Abstract
This paper presents a review on relevant studies and reports related to older drivers’ behavior and stress. Questionnaires, simulators, and on-road/in-vehicle systems are used to collect driving data in most studies. In addition, research either directly compares older drivers and the other drivers or considers participants according to various age groups. Nevertheless, the definition of ‘older driver’ varies not only across studies but also across different government reports. Although questionnaire surveys are widely used to affordably obtain massive data in a short time, they lack objectivity. In contrast, biomedical information can increase the reliability of a driving stress assessment when collected in environments such as driving simulators and on-road experiments. Various studies determined that driving behavior and stress remain stable regardless of age, whereas others reported degradation of driving abilities and increased driving stress among older drivers. Instead of age, many researchers recommended considering other influencing factors, such as gender, living area, and driving experience. To mitigate bias in findings, this literature review suggests a hybrid method by applying surveys and collecting on-road/in-vehicle data.
Keywords: older driver, biomedical signals, driving behavior, driving stress
1. Introduction
With global population ageing, more and more academics and governments are concerned with older drivers’ behavior, psychology, and driving safety [1,2,3]. Given its high proportion of older adults, Japan will have an estimated 25% more older drivers by 2030, increasing from 16 million in 2015 to 20 million in 2030 [4]. According to various official reports in the USA [5,6,7], the rate of older drivers has increased faster than that of any other age group, and the population over 70 years old is projected to increase by more than 70% between 2014 and 2030. Moreover, in 2013, the number of driving license holders over 70 years old in the UK surpassed 4 million (4,018,900) for the first time, and the rate of elderly drivers in the European Union will exceed one-quarter of the total by 2050 [8].
Given this trend, road traffic incidents involving older drivers represent a worldwide concern. The Older Drivers Task Force, a UK group of experts from a wide range of disciplines with an interest in the safety of older drivers, pointed that” this influx of older drivers has important economic and social value but it also presents road safety risks if we don’t adapt” [9]. Considering Japanese statistics, road safety in Tokyo is improving considerably, and the number of collisions has reduced by more than 50% since 2005. However, the ratio of older drivers involved in road traffic incidents has nearly doubled over the same time frame (from 10.9% to 20.4%) [4]. In the USA, 16 older adults were killed and 648 were injured in crashes, on an average, every day of 2014 [10,11]. The UK and Australia are facing the same problem, as statistics show that older drivers have the highest risk of death during road traffic incidents than drivers in any other age group in both countries [12,13].
Although reports suggest that the driving safety of older drivers is a serious global problem, its direct relation to older drivers’ physical and mental changes remains debatable.
Langford et al. [14] reached two findings: long periods of driving experience reduce the number of crashes per kilometer, and older adults drive less time than others. Hence, the relatively high crash rate of older drivers might be partially associated to their reduced driving periods. In this study, data from the Netherlands were analyzed to obtain the crash rates according to the yearly driving distance. However, the authors confirmed that most older adults (above 75 years old) drive safer than individuals in any other age group. Similarly, Mitchell [15] conducted a case study in Britain and concluded that the increase of fatality rates for older drivers may be caused by their fragility. Fildes et al. [16] confirmed that older road users are much more likely to be severely injured in crashes than their younger counterparts. A report by the Older Drivers Task Force in the UK [9] reached to a similar conclusion based on the statistics in 2014, indicating that older drivers account for only 7% of injured drivers but for 20% of driver deaths. Thus, older drivers’ fragility demands safer vehicles and transportation infrastructure with more effective protection in case of collisions. However, improving safety for older drivers should not depend only on external methods, which would be costly by the required upgrades to vehicles and infrastructure.
Improving the behavior and psychology of older drivers can be an alternative to improve safety if we can relate ageing to dangerous driving behaviors and stress. Various studies, including those mentioned above, have confirmed that older drivers have high death rates in cashes. However, as most crashes involve two or more road users, the responsibility of older drivers for accidents should be analyzed. Based on fatal crash data in the USA from 1999 to 2003, Tefft [17] found that the oldest drivers (above 85 years old) pose more risk to other road users than middle-aged drivers. In addition, Langford et al. [18] analyzed the insurance data from the state of Tasmania in Australia and found that older drivers (above 65 years old) were 1.5 times more likely to be responsible for crashes than middle-aged drivers. Likewise, Oxley et al. [19] summarized a series of studies and concluded that older road users were primarily responsible for crashes involving them. Thus, the study of older drivers’ behavior and psychology can be extremely important for improving road safety.
Before reporting the methods and findings from existing studies, we should define some important concepts:
Older driver. According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the USA [20], an older driver is a license holder who is 65 years old or above. In contrast, the United Nations (UN) [3] define elder as people who are 60 years old or above. Moreover, the age that defines an older driver varies according to the country and organization, as further detailed in Section 3.1.
Driving stress. The UN [21] defines stress as “the physical and psychological process of reacting to and coping with events or situations that place extraordinary pressure upon a human being. Such events are usually sudden and often involve physical or emotional loss, such as witnessing casualties or destruction from combat or disasters or the serious injury or death of a relative, friend or co-worker.” Accordingly, driving stress is caused by negative factors in driving environment, such as traffic jam, being followed closely, and long-distance driving [22,23]. Meanwhile, functional abilities of drivers may also affect their driving stress. For example, a visually cluttered environments will increase driver’s stress if he/she is suffering from eyesight problems [24,25,26,27].
Driving behavior. Considering the meanings of drive and behavior from the Cambridge Dictionary [28,29], driving behavior can be defined as a set of actions taken by a driver to control a vehicle.
Dangerous driving behavior. A dangerous driving behavior is a driving behavior that can directly or indirectly cause a crash, injury, or death on the road [30,31,32]. It is also called risky driving behavior or bad driving behavior [33,34].
In the remainder of this paper, we provide a survey about several studies on the relationship between aging and driving behavior or stress. These studies often report apparently contradictory statements. For instance, European Commission [35] claimed that “an increase in age does not cause higher crash rates per exposure, challenging the traditional concept of a direct association between age-related deterioration of safety-relevant driving skills and driving performance.” Although the studies mentioned in this report present sound statistics and results, the findings contradict the facts and conclusions that we mention in the paragraphs above. However, various studies may be limited by their methods, as most of them relied on questionnaire surveys, whereas few considered driving simulators, and on-road experiments were rarely conducted. The inconsistent methods may result in conflicting conclusions. In fact, by analyzing the introduction and conclusions of the corresponding papers, we found that previous hypotheses were either confirmed or denied only by asking different participant groups to report their driving status in many cases. Although questionnaire surveys can provide reliable results, this method seems to repeatedly induce conflicting conclusions. On the other hand, studies based on driving simulations or on-road experiments have their own strengths and weaknesses, as discussed in detail in Section 3 and Section 4. As noted by van Wee and Banister [36] on their reviews, we do not aim to criticize one type of conclusions or completely rebut a method. Instead, we expect to address three questions in this review:
What approaches and conclusions were used and deduced while evaluating older people’s driving behavior and stress?
What are the advantages, disadvantages, and reliabilities of these approaches?
Is it possible to apply novel methods to study older drivers’ behavior and stress?
In Section 2, we describe the methodology adopted for preparing this review. Then, we present an overview on different methods and results regarding older adults’ driving behavior and stress, and state the advantages and disadvantages of existing methods in Section 3. In addition, we suggest a more comprehensive hybrid method to study older adults’ driving behavior and stress in Section 4. Finally, we draw conclusions in Section 5.
2. Methodology
We used ScienceDirect, Google Scholar, Taylor & Francis Online, Scopus, Web of Science, and less often CiNii (in Japanese) and CNKI (in Chinese) to search for papers reporting studies in fields related to older adults’ driving behavior and stress. To collect policy information and official reports, we mainly relied on two search engines: Google and Yahoo. The search terms included two groups: (1) old, older, older driver, older adult, and similar terms; (2) driving behavior, driving stress, and similar terms. In addition, driving behaviors were replaced by specific keywords, such as speeding, acceleration, and left/right turn. Logic OR was used within the same group, and logic AND was used across groups. Previous reviews were also searched, collected, and analyzed [37,38,39,40]. We reviewed more than 200 relative papers and reports. For introducing and comparing the latest studies and conclusions, about 120 references are included in this manuscript, and only about ten of them were published in the last century. Moreover, most sources, including official statistics and government information (e.g., population, population rates, policies, regulations), were no earlier than 2013.
3. Studies on Older Drivers’ Behavior and Stress: Objectives, Methods, and Results
3.1. Definition of ‘Older Driver’
Aging is a natural worldwide phenomenon that is being increasingly investigated in different areas. However, a basic but mostly overlooked aspect is the definition of ‘older people’, which can vary across organizations, countries, and studies. These varying definitions may lead to different conclusions. Moreover, disagreements and debates may originate and increase around different concepts, especially if they share the same name [41].
Table 1 lists definitions of ‘older people/drivers’ adopted by different organizations and countries. In general, 60 and 65 years old are the main age thresholds that determine whether an individual is considered as an elder, but no consensus has been achieved among official definitions.
Table 1.
Definition of ‘older people/driver’ according to different organizations and countries.
Organization/Country | Definition of Older People/Driver | Reference |
---|---|---|
European Union | Above 65 years old (Younger group: 65–74 years old Older group: 75 years old and above) |
[8,43] |
UN | Above 60 years old (Younger group: 60–79 years old Older group: 80 years old and above) |
[3] |
Australia | Depends on source (60 or 65 years old and above, or 60 years old and above divided into age groups spanning 10 years) |
[44] |
China | 60 or 65 years old and above Driver over 70 years old must accept physical examination every year |
[45,46] |
France | 65 years old and above has letter S (senior) in vehicle sticker | [47] |
Germany | 60 years old and above (but retirement age is 65 years) | [48] |
Japan | 65 years old and above (by Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications statistics) 70 years old and above has vehicle sticker indicating senior 80 years old and above by the office of the Prime Minister of Japan |
[49,50,51] |
UK | 65 years old and above | [9] |
USA | 65 years old and above | [20,52] |
In addition, the meaning of ‘older driver’ sometimes varies depending on various conditions even in the same country. This definition may reflect cultural differences and current situations in different countries. Japan is a typical example of this phenomenon. According to data from the World Bank [42], Japan is facing the most serious problem of population ageing worldwide, and thus has the highest threshold in all countries. Another important aspect is in the subdivision of older people/drivers. The European Union and UN define younger and older groups of people/drivers. Although these subdivisions are different, we found that the age group definition is important, not only among older adults, but also among individuals in other age groups. Some examples of age thresholds and the group subdivisions are listed in Table 2 and Table 3.
Table 2.
Four main data for evaluating older people’s driving behavior and stress.
Data Source | Brief Introduction | Representative Example(s) | Reference |
---|---|---|---|
Self-reported data | Collected from questionaire surveys or telephone interviews. |
|
[53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60] |
Driving simulator data | Obtained from virtual driving environments. |
|
[61,62,63,64,65,66] |
On-road/in-vehicle data | Obtained from a variety of sensors, such as CAN (Controller are networks), cameras, and GPS. |
|
[67] |
Biomedical data | Detected by biomedical sensors |
|
[68,69,70,71,72] |
Table 3.
Methods and results from studies on older drivers’ behavior.
Result | Driving Behaviors Remain Stable throughout Lifespan | Driving Behaviors Deteriorate with Age | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Method | Reference | Age Defining Older Driver (Years) | Comparison Group(s) | Sample Size | Reference | Age defining Older Driver | Comparison Group(s) | Sample Size |
Questionnaire, telephone, or official statistic | [42] | ≥75 | None | 888 | [54] | ≥60 | None | 104 |
[95] | ≥55 | 18–34 years old, 35–54 years old |
18–34 years old: 1522, 35–54 years old: 2726, >55 years old: 1883 |
[96] | Unknown (average age of 71.24) | Unknown(average age of 21.29 years) | Younger group: 24, Older group: 21. |
|
[82] | 45–59: Older drivers, ≥60: senior drivers |
18–29 years old, 30–44 years old |
18–29 years old: 419, 30–44 years old: 357, 45–59 years old: 328, ≥60: 139 |
|||||
[97] | ≥55 | None | 1279 | |||||
[78] | 36–60 years old (older group) | 18–35 years old | 216 | |||||
[98] | ≥65 | None | 350 | |||||
[99] | – | – | – | |||||
Driving simulator | [84] | ≥60 | <25 years old: younger drivers *, 30–45 years old: middle-aged drivers |
Younger drivers: 10, Middle-aged drivers: 11, Older drivers: 10 |
[100] | ≥65 | 19–22 years old | Younger drivers: 12, Older drivers: 12 |
[101] | ≥60 | 18–40 years old | Younger drivers: 38, Older drivers: 38 |
|||||
[102] | ≥65 | None | 99 | |||||
[103] | ≥65 | 18–33 years old (younger adults) | Younger adults: 45, Older adults: 40 |
|||||
[104] | ≥65 | None | 297 | |||||
On-road/in-vehicle experiments | [79] | ≥75 | None | 344 | ||||
[80] | ≥75 | None | 182 | |||||
[83] | ≥65 | <25 years old: young drivers, 25–65 years old: middle-aged drivers |
Unknown |
*: The group name is the same as that in the original reference.
3.2. Data Usage, Collection and Analysis
Table 2 summarized four main kinds of data, which are mainly used for studies on older people’s driving behavior and stress.
By comparing data among different participants and driving conditions, researchers draw conclusions about older people’s driving and travel characteristics, and then give suggestions for driving safety.
The advantages and disadvantages of the different evaluation methods are discussed in Section 3.5.
3.3. Results about Older Drivers’ Behavior
Various studies [73,74,75,76,77] about driving behavior provide a gradual construction of a hierarchical system, in which behavior has different meanings for different transport users. Besides older driver, driving behavior should be defined in each study. In this review, we consider both driving behaviors (e.g., accelerating, decelerating, speeding, braking time/distance, turning time) and travel behaviors (e.g., road selection, left/right turn selection, trip frequency, trip length, non-home-based trip rate) as driving behaviors. Each indicator can evaluate the participant’s behavior as good or bad compared with a baseline, or as better or worse across groups. For instance, speeding can be measured directly during an experiment if the speed limit is known. In contrast, most indicators are relative and should be compared across age groups. In Section 3.4, common driving behavior indicators are listed along with biomedical indicators.
Two main and opposing results of studies on older drivers’ behavior can be summarized as follows:
Driving behaviors remain stable throughout the lifespan.
Driving behaviors deteriorate with age.
The gap between these two viewpoints has deepened enough to lead different research directions and vehicle/transportation design. Table 3 lists representative studies, in which 1/3 studies supported point 1 above. Although these papers do not represent the proportion of studies reaching to each result, we found that most studies determined that the driving behavior of older people is worse than that of people in other age groups.
As noted in Section 3.1, the definition of ‘older driver’ and comparison groups vary across studies. Although the studies seem to be focused on the same topic, we determined different study objects in most cases. For instance, 36-years-old and 75-years-old participants might belong to different age groups although some researchers defined both of them as “older drivers” [78,79,80,81]. Another example is that Delhomme et al. [82] divided participants into older and senior drivers groups but some other studies classified the participants into younger and middle-age groups [83,84]. Whether their conclusions relied on the classification method is still unclear [85].
From another viewpoint, Table 3 shows that most existing studies were based on self-reported information, and few considered experiments to collect on-road/in-vehicle data. It is difficult to determine the underlying reason for these study setups. We assume that questionnaire surveys are more appealing because they allow to gather much more data in a relatively shorter period compared to conducting experiments. However, each method has its own strengths and weaknesses. Despite its widespread use, the superiority of self-reported data over other methods is not conclusive. For example, several studies have reported that questionnaire and naturalistic driving data reached to the same conclusions only in a few situations (e.g., the habit of avoiding nighttime driving) [86,87,88]. Moreover, questionnaires may be more unreliable when applied to older drivers, possibly distorting conclusions. In Section 3.5, we discuss each method in more detail.
It is worth noting other conditions related to driving behaviors. Hakamies-Blomqvist and Wahlström [89] suggested that age–behavior relations must consider other factors such as gender. One of specific examples is that female older participants drove more carefully than male older drivers [90]. Some other researchers [91,92] also claimed that driving experience and living habits were real impact factors on driving and travel behaviors rather than age. For instance, retirement allows older adults to travel on weekdays and avoid rush hours. Therefore, driving behaviors such as speeding and road selection vary according to these specific factors. Shen et al. [93] confirmed that older adults are more likely to travel in the daytime, while younger adults drive more frequently after 16:00. Considering that fatal accidents in the daytime are only one-third of those at night [94], less road traffic incidents may be related to older drivers. Overall, results related to older drivers’ behavior suggest that although age is an important factor, it should be considered in the context of other factors.
3.4. Results about Older Drivers’ Stress
Stress has been proved to be a key factor in driving/transportation safety. Nearly fifty years ago, researchers claimed that stressful drivers were five times more likely to cause fatal accidents than others [105]. At the beginning of this section, we should note that stress is thought of as a result when coping resources might be insufficient to meet situational demands [106]. Thus, stress may be generated or escalated by not only age but also many subjective and objective factors [106,107,108,109].
Table 4 lists some representative factors that may influence driving stress.
Table 4.
Influence factors on driving stress.
Category | Influence Factors On Driving Stress |
---|---|
Subjective Factors of Drivers |
|
Objective Factors of Driving Environment |
|
Table 5 lists information from some representative studies on driving stress of older adults. Their research methods and conclusions, like the reviewed studies on driving behavior, have the following four characteristics:
The definition of ‘older driver’ either varies when comparing multiple age groups/single age group or is not established.
Most studies employed questionnaire surveys, and few considered on-road experiments.
Questionnaire surveys allowed collecting more data than studies based on simulators and on-road experiments.
Most studies concluded that driving stress increases with age, and few found significant differences between older adults and individuals in other age groups.
Table 5.
Methods and results from studies on older drivers’ stress.
Result | Driving Stress Remains Stable throughout Lifespan | Driving Stress Increases with Age | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Method | Reference | Age Defining Older Driver (Years) | Comparison Group(s) | Sample Size | Reference | Age Defining Older Driver (Years) | Comparison Group(s) | Sample Size |
Questionnaire, telephone interview, or official statistic | [110] | >34 | Others | Older drivers: 130, Others: 165 |
[111] | ≥67 | 48–67 years old: baby boomer | Baby boomer: 198, Older: 201 |
[89] | ≥70 | None | Unknown | |||||
[112] | Unknown | Unknown | 914 | |||||
[113] | ≥65 | ≤24 years old, 25–64 years old |
≤24 years old: 109 25–64 years old: 423 ≥65 years old: 83 |
|||||
Simulator | [84] | >60 | <25 years old: young drivers *, 30–45 years old: middle-aged drivers |
Young drivers: 10, Middle-aged drivers: 11, Older drivers: 10 |
[102] | ≥64 | None | 99 |
[114] | ≥65 | 18–25 years old: young | Young: 10, Old: 10 |
|||||
[103] | ≥65 | 18–33 years old: younger adults | Younger adults: 45, Older adults: 40 |
|||||
On-road/in-vehicle experiment | [115] ** | ≥65 | ≤25 years old: younger, 35–55 years old: middle-age |
Younger: 20, Middle-age: 20, Older: 14 |
*: The group name is the same as that in the original reference. **: Used biomedical data.
It is worth mentioning various results. Matthews et al. [106] evaluated the mechanism linking driving stress and behavior under various traffic situations. According to Dorn [107], although a stress–behavior relation exists, it is difficult to establish a fixed relation across age groups. Qu et al. [108] confirmed a correlation between driving stress and dangerous driving behaviors and claimed that gender should be considered as an interaction factor. These studies support the idea that age should not be evaluated as a single influencing factor when studying driving stress.
Unlike driving behavior, driving stress is difficult to measure directly. Lee and Winston [109] were clear: as the psychological reaction to driving situations often leads to dangerous driving behaviors, they can allow to evaluate driver’s stress. Therefore, there are two methods to determine driving stress through questionnaire surveys, either directly asking drivers to evaluate driving stress by themselves (i.e., driving stress questionnaire) or estimating the stress level from their driving behaviors. The second method is also adopted when using driving simulators or on-road experiments. Moreover, Lee and Winston [109] summarized stress-related driving behaviors and quantified stress–behavior correlations for estimation in other driving stress experiments. However, they also noted that the results are affected by participant’s emotional status, which is highly subjective and varies according to different factors, such as age, gender, and driving experience.
Stress is usually evaluated using physiological data. Although bio-signal collection and analysis technology have substantially evolved in the past decades, few studies on older adults’ driving stress have used this technology. This might be partially caused by the cost, because the same system setup should be used in each participant and in the same periods considering the influence of factors such as the circadian rhythm [116]. On the other hand, constructing relations among aging, stress, and physiological data may be difficult [117].
Table 6 lists common behavior indicators and biomedical indicators related to driving stress, where the latter can be measured by the corresponding medical devices.
Table 6.
Behavior indicators and biomedical indicators related to driving stress.
Type | Indicator | Performance, Trend or Response under Stress | |
---|---|---|---|
Driving behavior (general definition) |
Driving behavior | Speed | Unstable and frequently changing between low and high values |
Speeding | Driver fails to control speed by unawareness of speeding | ||
Acceleration/deceleration | Sudden | ||
Braking time/distance | Sudden braking with shorter distance by late reaction to dangerous objects/conditions | ||
Turning time | Takes longer than without stress at intersections | ||
Lane-keeping | Inability to maintain the vehicle in lane center and frequent steering to adjust vehicle position | ||
Travel behavior | Road selection | Avoid expressways | |
Left/right turn selection | Avoid turning right in countries where driving occurs on the left side of the road and vice versa | ||
Trip frequency | Avoid or give up driving | ||
Driving area and destination distribution | Prefer driving in familiar areas such as places near home | ||
Trip length | Avoid long-distance driving | ||
Driving time | Avoid driving at night, commuting times, and weekends | ||
Non-home-based trip rate | Low non-home-based trip rate when driving with stress and preference to home-based trips. | ||
Biomedical data | Skin potential | Skin conductance response | Increase |
Skin conductance level | Increase | ||
Skin potential response | Increase | ||
Cardiovascular system | Diastolic blood pressure | Increase | |
Heart rate | Increase | ||
Heart rate variability | Decrease | ||
Low-frequency component of heart rate variability | Increase | ||
Low-to-high frequency component ratio of heart rate variability | Increase | ||
RR interval | Decrease | ||
Systolic blood pressure | Increase | ||
Cerebrovascular system | Oxyhemoglobin | Increase | |
Deoxyhemoglobin | Decrease | ||
Respiration | Respiration interval | Decrease | |
Respiration rate | Increase | ||
Peripheral capillary oxygen saturation | Decrease | ||
Temperature | Skin temperature | Decrease |
3.5. Advantages and Disadvantages of Methods Related to Older Drivers’ Behavior and Stress
The methods we discuss in this section are related to data collection rather than analysis. As analysis includes tools such as regression models, significance tests, and other statistical methods, it should be discussed in mathematical contexts.
Studies based on questionnaire surveys allow obtaining a relatively large amount of data with reduced cost and collection time compared to other methods, so most researchers perform studies based on these surveys to evaluate older drivers’ behavior and stress. However, Ross et al. found that 3/4 older participants inclined to overrate their driving abilities in their study [98]. Therefore, the concealment and bias [118] are the disadvantages of questionnaire surveys.
The advantages and disadvantages of research using driving simulators somehow represent the midpoint between research using questionnaire surveys and on-road experiments. However, simulators can only reconstruct a simplified driving environment. The complexity of on-road environment may sometimes be even higher than that of flying environments because car drivers must face continuous interactions with on-road objects. In addition, the simulator environment may result unfamiliar to participants, possibly increasing the participants’ driving stress. Therefore, the participants’ behavior in a simulation experiment can differ from that in a real scenario [67,119].
On-road/in-vehicle experiments need pre-education for participants but are more subjective than self-report questionnaire. In addition, Vhaduri et al. [120] developed a GPS-stress model (GStress) to estimate driving stress and obtained better results than survey-based studies.
Biomedical data are also as subjective as on-road/in-vehicle data, but the participants are usually fewer than for questionnaire-based studies because of the high cost of biomedical sensors. Table 7 describes these factors for different data collection methods. Overall, researchers must consider a balance between these factors according to the expected results and available resources.
Table 7.
Factors influencing methods to study older drivers’ behavior and stress.
Method/Source | Cost | Experiment Period | Sample Size | Objectivity |
---|---|---|---|---|
Questionnaire, telephone, or official statistic | +++ | +++ | +++ | + |
Driving simulator data | ++ | ++ | + | ++ |
On-road/in-vehicle experiment data | ++ | + | ++ | +++ |
Biomedical data | + | ++ | +++ |
Markers +++, ++, and + indicate the factor rating from best to worst. Marker—indicates undefined rating. The experiment period and sample size for Biomedical data collection depend on the experimental method (simulator or on-road/in-vehicle experiment).
4. Improving Studies on Older Drivers’ Behavior and Stress
After comparing various methods to study older drivers’ behavior and stress, we can conclude that no single method can provide all the suitable experimental factors listed in Table 5. Alternatively, a possible improvement may be a hybrid method, in which researchers conduct two or more experiments in a study to leverage the advantages and compensate for the deficiencies of different methods.
Ross et al. [98] conducted a questionnaire survey study and driving simulator experiments. Kanamori et al. [117] collected questionnaire answers, on-road data, and biomedical information, and jointly analyzed the obtained information. Yokoyama and Takahashi [121] evaluated both objective and subjective stress indicators in the same experiment and established their mathematical relation. These researchers claim that a hybrid method increases the accuracy of results. Moreover, Ross et al. [98] found that simulator-based driving training before paper-based learning can significantly improve older adults’ driving abilities.
While evaluating older people’s driving behaviors and stress, we suggest an hybrid method:
First, researchers should collect participants’ subjective data by questionnaires. In this step, Manchester Driver Behavior Questionnaire, Driving Habits Questionnaire, and Driving Behavior Rating Scale [55,56,57,58,59,60] can be used. In second step, simulator, on-road, or both-based experiments can be conducted to obtain objective data, and then the relationship between the previous two kinds of data will be established. It should be noted that biomedical data is especially important in this method because it can evaluate the reliability of relation model. Finally, reflex and reaction responses should be considered when analyzing driving behaviors. As some behaviors involve interaction between two or more parties, the actions of the all the parties should be recorded and considered. To the best of our knowledge, no study has jointly considered questionnaire, simulator, and on-road/in-vehicle camera data to date, possibly due to the high costs, intense work, and high complexity associated with the experimental setup and data processing. Nevertheless, we expect that the continuously increasing computational capabilities may enable researchers to choose on-road/in-vehicle video data for more comprehensive analyses. Moreover, recent optimized methods and technological progress may lead to better tools or systems for both experiments and analyses.
5. Conclusions
By reviewing academic papers and government reports related to older drivers’ behavior and stress, we can answer the three questions posed in the introduction. First, questionnaire, driving simulator, and on-road/in-vehicle data have been used to collect driving information in different studies. Biomedical data collection can be added to simulator and on-road/in-vehicle experiments to further evaluate driving stress. Researchers have either compared older drivers and drivers in other age groups directly or subdivided participants into multiple age groups. Most existing studies and reports have varying definitions of ‘older driver’. In different studies, the age threshold in years for older driver can be 60, 65, or even higher. Consequently, the diverse grouping methods may influence study conclusions. Regarding driving behavior, some studies have reported deteriorating driving abilities with ageing, whereas others have not found significant differences between older drivers and drivers in other age groups. Two main conclusions have been drawn about driving stress: (1) driving stress escalates with age and (2) driving stress remains stable throughout the lifespan. It is worth noting that gender, driving experience, living positions, and some other factors should be considered while establishing regression model.
We found that most studies have been based on self-reported data, possibly due to its advantage of obtaining a relatively large dataset with reduced costs and in a short period. However, self-reported data lack objectivity, especially when collected from older drivers. In driving stress research, biomedical data can increase the reliability of results when collected from simulators or on-road experiments.
Until a new research model or technological system becomes available, we suggest using a hybrid method, which has already been used in several studies. This method requires collection of both subjective and objective data, and their relation is obtained through mathematical models. Moreover, on-road/in-vehicle cameras should be used to consider the action of other transport users, who usually influence driving behaviors. Based on data and analytical results, older drivers’ behavior and stress can be evaluated or estimated more comprehensively.
The contributions from this literature review can be summarizes as follows.
The review sums up methods and results from research relating older drivers, driving behaviors, and driving stress. It may provide guidelines to other researchers on the dimension and approach to conduct studies in this field.
It identifies problems in the objectives and methods adopted in existing research. For example, we cannot easily affirm or refute the conclusions of other studies, because their objectives may be different despite using similar terminology and concepts.
It suggests a hybrid method to mitigate bias in findings. Researchers can conduct their studies more objectively by applying surveys and collecting on-road/in-vehicle data. Biomedical indicators require further research to evaluate driving stress more objectively.
Overall, incident data have shown that older drivers are responsible for most crashes involving two or more road users, and some studies concluded that older drivers show risky driving behaviors. However, we should not only focus on accident rate because “traffic conflict” and “accident” represent different dimensions while evaluation driving behavior. As noted by Risser [122], a driver’s accident history may not reflect the performance of his driving behavior. Similarly, even a bad driver can stay accident-free for a relatively long period. Risser’s study measured several driving behaviors. Based on his research, we know that we should not easily say one driver had “good” or “bad” behavior without clarifying what elements of behavior were considered.
Moreover, we should note that not only driving education but also the underlying traffic system and car design should be improved, considering older drivers. For example, intelligent intersection infrastructures and advanced driver-assistance systems may reduce older drivers’ incidents in the future. Policy makers should not simply enforce older people to avoid driving, because the social and economic costs (e.g., supply of goods and services) will remain very high until the fundamental problems of older drivers’ safety are solved.
Last but not least, we urge the researchers in the field of older drivers’ behavior and psychology take time to reexamine their methodologies and conclusions because of the varying definitions of older driver and the objectivity of methods/sources. New findings or even opposite conclusions may be obtained in some research.
Acknowledgments
This paper and related publications/research contributed to the first author’s doctoral dissertation [123], in which further details and more results can be found.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, Y.Z. and T.Y.; methodology, Y.Z. and T.Y.; software, Y.Z.; validation, Y.Z.; formal analysis, Y.Z. and T.Y.; investigation, Y.Z. and T.Y.; resources, Y.Z.; data curation, Y.Z. and T.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.Z. and T.Y.; writing-review and editing, Y.Z. and T.Y. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This study was financially supported by the Committee on Advanced Road Technology (CART), Ministry of Land, Infrastructure, Transport, and Tourism, Japan (Development of Evaluation Method of Mobility Space for the Next-generation Mobility Society, No. 26-1).
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Cabinet Office, Government of Japan Current Status of Traffic Accidents and Traffic Safety Policies (Ver. 2014) [(accessed on 21 July 2017)]; Available online: http://www8.cao.go.jp/koutu/taisaku/h27kou_haku/zenbun/genkyo/h1/h1b1s1_2.html.
- 2.Crampton A. Global aging: Emerging challenges. Pardee Papers. 2009;6:1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 3.United Nations World Population Ageing 2015. [(accessed on 21 July 2017)]; Available online: http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/publications/pdf/ageing/WPA2015_Report.pdf.
- 4.National Police Agency, Japan Driving License Statistics Ver. [(accessed on 21 July 2017)];2014 Available online: https://www.npa.go.jp/toukei/menkyo/index.htm.
- 5.National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) Clinician’s Guide to Assessing and Counseling Older Drivers. [(accessed on 21 July 2017)];2015 Available online: https://www.dmv.virginia.gov/drivers/pdf/older.pdf.
- 6.Thomas F.D., III, Blomberg R.D., Knodler M., Matthew R.E., Romoser M.R.E. Licensing Procedures for Older Drivers. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration; Washington, DC, USA: 2013. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]. Available online: https://www.nhtsa.gov/files/licensing-procedures-older-drivers. [Google Scholar]
- 7.US Census Bureau . Washington, DC, USA: 2014. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]. Projections of the Population by Sex and Age for the United States: 2015 to 2060. Available online: https://www2.census.gov/programs-surveys/popproj/tables/2014/2014-summary-tables/np2014-t9.xls. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Polders E., Brijs T., Vlahogianni E., Papadimitriou E., Yannis G., Leopold F., Durso C., Diamandouros K. Elder Safe—Risks and Countermeasures for Road Traffic of the Elderly in Europe. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/transport/road_safety/sites/roadsafety/files/pdf/studies/eldersafe_final_report.pdf.
- 9.Older Driver Task Force, UK. A National Older Driver Strategy. Supporting Safe Driving into Old Age. [(accessed on 9 May 2021)];2017 Available online: https://roadsafetyfoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/11/modsfl.pdf.
- 10.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Injury Prevention & Control. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2017 Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisqars/index.html.
- 11.National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) Washington, DC, USA: 2016. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]. Traffic Safety Facts 2014: Older Population. Available online: https://crashstats.nhtsa.dot.gov/Api/Public/ViewPublication/812273. [Google Scholar]
- 12.British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) Who, What, Why: How Dangerous are Elderly Drivers? [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2013 Available online: http://www.bbc.com/news/magazine-24204489.
- 13.Transport Accident Commission (TAC), Australia Road Safety for Ageing Road Users. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: http://www.tac.vic.gov.au/road-safety/safe-driving/older-drivers.
- 14.Langford J., Methorst R., Hakamies-Blomqvist L. Older drivers do not have a high crash risk—A replication of low mileage bias. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2006;38:574–578. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mitchell C.G. The licensing and safety of older drivers in Britain. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013;50:732–741. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fildes B.N., Corben B., Kent S.M., Oxley J., Le T.M., Ryan P. Older Road User Crashes. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];1994 Available online: https://trid.trb.org/view/415341.
- 17.Tefft B.C. Risks older drivers pose to themselves and to other road users. J. Saf. Res. 2008;39:577–582. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Langford J., Koppel S., Andrea D., Fildes B. Determining older driver crash responsibility from police and insurance data. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2006;7:343–351. doi: 10.1080/15389580600840706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Oxley J., Corben B., Fildes B., O’Hare M., Rothengatter T. Older vulnerable road users-Measures to reduce crash and injury risk. Monash Univ. Accid. Res. Cent. Rep. 2004;218:162. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Older Adult Drivers. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/motorvehiclesafety/older_adult_drivers/index.html.
- 21.United Nations UN Stress Management Booklet. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];1995 Available online: https://drc.ngo/media/2113524/un-stress-management-booklet.pdf.
- 22.Gulian E., Glendon A.I., Matthews G., Davies D.R., Debney L.M. The stress of driving: A diary study. Work Stress. 1990;4:7–16. doi: 10.1080/02678379008256960. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Anstey K.J., Wood J., Lord S., Walker J.G. Cognitive, sensory and physical factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2005;25:45–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ho G., Scialfa C.T., Caird J.K., Graw T. Visual Search for Traffic Signs: The Effects of Clutter, Luminance, and Aging. Hum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. 2001;43:194–207. doi: 10.1518/001872001775900922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Swain T.A., McGwin G., Wood J.M., Antin J.F., Owsley C. Naturalistic Driving Techniques and Association of Visual Risk Factors With At-Fault Crashes and Near Crashes by Older Drivers With Vision Impairment. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2021 doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2021.0862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wood J., Chaparro A., Hickson L. Interaction between visual status, driver age and distracters on daytime driving performance. Vision Res. 2009;49:2225–2231. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ortiz-Peregrina S., Ortiz C., Casares-López M., Castro-Torres J.J., Jiménez Del Barco L., Anera R.G. Impact of Age-Related Vision Changes on Driving. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:7416. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17207416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cambridge Dictionary Meaning of Drive in English. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/drive.
- 29.Cambridge Dictionary Meaning of Behaviour in English. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/behaviour.
- 30.Eby D.W. Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2004. Risky Driving; pp. 627–632. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dula C.S., Geller E.S. Risky, aggressive, or emotional driving: Addressing the need for consistent communication in research. J. Saf. Res. 2003;34:559–566. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2003.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hayley A.C., de Ridder B., Stough C., Ford T.C., Downey L.A. Emotional intelligence and risky driving behaviour in adults. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2017;49:124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2017.06.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jafarpour S., Rahimi-Movaghar V. Determinants of risky driving behavior: A narrative review. Med. J. Islam. Repub. Iran. 2014;28:142. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Yang L., Zhang X., Zhu X., Luo Y., Luo Y. Research on risky driving behavior of novice drivers. Sustainability. 2019;11:5556. doi: 10.3390/su11205556. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.European Commission (EC) Driver Licensing Legislation. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2013 Available online: http://www.consolproject.eu/attachments/article/16/CONSOL%20Report_WP5.1_final.pdf.
- 36.Van Wee B., Banister D. How to write a literature review paper? Transp. Rev. 2016;36:278–288. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kendrick D., Drummond A., Logan P., Barnes J., Worthington E. Systematic review of the epidemiology of non-collision injuries occurring to older people during use of public buses in high income countries. J. Transp. Health. 2015;2:394–405. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2015.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Pacheco M.T., Parmigiani M.M., de Fatima Andrade M., Morawska L., Kumar P. A review of emissions and concentrations of particulate matter in the three major metropolitan areas of Brazil. J. Transp. Health. 2017;4:53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2017.01.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Siren A., Haustein S. Driving licences and medical screening in old age: Review of literature and European licensing policies. J. Transp. Health. 2015;2:68–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2014.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Van Wee B., Ettema D. Travel behaviour and health: A conceptual model and research agenda. J. Transp. Health. 2016;3:240–248. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2016.07.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wittgenstein L. Philosophical investigations. Philosophische Untersuchungen. Basil Blackwell; Oxford, UK: 1953. [Google Scholar]
- 42.The World Bank Population Ages 65 and Above (% of Total Population) [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SP.POP.65UP.TO.ZS?most_recent_value_desc=true&view=chart.
- 43.European Commission (EC) Population. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2016 Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/transport/road_safety/specialist/knowledge/old/which_factors_will_influence_the_future_number_of_fatalities_among_older_drivers/population_en#_3.1_Population.
- 44.Australia Government Road Safety of Older Australians: Recent Statistics. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2016 Available online: https://bitre.gov.au/publications/2014/files/is_50_amended_2016_III.pdf.
- 45.State Information Center (China) China’s Population Aging Trend and Its Impact. [(accessed on 21 July 2017)]; Available online: http://www.sic.gov.cn/News/455/5900.htm.
- 46.The Ministry of Public Security of People’s Republic of China Regulations of Application and Use for Motor Vehicle Driver License (No. 139) [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: http://www.bjgaj.gov.cn/web/detail_getArticleInfo_429286_col1167.html.
- 47.Signal Senior Association. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: http://www.signalsenior.com/
- 48.Statistisches Bundesamt, Federal Statistical Office Wiesbaden Germany’s Population by 2050. Results of the 11th Coordinated Population Projection. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2006 Available online: https://www.destatis.de/EN/Publications/Specialized/Population/GermanyPopulation2050.pdf?__blob=publicationFile.
- 49.Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications (MIC), Japan the White Paper on Police Ver. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2015 Available online: https://www.npa.go.jp/hakusyo/h17/hakusho/h17/html/G1030000.html.
- 50.National Police Agency, Japan Road Traffic Law. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2002 Available online: http://www.houko.com/00/01/S35/105.HTM.
- 51.Prime Minister of Japan and His Cabinet Ministerial Meeting on Older Driver-Caused Accident Prevention. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2016 Available online: http://www.kantei.go.jp/jp/97_abe/actions/201611/15kakuryokaigi.html.
- 52.Granda T.M., Thompson S. The older driver comes of age. Public Roads. 2006;69:26–33. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Baker A.S., Litwack S.D., Clapp J.D., Beck J.G., Sloan D.M. The driving behavior survey as a measure of behavioral stress responses to MVA-related PTSD. Behav. Ther. 2014;45:444–453. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Baldock M.R.J., Mathias J.L., Mclean A.J., Berndt A. Self-regulation of driving and its relationship to driving ability among older adults. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2006;38:1038–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Owsley C., Stalvey B., Wells J., Sloane M.E. Older drivers and cataract: Driving habits and crash risk. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 1999;54:M203–M211. doi: 10.1093/gerona/54.4.M203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Af Wåhlberg A., Dorn L., Kline T. The Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire as a predictor of road traffic accidents. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 2011;12:66–86. doi: 10.1080/14639220903023376. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sârbescu P. Psychometric properties of the Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire in Romania: Validation of a cross-cultural version. Int. J. Traffic Transp. Psychol. 2013;1:20–27. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Reason J., Manstead A., Stradling S., Baxter J., Campbell K. Errors and violations on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics. 1990;33:1315–1332. doi: 10.1080/00140139008925335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Clapp J.D., Olsen S.A., Beck J.G., Palyo S.A., Grant D.M., Gudmundsdottir B., Marques L. The Driving Behavior Survey: Scale construction and validation. J. Anxiety Disord. 2011;25:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lajunen T., Parker D., Summala H. The Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire: A cross-cultural study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2004;36:231–238. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(02)00152-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lewis K., Hulme K., Kasprzak E., Moore-Russo D., Fabiano G. Motion simulation experiments for driver behavior and road vehicle dynamics. J. Comput. Inf. Sci. Eng. 2011;11:041001. doi: 10.1115/1.3617437. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lorentzen T., Kobayashi Y., Ito Y. Virtual reality driving simulation: Integrating infrastructure plans traffic models and driving behaviors; Proceedings of the 20th Annual Meeting and Exposition of the Intelligent Transportation Society of America; Houston, TX, USA. 3–5 May 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 63.The Ohio State University Driving Simulation Laboratory. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)]; Available online: http://drivesim.osu.edu/files/2015/08/Ohio_State-Driving_Simulation_Lab-handout.pdf.
- 64.Zeeb E. Daimler’s new full-scale, high-dynamic driving simulator—A technical overview; Proceedings of the Driving Simulation Conference Europe; Arts et Métiers ParisTech, France. 9–10 September 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Blissing B., Bruzelius F., Eriksson O. Driver behavior in mixed and virtual reality—A comparative study. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2019;61:229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2017.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Tateyama Y., Yamada H., Noyori J., Mori Y., Yamamoto K., Ogi T., Nishimura H., Kitamura N., Yashiro H. Observation of drivers’ behavior at narrow road in an immersive car driving simulator; Proceedings of the 9th ACM SIGGRAPH Conference on Virtual-Reality Continuum and its Applications in Industry; Seoul, Korea. 12–13 December 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Healey J.A., Picard R.W. Detecting stress during real-world driving tasks using physiological sensors. IEEE Trans. Intell. Transp. Syst. 2005;6:156–166. doi: 10.1109/TITS.2005.848368. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kreibig S.D. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 2010;84:394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Miyake S. Estimation of driver’s states. J. Soc. Automot. Eng. Japan. 2016;70:34–40. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 70.Sharma N., Gedeon T. Modeling a stress signal. Appl. Soft Comput. 2014;14:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.asoc.2013.09.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Singh R.R., Conjeti S., Banerjee R. A comparative evaluation of neural network classifiers for stress level analysis of automotive drivers using physiological signals. Biomed. Signal Process. Control. 2013;10:740–754. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2013.06.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Keskinen E., Hatakka M., Laapotti S., Katila A., Peräaho M. Traffic Transport Psychol. Elsevier; Bern, Switcerland: 2004. Driver behaviour as a hierarchical system. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Hatakka M., Keskinen E., Gregersen N.P., Glad A., Hernetkoski K. From control of the vehicle to personal self-control; broadening the perspectives to driver education. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2002;5:201–215. doi: 10.1016/S1369-8478(02)00018-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hatakka M. Novice Drivers’ Risk- and Self-Evaluations: Use of Questionnaires in Traffic Psychological Research, Method Development, General Trends in Four Sample Materials, and Connections with Behavior. Turun Yliopisto; Turku, Filand: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Michon J.A. A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know, what should we do? In: Evans L., Schwing R.S., editors. Human Behaviour and Safety. Springer; Boston, MA, USA: 1985. pp. 485–524. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Michon J.A. Transportation Planning for a Better Environment. Springer; Boston, MA, USA: 1976. The mutual impacts of transportation and human behaviour; pp. 221–235. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Newnam S., Mamo W.G., Tulu G.S. Exploring differences in driving behaviour across age and years of education of taxi drivers in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. Saf. Sci. 2014;68:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2014.02.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Chevalier A., Chevalier A.J., Clarke E., Coxon K., Brown J., Rogers K., Boufous S., Ivers R., Keay L. Naturalistic rapid deceleration data: Drivers aged 75 years and older. Data Brief. 2016;9:909–916. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2016.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Chevalier A., Coxon K., Chevalier A.J., Clarke E., Rogers K., Brown J., Boufous S., Ivers R., Keay L. Predictors of older drivers’ involvement in rapid deceleration events. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017;98:312–319. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Siren A., Meng A. Older drivers’ self-assessed driving skills, driving-related stress and self-regulation in traffic. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2013;17:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2012.10.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Delhomme P., Cristea M., Paran F. Self-reported frequency and perceived difficulty of adopting eco-friendly driving behavior according to gender, age, and environmental concern. Transp. Res. D Transp. Environ. 2013;20:55–58. doi: 10.1016/j.trd.2013.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Koththigoda S., Dissanayake S. Factors associated with severity of single-vehicle crashes involving older drivers with and without passengers; Proceedings of the Transportation Research Board 96th Annual Meeting; Washington, DC, USA. 8–12 January 2017; pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Horberry T., Anderson J., Regan M.A., Triggs T.J., Brown J. Driver distraction: The effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2006;38:185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2005.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Tahara Y., Iwadare M. Where the elderly move to: A review and a study of elderly migration flows in Japan. Komaba Stud. Hum. Geogr. 1999;13:1–53. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Agramunt S., Meuleners L., Chow K.C., Ng J.Q., Morlet N. A validation study comparing self-reported travel diaries and objective data obtained from in-vehicle monitoring devices in older drivers with bilateral cataract. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017;106:492–497. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Blanchard R.A., Myers A.M., Porter M.M. Correspondence between self-reported and objective measures of driving exposure and patterns in older drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010;42:523–529. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Molnar L.J., Charlton J.L., Eby D.W., Bogard S.E., Langford J., Koppel S., Kolenic G., Marshall S., Man-Son-Hing M. Self-regulation of driving by older adults: Comparison of self-report and objective driving data. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2013;20:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2013.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Hakamies-Blomqvist L., Wahlström B. Why do older drivers give up driving? Accid. Anal. Prev. 1998;30:305–312. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(97)00106-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Charlton J.L., Oxley J., Fildes B., Oxley P., Newstead S., Koppel S., O’Hare M. Characteristics of older drivers who adopt self-regulatory driving behaviours. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2006;9:363–373. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2006.06.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Andrews E., Westerman S.J. Age differences in simulated driving performance: Compensatory processes. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012;45:660–668. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.09.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.National Institute on Aging, USA Older Drivers. [(accessed on 25 April 2020)];2015 Available online: https://nihseniorhealth.gov/olderdrivers/howagingaffectsdriving/01.html.
- 92.Shen S., Koech W., Feng J., Rice T.M., Zhu M. A cross-sectional study of travel patterns of older adults in the USA during 2015: Implications for mobility and traffic safety. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e015780. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-015780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Varghese C., Shankar U. Traffic Safety Facts. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration; Wshington, DC, USA: 2007. Passenger vehicle occupant fatalities by day and night—A contrast; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Wickens C.M., Mann R.E., Stoduto G., Ialomiteanu A., Smart R.G. Age group differences in self-reported aggressive driving perpetration and victimization. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2011;14:400–412. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2011.04.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Bunce D., Young M.S., Blane A., Khugputh P. Age and inconsistency in driving performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012;49:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.MacLeod K.E., Satariano W.A., Ragland D.R. The impact of health problems on driving status among older adults. J. Transp. Health. 2014;1:86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2014.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ross L.A., Dodson J.E., Edwards J.D., Ackerman M.L., Ball K. Self-rated driving and driving safety in older adults. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012;48:523–527. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Rothe J.P. Rethinking Young Drivers. Transaction Publishers; Piscataway, NJ, USA: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Choi M., Mezuk B., Lohman M.C., Edwards J.D., Rebok G.W. Gender and racial disparities in driving cessation among older adults. J. Aging Health. 2013;25:147S–162S. doi: 10.1177/0898264313519886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Doroudgar S., Chuang H.M., Perry P.J., Thomas K., Bohnert K., Canedo J. Driving performance comparing older versus younger drivers. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2017;18:41–46. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2016.1194980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Joanisse M., Gagnon S., Voloaca M. The impact of stereotype threat on the simulated driving performance of older drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013;50:530–538. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.05.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Mather M., Gorlick M.A., Lighthall N.R. To brake or accelerate when the light turns yellow? Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:174–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02275.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Wood J.M., Horswill M.S., Lacherez P.F., Anstey K.J. Evaluation of screening tests for predicting older driver performance and safety assessed by an on-road test. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013;50:1161–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Brenner B., Selzer M.L. Risk of causing a fatal accident associated with alcoholism, psychopa-thology, and stress: Further analysis of previous data. Behav. Sci. 1969;14:490–495. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830140607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Dorn L. Driver Behaviour and Training. Volume 3 Ashgate Publishing; Farnham, UK: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 106.Matthews G., Dorn L., Hoyes T.W., Davies D.R., Glendon A.I., Taylor R.G. Driver stress and performance on a driving simulator. Hum. Factors. 1998;40:136–149. doi: 10.1518/001872098779480569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Qu W., Zhang Q., Zhao W., Zhang K., Ge Y. Validation of the Driver Stress Inventory in China: Relationship with dangerous driving behaviors. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016;87:50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Lee Y.C., Winston F.K. Stress induction techniques in a driving simulator and reactions from newly licensed drivers. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016;42:44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2016.06.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Argandar G.D., Gil F.T., Berlanga J.F. Measuring situations that stress Mexicans while driving. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016;37:154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2015.12.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Conlon E.G., Rahaley N., Davis J. The influence of age-related health difficulties and attitudes toward driving on driving self-regulation in the baby boomer and older adult generations. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017;102:12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Hill J.D., Boyle L.N. Driver stress as influenced by driving maneuvers and roadway conditions. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2007;10:177–186. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2006.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Staubach M. Factors correlated with traffic accidents as a basis for evaluating Advanced Driver Assistance Systems. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2009;41:1025–1033. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Kaber D., Zhang Y., Jin S., Mosaly P., Garner M. Effects of hazard exposure and roadway complexity on young and older driver situation awareness and performance. Transp. Res. F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2012;15:600–611. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2012.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Miller E.E. Master’s Thesis. University of Washington; Seattle, WA, USA: 2013. Effects of Roadway on Driver Stress: An On-Road Study Using Physiological Measures. [Google Scholar]
- 115.Tsuchikawa S., Iwakura S., Andoh A. Examination of the stress measuring on long distance trip using heart beat interval index; Proceedings of the Infrastructure Planning; Nagoya, Japan. 12–14 November 2002; (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 116.Kanamori R., Kubota J., Ando A., Yamamoto T., Morikawa T. A basic analysis about driving stress based on multiple biological data measurement; Proceedings of the 51th Infrastructure Planning and Management; Fukuoka, Japan. 6–7 June 2015; (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 117.McDonald J.D. Measuring personality constructs: The advantages and disadvantages of self-reports, informant reports and behavioural assessments. Enquire. 2008;1:1–19. [Google Scholar]
- 118.Brooks J.O., Goodenough R.R., Crisler M.C., Klein N.D., Alley R.L., Koon B.L., Logan W.C., Jr., Ogle J.H., Tyrrell R.A., Wills R.F. Simulator sickness during driving simulation studies. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010;42:788–796. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Reed M.P., Green P.A. Comparison of driving performance on-road and in a low-cost simulator using a concurrent telephone dialling task. Ergonomics. 1999;42:1015–1037. doi: 10.1080/001401399185117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Vhaduri S., Ali A., Sharmin M., Hovsepian K., Kumar S. Estimating drivers stress from GPS traces; Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications; Seattle, WA, USA. 17–19 September 2014; pp. 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Yokoyama Y., Takahashi I. Feasibility study on estimating subjective fatigue from heart rate time series. IEICE Trans. Fund. Electron. Commun. Comput. Sci. 2013;96:756–762. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 122.Risser R. Behavior in traffic conflict situations. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1985;17:179–197. doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(85)90020-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Zhao Y. Ph.D. Thesis. Nagoya University; Nagoya, Japan: 2017. A Study of Older Drivers’ Travel Patterns, Driving Behaviors, and Driving Stress. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.