Abstract
Understanding the mechanisms underlying the effects of behaviour change interventions is vital for accumulating valid scientific evidence, and useful to informing practice and policy-making across multiple domains. Traditional approaches to such evaluations have applied study designs and statistical models, which implicitly assume that change is linear, constant and caused by independent influences on behaviour (such as behaviour change techniques). This article illustrates limitations of these standard tools, and considers the benefits of adopting a complex adaptive systems approach to behaviour change research. It (1) outlines the complexity of behaviours and behaviour change interventions; (2) introduces readers to some key features of complex systems and how these relate to human behaviour change; and (3) provides suggestions for how researchers can better account for implications of complexity in analysing change mechanisms. We focus on three common features of complex systems (i.e., interconnectedness, non-ergodicity and non-linearity), and introduce Recurrence Analysis, a method for non-linear time series analysis which is able to quantify complex dynamics. The supplemental website provides exemplifying code and data for practical analysis applications. The complex adaptive systems approach can complement traditional investigations by opening up novel avenues for understanding and theorising about the dynamics of behaviour change.
Keywords: complex systems, wellbeing, methodology, behaviour change
1. Introduction
Behavioural interventions often fail to produce sustainable effects [1], especially when transferred from one context to another. One core interest of behaviour change science then, is to improve our understanding of interventions’ mechanisms of action. Behavioural theories identify hundreds of potential “determinants” of behaviour, that is, factors that potentially influence the behaviour of interest. These determinants constitute the channels through which behaviour change techniques might influence behaviour [2]. Determinants range from social cognitions such as self-efficacy and attitudes, to biological factors, and elements of the social and built environments in which behaviours take place [3]. When studied using typical factorial designs and linear statistical models, the relationships between causal precedents and behaviour change are assumed additive, constant and linear (i.e., the outputs are proportional to the inputs). However, it is our position that this offers researchers and the public an inaccurate and imprecise understanding of behaviour change. We should instead consider the relevant factors as complex, potentially non-linear, and dynamic.
The evaluation of behaviour change interventions often involves randomly assigning participants to receive an intervention of interest or a comparator, and measuring subjective and objective indicators of behaviour [4]. Usually, these measurements occur immediately before and after the delivery of the intervention, though sometimes additional follow-up measurements may take place weeks or months later. This is the classic Randomised Controlled Trial (RCT) design and the data from such studies are most often analysed using statistical techniques that are specific cases of the General Linear Model. In this paper, we refer to this as the “conventional approach.” To assess whether an intervention was more effective, on average, than a comparator, comparing averages in RCTs can be purposeful and acceptable. This method can answer questions such as “Does the intervention influence the target behaviour?”, and “Do cohorts differ from each other?” However, using so few measurement points to study behaviour change mechanisms (“How do intervention participants change?”) may present problems.
Currently, mechanisms of change within behaviour change interventions are typically studied using mediation analysis [5], where the impact of X (e.g., an intervention) on Y (e.g., a behavioural outcome) is modelled to pass through a third variable M (e.g., a theoretical determinant of the behavioural outcome targeted by the intervention). In the presence of classical mediation, the path X-Y would be reduced to near zero when adding M to the model. If this were observed, the researcher would conclude that there is evidence for mediation. In the case of behaviour change interventions then, one would conclude that the intervention (X) changed behaviour (Y) because it changed important theoretical determinants of the behaviour (M). For a case example from members of the current author group see [6].
Inferring mechanisms from contemporary mediation analyses is problematic on various grounds [7,8,9,10,11,12]. Of particular importance to human behaviour change, however, is that the accuracy of mediation analysis depends on four key assumptions [13]: (1) The number of variables involved is small, and dynamics can be meaningfully assessed with only a few time points; (2) The process of change is the same for all individuals, e.g., follows the same sequence; (3) The dynamic between variables is linear, additive, and does not change in time; and (4) The included variables are not entangled with the context, omitted variables, or each other in bi-directional recursive relationships. Researchers can, of course, include more variables (leading to new issues, e.g., mixing up mediators, confounders and colliders [14,15] or lowering the already worrisome statistical power [16]), try to incorporate non-linear effects [17], and add more time points (for caveats regarding latent variable modelling, see, e.g., [18,19]). However, limiting the notion of a mechanism to a (multiple) mediation/moderation problem narrows our understanding of how changes occur over time [20,21].
During the first two decades of the 21st century, behaviour change researchers began extending the traditional approach and embracing designs with an increased focus on temporal processes [22,23]. Recently, alternative solutions stemming from complex systems science [24,25] have become increasingly accessible and helpful in understanding change processes. We will explore these ideas and how they help us surpass traditional assumptions. In what follows, this paper will (1) outline the complexity of behaviours and behaviour change interventions; (2) introduce readers to some key features of complex systems and how these can be applied to human behaviour; and (3) provide concrete suggestions for how researchers can better account for the implications of complexity in analysing behaviour change mechanisms.
1.1. What Are Complex Systems?
A system is “a delineated part of the universe which is distinguished from the rest by an imaginary boundary” [26], although other definitions exist [27]. Many things—an airplane, a car, a robot, a central nervous system, a school, a community, a society—can be conceptualised as systems. This paper focuses on individual people, which are complex systems. Complex systems can be characterised as webs of many interdependent self-organising parts that operate without central control, whose interactions give rise to emergent properties and behaviours, irreducible to a sum of the parts [28]. The future behaviour of a complex system strongly depends on its unique history of interactions, that is, past experience. Additionally, the system adapts to its environment and actors therein, coevolving with each other to create macro-level behaviour. This dynamic is difficult to predict and is usually not changeable in a stepwise engineering sense [29]. The italicized characteristics above distinguish complex systems from those which are just complicated. Highly complicated systems (e.g., an airplane), unlike complex ones (e.g., an organism), cannot self-organise to function adaptively when one part is removed [30]. Guides to basic terminology of complexity for scientists working with health behaviours can be found in [30] as well as Table 1 of [29].
1.2. The Relevance of Complexity for Behaviour Change
The promise of complex adaptive systems approaches in health behaviour change research has been previously discussed by, e.g., [31], but over a decade ago, not many empirical solutions were easily accessible to investigators in this field. Recently, methods to study complex adaptive systems in health behaviour change have been presented [32], with a focus on simulation and qualitative methods. This article discusses novel quantitative solutions, which have recently become available, to investigate behaviour change phenomena with a complex systems lens.
To paint a picture of just how complex the behavioural world is, take the case of physical activity. Already three and a half decades ago, more than 30 influences on (or “determinants of”) physical activity were being considered, along with calls for better understanding of their dynamics, interactions, and the time scales over which these develop [33]. While any influence (e.g., intention, attitude) could have a direct relationship with physical activity, some rely on interactions with other influences to affect behaviour [34,35]. Furthermore, these interactions may be moderated by additional factors, and by other variables which themselves have no direct relationship with physical activity, with synergistic and opposing effects which may themselves depend on whether some threshold is exceeded. The extent to which all known (and unknown) influences on physical activity interact with one another presents a map of practically infinite, intertwined ‘routes’ to initiating and maintaining physical activity.
The role of time brings added complexity to this behavioural world, as dynamic patterns change over time and at varying frequencies [36,37]. For example, fluctuations in physical activity clearly occur within a day, as most individuals are (at least in the absence of highly sedentary working conditions and considerable somnambulism) more active while awake than while asleep. Fluctuation also occurs over the course of a week, as activity levels tend to be higher on weekdays than on weekends [38]; over the course of months, as activity levels are higher in warmer seasons and lower in colder ones [39] and over the course of years, as activity levels tend to decline with age [40]. How determinants—which are postulated to comprise the mechanisms underlying changes in behaviour—fluctuate and interact with the fluctuations in behaviour, is largely unknown.
Human behaviour is complex, and while we have formulated theoretical constructs to be as amenable as possible to linear methods of analysis, this may obscure important characteristics of behaviour change. Why are linear models inappropriate for many of our research questions in the behavioural sciences? First, with many non-linear interactions across time scales, mechanistic causality (including mediation and moderation) becomes suspect or intractable [19,41,42]. Second, traditional statistical analyses start from the simplification that everything is independent from everything else. In actuality, nearly everything eventually depends on everything else, contributing to what Paul E. Meehl [43] seminally coined as “the crud factor”. Jacob Cohen, the developer of power analysis, similarly exclaimed that (in the absence of randomisation), the nil hypothesis of no effect is always a priori false [44]. These well-known ideas demonstrate violations of the classical assumptions regarding independence and interference [45,46]. In the same vein, forecasting in complex systems is notoriously difficult [47,48,49], making hypothesis testing—which is, after all, the test of a prediction—in intervention evaluation, a curious challenge.
Complexity science starts from the assumption that everything is intertwined, and can provide us with new hypotheses that respect the complexity of the phenomena under study [50]. This is necessary, because a conventional linear analysis will only yield accurate results when underlying assumptions are met: that the components in the model are independent, with additive effects that can be decomposed and attributed to their causes (e.g., beta coefficients in multiple regression). If, on the other hand, these “component-dominant” dynamics are not driving the system, and instead, the effects are intertwined, overlapping and inseparable [51], the dynamics are “interaction-dominant”, and the replication and generalisation issues for results stemming from the linear analysis are almost inevitable [46]. Instead, intensive longitudinal methods are necessary to monitor how processes unfold. This information can then be used to dynamically tune interventions in real time, making success less dependent on having a correct program theory at the outset [52,53]. Made possible by N-of-1 methodologies [54], this goal has been recently pursued by using control systems engineering approaches [55] and just-in-time adaptive interventions [56], among others.
Although behaviour change maintenance has been theorised at length [1], existing theories do not incorporate complex systems principles, which might overcome the aforementioned issues. From the viewpoint of complex systems science, the effects of behaviour change interventions can be considered as shocks to the system in which they take place. The aim of the intervention shock is to alter the system’s status, pushing against existing forces to affect change [57,58]. This is akin to attempts to work against gravity, which pulls a ball to the bottom of a valley. In this example, the valley represents a relatively stable state, also known as an attractor (see Figure 1) [59,60]. Taking the analogy further, pushing the ball outside of the valley may lead it to roll off a peak, ending up in a deeper valley than where it started (i.e., a more stable, deep-rooted state). A complex systems perspective implies that, even in the event of a successful intervention, stabilizing a system in a more functional state may require at least as many resources as the initial change itself [61]. In general, while complex systems may often be impossible to control precisely, they can be stewarded approximately, while allowing for variability stemming from self-organisation to flourish instead of trying to iron it out [62,63]. The necessity of a complex systems approach is increasingly recognized, including within the UK Medical Research Council’s recently updated guidance for developing and evaluating complex interventions [64].
Having now undergone a brief conceptual introduction to complexity, we can describe behaviour change as “a collection of contextualised processes that are nontrivially specific to each individual, and which form a complex interconnected system that is not restricted to linear dynamics” (see [65], p. 4). We highlight three features of this definition:
A complex interconnected system: A multitude of variables and timescales which are interwoven, interdependent, and interacting.
Contextualised processes, specific to each individual: Individuals follow meaningfully different change trajectories that develop and change with time.
Not restricted to linear dynamics: Inputs are not necessarily proportional to outputs, and long periods of apparent stability can precede short periods of rapid change.
2. Behaviour Change Mechanisms under Complexity: Three Key Features
In the following three sections, we drill further down into these ideas. In the first, we introduce interconnectedness via interaction-dominant dynamics, which flow from point 1 above; second, we present how idiosyncratic, non-stationary change trajectories lead to non-ergodicity, a technical term for point 2; third, we highlight that the flexibility of complex systems leads to ubiquitous non-linear dynamics as alluded to in point 3. Table 1 provides an overview of these ideas, which are elaborated further in the subsequent sections.
Table 1.
Interconnectedness | Non-Ergodicity | Non-Linear Dynamics | |
---|---|---|---|
Description | The structure of a system—how it is organised and the relationships between its component parts—can matter more than the component parts themselves. This includes interconnectedness of different variables such as attitudes or perceived norms, as well as that of their temporal dependence; dynamic dependencies of complex systems are not restricted to one or a few previous time points [41,46,66]. | Psychological processes are non-stationary and heterogeneous, hence non-ergodic (group-level measurements do not correspond to those of individuals in time). This means within-individual processes cannot, in general, be inferred from between-individual data. The lack of group-to-individual generalisability implies a threat to validity of results in many areas of science [67,68,69]. | In a linear progression of a phenomenon, the whole is exactly the sum of its parts: You can calculate how much each influencer of behaviour changes, and add them together to get the total effect. Non-linearity occurs when a system’s inputs are disproportionate to its outputs. For example, an effect might be imperceptible for a long time, then explode (as in exponential growth), or suddenly switch states upon reaching a threshold [70,71,72]. |
Main lesson | Dynamic, intertwined processes do not exist in a vacuum. They are always co-dependent and cannot be partialed out into variance components without losing essential information on how the system as a whole operates. | Drawing individual-level inferences from group-level data (the ecological fallacy) leads to misleading or incorrect inferences regarding individual behaviour. A statistical relationship in the population may not hold for any of the individuals. | Viewing the world solely from the lens of linear phenomena and relationships, leads to missed opportunities and misunderstood impacts of interventions. |
Recommendations for the research community | Move from traditional regression-based approaches, which are inspired by component-dominant, additive dynamics, to methods developed for interaction-dominant dynamics, able to cope with multiplicative effects and heavy-tailed distributions. | Move from large-sample research with many variables and many people but few time points (one model per sample), to N-of-1 and intensive longitudinal time series designs, with usually fewer people and variables, but more data per variable (one model per individual). | Move from linear approximations with the illusion of predictability, to methods that can accommodate non-linear patterns and disproportionate influences. |
2.1. Interconnectedness
When processes in complex systems are not independent, they are said to be coupled. Coupling can be unidirectional (for example, physical activity increases muscle mass but not the other way around), or bidirectional, where the elements of a system simultaneously reinforce or suppress each other over time, demonstrating a type of circular causality (e.g., good performance and rewards). Dynamics in living systems tend to be dominated by synergies (“interaction-dominant causation”) instead of their component parts [41,46,73]. Many psychological and behaviour change theories, at least implicitly, assume the presence of reciprocal causation and intertwined processes (e.g., [74], p. 6), but empirical testing of such processes has, to date, been limited.
As mentioned earlier, within the conventional approach to behaviour change intervention evaluation, researchers commonly employ mediation analyses to examine mechanisms. However, the clean independent variable mediator dependent variable type of path analysis can be misleading when change is in fact driven by self-reinforcing, “autocatalytic” interactions that occur over time. In component-dominant causation, effects follow causes in a billiard-ball fashion, and one variable can change without everything else changing. For example, a study developed with the component-dominant mindset could explore how a specific behaviour change technique, say goal setting, affects behaviour. On the other hand, variables of interest to behaviour change researchers are unlikely to change without affecting a large amount of other related variables [51], producing context-dependent effects [75]. This, too, implies that interaction-dominant causation is a more plausible framework for the behaviour change domain, wherein effects emerge (and are conditional upon) the system’s holistic multivariate dynamics, with everything potentially taking place simultaneously in a circularly causal manner. Interaction-dominant dynamics are also characterised by heavy-tailed distributions [49,66,76] such as the log-normal distribution [77], which are common in psychological data [78,79], as well as the presence of long-range temporal correlations and power-law scaling [72,80,81]. Importantly, interplay happens not just between variables, but also between their temporal dynamics: Processes taking place on fast timescales (e.g., lack of physical activity) modulate slow-timescale processes (e.g., development of obesity, lower energy levels), which then feed back, affecting fast-timescale processes [41].
One way of looking at mutually interacting processes with reciprocal causality is to consider the system as a network. Network science is a well-established field with applications ranging from physiology to the organisation of cities [82], and health [83,84]. An illustrative example comes from the study of depression, where the traditional thinking assumes that a latent factor—depression—causes the symptoms. On the other hand, a network science perspective leads to an alternative view, where the network of mutually interacting symptoms constitutes the phenomenon [85,86]. This approach has provided new avenues into understanding and treating depression, such as locating the symptoms which are most relevant to the activation of the network (i.e., the emergence of depression). In addition, this network approach provides insights into how intervening on specific symptoms might affect the system as a whole, given the dynamic interconnected relationships between symptoms.
Although the network theory of mental disorders [85] aligns with and stems from complexity science, the psychological network models usually associated with the approach [87,88] rely on many assumptions stemming from their grounding in multiple regression, including multivariate normality (i.e., linearity) and stationarity [89]. Despite their valuable richness, they were recently shown to not reliably inform about the underlying system dynamics [90]. Still, the conceptual frameworks such models represent—coupled processes interacting in a system, instead of “root causes” [91]—ought to be the primary ontology considered by behaviour change researchers. In the later section on empirical solutions, we present a recurrence-based network modelling approach to consider when investigating these coupled processes [92].
2.2. Non-Ergodicity
To be useful to individuals, processes postulated by psychology should work on the individual level [93]. Whether group-level variation is informative of individual-level dynamics depends on a condition known as ergodicity. Ergodicity has the following properties: “Only if the ensemble of time-dependent trajectories in behaviour space obeys two rigorous conditions will an analysis of interindividual variation yield the same results as an analysis of intraindividual variation […] First, the trajectory of each subject in the ensemble has to obey exactly the same dynamical laws (homogeneity of the ensemble). Second, each trajectory should have constant statistical characteristics in time (stationarity, i.e., constant mean level and serial dependencies)” ([94]; see also [68]).
In other words, in a 100 × 100 spreadsheet, where participants are rows and measurement occasions are columns, calculating an average of values within one column (“ensemble average”), should give the same result as calculating the same statistic from one row (“time average”). For example, in an ergodic process, the mean and standard deviation of each person’s daily minutes of physical activity over a 100-day period would be the same as the mean and standard deviation of 100 people’s daily physical activity minutes measured once. Or, observing that 20% of a given population are smokers, would mean that everyone is a smoker for 20% of their lives. In terms of coupled processes, the correlation between physical activity and intention would be the same in the population measured once, as it is for one person over time.
Going back to the two “rigorous conditions”, the condition of homogeneity almost by definition rules out the behaviour change researcher’s interests, as we are interested in how people (can) change, and it is quite clear that people do not all follow the same behaviour change processes. Indeed, it would seem preposterous to suggest that a process like self-regulation would remain constant across an individual’s life span. The mathematical proof for the non-equivalence of inter-individual and intra-individual data structures was published over a decade ago [95], and recent research has attempted to quantify the threats stemming from lack of group-to-individual generalisability [67]. This preliminary work indicates that even if we could work with “generalisable” ideal random samples from well-defined populations, we would still be committing the ecological fallacy (i.e., drawing individual-level inferences from group-level data) if we wanted to apply our knowledge to individuals.
The second condition, generally referred to as stationarity, is that the statistical properties of these processes must not change over time. In the context of physical activity, the factors that influence behaviour are likely to change over time. For example, the effect of discomfort on physical activity is likely to change in a non-linear manner over time as fitness and tolerance of discomfort fluctuate, not only because of randomness, but as core features of the phenomena itself [96]. The tools most often used in research to analyse behaviour change, such as linear regression, do not account for these kinds of temporal dynamics. This is because temporal cognitive change fundamentally violates the assumption of stationarity.
For the processes underlying physical activity to be considered stationary, the average level of discomfort must remain stable across time for all individuals. In addition, the sequential dependence between repeated measures must be stable [97]. In terms of the relationships between variables, the assumption of stationarity requires that the causal structure which leads to a particular outcome is unchanging across time [98]. Examining behaviour change usually involves an attempt to change the causal structure underlying a behaviour. For example, after making coping plans to tackle barriers to physical activity, the causal relationship between perceived barriers and low physical activity ought to be diminished. Generally, this also means that behaviour can be expected to change as learning and development progress. Stationary data is therefore rare in behaviour change research. This lack of stationarity is rarely acknowledged or (statistically) accounted for in empirical studies evaluating behavioural processes. The result is analogous to the ecological fallacy of taking a population-level mean and extrapolating to individual-level attributes; an average over an individual’s time series describes that individual better than the population-level snapshot, but still might not apply to any particular time period. As a simple example, consider 100 days of data in which a dependence relationship is strongly positive for the first 50, and strongly negative the other: In this case, the average association over the whole time series might be zero, and this misses the abrupt change in this dynamic over time.
Figure 2 illustrates non-stationarity in the case of work motivation, a key feature of occupational health psychology. In these data, (taken from one participant in an observational intensive longitudinal study of work motivation; Heino et al., in prep), the observed relationships between variables shift drastically as the study progresses.
Idiographic science, which tries to unveil person-level processes, does not aim to go inductively from data to universal or statistical laws that hold in hypothetical infinitely large populations [99,100]. Instead, it applies general principles, such as universal properties of complex systems, to study how individuals behave in their particular contexts. Answering more than half a century of calls to expand focus beyond outcomes to processes, new technology in data collection and analysis has now made the idiographic approach possible [101]. The basic solution is to not average individuals and then model the behaviour of the averages, but to first model individuals, and then aggregate those models to search for commonalities [65]. Recent work has made use of methods such as ecological momentary assessment [102] to gather intensive longitudinal data on behaviour and determinants from one or more individuals, which can then be represented as time series. In the case of smoking, analyses of such idiographic data have yielded individualised models that can predict behaviour with stunning accuracy [103,104].
If the mechanisms of behaviour change occur within an individual, then we need to also study them within individuals. However, when we study individual time series data, the methods used in the conventional approach for studying group averages (e.g., pre-post measurements with a long time between them) leave us wanting. Figure 3 illustrates the effects of within-individual sampling rate on perceived trends. When the sampling rate does not match the rate of progression of a phenomenon, a deceptively linear picture of the process might emerge (see also [53], p. 3). The same logic applies if we are studying groups but cannot rely on the means being informative due to a lack of power (as demonstrated in [105]).
In sum, to study individual behaviour change, we need to not only collect intensive longitudinal data on the individual-level, but we must also consider the time evolution of the phenomenon and apply statistical analyses which can accurately model non-stationary data. In the health psychology context, Bolger and Zee [37] argue that not only temporal processes need to be considered, but also the heterogeneity therein. Consistent with the idiographic approach outlined above, every individual may exhibit idiosyncratic dynamics. As we will see next, the possibilities are vast when stepping outside the linear worldview.
2.3. Non-Linear Dynamics
The linear methods traditionally used in psychology (e.g., multiple linear regression, ANOVA, and other cases of the general linear model) view psychological phenomena as following gradual changes over time. While sometimes useful as approximations, the assumptions of linear models are usually violated in practice [24]. Furthermore, linear models may be invalid when ceiling or floor effects are present [106,107], or under hysterisis, when the temporal direction of a relationship matters for its impact (e.g., prevention is important precisely because it takes more effort to exit the state of having a lifestyle disease, than to enter it) [108,109].
While a reliance on linear models simplifies the analytical approaches needed to explore relationships between variables, it does not contribute to our understanding of how the world works, as “most of everyday life is non-linear” [110] and outside the physical sciences, non-linear systems are “the rule, not the exception” [111]. As an intuitive example, consider that falling from 10 m is likely to kill you, but falling from 1 m does not make you 1/10th dead—in fact, it makes you stronger [112,113]. Or that eating twice the size of a normal meal rarely results in twice the pleasure.
Non-linear dynamics can be very useful albeit unintuitive to grasp, as the world discovered during the COVID-19 pandemic: An exponential growth starting from 20 cases on day 1 with a growth rate of 20% can lead to 4030 cases by day 30, and 81,030 cases by day 45. Reducing this growth rate by a mere one percentage-point would result in approximately 29,000 fewer cases by that time. Theories and methods to understand non-linear change phenomena in individuals can provide different types of answers than linear analyses. The most important factors in predicting behaviour change may not be the strength of a variable’s relationship with behaviour (e.g., regression weights), but rather the type of fluctuation that the variable exhibits in response to an intervention [73,114,115], or how fast the dynamics recover after shocks [116].
Another key insight is that while we cannot usually predict what the value of the next observation will be, we can “predict” which system states are possible, and evaluate the risks and opportunities for intervention from there. This can be done with, e.g., simulation approaches, which can potentially “replicate the global tendencies of the dynamics” [117], without being precisely correct about any specific instance. Indeed, exact prediction beyond some short-term horizon is convincingly shown to be impossible in complex and not-so-complex systems across sciences, as indicated since Poincare’s discovery of the famous three-body problem [110]. For a discussion on evaluating the aforementioned prediction time horizon in psychological self-ratings, see [72].
Polynomial regression is perhaps the most commonly used model when linearity is questioned. This method allows for identifying curves that may better fit data on the relationships between variables than a straight line [118], and can also be used to represent non-linear changes that occur over time. Polynomial regression models do not, however, adequately capture the essence of complex systems; non-linear, irregular changes, periodic peaks and plateaus, and with recoveries after negative shocks and deterioration after positive ones [119].
When we consider the situation where all components of a system interact, many features evident in everyday life but ambiguous in linear modelling become salient. Long periods with no discernible changes in outcomes might be followed by short bursts with large shifts. For example, a person’s conscious intention to smoke may remain stable, while social norms keep changing, until one day when a seemingly innocuous event causes the person to quit. When a system finally reaches a “tipping point” (e.g., an individual’s behaviour changes), conventional analytic methods have difficulty determining whether the effect was caused by a critically important incident, or by less obvious, small, cumulative effects over time which preceded the so-called phase transition. Obviously, in such situations, the consequences of an incident (i.e., the camel’s back breaking) do not relate linearly to the intensity of the event (i.e., loading the last straw on the camel). This is a common dynamic in complex systems [63], but it is extremely difficult to evaluate if information regarding the system is only available for a few points in time. Intensive longitudinal data are therefore needed.
3. Empirical Solutions
To model intensive longitudinal data, models developed within the literature on time series analysis are necessary [65,120]. A time series in this case is a sequence of values representing one variable in one individual, and time series analysis consists of methods for studying time evolution of one or more data generating processes.
The most common modelling framework, lag-1 autoregression, uses one previous time point as input to predicting the next one. In behavioural science, vector autoregression—vectors being sequences of numbers, representing values of variables—is often used to test the effects of several variables on the outcome of interest. One drawback of such autoregressive models is that they assume that there exists an average value around which the process fluctuates, which also motivates the common practice of “detrending”. In detrending, the researcher transforms the data by fitting a linear regression line and continuing the analysis with the residuals, often not taking into account that there can be several trends in subsections of the data (i.e., the trend is non-stationary), which all contribute to what the linear model interprets as normally distributed “errors.” Moreover, the supposed mean value—as well as variance around it—may not remain the same across time (i.e., the level is non-stationary), and the impact of previous time points on future ones is assumed to remain constant [121]. One way to overcome this shortcoming, is to let the parameters in autoregressive models vary across time, leading to the time-varying autoregressive model depicted in Figure 2. But even time-varying autoregressive models operate under the linear regression framework, with its accompanying assumptions, such as normally distributed errors. Furthermore, in Figure 2, we have limited ourselves to investigating the lag-1 relationships, whereas long-range dependencies are common in ecological momentary assessment data [72,92,115,122].
Time-varying autoregressive models are regression-based, and are only appropriate when the dynamics of all variables in the model conform to the required assumptions. Empirical researchers have a wide variety of assumption tests at their disposal. The supplementary website (section available online: https://git.io/JfLmm (accessed on 1 May 2021)) presents a plethora of these tests applied to a sample of 20 individuals collecting data for 9 motivation variables. We can see that many or most time series indeed exhibit non-stationary trends and levels, as well as non-linearities. Additionally, longer time series reject more of the assumptions, as the deviations from assumptions are not necessarily present in small samples, and larger samples confer higher statistical power. This does not suggest that we ought to only gather short time series, as doing so would limit our abilities to detect deviations from assumptions and generalise to data outside the sample.
There are many ways to study non-linear change processes in complex systems. Behavioural researchers may find the generalised logistic model [107] a good starting point. This method produces readily-interpretable parameters indicating the floors and ceilings of the variables intervened upon, as well as the growth rate and timing of changes. Researchers may also be interested in identifying critical transformations taking place in a system (e.g., a person’s motivational system). In complex systems, these state transitions may be preceded by warning signs such as increased turbulence (quantified as e.g., dynamic complexity; [123]), or critical slowing down (i.e., heightened autocorrelations in a time series), before (re)lapses occur [124,125]. In clinical psychology, intensive monitoring of psychopathological symptoms has allowed researchers to examine symptoms’ variability, autocorrelations and other indicators of dynamics during interventions. This has yielded considerable advances in the prediction of phase transitions between adaptive and maladaptive psychological states [58,126,127,128]. A conceptual replication in a population undergoing a weight loss intervention [129] recently found that sudden drops in physical activity levels could be predicted by the emergence of erratic fluctuations in day-to-day physical activity. While the presence of critical fluctuations is a key indicator of the effectiveness of psychotherapy for mood disorders [58], similar investigations have not yet been undertaken in other behaviour change contexts.
Multilevel models are often proposed as a method to handle nested timescales and temporal dynamics. It should be noted though, that such models assume, e.g., that individuals depart from group-level means according to some known distribution—and if distributional assumptions are incorrect, so are the results [100]. For a use case of complexity-informed multilevel modelling, see [129].
In the next section, we present one family of analysis methods, recurrence quantification, that is suitable for analysing longitudinal data sets while making fewer a priori assumptions.
Modelling Complex Time Series Data with Recurrence-Based Analyses
Recurrence quantification analysis, unlike regression-based methods, makes no assumptions about distributional shapes of observations or their errors, about linearity, nor about the time-lags involved. Researchers can therefore use it to explore the dynamics of a phenomenon, obtaining robust visually-intuitive information about the organisation of a system. Recall from Table 1 that, in complex systems, the organisation of components can be more important than the components themselves.
Recurrence networks display relationships between multivariate observations in a time series in an intuitive way. The results of a multidimensional recurrence quantification analysis can be thought of as displaying a type of multivariate “correlation”, indicating the occasions in a time series that repeat a previously observed pattern. This could be patterns of single values or combinations of values of different variables, analogous to a system state. These patterns or configurations can be thought of as the attractor states towards which the system is drawn.
An in-depth walkthrough of the analysis with code is provided in the supplementary website (see section available online: https://git.io/JfLs3 (accessed on 1 May 2021)), and a tutorial is available in [130], hence we will be brief in the background and focus on the results. Data for the demonstration below comes from a single participant, who at each time point, completed six questions about their self-determined motivation. A more detailed exposition of the data is found at the supplementary website (see section available online: https://git.io/JfLmQ (accessed on 1 May 2021)).
Figure 4 demonstrates a multidimensional recurrence network, where each point is a measurement occasion. Lines between measurement occasions indicate recurrences of a system state, in this case, a “motivation profile” consisting of the six motivation-related questions. We can see that most of the recurrent states take place in the second half of the data. In addition, patterns that appear only once (white dots) take place exclusively in the first half of data collection. Had we only measured the first 50% of observations, many of the recurrent system states would have been missed. This network demonstrates that this person’s motivational system gravitates between several observable recurrent states.
While Figure 4 shows how various states recur over time within the system, Figure 5 depicts the actual values of variables within each recurrent state. With this information, one might attempt to assign qualitative meaning to the observed recurrent profiles. In this data, we can observe that about a fifth of the participant’s responses fall into a relatively balanced profile (category 1st, yellow), while ca. 15% display what self-determination theory [132] would consider an “optimal” motivation profile—high in autonomous forms of motivation and low in controlled ones (categories 3rd and 4th, purple and dark blue, respectively).
We can collapse the information in Figure 4 to the percentages with which one state follows another, demonstrating a transition network. It answers the question “If you are in state x, what is the probability of transitioning to state y?” Figure 6 (panel A) depicts the relative frequencies with which each state precedes the others. If the system displays linear dynamics, then the matrix should be roughly symmetrical, with similar values observed above and below the diagonal drawn from the bottom left cell to the top right cell. Panel B of Figure 6 presents the same information as a network.
To distinguish whether the results reflect a non-linear data structure, or whether they are merely a product of randomness, researchers can use a technique called surrogate data analysis [133]. In this method, temporally disordered versions of the data—called “surrogates”—are created, and the observed data is then compared to those. The surrogates represent the hypothesis that the data were generated by a rescaled Gaussian linear process. By analysing the surrogates, we ask whether the data can be understood to have arisen from a process that is essentially stochastic and linear instead of highly interdependent and non-linear. The analysis indicates that it would indeed be very unlikely to see these results, if the dynamics were Gaussian. Surrogate analysis of this dataset is presented in the supplementary website (section available online: https://git.io/JqRTQ (accessed on 1 May 2021)).
Following the idiographic approach outlined earlier, this information could now be used to develop a personalised intervention. The designer of such an intervention—possibly the person themself, using self-enactable behaviour change techniques [134]—has several new perspectives to consider: How could transitions to the “optimal” profiles (3rd and 4th) be increased? Could these optimal states be made more “sticky”, and the 1st state less so (as indicated by self-loops in Figure 6, panel B)? Could just-in-time adaptive interventions [56]—such as prompts on a mobile device—be used to inform the person of the state they are in, and prompt enactment of techniques that can help them stay in or leave that state?
To summarise: Having looked at all the time scales instead of just the previous time points, while not restricting ourselves to linear dynamics, we observe the features outlined in Table 1. The recurrent states of the system are connected across time, demonstrating interconnectedness instead of independence. They are not equally spaced in time, demonstrating non-stationarity and hence non-ergodicity. In addition, going from state a to state b does not generally happen with the same probability as the reverse, hence demonstrating non-linear dynamics. These features offer potent information for formulating interventions and understanding the dynamics at play in a system, but would be overlooked by more traditional analysis methods.
4. Discussion
Applied behavioural sciences have always studied phenomena, like behaviour change mechanisms, which take place within complex ecological systems [135]. Past efforts to understand these phenomena used linear models, even though the tools of complexity science would have been more appropriate [136]. Behavioural scientists have an opportune moment to start considering complexity, as the field of behavioural intervention research is now taking committed first steps in this direction [64,75]. There is a growing interest toward intervention programme theories that explicitly model complexity, such as recursive causality, disproportionate relationships, “tipping points”, and emergent outcomes [137]. In addition, novel analytical methods that are compatible with complexity science are continually being developed [131].
By examining intensive longitudinal data, we have shown similar results to a plethora of studies, which find complex dynamics in ecological momentary assessment data [72,115,122,138]: Non-linear, non-ergodic, non-independent dynamics, which defy traditional assumptions. Moreover, the empirical case example of a single person shows the importance of observing change over long periods of time: There are several recurring “ways of being”, patterns of experience which change dynamically within the individual. These recurrence patterns, and the nature of states that recur, are likely to differ across individuals. By using multivariate recurrence-based methods [92], researchers are free to examine conceptually overlapping variables from multiple theories in the same analysis [139], as such methods do not require partialing out variance. This makes intervention process evaluation possible from a more holistic perspective—that is, looking for changes in, e.g., attractor states or complexity measures.
Critically appraising the often hidden assumptions of models, especially in the context of complex systems such as human behaviour change interventions, is necessary for understanding the phenomena of interest and building a credible science. Researchers who study stable phenomena and who only wish to draw group-level inferences (e.g., to select promising public health interventions) are probably best served with traditional models. This is rarely the case for psychologists and behaviour change intervention researchers who wish to understand time- and context-embedded change mechanisms. For theory to advance, assumptions need to be justified: We cannot conclude, both, that our models for empirical testing omit crucial facets of reality, and at the same time imply real-life consequences. We propose that a more fruitful approach would be to investigate coupled processes with individual-level psychological data from intensive longitudinal designs, and to use analyses that are reasonably free from assumptions regarding independence, ergodicity and linearity.
By studying what other sciences know about change processes in complex systems and replicating studies where the ideas have been applied to human behaviour change, researchers can work towards uncovering more general principles of behaviour change. As Molenaar [140] pointed out, “the set of person-specific time series models thus obtained then can in the next step be subjected to standard analysis of inter-individual variation in order to detect subsets of subjects who are homogeneous with respect to particular aspects of the dynamical laws concerned”. In other words, information obtained from individual-level studies of dynamic patterns can then possibly inform models of larger groups, leading to better (or at least humbler and more nuanced) social scientific theories [141]. Generating theory in this way would answer calls to address the issue of time more clearly in theories of health behaviour [36,37]. It could also lay the foundation for more formal theories of behaviour change to be developed [50,90,142], as these typically hypothesise how relationships between variables unfold over time, and a more coherent correspondence between theoretical cycles and empirical cycles in behaviour change research [143].
5. Limitations
The field of complexity science and aligned novel methods is fast-moving, with new developments always on the horizon. However, there remain many practical and methodological barriers to fully embracing the complexity perspective in behaviour change research. Many of these barriers relate to data collection. While the development of smartphones and wearable devices for ambulatory assessment allow the convenient collection of intensive longitudinal data, there are few stable and user-friendly open-source options. This has resulted in large variability in the data collection tools used to produce intensive longitudinal data [144]. Ensuring good adherence to these forms of data collection can be a challenge for researchers. For participants, adapting to intensive assessment is a behaviour change in itself—particularly if they are required to use a specific device or smartphone application. Although measurement burst designs [145] might alleviate some challenges, they bring about other, perhaps graver ones, such as a mismatch between the sampling rate and the time scales in which the phenomena of interest unfold.
Long time series data can be time-consuming and effortful to collect. It also creates a much greater burden on participants than traditional questionnaires and fewer time points. However, in behaviour change research and health psychology, much of the core research interests of our theories—influences on behaviours—have traditionally been subjective factors (e.g., sense of self-efficacy, motivations and motives, outcome expectancies), only—by definition—accessible via self-report. This presents an undeniable practical challenge, along with the fact that observations often need to be spread equidistantly in time, allowing one individual to collect only 1–2 data points per day. Still, examples of more than a hundred time points being collected are found from weight loss maintenance [146] to psychotherapy [127], with some studies collecting more than 1000 observations per participant [115].
Several methodological challenges for the study of dynamic systems in behavioural science have been identified [147], including measurement reactivity, the optimal choice of measurement intervals, and measurement quality. To properly address measurement reactivity, it is necessary to know whether the anticipation of measurement or the self-monitoring process itself (or both) interact with the outcomes of interest. Choosing an optimal measurement interval requires knowledge of the timescale of the behaviour change dynamics, which is rare. As regards to measurement quality, we still lack a comprehensive approach to developing and establishing the quality of momentary measures of psychological constructs. Ensuring the validity and reliability of these measures can be difficult due to the requirement to use few items, not to mention that the questionnaire scales are themselves bounded, whereas experience hardly is. One solution for this is to inspect change profiles of responses [92] instead of raw scores. Another solution would naturally be tapping into wearable data; for example, electronically-activated recorders [148] are maturing as a technology, and complexity methods have already been applied to physical activity data during a weight loss intervention [129].
6. Conclusions
When a study finds that variables have explained an unsatisfactory proportion of behaviour, researchers often follow the pattern seen in social and organisational sciences and conclude that either: “(a) significant, explanatory variables have been omitted from the study, (b) the measurement instrument is too imprecise and ‘rough’, or that (c) the random or stochastic part of the problem has overwhelmed the patterned part” [149]. However, if the result stems from a statistical model that makes unfounded assumptions regarding independence, ergodicity and linearity, is it any wonder that it fails to satisfactorily describe reality? In this paper, we have attempted to show that many common modelling strategies fail to adequately capture real-world dynamics of behaviour change, and that a change in approach can advance our understanding of behaviour and behaviour change processes. Behaviour change researchers should further utilize intensive longitudinal designs to collect individual-level psychological and behavioural data from participants, and should utilise analytical methods that are reasonably free from assumptions of independence, ergodicity and linearity. This has practical implications from replicability to outcome and intervention selection. In our view, further embracing complexity science and its methods will advance research on behaviour change and could unearth new evidence of the dynamics of behavioural processes.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Daniele Proverbio and Jonas Haslbeck for their comments, as well as Anton Kunnari for the idea of transition networks.
Author Contributions
M.T.J.H. wrote the analysis code, including the full online supplement, formulated the initial draft of the manuscript and revised it. K.K., C.N., N.H. and F.H. revised the paper for its intellectual content in several rounds. M.T.J.H., K.K. and N.H. contributed to collecting the empirical data. F.H. wrote and adapted the R package used for analysis, and provided expertise in the analytical approach. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
M.T.J.H. and K.K. were supported by Academy of Finland (grant number 295765 to principal investigator N.H.). M.T.J.H. was also supported by Gyllenberg Foundation (grant number 5177). N.H. was supported by an Academy of Finland Research Fellowship (grant number 285283).
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and received a statement of ethical acceptability from the University of Helsinki Ethical Review Board in the Humanities and Social and Behavioural Sciences (statement #29/2018 dated 6 August 2018).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
Data is available on the supplementary website (section available online: https://git.io/JfLmQ (accessed on 1 May 2021)).
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Reporting
We report all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study on the supplementary website (section available online: https://git.io/JfLmQ (accessed on 1 May 2021)).
Other Materials and Online Resources
All analyses and code are available on the supplementary website (available online: https://git.io/Jffrm (accessed on 1 May 2021)). The Open Science Framework repository for this project is available online: https://osf.io/hnpse/ (accessed on 1 May 2021). This manuscript was created with the R package papaja [150], and the source code is available online: https://git.io/JfLY1 (accessed on 1 May 2021). GitHub repository available online: https://git.io/JfLYr (accessed on 1 May 2021)).
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Kwasnicka D., Dombrowski S.U., White M., Sniehotta F. Theoretical Explanations for Maintenance of Behaviour Change: A Systematic Review of Behaviour Theories. Health Psychol. Rev. 2016;10:277–296. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2016.1151372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Carey R.N., Connell L.E., Johnston M., Rothman A.J., de Bruin M., Kelly M.P., Michie S. Behavior Change Techniques and Their Mechanisms of Action: A Synthesis of Links Described in Published Intervention Literature. Ann. Behav. Med. 2019;53:693–707. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Michie S., West R., Campbell R., Brown J., Gainforth H. ABC of Behaviour Change Theories. Silverback; Sutton, UK: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Matthews L., Simpson S.A. Evaluation of Behavior Change Interventions. In: Hamilton K., Cameron L.D., Hagger M.S., Hankonen N., Lintunen T., editors. The Handbook of Behavior Change. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2020. pp. 318–332. Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hagger M.S., Moyers S., McAnally K., McKinley L.E. Known Knowns and Known Unknowns on Behavior Change Interventions and Mechanisms of Action. Health Psychol. Rev. 2020;14:199–212. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1719184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hankonen N., Heino M.T.J., Kujala E., Hynynen S.-T., Absetz P., Ara’ujo-Soares V., Borodulin K., Haukkala A. What Explains the Socioeconomic Status Gap in Activity? Educational Differences in Determinants of Physical Activity and Screentime. BMC Public Health. 2017;17:144. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3880-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bullock J.G., Green D.P., Ha S.E. Yes, but What’s the Mechanism? (Don’t Expect an Easy Answer) J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010;98:550–558. doi: 10.1037/a0018933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fiedler K., Schott M., Meiser T. What Mediation Analysis Can (Not) Do. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011;47:1231–1236. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Green D.P., Ha S.E., Bullock J.G. Enough Already About “Black Box” Experiments: Studying Mediation Is More Difficult Than Most Scholars Suppose. Ann. Am. Acad. Political Soc. Sci. 2010;628:200–208. doi: 10.1177/0002716209351526. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kline R.B. The Mediation Myth. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2015;37:202–213. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2015.1049349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Roe R. What Is Wrong with Mediators and Moderators? Eur. Health Psychol. 2012;14:4–10. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Keele L. Causal Mediation Analysis: Warning! Assumptions Ahead. Am. J. Eval. 2015;36:500–513. doi: 10.1177/1098214015594689. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hofmann S.G., Curtiss J.E., Hayes S.C. Beyond Linear Mediation: Toward a Dynamic Network Approach to Study Treatment Processes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2020;76:101824. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rohrer J.M. Thinking Clearly About Correlations and Causation: Graphical Causal Models for Observational Data. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2018;1:27–42. doi: 10.1177/2515245917745629. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rohrer J.M., Hünermund P., Arslan R.C., Elson M. That’s a lot to PROCESS! Pitfalls of Popular Path Models. PsyArxiv. 2021 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/paeb7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Loeys T., Moerkerke B., Vansteelandt S. A Cautionary Note on the Power of the Test for the Indirect Effect in Mediation Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2015;5:1549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Knafl G.J., Knafl K.A., Grey M., Dixon J., Deatrick J.A., Gallo A.M. Incorporating Nonlinearity into Mediation Analyses. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2017;17:45. doi: 10.1186/s12874-017-0296-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hamaker E.L., Kuiper R.M., Grasman R.P.P.P. A Critique of the Cross-Lagged Panel Model. Psychol. Methods. 2015;20:102–116. doi: 10.1037/a0038889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dawid A.P. In: Beware of the DAG. Guyon I., Janzing D., Schölkopf B., editors. Volume 6. PMLR; Whistler, BC, Canada: 2010. pp. 59–86. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bechtel W., Abrahamsen A.A. Thinking Dynamically About Biological Mechanisms: Networks of Coupled Oscillators. Found. Sci. 2013;18:707–723. doi: 10.1007/s10699-012-9301-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Golonka S., Wilson A.D. Ecological Mechanisms in Cognitive Science. Theory Psychol. 2019;29:676–696. doi: 10.1177/0959354319877686. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bolger N., Laurenceau J.-P. Intensive Longitudinal Methods: An Introduction to Diary and Experience Sampling Research. Guilford Press; New York, NY, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Reichert M., Giurgiu M., Koch E.D., Wieland L.M., Lautenbach S., Neubauer A.B., von Haaren-Mack B., Schilling R., Timm I., Notthoff N., et al. Ambulatory Assessment for Physical Activity Research: State of the Science, Best Practices and Future Directions. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2020;50:101742. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Siegenfeld A.F., Bar-Yam Y. An Introduction to Complex Systems Science and Its Applications. arXiv. 20191912.05088 [Google Scholar]
- 25.Newman M.E.J. Complex Systems: A Survey. Am. J. Phys. 2011;79:800–810. doi: 10.1119/1.3590372. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bar-Yam Y. Concepts Map: System; New England Complex Systems Institute: 2018. [(accessed on 1 May 2021)]; Available online: https://web.archive.org/web/20181009095010/http://necsi.edu/guide/concepts/system.html.
- 27.Wright D., Meadows D.H. Thinking in Systems: A Primer. 1st ed. Earthscan; London, UK: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mitchell M. Complexity: A Guided Tour. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: New York, NY, USA: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Brand S.L., Fleming L.E., Wyatt K.M. Tailoring Healthy Workplace Interventions to Local Healthcare Settings: A Complexity Theory-Informed Workplace of Well-Being Framework. Sci. World J. 2015;2015:340820. doi: 10.1155/2015/340820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rickles D., Hawe P., Shiell A. A Simple Guide to Chaos and Complexity. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health. 2007;61:933–937. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.054254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Resnicow K., Vaughan R. A Chaotic View of Behavior Change: A Quantum Leap for Health Promotion. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2006;3:25. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-3-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gomersall T. Complex Adaptive Systems: A New Approach for Understanding Health Practices. Health Psychol. Rev. 2018;12:405–418. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1488603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dishman R.K., Sallis J.F., Orenstein D.R. The Determinants of Physical Activity and Exercise. Public Health Rep. 1985;100:158–171. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kok G., Peters G.-J.Y., Kessels L.T., Ten Hoor G.A., Ruiter R.A. Ignoring Theory and Misinterpreting Evidence: The False Belief in Fear Appeals. Health Psychol. Rev. 2018;12:111–125. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1415767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Peters G.-J.Y., Ruiter R.A., Ten Hoor G.A., Kessels L.T., Kok G. Towards Consensus on Fear Appeals: A Rejoinder to the Commentaries on Kok, Peters, Kessels, Ten Hoor, and Ruiter. Health Psychol. Rev. 2018;12:151–156. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1454846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Scholz U. It’s Time to Think About Time in Health Psychology. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being. 2019;11:173–186. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bolger N., Zee K.S. Heterogeneity in Temporal Processes: Implications for Theories in Health Psychology. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being. 2019;11:198–201. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Matthews C.E., Ainsworth B.E., Thompson R.W., Bassett D.R. Sources of Variance in Daily Physical Activity Levels as Measured by an Accelerometer. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2002;34:1376–1381. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200208000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cepeda M., Koolhaas C.M., van Rooij F.J.A., Tiemeier H., Guxens M., Franco O.H., Schoufour J.D. Seasonality of Physical Activity, Sedentary Behavior, and Sleep in a Middle-Aged and Elderly Population: The Rotterdam Study. Maturitas. 2018;110:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dumith S.C., Gigante D.P., Domingues M.R., Kohl H.W. Physical Activity Change During Adolescence: A Systematic Review and a Pooled Analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011;40:685–698. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Richardson M.J., Kallen R.W., Eiler B.A. Computational Social Psychology. Routledge; New York, NY, USA: 2017. Interaction-Dominant Dynamics, Timescale Enslavement, and the Emergence of Social Behavior; pp. 121–142. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rickles D. Causality in Complex Interventions. Med. Health Care Philos. 2009;12:77–90. doi: 10.1007/s11019-008-9140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Meehl P.E. Why Summaries of Research on Psychological Theories Are Often Uninterpretable. Psychol. Rep. 1990;66:195–244. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1990.66.1.195. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cohen J. The Earth Is Round (P0.6em.05) Am. Psychol. 1994;49:997–1003. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.12.997. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fink D.S., Keyes K.M., Cerd’a M. Social Determinants of Population Health: A Systems Sciences Approach. Curr. Epidemiol. Rep. 2016;3:98–105. doi: 10.1007/s40471-016-0066-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wallot S., Kelty-Stephen D.G. Interaction-Dominant Causation in Mind and Brain, and Its Implication for Questions of Generalization and Replication. Minds Mach. 2017;28:353–374. doi: 10.1007/s11023-017-9455-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Makridakis S., Taleb N. Decision Making and Planning Under Low Levels of Predictability. Int. J. Forecast. 2009;25:716–733. doi: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2009.05.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Makridakis S., Hyndman R.J., Petropoulos F. Forecasting in Social Settings: The State of the Art. Int. J. Forecast. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2019.05.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Taleb N.N. Statistical Consequences of Fat Tails: Real World Preasymptotics, Epistemology, and Applications. STEM Academic Press; New York, NY, US: 2020. Illustrated Edition. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Heino M.T.J., Fried E.I., LeBel E.P. Commentary: Reproducibility in Psychological Science: When Do Psychological Phenomena Exist? Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1004. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Peters G.-J.Y., Crutzen R. Pragmatic Nihilism: How a Theory of Nothing Can Help Health Psychology Progress. Health Psychol. Rev. 2017;11:103–121. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1284015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hekler E.B., Klasnja P., Chevance G., Golaszewski N.M., Lewis D., Sim I. Why We Need a Small Data Paradigm. BMC Med. 2019;17:133. doi: 10.1186/s12916-019-1366-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schiepek G., Aichhorn W., Gruber M., Strunk G., Bachler E., Aas B. Real-Time Monitoring of Psychotherapeutic Processes: Concept and Compliance. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:604. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kwasnicka D., Inauen J., Nieuwenboom W., Nurmi J., Schneider A., Short C.E., Dekkers T., Williams A.J., Bierbauer W., Haukkala A., et al. Challenges and Solutions for N-of-1 Design Studies in Health Psychology. Health Psychol. Rev. 2019;13:163–178. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1564627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hekler E.B., Rivera D.E., Martin C.A., Phatak S.S., Freigoun M.T., Korinek E., Klasnja P., Adams M.A., Buman M.P. Tutorial for Using Control Systems Engineering to Optimize Adaptive Mobile Health Interventions. J. Med. Internet Res. 2018;20:e214. doi: 10.2196/jmir.8622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hardeman W., Houghton J., Lane K., Jones A., Naughton F. A Systematic Review of Just-in-Time Adaptive Interventions (JITAIs) to Promote Physical Activity. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2019;16:31. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0792-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hawe P., Shiell A., Riley T. Theorising Interventions as Events in Systems. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2009;43:267–276. doi: 10.1007/s10464-009-9229-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Olthof M., Hasselman F., Strunk G., Aas B., Schiepek G., Lichtwarck-Aschoff A. Destabilization in Self-Ratings of the Psychotherapeutic Process Is Associated with Better Treatment Outcome in Patients with Mood Disorders. Psychother. Res. 2019;30:520–531. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2019.1633484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Scheffer M., Bolhuis J.E., Borsboom D., Buchman T.G., Gijzel S.M.W., Goulson D., Kammenga J.E., Kemp B., van de Leemput I.A., Levin S., et al. Quantifying Resilience of Humans and Other Animals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018:201810630. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1810630115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ashwin P., Wieczorek S., Vitolo R., Cox P. Tipping Points in Open Systems: Bifurcation, Noise-Induced and Rate-Dependent Examples in the Climate System. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2012;370:1166–1184. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2011.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bar-Yam Y. Making Things Work: Solving Complex Problems in a Complex World. NECSI/Knowledge Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Navarro J., Rueff-Lopes R. Healthy Variability in Organizational Behavior: Empirical Evidence and New Steps for Future Research. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 2015;19:529–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Taleb N.N., Blyth M. The Black Swan of Cairo. Foreign Aff. 2011;90:33–39. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Skivington K., Matthews L., Craig P., Simpson S., Moore L. Developing and Evaluating Complex Interventions: Updating Medical Research Council Guidance to Take Account of New Methodological and Theoretical Approaches. Lancet. 2018;392:S2. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32865-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wright A.G.C., Woods W.C. Personalized Models of Psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2020;16 doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-102419-125032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.van Rooij M.M.J.W., Nash B.A., Rajaraman S., Holden J.G. A Fractal Approach to Dynamic Inference and Distribution Analysis. Front. Physiol. 2013;4 doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fisher A.J., Medaglia J.D., Jeronimus B.F. Lack of Group-to-Individual Generalizability Is a Threat to Human Subjects Research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018:201711978. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711978115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Molenaar P.C.M. On the Implications of the Classical Ergodic Theorems: Analysis of Developmental Processes Has to Focus on Intra-Individual Variation. Dev. Psychobiol. 2008;50:60–69. doi: 10.1002/dev.20262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Time to Move Beyond Average Thinking. Nat. Phys. 2019;15:1207. doi: 10.1038/s41567-019-0758-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Helmich M.A., Wichers M., Olthof M., Strunk G., Aas B., Aichhorn W., Schiepek G., Snippe E. Sudden Gains in Day-to-Day Change: Revealing Nonlinear Patterns of Individual Improvement in Depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2020;88:119–127. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kelty-Stephen D.G., Wallot S. Multifractality Versus (Mono-) Fractality as Evidence of Nonlinear Interactions Across Timescales: Disentangling the Belief in Nonlinearity from the Diagnosis of Nonlinearity in Empirical Data. Ecol. Psychol. 2017;29:259–299. doi: 10.1080/10407413.2017.1368355. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Olthof M., Hasselman F., Lichtwarck-Aschoff A. Complexity in Psychological Self-Ratings: Implications for Research and Practice. BMC Med. 2020;18:317. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-01727-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bak P., Tang C., Wiesenfeld K. Self-Organized Criticality: An Explanation of the 1/F Noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1987;59:381–384. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.59.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Prentice-Hall, Inc.; Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Craig P., Di Ruggiero E., Frolich K.L., Mykhalovskiy E., White M., Campbell R., Cummins S., Edwards N., Hunt K., Kee F. Taking Account of Context in Population Health Intervention Research: Guidance for Producers, Users and Funders of Research. Trials and Studies Coordinating Centre; Southampton, UK: 2018. NIHR Evaluation. [Google Scholar]
- 76.West B.J. Homeostasis and Gauss Statistics: Barriers to Understanding Natural Variability: Homeostasis and Gauss Statistics. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2010;16:403–408. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01459.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Trafimow D., Wang T., Wang C. Means and Standard Deviations, or Locations and Scales? That Is the Question! New Ideas Psychol. 2018;50:34–37. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Cain M.K., Zhang Z., Yuan K.-H. Univariate and Multivariate Skewness and Kurtosis for Measuring Nonnormality: Prevalence, Influence and Estimation. Behav. Res. Methods. 2017;49:1716–1735. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0814-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bono R., Blanca M.J., Arnau J., G’omez-Benito J. Non-Normal Distributions Commonly Used in Health, Education, and Social Sciences: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1602. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Van Orden G.C., Holden J.G., Turvey M.T. Human Cognition and 1/F Scaling. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2005;134:117–123. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Wijnants M.L. A Review of Theoretical Perspectives in Cognitive Science on the Presence of 1/F Scaling in Coordinated Physiological and Cognitive Processes. J. Nonlinear Dyn. 2014;2014:1–17. doi: 10.1155/2014/962043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Barab’asi A.-L. Network Science. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Centola D. The Truth about Behavioral Change. MIT Sloan Management Review. Oct 3, 2018.
- 84.Zhang J., Centola D. Social Networks and Health: New Developments in Diffusion, Online and Offline. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2019;45:91–109. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-073117-041421. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Borsboom D. A Network Theory of Mental Disorders. World Psychiatry. 2017;16:5–13. doi: 10.1002/wps.20375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cramer A.O.J., van Borkulo C.D., Giltay E.J., van der Maas H.L.J., Kendler K.S., Scheffer M., Borsboom D. Major Depression as a Complex Dynamic System. PLoS ONE. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0167490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Heino M.T.J., Knittle K., Fried E., Sund R., Haukkala A., Borodulin K., Uutela A., Araujo-Soares V., Vasankari T., Hankonen N. Visualisation and Network Analysis of Physical Activity and Its Determinants: Demonstrating Opportunities in Analysing Baseline Associations in the Let’s Move It Trial. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. 2019;7:269–289. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2019.1646136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Mkhitaryan S., Crutzen R., Steenaart E., de Vries N.K. Network Approach in Health Behavior Research: How Can We Explore New Questions? Health Psychol. Behav. Med. 2019;7:362–384. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2019.1682587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Epskamp S., Waldorp L.J., Mõttus R., Borsboom D. The Gaussian Graphical Model in Cross-Sectional and Time-Series Data. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2018;53:453–480. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2018.1454823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Haslbeck J.M.B., Ryan O., Robinaugh D.J., Waldorp L.J., Borsboom D. [(accessed on 1 May 2021)];Modeling Psychopathology: From Data Models to Formal Theories. 2019 doi: 10.1037/met0000303. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/jgm7f/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 91.Bringmann L.F., Eronen M.I. Don’t Blame the Model: Reconsidering the Network Approach to Psychopathology. Psychol. Rev. 2018;125:606–615. doi: 10.1037/rev0000108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hasselman F., Bosman A.M.T. Studying Complex Adaptive Systems with Internal States: A Recurrence Network Approach to the Analysis of Multivariate Time Series Data Representing Self-Reports of Human Experience. Front. Appl. Math. Stat. 2020;6 doi: 10.3389/fams.2020.00009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Johnston D.W., Johnston M. Useful Theories Should Apply to Individuals. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2013;18:469–473. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Molenaar P.C.M. Handbook of Cognitive Aging: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. SAGE Publications, Inc.; Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: 2008. Consequences of the Ergodic Theorems for Classical Test Theory, factor Analysis, and the Analysis of Developmental Processes; pp. 90–104. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Molenaar P.C.M. A Manifesto on Psychology as Idiographic Science: Bringing the Person Back into Scientific Psychology, This Time Forever. Measurement. 2004;2:201–218. doi: 10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Hall P.A., Fong G.T. Temporal Self-Regulation Theory: A Neurobiologically Informed Model for Physical Activity Behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:117. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Molenaar P.C., Campbell C.G. The New Person-Specific Paradigm in Psychology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009;18:112–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Cole D.A., Maxwell S.E. Testing Mediational Models with Longitudinal Data: Questions and Tips in the Use of Structural Equation Modeling. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2003;112:558–577. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Gayles J.G., Molenaar P.C.M. The Utility of Person-Specific Analyses for Investigating Developmental Processes: An Analytic Primer on Studying the Individual. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2013;37:549–562. doi: 10.1177/0165025413504857. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Piccirillo M.L., Rodebaugh T.L. Foundations of Idiographic Methods in Psychology and Applications for Psychotherapy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2019;71:90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Hamaker E.L., Grasman R.P.P.P., Kamphuis J.H. Modeling BAS Dysregulation in Bipolar Disorder: Illustrating the Potential of Time Series Analysis. Assessment. 2016;23:436–446. doi: 10.1177/1073191116632339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Burke L.E., Shiffman S., Music E., Styn M.A., Kriska A., Smailagic A., Siewiorek D., Ewing L.J., Chasens E., French B., et al. Ecological Momentary Assessment in Behavioral Research: Addressing Technological and Human Participant Challenges. J. Med. Internet Res. 2017;19 doi: 10.2196/jmir.7138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Fisher A.J., Soyster P.D. Generating Accurate Personalized Predictions of Future Behavior: A Smoking Exemplar. [(accessed on 1 May 2021)];2019 Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/e24v6.
- 104.Soyster P.D., Fisher A.J. Involving Stakeholders in the Design of Ecological Momentary Assessment Research: An Example from Smoking Cessation. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0217150. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Carello C., Moreno M. Tutorials in Contemporary Nonlinear Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. US National Science Foundation; Alexandria, VA, USA: 2015. Why nonlinear methods; pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 106.Gonz’alez M., Coenders G., Saez M., Casas F. Non-Linearity, Complexity and Limited Measurement in the Relationship Between Satisfaction with Specific Life Domains and Satisfaction with Life as a Whole. J. Happiness Stud. 2010;11:335–352. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9143-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Verboon P., Peters G.-J.Y. Applying the Generalized Logistic Model in Single Case Designs: Modeling Treatment-Induced Shifts. Behav. Modif. 2020;44:27–48. doi: 10.1177/0145445518791255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Kelso J.A.S. Haken-Kelso-Bunz Model. Scholarpedia. 2008;3:1612. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1612. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.van Rooij M.M.J.W., Favela L.H., Malone M., Richardson M.J. Modeling the Dynamics of Risky Choice. Ecol. Psychol. 2013;25:293–303. doi: 10.1080/10407413.2013.810502. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Strogatz S.H. Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos: With Applications to Physics, Biology, Chemistry, and Engineering. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 2018. ISBN-13 978-0813349107. [Google Scholar]
- 111.May R.M. Simple Mathematical Models with Very Complicated Dynamics. Nature. 1976;261:459–467. doi: 10.1038/261459a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Taleb N.N. Antifragile: Things That Gain from Disorder. 1st ed. Random House; New York, NY, USA: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 113.Taleb N.N. ’Antifragility’ as a Mathematical Idea. Nature. 2013;494:430. doi: 10.1038/494430e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Almurad Z.M.H., Roume C., Blain H., Deligni‘eres D. Complexity Matching: Restoring the Complexity of Locomotion in Older People Through Arm-in-Arm Walking. Front. Physiol. 2018;9:1766. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Delignieres D., Fortes M., Ninot G. The Fractal Dynamics of Self-Esteem and Physical Self. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 2004;8:479–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Van Orden G.C., Kloos H., Wallot S. Living in the Pink: Intentionality, Wellbeing, and Complexity. In: Hooker C., editor. Philosophy of Complex Systems. Volume 10. North-Holland; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2011. pp. 629–672. Handbook of the Philosophy of Science. [Google Scholar]
- 117.Tobias R. Changing behavior by memory aids: A social psychological model of prospective memory and habit development tested with dynamic field data. Psychol. Rev. 2009;116:408–438. doi: 10.1037/a0015512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Chatzisarantis N.L.D., Yli-Piipari S., Schriefer L.S., Wang D., Barkoukis V., Hagger M.S. Is the Relationship Between Physical Activity Intentions and Behaviour Convex? A Test across 13 Studies. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019;43:114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.01.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hofmans J., Vantilborgh T., Solinger O.N. K-Centres Functional Clustering: A Person-Centered Approach to Modeling Complex Nonlinear Growth Trajectories. Organ. Res. Methods. 2017:1094428117725793. doi: 10.1177/1094428117725793. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Bradley E., Kantz H. Nonlinear Time-Series Analysis Revisited. Chaos Interdiscip. J. Nonlinear Sci. 2015;25:097610. doi: 10.1063/1.4917289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Bringmann L.F., Hamaker E.L., Vigo D.E., Aubert A., Borsboom D., Tuerlinckx F. Changing Dynamics: Time-Varying Autoregressive Models Using Generalized Additive Modeling. Psychol. Methods. 2017;22:409. doi: 10.1037/met0000085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Olthof M., Hasselman F., Wijnants M., Lichtwarck-Aschoff A. Selbstorganisation–ein Paradigma für die Humanwissenschaften. Springer; Wiesbaden, Germany: 2020. Psychological dynamics are complex: A comparison of scaling, variance, and dynamic complexity in simulated and observed data; pp. 303–316. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Schiepek G., Strunk G. The Identification of Critical Fluctuations and Phase Transitions in Short Term and Coarse-Grained Time Seriesa Method for the Real-Time Monitoring of Human Change Processes. Biol. Cybern. 2010;102:197–207. doi: 10.1007/s00422-009-0362-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.van de Leemput I.A., Wichers M., Cramer A.O.J., Borsboom D., Tuerlinckx F., Kuppens P., van Nes E.H., Viechtbauer W., Giltay E.J., Aggen S.H., et al. Critical Slowing down as Early Warning for the Onset and Termination of Depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:87–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312114110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Wichers M., Groot P.C., Psychosystems E.G. Critical Slowing down as a Personalized Early Warning Signal for Depression. Psychother. Psychosom. 2016;85:114–116. doi: 10.1159/000441458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Jeronimus B.F. Dynamic system perspectives on anxiety and depression. In: Kunnen E.S., de Ruiter N.M.P., Jeronimus B.F., van der Gaag M.A.E., editors. Psychosocial Development in Adolescence. Routledge; Abingdon on Thames, UK: 2019. pp. 100–126. [Google Scholar]
- 127.Olthof M., Hasselman F., Strunk G., van Rooij M., Aas B., Helmich M.A., Schiepek G., Lichtwarck-Aschoff A. Critical Fluctuations as an Early-Warning Signal for Sudden Gains and Losses in Patients Receiving Psychotherapy for Mood Disorders. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2019:2167702619865969. doi: 10.1177/2167702619865969. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Schiepek G., Schöller H.J., de Felice G., Steffensen S.V., Bloch M.S., Fartacek C., Aichhorn W., Viol K. Convergent Validation of Methods for the Identification of Psychotherapeutic Phase Transitions in Time Series of Empirical and Model Systems. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1970. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Chevance G., Baretta D., Heino M.T.J., Perski O., Klasnja P., Hekler E., Godino J. Characterizing and Predicting Person-Specific, Day-to-Day, Fluctuations in Walking Behavior. [(accessed on 1 May 2021)];2020 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251659. Available online: https://osf.io/preprints/sportrxiv/bzj6s/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 130.Wallot S., Leonardi G. Analyzing Multivariate Dynamics Using Cross-Recurrence Quantification Analysis (CRQA), Diagonal-Cross-Recurrence Profiles (DCRP), and Multidimensional Recurrence Quantification Analysis (MdRQA) A Tutorial in R. Front. Psychol. 2018;9 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Hasselman F. [(accessed on 1 May 2021)];Casnet: A Toolbox for Studying Complex Adaptive Systems and Networks. 2020 Available online: https://github.com/FredHasselman/casnet.
- 132.Ryan R.M., Deci E.L. Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. Guilford Press; New York, NY, USA: 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 133.Schreiber T., Schmitz A. Surrogate Time Series. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 2000;142:346–382. doi: 10.1016/S0167-2789(00)00043-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Knittle K., Heino M., Marques M.M., Stenius M., Beattie M., Ehbrecht F., Hagger M.S., Hardeman W., Hankonen N. The Compendium of Self-Enactable Techniques to Change and Self-Manage Motivation and Behaviour V.1.0. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:215–223. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0798-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Bronfenbrenner U., Morris P.A. Handbook of Child Psychology: Theoretical Models of Human Development. 5th ed. Volume 1. John Wiley & Sons Inc.; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 1998. The ecology of developmental processes; pp. 993–1028. [Google Scholar]
- 136.Navarro J., Roe R.A., Artiles M.I. Taking Time Seriously: Changing Practices and Perspectives in Work/Organizational Psychology. Rev. Psicol. Trab. Organ. 2015;31:135–145. doi: 10.1016/j.rpto.2015.07.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Rogers P.J. Using Programme Theory to Evaluate Complicated and Complex Aspects of Interventions. Evaluation. 2008;14:29–48. doi: 10.1177/1356389007084674. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Navarro J., Arrieta C., Ball’en C. An Approach to the Study of the Dynamics of Work Motivation Using Diary Method. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 2007;11:473–498. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Gordon R.A. Issues in Multiple Regression. Am. J. Sociol. 1968;73:592–616. doi: 10.1086/224533. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Molenaar P.C.M. Psychological Methodology Will Change Profoundly Due to the Necessity to Focus on Intra-Individual Variation. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2007;41:35–40. doi: 10.1007/s12124-007-9011-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Smaldino P.E., Calanchini J., Pickett C.L. Theory Development with Agent-Based Models. Organ. Psychol. Rev. 2015;5:300–317. doi: 10.1177/2041386614546944. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Chevance G., Perski O., Hekler E. Innovative Methods for Predicting and Changing Complex Health Behaviors: Four Propositions. Transl. Behav. Med. 2021;11:676–685. doi: 10.1093/tbm/ibaa026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.van Rooij I., Baggio G. Theory Before the Test: How to Build High-Verisimilitude Explanatory Theories in Psychological Science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021:1745691620970604. doi: 10.1177/1745691620970604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Trull T.J., Ebner-Priemer U.W. Ambulatory Assessment in Psychopathology Research: A Review of Recommended Reporting Guidelines and Current Practices. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2020;129:56–63. doi: 10.1037/abn0000473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Stawski R.S., MacDonald S.W.S., Sliwinski M.J. The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ, USA: 2015. Measurement Burst Design; pp. 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- 146.Kwasnicka D., Dombrowski S.U., White M., Sniehotta F.F. N-of-1 Study of Weight Loss Maintenance Assessing Predictors of Physical Activity, Adherence to Weight Loss Plan and Weight Change. Psychol. Health. 2017;32:686–708. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1293057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Hamaker E.L., Wichers M. No Time Like the Present: Discovering the Hidden Dynamics in Intensive Longitudinal Data. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2017;26:10–15. doi: 10.1177/0963721416666518. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Kaplan D.M., Rentscher K.E., Lim M., Reyes R., Keating D., Romero J., Shah A., Smith A.D., York K.A., Milek A., et al. Best Practices for Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR) Research: A Practical Guide to Coding and Processing EAR Data. Behav. Res. 2020;52:1538–1551. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01333-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Mathews K.M., White M.C., Long R.G. Why Study the Complexity Sciences in the Social Sciences? Hum. Relat. 1999;52:439–462. doi: 10.1177/001872679905200402. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Aust F., Barth M. Papaja (Preparing APA Journal Articles) [(accessed on 1 May 2021)];2020 Available online: crsh.github.io/papaja_man/
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data is available on the supplementary website (section available online: https://git.io/JfLmQ (accessed on 1 May 2021)).