Skip to main content
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health logoLink to International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
. 2021 May 15;18(10):5277. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18105277

Infant Cognitive Development and Stimulating Parenting Practices in Rural China

Hannah Johnstone 1, Yi Yang 1, Hao Xue 1,*, Scott Rozelle 1
Editor: Cheng-Fang Yen1
PMCID: PMC8155833  PMID: 34063561

Abstract

This study examines the prevalence of cognitive delay among infants and toddlers in rural China and its relationship with one of the potential sources of the observed delay: low levels of stimulating parenting practices (SPPs). Data were compiled from five distinct studies, resulting in a pooled sample of 4436 caregivers of 6–29-month-old infants. The sampling sites span five provinces in rural China. According to the data, on average, rates of delay are high—51 percent. The low rates of SPPs among our sample demonstrate that this may be one source of the high prevalence of delays. The results of the multivariate regression analysis reveal that reading books and singing songs are each significantly associated with an increase in infant cognitive score by 1.62 points (p = 0.003) and 2.00 points (p < 0.001), respectively. Telling stories to infants, however, is not significantly associated with infant cognitive scores. Our findings indicate that caregivers with different characteristics engage in various levels of stimulating practices and have infants with different rates of delay. Specifically, infants of better-educated mothers who have greater household assets are in families in which the caregivers provide more SPPs and have infants who score higher on the study’s cognitive abilities scales.

Keywords: infant and toddler cognitive development, infant and toddler stimulation, stimulative parenting, rural China

1. Introduction

Early childhood development research in developed countries has increasingly focused on measuring the relationship between stimulating parenting practices (SPPs) and childhood development, with many studies finding positive associations between SPPs and the development of the basic cognitive functions of their children [1]. The majority of this research has been conducted in subpopulations in developed countries, using samples from relatively affluent and healthy populations (with relatively low percentages, or normal shares, of populations with cognitive delays). These studies have shown that physical and mental stimulation are crucial to the development and growth of cognitive, language, social-emotional, and motor skills in children and infants [2,3,4].

Less research on this topic has been done in developing nations. Worldwide, it is estimated that nearly 40% of children and infants are not reaching their potential for development [5], the majority of who are located in low-and-middle-income countries (LMICs). Although less research has been conducted in LMICs, the available literature similarly shows that quality parent-child engagement in the form of physical and verbal interactions has positive and significant associations with executive functions and performance intelligence in infants [6,7]. Moreover, childhood developmental outcomes have been shown to predict higher academic performance, future earnings for individuals, and greater national economic growth of populations [8].

While the developmental studies referred to in the previous paragraph show that research on early childhood development has been conducted in LMICs, relatively few studies that examine the nature of development outcomes of infants have been conducted in rural China, despite the fact that a substantial percentage of China’s population lives in rural provinces. It is estimated that approximately 70% of all infants in China are born and reside in rural provinces or live in migrant communities in the suburbs of China’s cities [9].

Recent literature measuring infant cognitive delay in rural China has found that rates of cognitive delay during infancy are high and parental engagement in stimulating parenting practices is low [10,11]. These individual studies, however, were only conducted in relatively isolated communities (geographically) and are limited in sample size. Therefore, based on any one of the papers, it is difficult to draw general conclusions about infant development in rural China or the relationship between infant cognitive development and stimulating parenting practices.

There also has been scarce research that seeks to analyze the characteristics of the families and caregivers who are (and are not) engaging in SPPs either internationally or in rural China. In the international literature, a small set of studies focuses on identifying family characteristics associated with low levels of SPPs. One study determined that there was a significant positive relationship between caregiver educational attainment and SPPs [12]. Ghosh et al. (2015) identified a positive relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and SPPs [13]. Obradovic et al. (2016) found that when the primary caregiver was the child’s mother (rather than the child’s grandmother or other caregiver types), the intensity of SPPs rose [7]. To our knowledge, however, no study in rural China has identified and analyzed the factors associated with poor SPPs as one of its main focal points.

The overall goal of our paper is to measure the prevalence of development delays in rural China and then to identify the factors and outcomes associated with SPPs across multiple provinces in rural China. To achieve this goal, our study has four objectives. First, we determine the prevalence of cognitive delay in our sample areas. Next, we document the prevalence of SPPs (drawing on questions derived from the Family Care Index) across rural China. Third, we investigate the associations between different SPPs and cognitive development within our samples. Finally, we measure the relationship between family characteristics (characteristics of caregivers, their households, and infants) and the frequency and type of SPP.

2. Data and Methods

2.1. Pooled Dataset

In this section, we describe the characteristics of the studies that are pooled and used in this study, as well as give a brief description of each individual study. Next, we provide an overview of the sample selection processes, followed by a description of the data collection and survey instruments that were utilized. Finally, we present the statistical analysis used in this paper as well as explain the models that were implemented to examine the data.

The data used in this study were aggregated from five studies across China and will be referred to as the pooled dataset. Every study included in the pooled dataset used the same measures to assess cognitive outcomes, using the Bayley-III scale (with the exception of study 1, which used the Bayley-I MDI scale) and SPPs (reading books, singing songs, and telling stories). Taken together, the pooled dataset included observations on caregiver-infant/toddler dyads from five provinces, 34 counties, and 4436 individuals. A summary of the five studies with eight study sites and corresponding publications is presented in Appendix A Table A1.

2.2. Individual Datasets

Study 1 was carried out in Shaanxi, and data collection was conducted during a four-week period in 2014. Household surveys were administered to the caregivers of 525 randomly selected infants and toddlers. The data collection effort covered 131 villages across four nationally designated poverty counties in southern Shaanxi Province. In 2015, research teams carried out study 2 (Yunnan and Hebei), for which surveys were administered to 440 caregiver-infant/toddler dyads who were living in 43 villages across two designated poverty counties (one in each of the study provinces). The second Shaanxi study (study 3) was conducted in 2016. Data collection teams administered surveys to 1832 randomly selected households in 100 villages across 22 nationally designated poverty counties. Study 4 was carried out in 2017, for which surveys were conducted in three provinces, Beijing, Henan, and Shaanxi. Although a share of the families who were surveyed was chosen from subpopulations that were living in urban migrant communities, all of the respondents were rural residents (had rural hukou). In total, 625 caregiver-infant/toddler dyads were selected across the three provinces. The final dataset (study 5) was collected in Yunnan in 2019. Research teams administered surveys to 1014 randomly selected caregiver-infant/toddler dyads across 224 villages in two townships located in a nationally designated poverty county in Yunnan.

2.3. Sample Selection

Studies 1, 2, 3, and 5 used nearly identical four-step sampling protocols to select sample households for the individual studies. First, the research teams selected nationally designated poverty counties. Second, the research teams gathered a list of all townships, excluding those that housed the county seat (as those tend to be more urbanized) and those without villages with a population of 800 or more. Next, the sample villages were randomly selected. Finally, the local bureau of health provided lists of households with infants and toddlers in each of the sample villages in the desired age range. From those lists, the teams selected all infants and toddlers within the village.

The sample selection for study 4 varied somewhat from the other studies, although the targeted infants and toddlers were the same (that is, they were from families with rural hukou). In counties of Henan province, sample towns and villages were randomly chosen from a comprehensive list of all towns and villages. Next, the researchers randomly chose households with infants and toddlers aged 6 to 30 months. To obtain a sampling from Beijing and Shaanxi, the research team visited urban government offices (the metropolitan Family Planning offices) and obtained lists of neighborhoods that were known to house migrant communities with rural hukou. From this list of neighborhoods, the researchers randomly selected migrant households with infants aged 6 to 30 months.

While it is true that the sample migrant households from study 4 were living in migrant communities (and not in rural villages), all sample families in this study had rural hukou, i.e., rural residency status [14]. Although relatively little is known about ECD in rural migrant communities, studies have found that migrant children have similar scores on cognitive scales when they are school-aged [15,16]. Given these two sets of findings, we believe that the migrant sample of infants is suitable for our purposes of examining rates of cognitive delay.

2.4. Data Collection and Survey Instruments

Although the data are aggregated from multiple surveys, all five surveys used similar data collection strategies. In each survey, questionnaires were distributed by local undergraduate and graduate students who were recruited from academic departments by the research teams relevant to the content of the survey. All students underwent comprehensive, multi-day training for each survey.

The first survey block collected information on the cognitive development of sampled infants, using the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition (BSID-III). Enumerators attended a week-long training course on how to administer the BSID-III, which included a two and a half day experiential learning program in the field. Accompanied by their primary caregiver, infants completed a standardized series of developmental tasks (which included items such as attention to familiar and unfamiliar objects, looking for a fallen object, and pretend play) to assess their performance as related to information processing, counting, and number skills [17]. The test also considered the infant’s gestational and chronological age. This test is listed by the American Psychiatric Association as a standard way to diagnose certain developmental disorders and has been translated and adapted to the Chinese context. The BDSI-III scale used in China has been tested for inter-item consistency, test-retest reliability, and inter-rater reliability, and it has been shown to be reliable and valid [17].

The BSID-III data allowed the research teams to measure the rates of cognitive delay in the samples. Cognitive delay was defined according to documented distributions of BSID scores in reference populations. In a healthy population, the mean cognitive score is 105 with a standard deviation (SD) of 9.6 [18]. Mild delay is defined as a score between one and two SDs below the mean. An infant is considered to have mild cognitive delay if the infant scores between 85.8 and 95.4 (inclusive). A moderate or severe delay is defined as a score that is more than two SDs below the mean. Infants with scores below 85.8 are considered moderately or severely delayed. In this study, cognitive delay (in general) is defined as a score below 95.4 [18].

In the second part of the survey, enumerators collected additional survey data from the sample households. For this part of the survey, enumerators underwent a three-day training course on how to administer household surveys, after which all enumerators participated in a two day pilot program in non-sampled villages to practice survey presentation. During the survey, enumerators presented each household with a standardized survey questionnaire and read aloud each question as it was written. For example, to assess a household’s SES, enumerators asked families whether they possessed each of the following ten items: a flush toilet, running water, car, motorcycle, refrigerator, water boiler, laundry machine, computer, internet, and air conditioning. These questions were adapted from the Family Care Indicators (FCI) survey. The FCI has been shown to have adequate test-retest reliability. Previous studies have shown significant relationships between FCI and cognitive development in LMICs [19]. This indicator has been translated and adapted for use in the context of rural China [20,21]. Subsets of FCI measures are commonly used in studies associated with child development [22,23,24,25,26].

The last survey block collected demographic and socioeconomic information from all sample households. This included each infant’s gender and age and whether the infant was born prematurely. The data collected also contained socioeconomic information, including whether the primary caregiver was the mother, mother’s educational attainment, mother’s age, household SES, and whether the household received a rural subsistence allowance. In this study, household SES was determined using an asset-based measure. Enumerators asked families whether they possessed each of the following ten items: a flush toilet, running water, car, motorcycle, refrigerator, water boiler, laundry machine, computer, internet, and air conditioning. For each item, a dummy variable was generated to indicate an absence or possession of each of the listed items, where 0 means absence and 1 means possession. SES was an interval measure obtained by adding the values of each of those 10 measures and running a principal component analysis to generate a standardized composite scale from −0.53 to 2.53, with an SD of 1 as a proxy for family asset index (the higher the number, the higher the SES).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Our statistical analysis is comprised of three parts. First, we run descriptive statistics of our samples and break them down by province to show the prevalence of cognitive scores, cognitive delays, and SPPs across rural communities. To examine the prevalence of cognitive delay and SPPs, we first report descriptive statistics from the pooled dataset as a whole. Following, we report the prevalence of cognitive delay and SPPs for each dataset.

Second, we use multivariate regression analysis to examine the associations between different SPPs and infant cognitive developmental outcomes within the samples. Equations (1) and (2) define the specifications of the multivariate regressions that we use to demonstrate cognitive outcomes are associated with SPPs:

Cogi=α+β1SPPi+β2Ci+β3Hi+β4FE+ε (1)
Cogi=α4+γ2PP+β41SES+β42CE+Xiθ+ε (2)

where Cogi  stands for the cognitive score of infant i measured by the Bayley-III cognitive test, which is a continuous number that ranges from 49 to 145; SPPi is a dummy variable that indicates whether the caregiver did (or did not) engage in one of the SPPs; Ci is infant i’s characteristics, including the infant’s age in months, the infant’s gender, and whether the infant is premature; Hi is vector that includes the household characteristics, including mother’s age, mother’s educational attainment, household SES, and whether the household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao); FE is province and year fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level.

Third and finally, we used multivariate regression analysis to examine the relationship between the frequency of SPPs and the study’s control variables:

SPP=α1+β11SES+β12CE+X θ+ε (3)

where SPP denotes the frequency of SPPs, and α is the constant. In all equations, SES is socioeconomic status, and CE is mother’s educational attainment. In all equations we control for province and year fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. The term Xi is a vector of covariates that are included to capture characteristics of infants, such as gender, age, premature birth, and province, and ε is an error term for variance not captured by those variables in the model PP (which is a binary outcome, where 1 indicates presence of the specific SPP, and 0 indicates absence of such a practice), which represents three different measures of SPP: reading books, singing songs, and telling stories, each separately modeled. The regressors are indicated by Bji, where i is the coefficient of regressor i in equation j.

In addition to our regression analysis, we also conduct a structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis to check for robustness. Our SEM analysis followed the guidelines of Schumacker and Lomax (2016) [27].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Characteristics

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of the infants and caregivers in our sample. This table comprises five studies with eight study sites for the survey respondents in five provinces. In total, the combined sample includes 4436 caregiver-infant/toddler dyads. The average age of the sampled infants across the eight study sites was 15 months, approximately half were male (52%), and only 5% of the sampled infants were born prematurely. The majority of primary caregivers were mothers (72%), and the average age of our sampled mothers was 28 years. These mothers had relatively lower levels of education, with the average level of educational attainment across all of the observations being junior high school (nine years of schooling). Within their communities, about 16% of households received a rural subsistence allowance.

Table 1.

Household and infant characteristics.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)
All Shaanxi 2014 Yunnan 2015 Hebei 2015 Shaanxi 2016 Beijing 2017 Henan 2017 Shaanxi 2017 Yunnan 2019
(N = 4436) (N = 525) (N = 210) (N = 230) (N = 1832) (N = 81) (N = 271) (N = 273) (N = 1014)
Panel A: household characteristics
The primary caregiver is mother (1 = yes) 3179 350 164 185 1286 60 170 167 797
0.72 0.67 0.78 0.80 0.70 0.74 0.63 0.61 0.79
Mother’s age (years) 27.5 26.42 26.81 29.15 27.98 28.52 27.62 28.61 26.55
5.06 4.55 5.43 4.81 5.01 3.96 4.13 4.2 5.59
Mother’s years of schooling (years) 9.09 8.51 8.50 9.97 9.22 13.1 12.7 12.6 6.87
3.56 2.53 3.10 2.94 2.64 2.51 3.08 2.85 4.14
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 2139 120 17 174 930 74 260 160 404
0.48 0.23 0.081 0.76 0.51 0.91 0.96 0.59 0.40
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) 831 145 31 24 202 6 19 33 371
0.19 0.28 0.15 0.10 0.11 0.07 0.07 0.12 0.37
Panel B: infant characteristics
Infant age (months) 16.3 24.5 11.7 13.1 14.5 18.2 20.3 18.8 15.3
6.79 3.20 3.62 3.14 5.47 9.21 9.52 8.75 5.59
Male infant (1 = yes) 2284 251 111 125 944 44 152 133 524
0.51 0.48 0.53 0.54 0.52 0.54 0.56 0.49 0.52
Infant is premature (1 = yes) 249 56 9 10 85 4 17 20 48
0.06 0.11 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.07 0.05

Notes: Data are frequency and percent for binary variables (1 = yes; 0 = no), and mean and standard deviation for numeric variables (years or months).

3.2. Cognitive Scores

Table 2 presents the cognitive scores and rates of cognitive delays among infants and toddlers across the eight study sites (Columns 2–9). Row 1, Columns 2 to 9 present the average cognitive score of each dataset. On average, the infant cognitive score ranges from 103.8 to 83.7, with the weighted average across all pooled datasets being 95.6. Row 2, Columns 2 to 9 provide the average cognitive delay of the infants and toddlers in each study (for which an infant is counted as delayed if his or her score is below 95.4 and where delay is defined as a score that is less than −1 SD below the international mean). Of the 4436 sampled infants and toddlers, between 37% and 54% are measured as having cognitive delay. The weighted average across the pooled datasets shows that 51% of infants and toddlers suffer from cognitive delay (Row 2, Column 1). Rows 3 and 4 indicate that, out of the 51% of infants and toddlers with cognitive delay, an average of 30% are mildly delayed (with scores between 95.4 and 85.8), and as many as 21% are moderately to moderately/severely delayed (with scores below 85.8).

Table 2.

Prevalence of cognitive delay.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)
All Shaanxi 2014 Yunnan 2015 Hebei 2015 Shaanxi 2016 Beijing 2017 Henan 2017 Shaanxi 2017 Yunnan 2019
(N = 4436) (N = 525) (N = 210) (N = 230) (N = 1832) (N = 81) (N = 271) (N = 273) (N = 1014)
Cognitive score 95.62 83.72 95.93 100.61 95.91 98.70 103.84 98.42 96.87
15.02 20.78 12.61 12.02 12.57 12.54 13.77 16.74 13.15
Cognitive delay (1 = yes) 2225 221 119 93 988 38 101 139 526
0.50 0.42 0.57 0.40 0.54 0.47 0.37 0.51 0.52
Mild delay (1 = yes) 1247 81 73 61 538 24 78 78 314
0.28 0.15 0.35 0.27 0.29 0.30 0.29 0.29 0.31
Moderate-severe delay (1 = yes) 978 140 46 32 450 14 23 61 212
0.22 0.27 0.22 0.14 0.25 0.17 0.08 0.22 0.21
Moderate-severe delay (1 = yes) 978 140 46 32 450 14 23 61 212
0.22 0.27 0.22 0.14 0.25 0.17 0.08 0.22 0.21

Notes: a Data are frequency and percent for binary variables (1 = yes; 0 = no), and mean and standard deviation for numeric variables (years or months). b The mean score (standard deviation) of Bayley III scales of Infant Development (BSID-III) is expected to be 105 (9.6) for the cognitive scale in a healthy population. Mild delay is defined as a score between one and two SDs below the mean. This translates to a score between 85.8 (inclusive) and 95.4 on the cognitive scale. A moderate or severe delay is defined as a score that is more than two SDs below the mean, which is below 85.8 for the cognitive scale. c Bayley I Scales of Infant Development (BSID-I) was used for collecting data from Shaanxi in 2014 (Column2). For BSID-I, mild delay is defined as a score between 70 (inclusive) and 80 on the cognitive scale; A moderate or severe delay is defined as a score below 70 for the cognitive scale.

3.3. Stimulating Parenting Practices (SPPs)

Table 3 presents the findings for the prevalence of SPPs in which caregivers had engaged with their infants on the previous day (Columns 2–9). The results in Row 1 concern whether caregivers had read a book to their infant on the previous day. Across the pooled datasets, the results Columns 2 to 9 indicate that between 4% and 67% of caregivers had read a book to their infant, with the weighted average of the eight study sites indicating that only 22% of caregivers read a book to their infant. Rows 2 and 3 in Table 3 concern the prevalence of telling stories and singing songs with infants, respectively. As shown in Row 2, between 10% and 37% of caregivers told stories to their infants on the previous day. The weighted average of all datasets shows that only 17% of caregivers had told a story to their infant on the previous day (Column 1). Row 3 shows that between 2% and 64% of caregivers sang songs to their infants on the previous day. The weighted average across all datasets revealed that only 38% of caregivers sang songs to their infant on the previous day.

Table 3.

Prevalence of stimulating parenting practices.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)
All Shaanxi 2014 Yunnan 2015 Hebei 2015 Shaanxi 2016 Beijing 2017 Henan 2017 Shaanxi 2017 Yunnan 2019
(N = 4436) (N = 525) (N = 210) (N = 230) (N = 1832) (N = 81) (N = 271) (N = 273) (N = 1014)
Read book to infant
(1 = yes)
960 21 107 155 384 25 78 64 126
0.22 0.04 0.51 0.67 0.21 0.31 0.29 0.23 0.12
Told story to infant
(1 = yes)
742 57 20 42 338 30 66 71 118
0.17 0.11 0.10 0.18 0.18 0.37 0.24 0.26 0.12
Sang song to infant
(1 = yes)
1678 195 5 20 795 52 135 115 361
0.38 0.37 0.02 0.09 0.43 0.64 0.50 0.42 0.36
At least one practice
(1 = yes)
2168 207 113 162 923 58 157 130 418
0.49 0.39 0.54 0.70 0.50 0.72 0.58 0.48 0.41
Two or more practices
(1 = two or more practices)
848 50 17 36 414 33 79 78 141
0.19 0.10 0.08 0.16 0.23 0.41 0.29 0.29 0.14

Notes: a Data are frequency and percent. b The simulating parent practices (read book to infant, told story to infant, and sang song to infant) reported in columns 2–5 measured caregiver engagement in the previous day; the columns 6–9 measured caregiver engagement in the past three days.

Row 4 of Table 3 presents the share of caregivers who engaged in any of the three SPPs measured. The results indicate that 39% to 70% of caregivers engaged in at least one SPP on the previous day, with the weighted average of all datasets showing that approximately half (49%) of caregivers engaged in at least one SPP on the previous day. The share of caregivers who engaged in multiple SPPs is considerably lower (where multiple is defined as engagement in two or more parenting practices), with the data showing that 8% to 41% of caregivers engaged in multiple SPPs on the previous day, with the weighted average across all datasets as revealing that only 19% of caregivers engage in multiple SPPs.

3.4. Correlates of Cognitive Development

Table 4 provides the correlations between individual SPP (reading books, telling stories, singing songs, engaging in at least one SPP, engaging in two or more SPPs) and infant cognitive development, holding household characteristics constant. Rows 1 to 3 indicate that reading books and singing songs are each significantly associated with an increase in infant cognitive score by 1.62 points (p = 0.003) and 2.00 points (p < 0.001), respectively. Telling stories to infants, however, is not significantly associated with infant cognitive scores.

Table 4.

Correlations between stimulating parenting practices and infant cognitive scores.

(1) (3) (3) (4) (5)
Dependent Variable: Infant Cognitive Score
β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p
Read book to infant (1 = yes) 1.62 ** 0.56, 2.69 0.003
Told story to infant (1 = yes) 1.08 −0.06, 2.21 0.062
Sang song to infant (1 = yes) 2.00 *** 0.95, 3.05 <0.001
At least one practice (1 = yes) 2.35 *** 1.38, 3.32 <0.001
Two or more practices (1 = yes) 1.30 * 0.20, 2.40 0.020
The primary caregiver is mother −0.73 −1.78, 0.32 0.174 −0.63 −1.67, 0.42 0.238 −0.91 −1.95, 0.13 0.088 −1.01 −2.06, 0.03 0.057 −0.68 −1.73, 0.37 0.203
Mother’s age (years) 0.06 −0.03, 0.14 0.169 0.06 −0.02, 0.14 0.159 0.06 −0.02, 0.15 0.143 0.06 −0.02, 0.15 0.145 0.06 −0.03, 0.14 0.172
Mother’s years of schooling (years) 0.37 *** 0.22, 0.52 <0.001 0.38 *** 0.23, 0.53 <0.001 0.34 *** 0.19, 0.49 <0.001 0.33 *** 0.17, 0.48 <0.001 0.37 *** 0.23, 0.52 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 3.01 *** 2.07, 3.94 <0.001 3.04 *** 2.10, 3.98 <0.001 2.84 *** 1.93, 3.76 <0.001 2.81 *** 1.90, 3.72 <0.001 3.00 *** 2.07, 3.94 <0.001
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) −1.66 * −2.98, −0.34 0.014 −1.64 * −2.97, −0.32 0.015 −1.66 * −2.99, −0.32 0.015 −1.63 * −2.96, −0.31 0.016 −1.67 * −3.00, −0.34 0.014
Adjusted R-squared 0.12 0.12 0.13 0.13 0.12
Number of observations 4434 4434 4433 4433 4434

Notes: a Cognitive score was measured using the Bayley-III scale and Bayley I Scales of Infant Development (BSID-I). Scores in this sample ranged from 49 to 145. b Results of the ordinary least squares model, controlling for province and year fixed effect, are reported; standard errors are clustered at the village level. c Logistic regressions also adjust for three infant characteristics: infant age (months), male infant (1 = yes), and infant premature (1 = yes). d A generalized structural equation model (GSEM) was used to test robustness of measures and can be found in Appendix A Table A2. e * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. f Regression 1 examines the correlation between infant cognition and reading books, holding household characteristics constant. In the next four regressions (2–5) we look at the correlation between infant cognition and telling stories/singing songs/engaging in at least one SPP/engaging in two or more SPPs (respectively). All household characteristics are held constant. g A generalized structural equation model (GSEM) was used to test robustness of measures and can be found in Appendix A Table A2.

Rows 6 to 9 of Table 4 provide the results of an examination of the relationships between caregiver and household characteristics and infant cognitive scores. Although whether the caregiver was the mother and mother’s age are not associated with infant cognitive scores, both mother’s educational attainment and the level of household assets are each significantly positively correlated with infant cognitive scores. Each additional year of mother’s educational attainment is associated with an increase in infant cognitive score by 0.33–0.38 points (p < 0.001). Whether a household has high assets is associated with an increase in infant cognitive score by 2.81–3.04 points (p < 0.001). The results also show that receiving a rural subsistence allowance is associated with a decrease of infant cognitive score by 1.63–1.67 points (p = 0.014/0.016).

3.5. Correlates of Stimulating Parenting Practices (SPPs)

Table 5 presents the correlations between SPPs and household characteristics (Columns 1–5). As seen in row 1, the primary caregiver as the mother is significantly associated with reading books to infants, telling stories to infants, singing songs to infants, engaging in at least one practice, and engaging in high levels of SPPs (p < 0.001). Row 2 shows that mother’s age is significantly associated with reading books to infants (p = 0.001), telling stories to infants (p < 0.001), and engaging in high levels of SPPs (p < 0.001). Similarly, mother’s educational attainment and higher household assets are associated with each SPP, engaging in at least one practice, and engaging in two or more SPPs (all p-values < 0.01). Surprisingly, Row shows that households’ receiving a rural subsistence allowance is not correlated with any of the three individual stimulating practices (Columns 1–3), engaging in one parenting practice, or engaging in two or more SPPs (Columns 4 and 5).

Table 5.

Correlations between stimulating parenting practices and household characteristics.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)
Read Book to Infant Told Story to Infant Sang Song to Infant At Least One Practice Two or More Practices
β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p
The primary caregiver is mother 0.09 *** 0.07, 0.12 <0.001 0.04 *** 0.02, 0.07 0.001 0.16 *** 0.13,0.19 <0.001 0.18 *** 0.15, 0.21 <0.001 0.08 *** 0.05, 0.11 <0.001
Mother‘s age (years) 0.004 ** 0.001
0.006
0.001 0.004 ** 0.001
0.006
0.002 0.001 −0.002
0.004
−0.366 0.001 −0.002
0.004
0.514 0.005 *** 0.002
0.007
<0.001
Mother’s years of schooling (years) 0.02 *** 0.01, 0.02 <0.001 0.01 *** 0.01, 0.02 <0.001 0.03 *** 0.02, 0.03 <0.001 0.03 *** 0.02, 0.03 <0.001 0.02 *** 0.01, 0.02 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 0.07 *** 0.04, 0.09 <0.001 0.07 *** 0.04, 0.10 <0.001 0.11 *** 0.08, 0.14 <0.001 0.12 *** 0.09, 0.16 <0.001 0.08 *** 0.06, 0.11 <0.001
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) 0.00 −0.03, 0.03 0.864 −0.02 −0.06, 0.02 0.375 0.01 −0.02, 0.04 0.589 −0.01 −0.04, 0.02 0.516 0.01 −0.03, 0.04 0.638
Number of observations 4434 4434 4433 4434 4434

Notes: a Logistic regressions are used and marginal effects are reported in model (1) to (5). b Logistic regressions control for provinces and years fixed effects; standard errors are clustered at the village level. c Logistic regressions also adjust for three infant characteristics: infant age (months), male infant (1 = yes), and infant premature (1 = yes). d ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. e The coefficient and 95% CI of Mother’s age (years) are shown in three decimal digits because all of them are smaller than 0.01. f A generalized structural equation model (GSEM) was used to test robustness of measures and can be found in Appendix A Table A2.

For robustness, we also re-ran the analysis that created the results reported in Table 4 and Table 5 using an SEM approach. The SEM results are reported in Appendix A Table A2. As can be seen from comparing the results from the SEM analysis with results in Table 4 and Table 5, we find that they are almost the same.

4. Discussion

The high rates of development delays found across our pooled dataset are consistent with other studies that examine infant development in rural China and exceed rates of cognitive delay found in many other LMICs. Across the five provinces, 51% of infants in our sample suffered from cognitive delay. These findings are consistent with other infant development studies conducted in rural China, which found rates of infant delay to be approximately three times higher than those found globally in healthy populations (approximately 15%) [28,29,30,31]. Further, rates of infant cognitive delay in our sample even exceeded rates of delay found in many LMICs. A developmental analysis, using the Early Childhood Development Index, found that, among 99,222 infants across 35 LMICs, the average rate of cognitive delay was approximately 36% [32]. Another study, which used national health surveys, found that, among 330,613 children across 63 LMICs, the average rate of cognitive delay was only 25% [33]. Our findings indicate that not only are average rates of cognitive delay in our rural China samples significantly higher than rates found in healthy populations but also are higher than rates of cognitive delay found in some LMICs.

The pooled dataset revealed one possible source of the high rates of delay: There are relatively low rates of SPPs across our pooled dataset when compared to other LMICs. Our study found that, on average, only 24% of caregivers had recently read to their infant, 25% had told stories to their infant, and 48% had sung to their infant in the past three days. In comparison, one study of parenting practices across 38 LMICs found that 19% of all surveyed caregivers had recently told stories to their infant, and an average of 56% had recently sung songs to their infant in the past three days [34]. Another study conducted across 39 LMICs found that an average of 22% of mothers had read to their infant in the past three days [35]. Given the high rates of cognitive delay found across our pooled dataset, the rates of SPPs in our study are low, even when compared to a subset of other LMICs.

When examining the relationship between infant cognitive development and SPPs, our study found that reading books and singing songs were highly correlated with infant cognitive outcomes, which suggests that these activities encourage high levels of caregiver-infant engagement. Multiple studies that examine the relationship between reading books and early cognitive development have found that reading books to young children fosters lifelong cognitive functions, regardless of family SES [36,37,38]. Similarly, studies that examine the relationship between singing songs to infants and cognitive outcomes have found that early exposure to singing is beneficial to general, lifelong cognitive outcomes [39,40,41].

In addition to finding significant associations between reading books, singing songs, and infant cognitive outcomes, we also found that both caregiver engagement in one SPP and caregiver engagement in two or more SPPs were significantly related to infant cognition. Surprisingly, engagement in one SPP was found to have a stronger association with infant cognitive development than with engagement in two or more SPPs, which may indicate that the quality of engagement is more important than the quantity. This theory aligns with a previous intervention study that was conducted in rural China, which found that improvement in the quality of parenting skills significantly increased infant development [42]. Although our study has shown that the quantity of caregiver investment in rural China is positively associated with infant cognitive outcomes, future studies should consider assessing the quality of engagement and its relationship to infant cognitive development.

Although there are other possible reasons behind high rates of delay, such as economic constraints or time constraints, evidence from recent studies in rural areas of China points to knowledge constraints as the main factor behind low levels of SPPs and high rates of developmental delays. Although rural households typically have fewer economic resources than urban households, recent studies have found that the majority of rural households have sufficient economic resources to invest in children’s development, and they have a desire to do so [10,43]. Moreover, in rural households in China, primary caregivers of young children typically do not work outside the home, meaning that unless they are caring for other family members (such as other children or elderly relatives), there are few time constraints that would prevent them from regularly engaging in SPPs with their children [43]. In contrast, multiple recent studies have found that rural caregivers do not have knowledge of how to effectively engage in SPPs, and rural caregivers often lack reliable sources of information about parenting [10,43,44], pointing to knowledge constraints as a primary cause for low levels of SPPs and high rates of delay.

Beyond examining SPPs and cognitive development, our analysis investigated the relationship between household characteristics, infant cognitive development, and SPPs. We found that whether the primary caregiver was the mother, mother’s age, and educational attainment were significantly positively associated with infant cognitive outcomes, engaging in all three SPPs (reading books, singing songs, telling stories), engaging in at least one practice, and engaging in two or more practices. These findings are consistent with the literature that shows that the education and age of caregivers are each positively associated with higher rates of parenting knowledge [45,46]. In turn, parenting knowledge has been linked to highly stimulating home environments, regardless of household SES [45,47]. Our findings indicate that, because our sampled mothers are less educated (junior high or lower), they may have limited exposure to parenting knowledge, and, thus, their engagement in SPPs is low. This also would suggest that future interventions specifically aimed at teaching caregivers essential parenting knowledge may be an effective way to increase SPP engagement, regardless of education or age.

5. Conclusions

This study is the first to examine large-scale developmental delays and SPPs across rural provinces in China. Our analysis of the parenting landscape in rural China used data from five studies with eight study sites, covering five rural provinces. Among our pooled sample of 4436 caregiver-toddler dyads, we examined the prevalence of infant cognitive delay, finding average rates of delay across these provinces to be higher than in many other LMICs. We also measured how SPPs and household characteristics relate to infant cognitive development. Finally, we assessed the relationship between caregiver and household characteristics and SPPs.

This paper has many strengths. It is the first to document rates of infant cognitive delay, SPPs, demographic characteristics, and their correlations across multiple datasets containing more than 4000 samples in rural China. The large sample size and the geographic range that our study covers provide evidence that cognitive delays are a problem across numerous rural communities in our sampled populations and provinces. This evidence can better inform policymakers about which types of families are at risk for high rates of cognitive delay and low SPPs and may be used to help inform community intervention projects.

We also acknowledge the limitations of this study. First, our study considers only cross-sectional data and is, therefore, unable to make causal claims. Second, given that China is an expansive and immensely diverse country with great regional disparities in indigenous culture and economic development, the five provinces in our study are not necessarily representative of all rural communities in China at a national level.

The results of this study suggest that increased investment in early childhood development and SPPs may have an important role in reducing cognitive delay in infants across rural China, which could be crucial to the future development of China’s economy. The literature shows that early cognitive delays have lifelong consequences, including lower levels of educational attainment and fewer career prospects and earnings [48,49]. On a larger scale, such widespread rates of cognitive delay are likely to hinder China’s human capital, which, in turn, could severely limit the future of China’s economy [50,51,52]. We recommend that future studies expand on ours by examining cognitive development and SPPs in rural communities in provinces that this study did not explore. Further research that identifies the extent of cognitive delays and SPPs across China will ultimately bring us closer to developing effective solutions and interventions for improving early childhood development across China’s most vulnerable populations.

Appendix A

Table A1.

Summary of studies included in the pooled dataset.

Study Location of the Study Date Corresponding Publication
Study 1 Shaanxi 2014 Sylvia, S., Warrinnier, N., Luo, R., Yue, A., Attanasio, O., Medina, A., Rozelle, S. From quantity to quality: delivering a home-based parenting intervention through China’s family planning cadres. Econ. J. 2021, 131, 1365–1400. doi:10.1093/ej/ueaa114 [42].
Study 2a Yunnan 2015 Luo, R., Jia, F., Yue, A., Zhang, L., Lyu, Q., Shi, Y., Yang, M., Medina, A., Kotb, S., Rozelle, S. Passive parenting and its association with early child development. Early Child Dev. Care. 2017, 189, 1–15. doi:10.1080/03004430.2017.1407318 [53].
Study 2b Hebei 2015
Study 3 Shaanxi 2016 Zhong, J., He, Y., Gao, J., Wang, T., Luo, R. Parenting knowledge, parental investments, and early childhood development in rural households in western China.” Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020, 17, 2792 [54].
Study 4a Beijing 2017 Wang, L., Liang, W., Zhang, S., Jonsson, L., Li, M., Yu, C., Sun, Y., Ma, Q., Bai, Y., Abbey, C., et al. Are infant/toddler developmental delays a problem across rural China? J. Comp. Econ. 2019, 47, 458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jce.2019.02.003 [55].
Study 4b Henan 2017
Study 4c Shaanxi 2017
Study 5 Yunnan 2019 Zhang, S., Wang, L., Xian, Y., Bai, Y. Mental health issues among caregivers of young children in rural China: prevalence, risk factors, and links to child developmental outcomes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021, 18, 197 [56].

Table A2.

Correlations between infant cognitive scores, stimulating parenting practices and household characteristics using generalized structural equation model (GSEM).

Variables Coefficient 95% CI p-Value
Panel A Dependent variable: infant cognitive score
Read book to infant (1 = yes) 2.67 *** 1.50, 3.84 <0.001
Told story to infant (1 = yes) −0.25 −1.39, 0.89 0.670
Sang song to infant (1 = yes) 1.12 * 0.07, 2.16 0.036
The primary caregiver is mother −1.31 * −2.34, −0.29 0.012
Mother’s age (years) 0.11 * 0.02, 0.20 0.015
Mother’s years of schooling 0.34 *** 0.16, 0.51 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 4.35 *** 3.34, 5.35 <0.001
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) −1.57 * −3.08, −0.06 0.042
Panel B Dependent variable: Read book to infant (1 = yes)
The primary caregiver is mother 0.10 *** 0.06, 0.13 <0.001
Mother’s age (years) 0.01 *** 0.00, 0.01 <0.001
Mother’s years of schooling 0.02 *** 0.01, 0.02 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 0.07 *** 0.04, 0.10 <0.001
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) −0.03 −0.07, 0.00 0.055
Panel C Dependent variable: Told story to infant (1 = yes)
The primary caregiver is mother 0.04 ** 0.02, 0.06 0.001
Mother’s age (years) 0.00 *** 0.00, 0.01 <0.001
Mother’s years of schooling 0.01 *** 0.01, 0.02 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 0.08 * 0.05, 0.11 0.05
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) −0.03 −0.06, 0.01 0.16
Panel D Dependent variable: Sang song to infant (1 = yes)
The primary caregiver is mother 0.16 0.12, 0.19 0.12
Mother’s age (years) 0.00 *** 0.00, 0.00 <0.001
Mother’s years of schooling 0.02 *** 0.02, 0.03 <0.001
Household has higher socioeconomic status (1 = yes) 0.12 *** 0.09, 0.16 <0.001
Household receives rural subsistence allowance (dibao) (1 = yes) 0.02 −0.02, 0.05 0.30

Notes: a Generalized structural equation model (GSEM) is used to estimate the correlation between infant cognitive scores, stimulating parenting practices and household characteristics. Standard errors account for clustering at the village level. b The GSEM also adjusts for three infant characteristics: infant age (months), male infant (1 = yes), and infant premature (1 = yes). c Logistic regressions are used, and marginal effects are reported in Panel B, C, and D. d * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.J. and Y.Y.; methodology, H.X.; formal analysis, H.X.; writing—original draft preparation, H.J. and Y.Y.; writing—review and editing, H.J., H.X., and S.R.; supervision, S.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The five datasets and the implemented surveys conducted by the authors and their colleagues were approved by the Stanford University Institutional Review Board (protocol code 25735, 2013; protocol code 35921, 2015; protocol code 50901, 2019). Study permissions also were obtained from the Chinese government.

Informed Consent Statement

All caregivers involved in the surveys provided oral consent.

Data Availability Statement

The Stata code and data permissible for sharing will be made available upon request from H.X. (haoxue@stanford.edu).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Fernald A.A. Getting beyond the convenience sample in research on early cognitive development. Behav. Brain Sci. 2010;33:91–92. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hamm E.M. Playfulness and the environmental support of play in children with and without developmental disabilities. OTJR Occup. Particip. Health. 2006;26:88–96. doi: 10.1177/153944920602600302. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.McEntire N. The Importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Child. Educ. 2009;85:208. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2009.10521395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Burriss K.G., Tsao L.L. Review of research: How much do we know about the importance of play in child development? Child. Educ. 2002;78:230–233. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2002.10522188. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Aboud F.E., Singla D.R., Nahil M.I., Borisova I. Effectiveness of a parenting program in Bangladesh to address early childhood health, growth and development. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;97:250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Aboud F.E., Yousafzai A.K. Global health and development in early childhood. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:433–457. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Obradovíc J., Yousafzai A.K., Finch J.E., Rasheed M.A. Maternal scaffolding and home stimulation: Key mediators of early intervention effects on children’s cognitive development. Dev. Psychol. 2016;52:1409–1421. doi: 10.1037/dev0000182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nonoyama-Tarumi Y., Ota Y. Early Childhood Development in Developing Countries: Pre-primary Education, Parenting, and Health Care. Background Paper Education for All. Global Monitoring Report. [(accessed on 18 June 2020)]; Available online: http://basiced.org/wpcontent/uploads/Publication_Library/UNESCOEarly_Childhood_Development_in_Developing_Countries-2010.pdf.
  • 9.National Bureau of Statistics of China. [(accessed on 10 December 2020)]; Available online: http://www.stats.gov.cn/english/
  • 10.Yue A., Shi Y., Luo R., Wang B., Weber A., Medina A., Kotb S., Rozelle S. Stimulation and early child development in china: Caregiving at arm’s length. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2019;40:458–467. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zhou S., Zhao C., Huang X., Li Z., Ye R., Shi H., Zhao Q., Zhou Y., Chen X., O’Sullivan M., et al. The effect of a community-based, integrated and nurturing care intervention on early childhood development in rural China. Public Health. 2019;167:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2018.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Peairson S., Austin A., de Aquino C.N., de Burro E.U. Cognitive development and home environment of rural Paraguayan infants and toddlers participating in Pastoral del Niño, an early child development program. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2008;22:343–362. doi: 10.1080/02568540809594632. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ghosh S., Chowdhury S.D., Chandra A.M., Ghosh T. Grades of undernutrition and socioeconomic status influence cognitive development in school children of Kolkata. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2015;156:274–285. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Liu Z. Institution and inequality: The hukou system in China. J. Comp. Econ. 2005;33:133–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jce.2004.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang X., Yuan Z., Min S., Rozelle S. School quality and peer effects: Explaining differences in academic performance between China’s migrant and rural students. J. Dev. Stud. 2020 doi: 10.1080/00220388.2020.1769074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zhao Q., Wang X., Rozelle S. Better cognition, better school performance? Evidence from primary schools in China. China Econ. Rev. 2019;55:199–217. doi: 10.1016/j.chieco.2019.04.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hua J., Li Y., Ye K., Ma Y., Lin S., Gu G., Du W. The reliability and validity of Bayley-III cognitive scale in China’s male and female children. Early Hum. Dev. 2019;129:71–78. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lowe J.R., Erickson S.J., Schrader R., Duncan A.F. Comparison of the Bayley II mental developmental index and the Bayley III cognitive scale: Are we measuring the same thing? Acta Paediatr. Int. J. Paediatr. 2012;101:55–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02517.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hamadani J.D., Tofail F., Hilaly A., Huda S.N., Engle P., Grantham-McGregor S.M. Use of family care indicators and their relationship with child development in Bangladesh. J. Health Popul. Nutr. 2010;28:23–33. doi: 10.3329/jhpn.v28i1.4520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wang B., Yue A. The Relationship between Family Environment and Early Child Development in Rural China. (REAP Work. Pap.) Stanford University; Stanford, CA, USA: 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wang L., Zheng I. Family Care and Early Childhood Development: Evidence from China’s Four Rural Subpopulations. (REAP Work. Pap.) Stanford University; Stanford, CA, USA: 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nakata T., Trehub S.E. Infants’ responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behav. Dev. 2004;27:455–464. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tamis-LeMonda C.S., Shannon J.D., Cabrera N.J., Lamb M.E. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: Contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004;75:1806–1820. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00818.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Karrass J., Braungart-Rieker J.M. Effects of shared parent-infant book reading on early language acquisition. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2005;26:133–148. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tomopoulos S., Dreyer B.P., Tamis-LeMonda C., Flynn V., Rovira I., Tineo W., Mendelsohn A.L. Books, toys, paren-child interaction, and development in young Latino Children. Ambul. Pediatr. 2006;6:72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Murray A., Egan S.M. Does reading to infants benefit their cognitive development at 9-months-old? An investigation using a large birth cohort survey. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2014;30:303–315. doi: 10.1177/0265659013513813. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Schumacker R., Lomax R. A Beginner’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. Routledge; New York, NY, USA: 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shi S., Shi J., Guan X., Zhang J., Hu M. Analysis of influential factors of infant development. Matern. Child Health Care China. 2001;16:635–637. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Xu M., Liu X., Du Y., Yang Y., Li Z. The analysis of infant’s mental and motor development level and influencing factors in the countryside of Shaanxi Province. J. Xian Jiaotong Univ. Med. Sci. 2009;30:181–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Boyle C.A., Decouflé P., Yeargin-Allsopp M. Prevalence and health impact of developmental disabilities in US children. Pediatrics. 1994;93:399–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Zhang L., Yi H., Luo R., Liu C., Rozelle S. The human capital roots of the middle income trap: The case of China. Agric. Econ. 2013;44:151–162. doi: 10.1111/agec.12059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McCoy D.C., Peet E.D., Ezzati M., Danaei G., Black M.M., Sudfeld C.R., Fawzi W., Fink G. Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: National, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13:e1002233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gil J.D.C., Ewerling F., Ferreira L.Z., Barros A.J.D. Early childhood suspected developmental delay in 63 low-and middle-income countries: Large within-and between-country inequalities documented using national health surveys. J. Glob. Health. 2020;10 doi: 10.7189/jogh.10.010427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bornstein M.H., Putnick D.L., Lansford J.E., Deater-Deckard K., Bradley R.H. A developmental analysis of caregiving modalities across infancy in 38 low- and middle-income countries. Child Dev. 2015;86:1571–1587. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bornstein M.H., Putnick D.L. Mothers’ and Fathers’ parenting practices with their daughters and sons in low- and middle-income countries. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2016;81:60–77. doi: 10.1111/mono.12226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Niklas F., Cohrssen C., Tayler C. The sooner, the better: Early reading to children. SAGE Open. 2016;6:1–11. doi: 10.1177/2158244016672715. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Habibi E., Sajedi F., Hatamizadeh N., Shahshahanipour S., Malek Afzali H. Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran. F1000Research. 2018;7:411. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.14078.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Houston S.M., Lebel C., Katzir T., Manis F.R., Kan E., Rodriguez G.G., Sowell E.R. Reading skill and structural brain development. Neuroreport. 2014;25:347–352. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schellenberg E.G. Music and cognitive Abilities. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2005;14:317–320. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00389.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Politimou N., Dalla Bella S., Farrugia N., Franco F. Born to speak and sing: Musical predictors of language development in pre-schoolers. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:948. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dumont E., Syurina E.V., Feron F.J.M., van Hooren S. Music interventions and child development: A critical review and further directions. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1694. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sylvia S., Warrinnier N., Luo R., Yue A., Attanasio O., Medina A., Rozelle S. From quantity to quality: Delivering a home-based parenting intervention through China’s family planning cadres. Econ. J. 2021;131:1365–1400. doi: 10.1093/ej/ueaa114. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Li R., Rose N., Zheng Y.M., Chen Y., Sylvia S., Wilson-Smith H., Medina A., Dill S.E., Rozelle S. Early childhood reading in rural china and obstacles to caregiver investment in young children: A mixed-methods analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021;18:1457. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Yue A., Zhang N., Liu X., Tang L., Luo R., Yang M., Rozelle S., Medina A. Do infant feeding practices differ between grandmothers and mothers in rural China? Evidence from rural Shaanxi province. Fam. Community Health. 2018;41:233–243. doi: 10.1097/FCH.0000000000000198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rowe M.L., Denmark N., Harden B.J., Stapleton L.M. The role of parent education and parenting knowledge in children’s language and literacy skills among White, Black, and Latino families. Infant Child Dev. 2016;25:198–220. doi: 10.1002/icd.1924. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bornstein M.H., Putnick D.L., Bradley R.H., Lansford J.E., Deater-Deckard K. Pathways among caregiver education, household resources, and infant growth in 39 low- and middle-income countries. Infancy. 2015;20:353–376. doi: 10.1111/infa.12086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Parks P.L., Smeriglio V.L. Relationships among parenting knowledge, quality of stimulation in the home and infant development. Fam. Relat. 1986;35:411. doi: 10.2307/584369. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Heckman J.J., Jacobs B. In: Perspectives on the Performance on the Continent’s Economies. Phelps E., Sinn H.W., editors. Vol. 1. MIT-Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2010. pp. 243–323. Chapter 9. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Gertler P., Heckman J., Pinto R., Zanolini A., Vermeersch C., Walker S., Chang S.M., Grantham-McGregor S. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344:998–1001. doi: 10.1126/science.1251178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Madsen J.B., Murtin F. British economic growth since 1270: The role of education. J. Econ. Growth. 2017;22:229–272. doi: 10.1007/s10887-017-9145-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Khor N., Pang L., Liu C., Chang F., Mo D., Loyalka P., Rozelle S. China’s looming human capital crisis: Upper secondary educational attainment rates and the middle-income trap. China Q. 2016;228:905–926. doi: 10.1017/S0305741016001119. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang L., Li M., Abbey C., Rozelle S. Human capital and the middle income trap: How many of China’s youth are going to high school? Dev. Econ. 2018;56:82–103. doi: 10.1111/deve.12165. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Luo R., Jia F., Yue A., Zhang L., Lyu Q., Shi Y., Yang M., Medina A., Kotb S., Rozelle S. Passive parenting and its association with early child development. Early Child Dev. Care. 2017;189:1–15. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1407318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Zhong J., He Y., Gao J., Wang T., Luo R. Parenting knowledge, parental investments, and early childhood development in rural households in western China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:2792. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wang L., Liang W., Zhang S., Jonsson L., Li M., Yu C., Sun Y., Ma Q., Bai Y., Abbey C., et al. Are infant/toddler developmental delays a problem across rural China? J. Comp. Econ. 2019;47:458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jce.2019.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Zhang S., Wang L., Xian Y., Bai Y. Mental health issues among caregivers of young children in rural China: Prevalence, risk factors, and links to child developmental outcomes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021;18:197. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The Stata code and data permissible for sharing will be made available upon request from H.X. (haoxue@stanford.edu).


Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES