigraphy, the following process was applied: if subjective bedtime was reported as ≥ 60 min before a substantial reduction in activity and light levels, bedtime was adjusted to the time of decrease in activity and light. If reported wake time was ≥ 60 min after a substantial increase in activity and light, wake time was shifted to the start of the sustained activity and light increase. These timings were determined via visual inspection by an independent researcher and then reviewed by two study researchers for consensus (JM and MM). From these data, bedtime and wake time were computed for main sleep episodes14,46.
For both the dynamic and statistical models, white light (photopic illuminance) was used51. Light data were extracted from the adjusted actigraphy data files in 1-min epochs for each participant. Light data were further cleaned, with removal of data with values < 1 lux during wake, as it was assumed that this was likely due to the device being covered52. Additionally, light levels during sleep were set to zero, as the eyelids are closed during sleep, minimizing retinal light exposure53.
Circadian phase assessment
Immediately following the 7 days of at-home sleep/wake and light assessment, participants attended a laboratory session to undertake a circadian phase assessment measuring salivary dim light melatonin onset (DLMO). Participants arrived at the laboratory 6 h prior to their subjectively reported habitual bedtime and following admission, were seated in dim lighting (< 3 lux). From 5 h prior to habitual bedtime until 2 h after habitual bedtime, saliva samples were taken every hour. DLMO was calculated as the time that melatonin concentrations crossed and remained above a threshold of 2.3 pg/mL, calculated from linear interpolation between the samples immediately before and after the threshold54. Details of this procedure have been reported previously14,46.
Dynamic model
The dynamic model used a mathematical model that has been previously tested and validated against data from carefully screened healthy humans in laboratory conditions4, as well as in field conditions10,30. The model includes light processing in the retina by photoreceptors, the effects of light (photic drive) and the effects of sleep/wake patterns (non-photic drive) on the circadian clock, and a limit-cycle oscillator model of the central circadian clock. For the full model equations and parameter values, see the original publication4.
The model includes the following equation for the photic drive to the circadian pacemaker
where is a constant of proportionality determining amplitude of the light PRC, is the rate at which retinal photoreceptors are being activated, is a constant determining the overall shape of the light PRC, and and are the variables of the circadian pacemaker. All parameters of the model have been previously fit against healthy participant data. The model generates predicted times of minima in the endogenous core body temperature rhythm. DLMO is estimated to occur 7 h before these minima, which is based on the average phase angle difference between DLMO and CBTmin55.
Here, the model was first used in its default form to predict DLMO times for all participants to determine whether the model fit against healthy participants could generalize to this patient population. In other papers that have used versions of the Jewett-Kronauer model, light data have either been input in 1-min epochs10 or set to the maximum value in 60-min epochs40. A range of epoch choices were therefore used here, with 60-min epochs for testing default parameters.
Subsequently, the model was trained against the DSWPD dataset to determine if model performance could be improved. The dataset was split into training (50%) and test (50%) sets, split evenly within each of the three test sites. For training, the following modifications were permitted: (1) changes in the intrinsic circadian period in steps of 0.05 h, allowing values equal to or above the default 24.15 h, given empirical evidence for longer tau in this population32; (2) changes in the parameter , in steps of 0.05, allowing values equal to or above the default 0.40, given empirical evidence for greater phase delays in this population33; (3) changes in the parameter ,in steps of 5%, allowing values equal to or greater than the default 37, given empirical evidence for greater light sensitivity in this population33,41; (4) binning of light data in 1, 2, 5, 10, 15, 30, 60, or 120 min bins, to either the mean value or maximum value in the bin; (5) since the model requires continuous light data with no missing values, we allowed missing intervals to be filled with either a value of 0 lux or with the average value for the previous 2 h. The maximum allowed gap length was allowed to be in the range 1–6 h, in steps of 1 h. The longest continuous portion of the light time series with no missing values was then used as input to the model. No other changes to model parameters or equations were made. The non-photic component of the model was driven by sleep/wake state. For each participant, light data were input repeatedly for 60 days, allowing the model to approach a steady cycle, as in previous papers using the model10,31. Predicted DLMO time was the average predicted DLMO time across the final input cycle.
Statistical model
A statistical model for predicting circadian phase was developed based on a multiple linear regression analysis, with DLMO as the dependent variable. The following predictors were included: (1) light data from the advance and delay portions of the PRC (Figure 2); (2) demographic information (age, sex); (3) actigraphically derived bedtime and wake time and; (4) composite morningness-eveningness score (cMEQ)50. Variables (2)–(4) were included in the model as covariates based on previous reports that these variables are associated with circadian phase56–59. All variables, with the exception of sex and cMEQ, were continuous variables. cMEQ was treated as a categorical variable with two levels: extreme evening type (scores between 16–30) and moderately evening type (scores between 31–41). The statistical model was applied to the same training and test datasets as those described for the dynamic model above.
To quantify the time points at which light would have maximal circadian effects, we used a PRC for light55 (Fig. 1) and classified the 6 h prior to core body temperature minimum (CBTmin) as the interval corresponding to the delay zone and the 6 h after CBTmin as the advance zone. Light data were log10-transformed, as has been done by others previously, to account for brief periods of high light exposure that may have skewed the data52 and zero values were assigned a value of 0.001 lux to allow transformation. As we did not have an actual measure of CBTmin in this study, to determine CBTmin we assumed a phase angle between DLMO and CBTmin of 7 h, as reported in other studies55,60,61. CBTmin was thus estimated by adding 7 h to the average DLMO time for the total sample (N = 154); average DLMO time for the sample was 22:10 h, and average estimated CBTmin time was 5:10 h. This CBTmin time was then applied to all participants to determine the delay and advance zones. Average light exposure in the delay and advance zones was then calculated for each individual on each day with valid data (minimum 5 consecutive days, maximum 7 consecutive days; 6.83 ± 0.47 days). The average delay and advance light zones from each day were then averaged across the days of recording, with each participant having one measure of delay light and one measure of advance light.
Data analysis and statistical methods
Matlab R2018a (Natick, Massachusetts) was used for the dynamic model simulations, using ode15s for solving differential equations and using a custom grid search method for finding optimal parameter values. SPSS Statistics Version 20.0 (IBM, Armonk, New York) was used for the statistical model and all other data analysis. Data are expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) unless otherwise stated. Significance level was set at 0.05. Variables were compared between training and test datasets using a chi-squared (goodness of fit) test or independent samples t-test. To evaluate the predictive performance of each method, the mean error, mean absolute error, and root mean square error of the difference between actual DLMO and predicted DLMO were calculated. Pearson’s correlation coefficients were computed between actual DLMO and predicted DLMO for the dynamic and statistical models and regression lines were fit for each correlation. To determine whether there were significant differences between Pearson’s correlation coefficients for the dynamic and statistical models, a Fisher’s r to z transformation was used. The regression slopes were also compared to determine whether there were significant differences between models. We additionally calculated the percentage of participants for whom predicted DLMO was within ± 30, ± 60, ± 90, ± 120, or > 120 min of actual DLMO.
The predictive value of each model was also assessed by classifying participants as “circadian” or “non-circadian” DWSPD, based on a previously published classification scheme14. Circadian DSPWD participants were defined as individuals whose DLMO time was within half an hour before, or any time after, desired bedtime, while non-circadian DSPWD participants were defined as those whose DLMO occurred more than half an hour before desired bedtime14. Sensitivity, specificity, F1, and accuracy scores were calculated for both models.
Data retention
Out of 182 participants, 28 participants (15%) were excluded. Participants were excluded from the dynamic modeling approach, due to (1) irregular actigraphy data (n = 3) in which the pattern of light exposure and/or activity was highly irregular with no discernable diurnal rhythm, or (2) actigraphy recordings that did not occur in the 7 days immediately prior to the circadian phase assessment (n = 25).
To ensure comparability between the dynamic and statistical models, the same participants were excluded from the statistical model. As such, the total number of participants included was 154. One participant was excluded from the dynamic model test dataset due to inability to construct at least 1 day of consecutive valid light data due to gaps in the dataset.
Acknowledgements
In addition to the authors, members of the Delayed Sleep on Melatonin (DelSOM) study group to be acknowledged for their contribution are as follows: E. Armstrong, Melbourne, VIC.; K. Chohan, Sydney, NSW; Y. Djavadkhani, Sydney, NSW; K. Dodds, Sydney, NSW; S. Gunaratnam, Melbourne, VIC; M. Hardy, Sydney, NSW; S. Joosten, Melbourne, VIC; J. Lee, Melbourne, VIC; G. Micic, Adelaide, SA; B. Raman, Melbourne, VIC; E. Roese, Sydney, NSW; M. Salkeld, Adelaide, SA; E. Verberne, Melbourne, VIC; K. Wong, Sydney, NSW; B. Yee, Sydney, NSW; A. Yeo, Adelaide, SA; K. Yu, Melbourne, VIC. The study was funded by a research grant from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) to SMWR, SWL, RRG, LCL, DJK, and research support from Philips Respironics and the NHMRC Australasian Sleep Trials Network. The study was also supported by the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. JMM was supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award from Monash University and PhD top-up scholarship from the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. RRG is supported by a NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship.
Author contributions
J.M.M., M.M., T.L.S., C.G., N.L., D.J.B., D.J.K., L.C.L., R.R.G., S.W.L., S.M.W.R., and A.J.K.P. were all responsible for study concept and design. Data were acquired by J.M.M., M.M., C.G., N.L., D.J.K., L.C.L., R.R.G., S.W.L., T.L.S., and S.M.W.R. Data were analyzed and interpreted by J.M.M., M.M., K.A., S.M.W.R., and A.J.K.P. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Data availability
Materials and data in this publication can be requested via the CRC for Alertness, Safety and Productivity (Alertness CRC) by emailing inquiries@alertnesscrc.com.
Competing interests
Dr. Murray served as a Project Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Dr Magee served as a Project Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Dr Sletten reports her institution has received equipment donations or other support from Philips Lighting, Philips Respironics, Optalert and Compumedics. Dr Sletten serves as a Project Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Dr Gordon serves as a Project Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Dr Lovato reports no conflicts of interest. Dr Bartlett reports no conflicts of interest. Ms Ambani reports no conflicts of interest. Dr Kennaway reports no conflicts of interest. Dr Lack is shareholder in Re-Time Pty Ltd. Dr Grunstein serves as a consultant to, and is a Program Leader for, the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. He has provided non-remunerated advice to Merck and has been a medico-legal expert witness for Queensland Health, NSW Nurses Federation, NSW Health and NSW Director of Public Prosecutions. Dr Lockley holds current consulting contracts with Delos Living LLC; Environmental Light Sciences LLC; Headwaters Inc.; Hintsa Performance AG; Pegasus Capital Advisors LP; PlanLED; Focal Point LLC; and Wyle Integrated Science and Engineering. In the past 5 years, he has received consulting fees from Carbon Limiting Technologies Ltd for work conducted with PhotonStar LED. He has also received consulting fees from Naturebright; Thomas Jefferson University; and minor consulting fees from 15 financial companies related to non-24-h sleep–wake disorder in the blind and the publicly-available clinical trial results. He has received unrestricted equipment gifts from Biological Illuminations LLC; Bionetics Corporation; Philips Lighting; an unrestricted monetary gift to support research from Swinburne University of Technology, Australia; a fellowship gift from Optalert, Pty, Melbourne, Australia; and holds equity in iSLEEP, Pty, Melbourne, Australia. SWL receives royalties from Oxford University Press; and received honoraria for editing a textbook section from Elsevier and for drafting website text for the National Sleep Foundation; and for an article in the Wall Street Journal. Dr Lockley has received honoraria plus support for travel, accommodation or meals for invited seminars, conference presentations or teaching from American Society for Photobiology; Bassett Research Institute; Brookline Adult Education; Brown University; Emergency Social Services Association Conference; Estee Lauder; Harvard University (CME); MediCom Worldwide, Inc (CME); North East Sleep Society; and Portland General Electric. He has received support for travel, accommodation and/or meals only (no honoraria) for invited seminars, conference presentations or teaching from 8th International Conference on Managing Fatigue; 14th Annual Tennessee Perfusion Conference; American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Bar Harbor Chamber of Commerce; Cantifix; Connecticut Business & Industry Association Health and Safety Conference; Emergency Services Steering Committee; Ferrari; Harvard University; Hintsa Performance AG; Illinois Coalition for Responsible Outdoor Lighting; Illuminating Engineering Society; Lighting Science Group Corp; Massachusetts General Hospital; Midwest Lighting Institute; National Research Council Canada; New England College of Occupational and Environmental Medicine; Ontario Association of Fire Chiefs; Rio Tinto; Sleep HealthCenters; University of Connecticut Health Center; UMass Memorial; University of Manchester; University of Texas Medical Branch; Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc.; Warwick Medical School; Woolcock Institute of Medical Research; Wyle Integrated Science and Engineering (NASA). SWL has completed investigator-initiated research grants from Alcon Inc. and Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc., and has ongoing investigator-initiated research grants from Biological Illumination LLC, Philips Lighting, and Philips Respironics Inc. He has completed service agreements with Rio Tinto Iron Ore and Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc., and has completed three sponsor-initiated clinical research contracts with Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc. Dr Lockley holds a process patent for the use of short-wavelength light for resetting the human circadian pacemaker and improving alertness and performance which is assigned to the Brigham and Women's Hospital per Hospital policy. He has also received minor revenue from a patent on the use of short-wavelength light which is assigned to the University of Surrey. Dr. Lockley has served as a paid expert on behalf of seven public bodies and one union in arbitrations in relation to sleep, circadian rhythms and work hours. Dr Rajaratnam is a Program Leader for the CRC for Alertness, Safety and Productivity, Australia, which funded this work. SMWR reports grants from Vanda Pharmaceuticals, Philips Respironics, Cephalon, Rio Tinto and Shell, and has received equipment support and consultancy fees through his institution from Optalert, Tyco Healthcare, Compumedics, Mental Health Professionals Network, and Teva Pharmaceuticals, which are not related to this paper. Dr Phillips is an investigator in projects for the CRC for Alertness, Safety and Productivity, Australia, which funded this work, and he has received research funding from Versalux and Delos.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Golombek DA, Rosenstein RE. Physiology of circadian entrainment. Physiol. Rev. 2010;90:1063–1102. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00009.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stone JE, et al. Computational approaches for individual circadian phase prediction in field settings. Curr. Opin. Syst. Biol. 2020;22:39–51. doi: 10.1016/j.coisb.2020.07.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jewett ME, Forger DB, Kronauer RE. Revised limit cycle oscillator model of human circadian pacemaker. J. Biol. Rhythms. 1999;14:493–499. doi: 10.1177/074873049901400608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.St. Hilaire MA, et al. Addition of a non-photic component to a light-based mathematical model of the human circadian pacemaker. J. Theor. Biol. 2007;247:583–599. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2007.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kronauer RE, Forger DB, Jewett ME. Quantifying human circadian pacemaker response to brief, extended, and repeated light stimuli over the phototopic range. J. Biol. Rhythms. 1999;14:500–515. doi: 10.1177/074873099129001073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mott C, Dumont G, Boivin DB, Mollicone D. Model-based human circadian phase estimation using a particle filter. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2011;58:1325–1336. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2011.2107321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kolodyazhniy V, et al. Estimation of human circadian phase via a multi-channel ambulatory monitoring system and a multiple regression model. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2011;26:55–67. doi: 10.1177/0748730410391619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kolodyazhniy V, et al. An improved method for estimating human circadian phase derived from multichannel ambulatory monitoring and artificial neural networks. Chronobiol. Int. 2012;29:1078–1097. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.700669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gil EA, Aubert XL, Møst EI, Beersma DG. Human circadian phase estimation from signals collected in ambulatory conditions using an autoregressive model. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2013;28:152–163. doi: 10.1177/0748730413484697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Woelders, T., Beersma, D. G. M., Gordijn, M. C. M., Hut, R. A. & Wams, E. J. Daily light exposure patterns reveal phase and period of the human circadian clock. J. Biol. Rhythms, 748730417696787 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Chang AM, Reid KJ, Gourineni R, Zee PC. Sleep timing and circadian phase in delayed sleep phase syndrome. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2009;24:313–321. doi: 10.1177/0748730409339611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Third Edition. 3rd edn, (AASM, 2014).
- 13.Morgenthaler T, et al. Practice parameters for the use of actigraphy in the assessment of sleep and sleep disorders: An update for 2007. Sleep. 2007;30:519–529. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.4.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Murray JM, et al. Prevalence of circadian misalignment and its association with depressive symptoms in delayed sleep phase disorder. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw002. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Micic G, Lovato N, Gradisar M, Lack LC. Personality differences in patients with delayed sleep–wake phase disorder and non-24-h sleep–wake rhythm disorder relative to healthy sleepers. Sleep Med. 2017;30:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pandi-Perumal SR, et al. Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO): A tool for the analysis of circadian phase in human sleep and chronobiological disorders. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;31:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Burgess HJ, et al. The relationship between the dim light melatonin onset and sleep on a regular schedule in young healthy adults. Behav. Sleep Med. 2003;1:102–114. doi: 10.1207/S15402010BSM0102_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wright KP, Jr, Gronfier C, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Intrinsic period and light intensity determine the phase relationship between melatonin and sleep in humans. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2005;20:168–177. doi: 10.1177/0748730404274265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sletten TL, Vincenzi S, Redman JR, Lockley SW, Rajaratnam SM. Timing of sleep and its relationship with the endogenous melatonin rhythm. Front. Neurol. 2010;1:137. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2010.00137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Flynn-Evans EE, et al. Circadian phase and phase angle disorders in primary insomnia. Sleep. 2017;40(12):zsx163. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wyatt JK. Delayed sleep phase syndrome: Pathophysiology and treatment options. Sleep. 2004;27:1195–1203. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.6.1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.von Schantz M, Skene DJ. Telling biological time from a blood sample: Current capabilities and future potential. Ann. Clin. Biochem. 2015;52:699–701. doi: 10.1177/0004563215597943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kasukawa T, et al. Human blood metabolite timetable indicates internal body time. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:15036–15041. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207768109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Laing EE, et al. Blood transcriptome based biomarkers for human circadian phase. Elife. 2017;6:e20214. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dijk DJ, Lockley SW. Integration of human sleep-wake regulation and circadian rhythmicity. J. Appl. Physiol. 2002;92:852–862. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lovato N, et al. Can the circadian phase be estimated from self-reported sleep timing in patients with delayed sleep wake phase disorder to guide timing of chronobiologic treatment? Chronobiol. Int. 2016;33:1376–1390. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1220386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Duffy JF, et al. Peak of circadian melatonin rhythm occurs later within the sleep of older subjects. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 2002;282:E297–303. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00268.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wyatt JK, Stepanski EJ, Kirkby J. Circadian phase in delayed sleep phase syndrome: Predictors and temporal stability across multiple assessments. Sleep. 2006;29:1075–1080. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.8.1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Crowley SJ, Acebo C, Fallone G, Carskadon MA. Estimating dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) phase in adolescents using summer or school-year sleep/wake schedules. Sleep. 2006;29:1632–1641. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.12.1632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stone JE, et al. Application of a limit-cycle oscillator model for prediction of circadian phase in rotating night shift workers. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:1–2. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47290-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Phillips AJK, et al. Irregular sleep/wake patterns are associated with poorer academic performance and delayed circadian and sleep/wake timing. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Micic G, et al. The endogenous circadian temperature period length (tau) in delayed sleep phase disorder compared to good sleepers. J. Sleep Res. 2013;22:617–624. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Watson LA, et al. Increased sensitivity of the circadian system to light in delayed sleep-wake phase disorder. J. Physiol. 2018;596:6249–6261. doi: 10.1113/JP275917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Keijzer H, Smits MG, Duffy JF, Curfs LM. Why the dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) should be measured before treatment of patients with circadian rhythm sleep disorders. Sleep Med. Rev. 2014;18:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2013.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lockley SW. Timed melatonin treatment for delayed sleep phase syndrome: The importance of knowing circadian phase. Sleep. 2005;28:1214–1216. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.10.1214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Duffy, J. F. et al. Workshop report. Circadian rhythm sleep–wake disorders: gaps and opportunities. Sleep. 10.1093/sleep/zsaa281 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37.Asgari-Targhi Ameneh, Klerman Elizabeth B. Mathematical modeling of circadian rhythms. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Systems Biology and Medicine. 2019;11(2):e1439. doi: 10.1002/wsbm.1439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Breslow, E. R., Phillips, A. J. K., Huang, J. M., Hilaire, M. A. St. & Klerman, E. B. A mathematical model of the circadian phase-shifting effects of exogenous melatonin. J. Biol. Rhythms28(1), 79–89. 10.1177/0748730412468081 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 39.Watson LA, et al. Sleep and circadian instability in delayed sleep-wake phase disorder. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2020;16:1431–1436. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.8516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Phillips AJK, et al. High sensitivity and interindividual variability in the response of the human circadian system to evening light. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. (USA) 2019;116:12019–12024. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820989116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Aoki H, Ozeki Y, Yamada N. Hypersensitivity of melatonin suppression in response to light in patients with delayed sleep phase syndrome. Chronobiol. Int. 2001;18:263–271. doi: 10.1081/CBI-100103190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Stone JE, et al. The role of light sensitivity and intrinsic circadian period in predicting individual circadian timing. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2020;35:628–640. doi: 10.1177/0748730420962598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lucas RJ, et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lockley SW, Brainard GC, Czeisler CA. High sensitivity of the human circadian melatonin rhythm to resetting by short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2003;88:4502–4505. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-030570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cain, S. W. et al. Evening home lighting adversely impacts the circadian system and sleep. Sci. Rep.10(1). 10.1038/s41598-020-75622-4 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 46.Sletten TL, et al. Efficacy of melatonin with behavioural sleep-wake scheduling for delayed sleep-wake phase disorder: A double-blind, randomized clinical trial. PLoS Med. 2018;15:e1002587. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Murray Jade M., Phillips Andrew J.K., Magee Michelle, Sletten Tracey L., Gordon Christopher, Lovato Nicole, Bei Bei, Bartlett Delwyn J., Kennaway David J., Lack Leon C., Grunstein Ronald R., Lockley Steven W., Rajaratnam Shantha M.W., Armstrong E., Chohan K., Djavadkhani Y., Dodds K., Gunaratnam S., Hardy M., Joosten S., Lee J., Micic G., Raman B., Roese E., Salkeld M., Verberne E., Wong K., Yee B., Yeo A., Yu K. Sleep regularity is associated with sleep-wake and circadian timing, and mediates daytime function in Delayed Sleep-Wake Phase Disorder. Sleep Medicine. 2019;58:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Revised: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. 2nd edn, (AASM, 2005).
- 49.Horne J, Ostberg O. A self assessment questionnaire to determine morningness eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Smith CS, Reilly C, Midkiff K. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J. Appl. Psychol. 1989;74:728–738. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.5.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Price L, Khazova M, O'Hagan J. Performance assessment of commercial circadian personal exposure devices. Light Res. Technol. 2012;44:17–26. doi: 10.1177/1477153511433171. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Scheuermaier K, Laffan AM, Duffy JF. Light exposure patterns in healthy older and young adults. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2010;25:113–122. doi: 10.1177/0748730410361916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hatonen T, Alila-Johansson A, Mustanoja S, Laakso ML. Suppression of melatonin by 2000-lux light in humans with closed eyelids. Biol. Psychiatry. 1999;46:827–831. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00357-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Klerman EB, Gershengorn HB, Duffy JF, Kronauer RE. Comparisons of the variability of three markers of the human circadian pacemaker. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2002;17:181–193. doi: 10.1177/074873002129002474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Khalsa SB, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, Czeisler CA. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J. Physiol. 2003;549:945–952. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.040477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yoon I-Y, et al. Age-related changes of circadian rhythms and sleep-wake cycles. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2003;51:1085–1091. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51356.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Duffy JF, et al. Sex difference in the near-24-hour intrinsic period of the human circadian timing system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108(Suppl 3):15602–15608. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010666108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Burgess HJ, Eastman CI. A late wake time phase delays the human dim light melatonin rhythm. Neurosci. Lett. 2006;395:191–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Baehr EK, Revelle W, Eastman CI. Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: With an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. J. Sleep Res. 2000;9:117–127. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00196.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Benloucif S, et al. Stability of melatonin and temperature as circadian phase markers and their relation to sleep times in humans. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2005;20:178–188. doi: 10.1177/0748730404273983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Brown EN, Choe Y, Shanahan TL, Czeisler CA. A mathematical model of diurnal variations in human plasma melatonin levels. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 1997;272:E506–E516. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1997.272.3.E506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Materials and data in this publication can be requested via the CRC for Alertness, Safety and Productivity (Alertness CRC) by emailing inquiries@alertnesscrc.com.