Abstract
Background: Recent studies have shown that parental educational attainment is associated with a larger superior temporal cortical surface area associated with higher reading ability in children. Simultaneously, the marginalization-related diminished returns (MDRs) framework suggests that, due to structural racism and social stratification, returns of parental education are smaller for black and other racial/ethnic minority children compared to their white counterparts. Purpose: This study used a large national sample of 9–10-year-old American children to investigate associations between parental educational attainment, the right and left superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading ability across diverse racial/ethnic groups. Methods: This was a cross-sectional analysis that included 10,817 9–10-year-old children from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Parental educational attainment was treated as a five-level categorical variable. Children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface area and reading ability were continuous variables. Race/ethnicity was the moderator. To adjust for the nested nature of the ABCD data, mixed-effects regression models were used to test the associations between parental education, superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading ability overall and by race/ethnicity. Results: Overall, high parental educational attainment was associated with greater superior temporal cortical surface area and reading ability in children. In the pooled sample, we found statistically significant interactions between race/ethnicity and parental educational attainment on children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface area, suggesting that high parental educational attainment has a smaller boosting effect on children’s superior temporal cortical surface area for black than white children. We also found a significant interaction between race and the left superior temporal surface area on reading ability, indicating weaker associations for Alaskan Natives, Native Hawaiians, and Pacific Islanders (AIAN/NHPI) than white children. We also found interactions between race and parental educational attainment on reading ability, indicating more potent effects for black children than white children. Conclusion: While parental educational attainment may improve children’s superior temporal cortical surface area, promoting reading ability, this effect may be unequal across racial/ethnic groups. To minimize the racial/ethnic gap in children’s brain development and school achievement, we need to address societal barriers that diminish parental educational attainment’s marginal returns for middle-class minority families. Social and public policies need to go beyond equal access and address structural and societal barriers that hinder middle-class families of color and their children. Future research should test how racism, social stratification, segregation, and discrimination, which shape the daily lives of non-white individuals, take a toll on children’s brains and academic development.
Keywords: socioeconomic factors, population groups, brain development, reading, school performance, cortical surface, child, adolescents, magnetic resonance imaging
1. Background
Researchers have documented large, sustained racial/ethnic gaps in school performance and academic achievement in the United States [1,2,3,4]. Compared to white and Asian children, black, American Indian, Alaskan Native, Native Hawaiian, and Pacific Islander (AIAN/NHPI) children fare poorer in various school performance domains [5,6]. Jim Crow policies created high levels of racial segregation throughout the US that persist to this day. Racial and ethnic inequalities in school performance have emerged and deteriorated due to the neighborhood and school segregation [7,8,9]. School quality is substantially worse for children from racial and ethnic minority backgrounds and low socioeconomic status (SES) [10]. Furthermore, children from historically marginalized communities are exposed to greater levels of financial difficulties and psychosocial stressors [11] that results in lower memory, cognitive function, behavior, and school performance [12]. Additionally, greater exposure to indoor and outdoor toxins, such as air pollutants and lead [13], deteriorate the brain development of racial and ethnic minority brain development. Due to these social and physical environmental factors, researchers have observed lower academic achievement for racial and ethnic minorities [14]. The black–white achievement gap, for example, is a very well described phenomenon in the fields of education and psychology and has been sustained for centuries due to historical injustice and lack of political will to undo them [15,16].
Although academic disparities exist for math and science performance [17,18,19], racial and ethnic disparities in reading ability are of particular interest [20]. Here, we list five reasons: First, these differences are substantial in size. According to the National Assessment for Education Progress (NAEP) report, black students’ reading abilities (grades) were three-quarters of a standard deviation below white students’ scores, which is equal to a difference of four years of learning [21]. According to the Annie E. Casey Foundation, white students are three times more likely to be proficient at reading among fourth-graders than their black peers [22,23]. Second, the black–white reading gap has been stable since the 1970s [24,25]. Third, the reading gap has long-term consequences. Reading ability in elementary school is a strong predictor of reading ability 10 years later [26]. Reading is core to learning, and low reading ability has spill-over effects on almost all school achievement domains [27]. This is consistent with the observation that reading ability mediates the effect of parental education on neurocognitive abilities, such as executive functioning [28]. Fourth, the brain regions behind reading ability are well-established (e.g., the superior temporal cortex), making reading ability an ideal outcome variable in the neuroscience of children’s brain development [29,30,31]. Finally, some evidence suggests that the racial gap in children’s reading ability may explain some of the diminishing returns of SES for black rather than white adults later in life [32,33]. Reading is a good indicator of academic achievement and has strong connections to race/ethnicity, SES, brain development, and future health and economic outcomes. These reasons collectively make studying the associations between race/ethnicity, parental education, the superior temporal cortex, and reading ability a unique opportunity that can help us understand how race/ethnicity, SES (parental education), and brain development interact and shape racial and ethnic gaps in school performance, particularly for middle-class families.
The superior temporal cortical surface area is commonly studied in brain imaging of reading ability [34,35,36,37]. Research has already established the effects of SES and race/ethnicity effects on this brain region as an underlying mechanism that helps us understand how race and SES contribute to social inequalities in reading abilities [38,39]. The superior temporal cortex’s role in the comprehension of sentence meaning and structure [39], as well as naming words [35], is well established. As such, the superior temporal cortical surface area can be seen as a candidate brain region that can explain social inequalities in children’s reading ability [35,40]. A recent study by Merz, Noble, et al. [34] used structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (sMRI) data, as well as home-observation of family interactions, and explored associations between family SES, home linguistic input, left perisylvian cortical surface area (e.g., the superior temporal cortex), and reading skills among five to nine year-old children. Authors analyzed data of naturalistic home audio recordings in addition to T1-weighted MRI scans of the greater left perisylvian (e.g., the superior temporal cortex) cortical surface area. The study showed that family SES was associated with parent–child conversational turns, as well as parent words, which in turn were linked to a larger cortical surface area in areas such as the superior temporal cortex. Interestingly, language input partially mediated the effect of parental education on the left surface area in brain regions such as the superior temporal cortex (i.e., left perisylvian cortical surface area). Similarly, the left perisylvian surface area mediated the association between parental education and children’s reading skills. Merz et al. [34] concluded that morphometric brain features, such as the superior temporal cortex area, may be one of the mechanisms that explain why children with highly educated parents have better reading abilities. The researchers suggest that this is partly because more educated parents talk more frequently with their children, who get more opportunities to discuss ideas than children with lower educated parents. These bidirectional conversations enhance children’s language experiences and associated brain area, which in turn predicts children’s reading skills [34].
While parental education, the superior temporal cortex, and reading ability are linked, recent research by Assari and colleagues have reported that the magnitude of the associations between parental education (and other SES indicators), brain structures (e.g., cortical surface area), and behavioral outcomes are unequal across race/ethnic groups [41]. Marginalization-related diminished returns (MDRs) refer to systematically weaker effects of SES resources, particularly parental education, on a wide range of health and developmental outcomes of children from marginalized families compared to privileged families [42]. As a result of such MDRs [43,44,45,46], black children from high SES families remain at risk of smoking [47], aggression [48], depression [49], suicide [50], internalization [51,52], externalization [51,52], and poor school functioning [48] compared to white children with identical SES. High SES black children also show worse than expected grades in reading, math, and science than their white counterparts [53,54]. This pattern is not specific to black children as it is also shown for children from AIAN/NHPI, Latino, Asian American, and even marginalized white families [53,54,55,56,57].
In the Monitoring the Future (MTF) study, high parental education was associated with a larger enhancement of school performance for white children than black and Latino children [53]. While in the pooled sample and in white participants, we found an apparent step-wise increase in youth school performance with higher parental education, this step-wise improvement was absent for black and Hispanic youth [53]. We also observed similar findings in college students from the Healthy Mind Study (HMS, 2016), which showed that parental education had a weaker effect on the GPA of black than white college students [55]. In another study, using data from the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH 2013–2014) data, we found that, although high parental educational attainment was predictive of a higher GPA, race and ethnicity showed significant interactions with parental educational attainment on students’ GPA. As such, Hispanic and black children with highly educated parents still had lower GPAs, while white youth with the same parental education showed a higher gain in terms of GPA due to their parental education [54]. All these studies show that, across samples and age groups, high parental education promotes school performance of white more than black children and youth (i.e., MDRs).
In line with the MDRs described above, our analysis of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) data shows that the effects of SES indicators, particularly parental education, on brain function and structure are weaker for black than white children. For example, the effects of parental education on attention, working memory, and inhibitory control are all weaker for black than white children [58,59,60]. Analysis of the sMRI and fMRI data from the same sample also indicates that SES effects on hippocampus and amygdala volume [61] and the cortical surface area [41] were weaker for black than white children. Similarly, some of the effects of age on brain function [62,63] were weaker in black than white children. These observations are consistent with weaker SES effects on behavioral (externalizing) and mental health (depression, suicide, and stress) outcomes for black than white children [50,51,64]. We are not aware of any studies conducted explicitly on differential associations between SES, the superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading ability by race/ethnicity. Thus, there is a need to test the relevance of MDRs for the associations between parental education, the superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading scores among American children.
Since any research in the field of race, SES, brain, and cognitive function has the potential to be misinterpreted, we emphasize that MDR theory does not attribute these racial and ethnic disparities in SES effects on brain development and cognitive outcomes to biology. Instead, MDRs attribute them to social forces, historical injustice, and inaction to undo these disparities. Thus, MDR research takes a social rather than biological perspective to explain the observed disparities. These MDRs are shaped by contextual factors that undermine marginalized people’s life chances, broadly defined to include immigrants, Latino, Asian, AIAN/NHPI, and black individuals. The racial and SES gap in school performance is due to contextual and social factors, not immutable biological differences across race/ethnic groups. As these patterns are shown in various racial and ethnic minorities, but also immigrants [64,65], LGBT [66,67], and even marginalized white people [57], suggesting that any type of social marginalization reduces the gains of SES, black communities should not be blamed for undesired outcomes. These gaps are believed to be due to social, contextual, and environmental (modifiable) factors, such as low school quality in urban areas [68], residential segregation [69], and labor market discrimination [45], which all reduce the return of educational attainment for black communities. Beyond the work by our team [43,44], other scholars, such as Hudson [70], Thorpe [71], Williams [72], Ferraro [73], and others [74], have also described the same phenomenon, showing that SES effects tend to be weaker for non-whites than whites. However, most of that literature has not used the term MDRs.
2. Aims
Built on the MDRs phenomenon overall [44,45,75,76], MDRs of parental education on cognitive ability [60,62,63,77], school performance [53,54,55,57], and brain structures [41] in particular, we used data from Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study [75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82] to compare the associations between parental educational attainment, the superior temporal cortical surface area, and readability across diverse racial/ethnic groups in a large, diverse national sample of American children. In line with a recent work by Merz [34], we hypothesized positive associations between parental educational attainment, the superior temporal cortical surface area, and children’s reading ability. However, guided by an MDR perspective [41], we hypothesized weaker associations between these constructs in non-white than white children. In other terms, we expected parental education to be associated with a smaller increase in the superior temporal cortical surface area and the reading ability for non-white than white children. Similarly, based on the MDRs, we expected an increase in the superior temporal cortical surface area associated with a smaller increase in reading ability for non-white than white children. For two reasons, we expect more pronounced differences between black and white children than other racial and ethnic groups: First, because research has demonstrated stronger MDRs for black individuals than other non-white groups [44,45,76], and second, because the history of oppression and suppression in black communities is not comparable with the experiences of other groups [83,84].
3. Methods
Our cross-sectional study used data from wave 1 of the ABCD study [75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82]. ABCD is a state-of-the-art study of brain development of 9–10-year-old American children [75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82]. The ABCD study includes a diverse sample regarding race/ethnicity, SES, and family structure. Although the sample is not nationally representative, the ABCD study has used sampling strategies to increase the results’ generalizability to the US sample [80]. Although ABCD has a national sample that is recruited from 21 sites across 15 states, the sample is not nationally representative.
The ABCD sample is mainly recruited from schools [80]. To increase the number of non-white participants, particularly black children, recruitment also occurred outside school contexts in collaboration with local community organizations [80]. Comparing racial/ethnic groups in the ABCD sample requires attention to these different sampling strategies. Nevertheless, over 100 papers support MDRs across national and local studies, with various sampling designs. Thus, issues with sampling design should not be consequential as MDRs are robust regardless of the sampling design [85].
Participants were enrolled between 2016 and 2018 by 21 ABCD sites that were located in the following 15 states: Maryland, California, Colorado, Connecticut, Florida, Michigan, Minnesota, Missouri, New York, Oklahoma, Oregon, Pittsburgh, South Carolina, Utah, Vermont, Virginia, and Wisconsin. The number of participants across each site is shown in Appendix C. The number of participants enrolled over time is shown in Appendix D.
3.1. Measures
This study used data on demographics, race/ethnicity, SES, sMRI, and reading tasks. Our demographic variables included age, sex, family structure, and parental education, and our sMRI data was limited to the superior temporal cortical surface area. Reading was measured using NIH Toolbox [86,87].
Race. This study conceptualized race as a social construction, which means we see race as not based on some innate and immutable scientific fact, but rather, the social meanings ascribed to racial categories. In this view, race is a proxy of racism, adversity, and living conditions, rather than genetics or biological differences.
Parental education. Parents reported the highest education level in their household, and this included the following categories: Less than high school, high school completed, some college, college graduated, and graduate studies. This variable captured both maternal and paternal education. This variable was a nominal variable, with less than high school as the reference group.
3.2. Scanning Protocol
Structural MRI was performed at 21 sites in the United States using a standardized protocol for imaging acquisition, processing, reconstruction, and quality control [77]. All structural MRI findings were screened for incidental findings by a neuroradiologist [77]. A full description of image acquisition and processing is available elsewhere [77]. Although the ABCD study has functional and diffusion MRI data, this study only used structural neuroimaging data (sMRI). In the ABCD study data, whole-brain coverage was achieved with the following indicators and features: isotropic voxel resolution of 1 × 1 × 1 × mm, 256 × 256 matrices, flip angle of 8°, an inversion delay of 1060 milliseconds, 176 to 225 sections, the field of view of 256 × 240 to 256, the field of view phase of 93.75% to 100%, repetition time of 2400 to 2500 milliseconds, echo time of 2 to 2.9 milliseconds, and parallel imaging of 1.5 × 2.2. The total image acquisition time varied between 5 min and 38 s to 7 min and 12 s [77].
3.3. Image Reconstruction
The ABCD study team generated structural MRI data from T1-weighted and T2-weighted images. These data were corrected for gradient non-linearity distortions to ensure reliability across multiple imaging sites [88]. Intensity nonuniformity correction was based on tissue segmentation and sparse spatial smoothing. The ABCD study team then resampled images with 1-mm isotropic voxels into rigid alignment within the brain atlas. The cerebral cortex was then reconstructed, and volumetric segmentation was performed using FreeSurfer software, version 5.3.0 (Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA) [89,90]. Skull and nonbrain material were stripped from images. White matter and gray matter segmentation were performed by the ABCD study team using mesh creation. Topologic defects were corrected using the procedures described by Fischl et al. [91] and Ségonne et al. [92]. Images underwent surface optimization [93] and nonlinear registration to a spherical surface-based atlas [94]. The ABCD team then parcellated and labeled the cortical regions using a surface-based atlas classification that provides brain region of interest (ROIs)-level results [95]. The ABCD study team conducted all the procedures described above, and the ROI data are freely available within the data releases. We also used pre-processed MRI data, which is available in the released ABCD data files. The only sMRI data used for this analysis was the superior temporal cortical surface area. This was a continuous variable in mm3, with a higher score indicating a larger surface area of our ROI.
3.4. Imaging Quality Control
A detailed harmonization process was used to maximize similarity in the process across scanning sites. As such, ABCD study has heavily relied on comprehensive quality control and harmonization protocol across imaging sites to ensure the integrity and comparatively of the brain imaging data [77]. Images were manually reviewed for data quality. Images with the most severe artifact, irregularities, and/or poor image quality were rejected and excluded from processing and analysis. Cortical surface reconstruction images were reviewed for motion, intensity inhomogeneity, white matter underestimation, pial overestimation, magnetic susceptibility artifacts, and susceptibility artifacts [77].
3.5. Reading Ability
The ABCD study uses a wide range of cognitive measures [86,87]. Reading ability was measured using the NIH Toolbox Oral Reading Recognition Test Age 3+ v2.0 Uncorrected Standard Score. The NIH reading measure is one of NIH toolbox’s cognitive performance batteries [96]. These measures are highly reliable and valid [97] and widely used to assess reading [98] and other cognitive function aspects of children and adults [99]. The reading measure provides a continuous variable, with a higher score reflecting higher reading ability and comprehension of words, text, and sentence structures [100].
4. Data Analysis
We conducted the data analysis in the Data Exploration and Analysis Portal (DEAP). DEAP is an online platform based on the R-package, specifically designed for the analysis of ABCD data. Given the nested nature of the ABCD data, DEAP applied a mixed-effects regression model for data analysis. We calculated and reported regression coefficients for the following models: Model 1, the Main Effect model, did not include any interaction terms. Model 2, the interaction model, did include interaction terms between race/ethnicity and our independent variables. Thus, Model 2 was Model 1 plus interaction terms. These models were run for (1) the effects of parental education on the superior temporal cortical surface area, (2) the effects of the superior temporal cortical surface area on reading ability, and (3) the effects of parental education on reading ability. We did not adjust for multiple comparisons because we did not run exploratory models, did not have specific hypotheses for the associations tested, and our main goal was to test statistical interaction, which requires more power than regular analysis. Appendix A shows our modeling strategy for the three aims listed above. Appendix B show the results of our tests of assumptions, such as normality of the error terms for our outcomes and our models. We limited our sample to those with valid data on our variables of interest for our primary statistical analysis, but we did not limit our sample to any specific race/ethnicity. We ran models in the pooled sample and sub-sample that met restrictive quality control of the imaging for our sensitivity analysis. As results did not change, we reported the results with larger sample size. Without showing the numbers, our sensitivity analyses and robustness check results are mentioned at the end of the results section.
5. Results
5.1. Descriptive Data
Our descriptive statistics are shown in Table 1. This table shows the distribution of demographics, SES (parental education), the superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading ability for the overall sample and for racial/ethnic groups. This sample was mainly white (n = 7091; unweighted 65.6%; weighted 68.3%), followed by a mixed (n = 1298 unweighted 12.0% weighted 7.5%) racial group. The remaining sample was either black (n = 1646; unweighted 15.2%; weighted 13.9%), other race (n = 468; unweighted 4.3%; weighted 5.3%), Asian (n = 246; unweighted 2.3%; weighted 3.7%), or AIAN/NHPI (n = 68 unweighted 0.6%; weighted 1.2%).
Table 1.
Level | All | White | Black | Asian | AIAN/NHPI | Other-Race | Mixed-Race | p | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | Weighted | |||||||||
n | 10,817 | 7091 | 1646 | 246 | 68 | 468 | 1298 | |||||||||
Age (Month) | 119.04 (7.47) | 119.32 (7.49) | 119.10 (7.49) | 119.38 (7.50) | 118.86 (7.27) | 119.14 (7.28) | 119.54 (7.79) | 119.91 (7.77) | 118.00 (7.30) | 118.60 (7.20) | 118.75 (7.60) | 118.96 (7.61) | 118.94 (7.48) | 119.13 (7.59) | 0.437 | 0.478 |
Right Superior Temporal Surface Area (mm2) | 3911.63 (456.34) | 3897.79 (457.60) | 3951.42 (454.51) | 3932.68 (457.36) | 3755.05 (430.82) | 3742.63 (428.40) | 3907.72 (429.86) | 3901.32 (429.11) | 3931.47 (523.94) | 3934.30 (584.94) | 3824.22 (430.71) | 3815.88 (436.33) | 3924.04 (461.85) | 3919.07 (452.95) | <0.001 | <0.001 |
Left Superior Temporal Surface Area (mm2) | 4124.01 (507.98) | 4110.51 (510.07) | 4168.12 (507.51) | 4149.13 (510.35) | 3967.99 (477.53) | 3957.39 (477.14) | 4093.99 (539.90) | 4082.89 (536.78) | 4092.22 (559.98) | 4070.71 (610.38) | 4026.10 (470.90) | 4018.28 (471.30) | 4123.51 (508.28) | 4129.08 (501.39) | <0.001 | <0.001 |
Reading Ability | 90.96 (6.85) | 90.68(6.94) | 91.76 (6.45) | 91.44 (6.57) | 87.35 (7.02) | 87.00 (7.02) | 94.67 (6.43) | 94.52 (6.50) | 88.41 (7.64) | 87.81 (8.22) | 88.79 (6.89) | 88.95 (7.05) | 91.39 (6.96) | 90.50 (6.95) | <0.001 | <0.001 |
Parental Education | ||||||||||||||||
<High School Diploma | 470 (4.3) | (5.4) | 194 (2.7) | (3.7) | 137 (8.3) | (9.6) | 6 (2.4) | (2.1) | 6 (8.8) | (9.7) | 92 (19.7) | (19.5) | 35 (2.7) | (4.4) | <0.001 | <0.001 |
High School Diploma/Graduate Equivalency Degree | 978 (9.0) | (10.7) | 362 (5.1) | (6.7) | 404 (24.5) | (27.1) | 4 (1.6) | (2.1) | 16 (23.5) | (26.7) | 107 (22.9) | (23.5) | 85 (6.5) | (9.2) | ||
Some College | 2799 (25.9) | (29.7) | 1514 (21.4) | (26.8) | 647 (39.3) | (40.6) | 18 (7.3) | (7.9) | 29 (42.6) | (40.8) | 162 (34.6) | (32.8) | 429 (33.1) | (42.5) | ||
Bachelor | 2799 (25.9) | (24.4) | 2091 (29.5) | (27.9) | 230 (14.0) | (12.5) | 65 (26.4) | (27.0) | 14 (20.6) | (19.7) | 63 (13.5) | (13.8) | 336 (25.9) | (21.9) | ||
Post Graduate Degree | 3771 (34.9) | (29.8) | 2930 (41.3) | (34.9) | 228 (13.9) | (10.3) | 153 (62.2) | (61.0) | 3 (4.4) | (3.1) | 44 (9.4) | (10.4) | 413 (31.8) | (21.9) | ||
Sex | ||||||||||||||||
Female | 5175 (47.8) | (48.9) | 3337 (47.1) | (48.2) | 824 (50.1) | (51.2) | 127 (51.6) | (51.9) | 37 (54.4) | (58.4) | 213 (45.5) | (45.7) | 637 (49.1) | (50.7) | 0.095 | 0.095 |
Male | 5642 (52.2) | (51.1) | 3754 (52.9) | (51.8) | 822 (49.9) | (48.8) | 119 (48.4) | (48.1) | 31 (45.6) | (41.6) | 255 (54.5) | (54.3) | 661 (50.9) | (49.3) | ||
Married Family | ||||||||||||||||
No | 3402 (31.5) | (38.1) | 1506 (21.2) | (29.5) | 1156 (70.2) | (76.8) | 35 (14.2) | (15.6) | 33 (48.5) | (45.5) | 224 (47.9) | (51.1) | 448 (34.5) | (45.4) | <0.001 | <0.001 |
Yes | 7415 (68.5) | (61.9) | 5585 (78.8) | (70.5) | 490 (29.8) | (23.2) | 211 (85.8) | (84.4) | 35 (51.5) | (54.5) | 244 (52.1) | (48.9) | 850 (65.5) | (54.6) |
Table 1 also reports the results of ANOVA and Chi-square to compare the study variables across race/ethnicity groups. As this table shows, Asian and white children had higher parental education than black, other-race, and mixed-race children. Additionally, Asian and white children had a higher reading ability, and black, other-race, and mixed-race children had a lower reading ability. Similarly, Asian and white children were more likely to have married families, and black, AIAN/NHPI, other-race, and mixed-race children were less likely to have married families. Black, other-race, and mixed-race children had smaller right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas and white children had a larger right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas.
5.2. Model Fit
As mentioned before, Model 1, the Main Effect model, did not include any interaction terms. Model 2, the interaction model, did include interaction terms between race/ethnicity or our independent variables. Thus, Model 2 was Model 1 plus interaction terms. Our models fit statistics and showed a better fit in the presence of statistical interaction terms, regardless of the association that was being tested. This suggests that, regardless of the outcome, the inclusion of interactions between race/ethnicity and our predictors always explained more of the outcome’s variance than that without the inclusion of interaction terms (Table 2).
Table 2.
Independent Variable: Parental Education | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Outcome: Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area | ||||
Right | Left | |||
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Main Effects + Interactions |
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Main Effects + Interactions |
|
Effect Size | Effect Size | Effect Size | Effect Size | |
n | 10817 | 10817 | 10817 | 10817 |
R-squared | 0.11395 | 0.12775 | 0.16911 | 0.17197 |
ΔR-squared | 0.00218 (0.22%) | 0.03066 (3.07%) | 0.00975 (0.98%) | 0.02859 (2.86%) |
Independent Variable: Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area | ||||
Outcome: Reading Ability | ||||
Right | Left | |||
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Main Effects + Interactions |
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Main Effects + Interactions |
|
n | 11,117 | 11,117 | 11,117 | 11,117 |
R-squared | 0.17809 | 0.17825 | 0.17914 | 0.18 |
ΔR-squared | 0.00151 (0.15%) | 0.01571 (1.57%) | 0.00278 (0.28%) | 0.01781 (1.78%) |
Independent Variable: Parental Education | ||||
Outcome: Reading Ability | ||||
Model 1 Main Effects |
Model 2 Main Effects + Interactions |
|||
n | 11,440 | 11,440 | ||
R-squared | 0.17007 | 0.17289 | ||
ΔR-squared | 0.06144 (6.14%) | 0.092 (9.2%) |
5.3. Main Effects (Overall Effects)
Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 show our models without interaction terms (Model 1, right panel) and models with interaction terms (Model 2, left panel), both in the overall sample. As shown in the rights panels of these tables, there were positive associations between parental educational attainment, children’s right superior temporal cortical surface area, children’s left superior temporal cortical surface area, and children’s reading ability. To be more specific, the results confirm these hypotheses: (1) higher parental educational attainment is associated with larger right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas in children, (2) larger right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas are associated with higher reading ability in children, and (3) higher parental educational attainment is positively associated with higher reading ability in children. All these models adjusted for the nested nature of the data and controlled for covariates.
Table 3.
b | SE | p | sig | b | SE | p | sig | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||||
Outcome: Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area | ||||||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) | 35.86 | 23.16 | 0.122 | 88.31 | 35.33 | 0.012 | * | |
Parental Education (Some College) | 71.15 | 20.93 | 0.001 | *** | 122.72 | 30.55 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Bachelor) | 118.21 | 21.78 | <0.001 | *** | 183.71 | 30.63 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) | 170.79 | 21.67 | <0.001 | *** | 237.53 | 30.37 | <0.001 | *** |
Age (Month) | −1.02 | 0.50 | 0.043 | * | −1.01 | 0.51 | 0.045 | * |
Sex (Male) | 264.53 | 7.83 | <0.001 | *** | 265.47 | 7.84 | <0.001 | *** |
Married Family | 12.23 | 10.13 | 0.227 | 14.10 | 10.15 | 0.165 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −141.71 | 13.80 | <0.001 | *** | −36.91 | 46.41 | 0.426 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −123.16 | 25.70 | <0.001 | *** | −96.23 | 142.54 | 0.500 | |
Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 13.46 | 46.87 | 0.774 | −221.73 | 162.06 | 0.171 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −100.33 | 20.61 | <0.001 | *** | 5.91 | 50.17 | 0.906 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −44.54 | 14.78 | 0.003 | ** | 147.04 | 74.65 | 0.049 | * |
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −83.31 | 54.48 | 0.126 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −88.15 | 50.03 | 0.078 | # | ||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −134.58 | 55.56 | 0.015 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −161.81 | 56.43 | 0.004 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 28.99 | 230.69 | 0.900 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 46.65 | 167.17 | 0.780 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −44.80 | 150.31 | 0.766 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −33.20 | 146.11 | 0.820 | |||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 335.45 | 185.50 | 0.071 | # | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 221.71 | 178.12 | 0.213 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 189.23 | 191.30 | 0.323 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 669.07 | 281.32 | 0.017 | * | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −134.97 | 66.83 | 0.043 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −69.39 | 60.12 | 0.248 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −166.31 | 72.22 | 0.021 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −170.03 | 79.18 | 0.032 | * | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −188.38 | 90.57 | 0.038 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −175.63 | 78.41 | 0.025 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −198.47 | 80.36 | 0.014 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −222.69 | 79.69 | 0.005 | ** |
# p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 4.
b | SE | p | sig | b | SE | p | sig | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||||
Outcome: Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area | ||||||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) | 46.29 | 25.59 | 0.071 | # | 76.17 | 39.05 | 0.051 | # |
Parental Education (Some College) | 81.02 | 23.13 | <0.001 | *** | 106.93 | 33.77 | 0.002 | ** |
Parental Education (Bachelor) | 136.35 | 24.07 | <0.001 | *** | 183.84 | 33.87 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) | 185.33 | 23.95 | <0.001 | *** | 232.14 | 33.57 | <0.001 | *** |
Age (Month) | 0.35 | 0.55 | 0.525 | 0.37 | 0.55 | 0.506 | ||
Sex (Male) | 368.59 | 8.62 | <0.001 | *** | 369.85 | 8.62 | <0.001 | *** |
Married Family | 8.97 | 11.19 | 0.423 | 11.60 | 11.21 | 0.301 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −143.15 | 15.23 | <0.001 | *** | −50.34 | 51.25 | 0.326 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −146.29 | 28.43 | <0.001 | *** | −352.68 | 156.40 | 0.024 | * |
Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | −41.51 | 51.88 | 0.424 | −343.02 | 179.68 | 0.056 | # | |
Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −106.76 | 22.77 | <0.001 | *** | −35.54 | 55.38 | 0.521 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −49.96 | 16.27 | 0.002 | ** | 68.54 | 82.16 | 0.404 | |
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −66.00 | 60.14 | 0.272 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −60.76 | 55.24 | 0.271 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −160.15 | 61.31 | 0.009 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −150.43 | 62.24 | 0.016 | * | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 54.25 | 254.70 | 0.831 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 143.94 | 183.88 | 0.434 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 241.60 | 165.06 | 0.143 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 206.67 | 160.38 | 0.198 | |||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 491.49 | 205.56 | 0.017 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 278.92 | 197.45 | 0.158 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 172.63 | 211.98 | 0.415 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 778.50 | 311.03 | 0.012 | * | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −102.06 | 73.80 | 0.167 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −16.62 | 66.35 | 0.802 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −126.67 | 79.73 | 0.112 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −155.30 | 87.40 | 0.076 | # | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −115.38 | 99.69 | 0.247 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −84.07 | 86.30 | 0.330 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −131.70 | 88.42 | 0.136 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −159.03 | 87.66 | 0.070 | # |
# p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 5.
b | SE | p | sig | b | SE | p | sig | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||||
Outcome: Reading Ability | ||||||||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) | 0.0016 | 0.0001 | <0.001 | *** | 0.00166 | 0.00017 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) | 2.08 | 0.35 | <0.001 | *** | 2.07 | 0.35 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Some College) | 3.97 | 0.31 | <0.001 | *** | 3.97 | 0.31 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Bachelor) | 5.57 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** | 5.56 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) | 6.88 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** | 6.88 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** |
Age (Month) | 0.21 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** | 0.21 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** |
Sex (Male) | −0.57 | 0.12 | <0.001 | *** | −0.57 | 0.12 | <0.001 | *** |
Married Family | 0.80 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** | 0.80 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** |
Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −2.21 | 0.21 | <0.001 | *** | −0.78 | 1.45 | 0.590 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 1.56 | 0.38 | <0.001 | *** | 2.43 | 3.23 | 0.452 | |
Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | −0.93 | 0.67 | 0.163 | −3.95 | 4.77 | 0.408 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −0.44 | 0.31 | 0.156 | 1.06 | 2.42 | 0.661 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −0.04 | 0.22 | 0.855 | 1.27 | 1.77 | 0.472 | ||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −0.0004 | 0.0004 | 0.323 | |||||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −0.0002 | 0.0008 | 0.789 | |||||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 0.0008 | 0.0012 | 0.524 | |||||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −0.0004 | 0.0006 | 0.532 | |||||
Right Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −0.0003 | 0.0005 | 0.456 |
*** p < 0.001.
Table 6.
b | SE | p | sig | b | SE | p | sig | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||||
Outcome: Reading Ability | ||||||||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) | 0.0016 | 0.0001 | <0.001 | *** | 0.0017 | 0.0002 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) | 2.08 | 0.35 | <0.001 | *** | 2.07 | 0.35 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Some College) | 3.97 | 0.31 | <0.001 | *** | 3.97 | 0.31 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Bachelor) | 5.57 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** | 5.56 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) | 6.88 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** | 6.88 | 0.33 | <0.001 | *** |
Age (Month) | 0.21 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** | 0.21 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** |
Sex (Male) | −0.57 | 0.12 | <0.001 | *** | −0.57 | 0.12 | <0.001 | *** |
Married Family | 0.80 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** | 0.80 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** |
Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −2.21 | 0.21 | <0.001 | *** | −0.78 | 1.45 | 0.590 | |
Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 1.56 | 0.38 | <0.001 | *** | 2.43 | 3.23 | 0.452 | |
Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | −0.93 | 0.67 | 0.163 | −3.95 | 4.77 | 0.408 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −0.44 | 0.31 | 0.156 | 1.06 | 2.42 | 0.661 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −0.04 | 0.22 | 0.855 | 1.27 | 1.77 | 0.472 | ||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −0.0004 | 0.0004 | 0.323 | |||||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −0.0002 | 0.0008 | 0.789 | |||||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 0.0008 | 0.0012 | 0.524 | |||||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −0.0004 | 0.0006 | 0.532 | |||||
Left Superior Temporal Cortical Surface Area (mm2) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −0.0003 | 0.0005 | 0.456 |
*** p < 0.001.
5.4. Interaction Effects (Heterogeneity of the Effects by Race/Ethnicity)
Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 also show the results of models with the interaction terms in the right panel. As shown in these tables, there were significant interactions between race/ethnicity and our predictors on our overall sample outcomes. In most cases, the statistical significance of the interaction terms was indicative of differences in the slopes for black, AIAN/NHPI, other-race, or mixed-race children relative to white children. The positive associations were weaker for black, AIAN/NHPI, other-race, or mixed-race children relative to their white counterparts. The exception was the association between parental education of reading ability, which was stronger for black than white children (reverse interaction was found).
However, Table 7 shows an interaction that is opposite to the interactions described above. We found a positive association between high parental education and reading ability, which was stronger in black than white children.
Table 7.
b | SE | p | sig | b | SE | p | sig | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||||
Outcome: Reading Ability | ||||||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) | 2.10 | 0.34 | <0.001 | *** | 1.06 | 0.53 | 0.044 | * |
Parental Education (Some College) | 4.03 | 0.31 | <0.001 | *** | 2.95 | 0.46 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Bachelor) | 5.78 | 0.32 | <0.001 | *** | 4.77 | 0.46 | <0.001 | *** |
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) | 7.18 | 0.32 | <0.001 | *** | 6.08 | 0.45 | <0.001 | *** |
Age (Month) | 0.20 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** | 0.20 | 0.01 | <0.001 | *** |
Sex (Male) | −0.15 | 0.12 | 0.194 | −0.16 | 0.12 | 0.184 | ||
Married Family | 0.85 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** | 0.82 | 0.15 | <0.001 | *** |
Race/Ethnicity (Black) | −2.39 | 0.21 | <0.001 | *** | −4.47 | 0.68 | <0.001 | *** |
Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 1.44 | 0.38 | <0.001 | *** | 0.75 | 1.91 | 0.694 | |
Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | −0.95 | 0.66 | 0.153 | −5.44 | 2.48 | 0.028 | * | |
Race/Ethnicity (Other) | −0.56 | 0.31 | 0.069 | # | −1.64 | 0.75 | 0.028 | * |
Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | −0.14 | 0.22 | 0.510 | −2.60 | 1.10 | 0.018 | * | |
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | 1.88 | 0.80 | 0.020 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | 2.28 | 0.74 | 0.002 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | 2.42 | 0.82 | 0.003 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Black) | 2.20 | 0.84 | 0.009 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | −1.91 | 3.37 | 0.571 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 2.62 | 2.33 | 0.261 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 0.47 | 2.03 | 0.816 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Asian) | 0.72 | 1.97 | 0.715 | |||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 3.07 | 2.80 | 0.273 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 6.47 | 2.67 | 0.015 | * | ||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 2.90 | 2.93 | 0.322 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (AIAN/NHPI) | 5.19 | 3.68 | 0.159 | |||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | 2.58 | 0.99 | 0.009 | ** | ||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | 0.34 | 0.90 | 0.702 | |||||
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | 0.08 | 1.08 | 0.944 | |||||
Parental Education (Post Graduate Degree) × Race/Ethnicity (Other) | 2.20 | 1.17 | 0.061 | # | ||||
Parental Education (HS Diploma/GED) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | 1.77 | 1.33 | 0.182 | |||||
Parental Education (Some College) × Race/Ethnicity (Mixed) | 2.48 | 1.15 | 0.032 | * |
# p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
5.5. Figures
Appendix D includes a series of figures that depict the results shown in the above tables. Figure A4a, Figure A5a, Figure A6a, Figure A7a and Figure A8a showed that, in the pooled sample, parental educational attainment, children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface area, and children’s reading ability were all positively correlated. As shown by Figure A4b and Figure A5b, race/ethnicity showed statistically significant interactions with parental educational attainment on children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface area. These interactions suggest that high parental educational attainment has a smaller boosting effect on children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas for black, other-race, and mixed-race children than white children (in line with MDRs). Figure A6b shows that the association between the right superior temporal cortical surface area and reading ability is different across various racial/ethnic groups. Figure A7b shows that the association between the left superior temporal cortical surface area and reading ability is weaker for AIAN/NHPI than white children (in line with MDRs). Figure A8b shows that the association between parental education and reading ability is weaker in white than black children (in contrast to MDRs).
5.6. Sensitivity Analysis (Robustness Check)
Given the replicability crisis of psychological and social studies [101,102,103,104] and the need to test the replicability of the results [105] overall and in the ABCD study [106], we ran several robustness checks to further test the validity of our results . These included: (1) models in the absence and presence of covariates, (2) models in the full sample and those that met imaging quality indicators, (3) models with and without a propensity score, and (4) models on the right and the left-brain hemispheres. Without exception, the results were replicated regardless of our modeling strategy. We present results for right and left-brain hemispheres, but not other outcomes. The results of our replications are available upon request.
6. Discussion
Our results support our hypotheses about MDRs, as evidenced by our finding that parental educational attainment, the superior temporal cortical surface area, and reading ability are all correlated in 9–10-year-old children, but the magnitude of the associations differed across racial/ethnic groups. While the associations between parental educational attainment and children’s right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas, as well as the association between children’s left superior temporal cortical surface areas and children’s reading ability, are weaker in some non-white racial/ethnic groups of children than their white peers, the association between parental education and reading ability is unexpectedly stronger in black than white children.
The weaker observed associations in non-white than white children are consistent with other analyses of MDRs, using ABCD study data for a wide range of neurocognitive domains [102], supported by brain imaging [41]. To the best of our knowledge, the MDRs reported here were not known before: (1) the link between parental educational attainment and right and left superior temporal cortical surface areas and (2) the link between children’s left superior temporal cortical surface areas and children’s reading ability. These MDRs were found for various non-white groups of children, such as black, other-race, mixed-race, or AIAN/NHPI. This observation is consistent with the notion that MDRs are not specific to black children because we found them for all marginalized groups [56,57,67,106,107].
The observation of a stronger association between parental education and reading ability in black than white children did not support our hypotheses. However, this finding may be due to compensatory effects. For example, black parents may understand, from generations of disadvantage, that they must work twice as hard as white people to achieve half as much [70]. Nevertheless, structural factors are stacked against them, in terms of fewer educational resources, if they live in a district with predominantly non-white families [10], teacher discrimination [103], differential disciplinary actions [104], differential teacher expectations and grading [104], differential teacher preparedness [105], and less access to academic enrichment opportunities [106]. More educated black families may be more likely than their white counterparts to help their children in ways that are in their control, such as reading to them and supporting activities that may help reading more than other areas, such as math and science, which may require more time and resources. In other words, reading is easier to do as a family than math- or science-related enrichment and is therefore less influenced by MDRs. This interpretation is somewhat supported by the fact that we did find differences in child brain structures. This reading ability result may also indicate that families can overcome the negative effects of MDRs when they engage in enrichment activities of their own, but this does not change the fact that fundamental structural change is necessary because such activities are essentially a tax for black families as a way to keep up with their more privileged white counterparts. In some recent ABCD study data analyses, the SES effects on neurocognitive domains are weaker for black than white children [60,77,105]. In one study, the effects of parental education on mental rotation (a specific cognitive task) was weaker in black than white children [107]. In another analysis, the effect of parental education on inhibitory control was weaker for black than white children [102]. In another analysis of ABCD study data, age had weaker effects of inhibitory control for black than white children [63]. Similar results were found for the effects of age on children’s dimensional card changing activities [62].
In addition to the ABCD study data [102], similar MDRs are shown in several national studies, such as the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) [69,108,109,110,111], Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) [47,112,113,114], Education Longitudinal Study (ELS) [115], Monitoring the Future (MTF) [53], and National Survey of American Life (NSAL) [50], all showing weaker effects of SES indicators on outcomes for black children compared to white children. For example, in the PATH data, SES effects on future tobacco use [47], in the FFCWS study, SES effects on ADHD, impulsivity, obesity, and self-rated health [69,108,109,110,111], in ELS, SES effects on school quality [115], and in MTF, SES effects on school performance were all weaker for black than white children [53]. In a longitudinal study of Flint, for MI adolescents, the effect of having married parents on subsequent anxiety symptoms was weaker for black than white children who were transitioning to young adulthood [116]. In the NSAL-adolescents data, high SES positively correlated with depression for black boys [50]. Thus, the MDRs reported here (for two of three aims) are consistent with past research and not specific to a particular brain mechanism. Our findings add to the growing body of empirical work that suggests that these patterns are nearly universal. They also support that MDR-related disparities result from structural and contextual factors and are not due to deficient culture or behaviors.
In our study, the MDR hypothesis was found for black, other, mixed-race, and AIAN/NHPI families. Researchers have documented MDRs for black [48], Hispanic [48], Asian [56], AIAN/NHPI [117], and even marginalized white families [57]. These MDRs are also relevant to other types of marginalization based on sexual orientation and nativity (immigration) status. Individuals with LGBT [67,118] and immigrant status [64,65,119,120] show weaker health effects of SES than those with a non-LGBT and non-immigrant status. This non-specificity of MDRs across measures of the SES resource, outcomes, populations, and age groups suggests that MDRs are likely due to structural racism and social stratification, which cause disparities across domains [57]. Again, this universality of the patterns suggests that any deviation from majority norms and expectations (e.g., whiteness, straightness) comes with a penalty for the marginalized group. The privileges associated with the majority expectations include policy and laws, resource distribution, and opportunities for educational and economic advancement that contribute to inequality, and the odds are stacked against non-majority individuals, even at similar SES levels. Our results suggest that MDRs are strongest for black people whose American experience has been centuries and generations in the making [121]. These MDRs can reflect the existing structural racism in the US [83,84]. Another explanation of this phenomenon is that, in the absence of government interventions that undo some of the inequalities, the rich become richer and the poor, marginalized, and non-white become poorer [74]. This is because the Haves lead the making and take advantage of policies rather than the Have-Nots [74]. Unless some equalizing forces are in place, we cannot expect inequalities to narrow over time [122]. A recent report shows that governmental spending minimizes [123,124,125] the existing inequalities. For example, some research suggests that the Affordable Care Act (ACA) has reduced inequalities [126,127,128,129,130,131,132], in part because some of these applied policies may have larger effects on the underserved than privileged populations [133].
Reduced effects of parental education in black and white families might be a function of labor market discrimination and other unequal processes that hinder black people across SES and class lines [134]. Upward social mobility is very taxing and costly for black families, particularly black men [70]. Highly educated black people work in worse jobs than their white counterparts with similar education levels and receive lower salaries [135,136]. As a result, highly educated black people generate less wealth than white people with similar education [45]. Zajacova has argued that, instead of being an equalizer, education contributes and even generates inequality across populations [137,138]. These MDRs generate inequality without racist actors. This is why we interpret MDRs as indicators of structural racism. In the presence of MDRs, inequalities are regenerated, and poor outcomes are transitioned from one to the next generation. As such, MDRs can explain why inequalities persist over time, despite the overall improvement in SES and health [139].
In high SES families, stress remains at a higher level in black than white families [135,140]. Highly educated black families are more likely to remain in neighborhoods with high stress [69] and poor schools [115] than high SES white people. When high SES black families move for opportunity, they are heavily discriminated against at schools [141] and workplaces [142]. Such discrimination reduces black children’s GPA [103], mental health [143,144,145], and physical health [146,147,148]. Surprisingly, SES and discrimination are positively associated in black families [149,150,151]. The same association is reversed for white families [152]. As a result of all these structural inequalities, non-white children show worse than expected outcomes in high SES families, a pattern opposite to high SES white families. As explained, these processes are social, not biological, suggesting that these MDRs are shaped by racism, not genetics [43,44,45].
Our results also suggest that race needs to be conceptualized as a proxy of racism, social stratification, adversity, differential treatment, the legacy of slavery, Jim Crow, and other processes, such as interpersonal discrimination. In our framework, race is a proxy of social forces and constraint choices, rather than genes. We argue that racism alters the SES effects, so we observe race by SES interactions, as discussed here. As such, inequalities are modifiable via effective public policy that contributes to a more fair and equal American society. That said, MDRs indicate a social and physical context, educational opportunities, access to resources, and daily lived experiences of non-whites across SES levels and class lines. SES differently changes experiences and opportunities for whites and non-whites and other non-majority groups. As one’s race and ethnicity are more apparent than one’s SES, high SES non-whites are frequently discriminated against in society, a pattern which is also seen in schools [103]. Thus, we need to address societal barriers that diminish parental education’s marginal returns for middle-class racial and ethnic minority families if we want to minimize the racial/ethnic gap in children’s brain development, school achievement, and reading ability. Social and public policies need to go beyond equal access by addressing structural and societal barriers that hinder minority families and their children. Multi-level barriers, such as social stratification, unfair policing, poor schooling, segregation, racism, and discrimination, are common in the daily lives of communities of color. These experiences take a tremendous toll on their children’s brain and academic development, even if their SES is average or higher. Consequently, solutions to MDRs must go beyond individual victim-blaming approaches and require multi-level policy solutions that begin to break down the structural factors that underlie the inequality in the first place.
As this paper shows, we have distanced ourselves from any biological argument, such that brain structure is under the influence of race as a biological variable. However, there are modifiable biological changes that may occur under the influence of racism, toxic stress, oppression, and adversity. Hundreds of years under slavery, poverty, intimidation, discrimination, and murder have likely had an enormous effect on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, inflammation, allostatic load, epigenetics, and other mechanisms that mediate the effect of racism on the body [153]. For example, a growing body of research has documented epigenetic changes as a function of discrimination, stress, and racism [154,155,156]. Environments, such as lead, particular matters, and other toxins, also have epigenetic effects [154,155,156]. Pollutions in air and water, poor nutrition, and toxic stress can increase the epigenetic risk of black children and adults [154,155,156]. Arline Geronimus has worked extensively on biological changes that occur in response to adversity [157].
Still, it is unclear how these racism-related biological changes would explain our findings of racial variation in the associations between parental education, brain, and reading ability among American children. We still believe that a straightforward theory is a social mechanism: As a result of racism, living conditions of highly educated black and white families are qualitatively different, so parental education has a differential association with child brain and child reading, because black children are exposed to less enriching environments than their white counterparts, given social stratification and segregation [158]. Addressing racism may also help black parents engage in more enrichment activities, so we will see a higher brain development and a better reading ability of black children in highly educated families. Currently, highly educated black families are paying the extra “Black tax” [159], which may hinder their children’s opportunities to use enriched activities that can help to overcome the reading deficiencies seen amongst so many black children.
This paper acknowledges the MDR framework shown in so many studies investigating educational outcomes among children of color in the US [43,44]. As such, this paper provides additional evidence showing that significant effects of structural racism are still evident when SES is controlled. Structural racism may shape how parental education, brain structure, and reading ability correlate in American children.
At the same time, we do not deny the enormous progress made over the past centuries. We identify a great degree of progress being made among black families with higher education. The US has had a black president, there are more black people in congress and the senate than ever, Black Lives Matter receives enormous support, people of all colors and races participate in demonstrations for black people’s rights, and police officers were recently convicted for the killing of a Black man [160]. Thus, the US has made considerable progress and we do not deny it [161]. We simply say that this progress is not enough, and there is a need for additional work to be done [162].
Our results support past research that MDRs are relevant for black individuals and the idea that deviation from whiteness might be associated with a reduced gain in outcomes expected to follow access to SES resources [163]. Structural racism may affect biology, as well allostatic load and the HPA axis [153]. More research is needed on how racism gets under the skin and the role of toxins, allostatic load, and the HPA axis in this regard.
7. Limitations
Several limitations of our study should be noted. First, our study design is cross-sectional. As such, we cannot conclude or infer any causal effects between race, SES, brain development, or reading ability. Second, the sample was imbalanced across race/ethnic groups. Most participants were white, and a small proportion was Asian, AIAN/NHPI, and other-race. Third, the sample was not nationally representative. As such, we cannot generalize the results to all US children. Fourth, we only used a single sMRI indicator rather than multiple sMRI, dMRI, or fMRI measures across ROIs and brain structures. Fifth, we did not use other sources of data, such as school records of reading scores. Nevertheless, our study adds to the growing body of work on MDRs by focusing attention to brain structures in a large sample from across the US, application of a widely accepted and validated neurocognitive measure, highly homogenous samples in terms of age and development (resulting in high internal validity), and multiple robustness checks, suggesting that our results were not anomalous in the data. Our use of propensity score also increases the generalizability of our results and comparability of race/ethnic groups.
8. Future Directions
There is plenty of research to support the adverse effects of racism on children’s development, but there are many other factors that may contribute to poor reading ability in children that should be considered. In addition to racism, marginalization is also linked to poverty and adverse childhood experiences, as well as a wide range of other exposures, such as toxins and all types of stress. In this study, we focused on the indirect effect of race when SES is controlled. Future research may focus on other factors, such as poverty, toxins, epigenetics, HPA axis, allostatic load, adverse childhood experiences, stress, housing and food insecurity, education quality, neighborhood conditions, and other explanatory factors.
9. Conclusions
In summary, associations between parental education and children’s superior temporal cortical surface area were weaker for black, other-race, and mixed-race children than white children. The effect of the left superior temporal cortical surface area on reading ability was also weaker in AIAN/NHPI children than white children. Most of these findings are in line with MDRs. Using a MDR lens to understand the finding that parental educational attainment and children’s reading ability showed the opposite pattern (i.e., stronger, not weaker, effects for black compared to white children) suggests that reading may be one skill for children that black families have some control over (versus structural inequities) and therefore can take time to help their children with their reading ability. However, this tax creates other inequities in what black families may have to do to maintain some semblance of equitable developmental opportunities. Our study suggests that research into the socioecological and environmental mechanisms that explain why MDRs emerge for some predictors, processes, and outcomes, but not others, is necessary.
Appendix A. Model Formula
Appendix B. Model Assumptions
Appendix C. Sample Size across Study Sites
Appendix D. Recruitment Time Frame in the ABCD Study
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, all authors; formal analysis, S.A.; resources, M.B.; writing—original draft preparation, S.A.; writing—review and editing, all authors; supervision, M.A.Z. and C.H.C.; funding acquisition, S.A. and S.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, U24DA041147, U01DA041093, and U01DA041025. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html (accessed on 5 May 2021). A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/Consortium_Members.pdf (accessed on 5 May 2021). ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The current paper used the Curated Annual Release 2.0, also defined in NDA Study 634 (doi:10.15154/1503209). DEAP is a software provided by the Data Analysis and Informatics Center of ABCD, located at the UC San Diego, with generous support from the National Institutes of Health and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention under award number U24DA041123. The DEAP project information and links to its source code are available under the resource identifier RRID: SCR_016158. Shervin Assari is supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants 5S21MD000103, D084526-03, D084526-03, CA201415 02, DA035811-05, U54MD008149, U54MD007598, and U54CA229974.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of UCSD. However, for this specific analysis, ethical review and approval were waived for this study, due to use of fully deidentified data.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all adult subjects involved in the study. Assent was received from all participating children.
Data Availability Statement
Data are available at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd (accessed on 5 May 2021).
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH Data Archive or the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Reardon S.F., Galindo C. The Hispanic-White Achievement Gap in Math and Reading in the Elementary Grades. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2009;46:853–891. doi: 10.3102/0002831209333184. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hemphill F.C., Vanneman A. Achievement Gaps: How Hispanic and White Students in Public Schools Perform in Mathematics and Reading on the National Assessment of Educational Progress. National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC, USA: 2011. Statistical Analysis Report NCES 2011-459. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chavous T.M., Rivas-Drake D., Smalls C., Griffin T., Cogburn C. Gender matters, too: The influences of school racial discrimination and racial identity on academic engagement outcomes among African American adolescents. Dev. Psychol. 2008;44:637–654. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Chavous T.M. An Intergroup Contact-Theory Framework for Evaluating Racial Climate on Predominantly White College Campuses. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2005;36:239–257. doi: 10.1007/s10464-005-8623-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barton P.E. Parsing the Achievement Gap: Baselines for Tracking Progress. Policy Information Report. [(accessed on 5 May 2021)];2003 Available online: https://www.ets.org/research/policy_research_reports/publications/report/2003/gugt.
- 6.Berlak H. Race and the Achievement Gap. Critical Social Issues in American Education: Democracy and Meaning in a Globalizing World. [(accessed on 5 May 2021)];2005 Available online: https://rethinkingschools.org/articles/race-and-the-achievement-gap/
- 7.Howard T.C. Why Race and Culture Matter in Schools: Closing the Achievement Gap in America’s Classrooms. Teachers College Press; New York, NY, USA: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wiggan G. Race, School Achievement, and Educational Inequality: Toward a Student-Based Inquiry Perspective. Rev. Educ. Res. 2007;77:310–333. doi: 10.3102/003465430303947. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Talbert-Johnson C. Structural Inequities and the Achievement Gap in Urban Schools. Educ. Urban Soc. 2004;37:22–36. doi: 10.1177/0013124504268454. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Grogger J. Does School Quality Explain the Recent Black/White Wage Trend? J. Labor Econ. 1996;14:231–253. doi: 10.1086/209810. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Conger R.D., Wallace L.E., Sun Y., Simons R.L., McLoyd V.C., Brody G.H. Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Dev. Psychol. 2002;38:179. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bono K.E., Sy S.R., Kopp C.B. School readiness among low-income black children: Family characteristics, parenting, and social support. Early Child. Dev. Care. 2015;186:419–435. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1039528. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Vargas T., Damme K.S., Mittal V.A. Neighborhood deprivation, prefrontal morphology and neurocognition in late childhood to early adolescence. NeuroImage. 2020;220:117086. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Robison S., Jaggers J., Rhodes J., Blackmon B.J., Church W. Correlates of educational success: Predictors of school dropout and graduation for urban students in the Deep South. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2017;73:37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.11.031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Norman O., Ault C.R., Jr., Bentz B., Meskimen L. The black–white “achievement gap” as a perennial challenge of urban science education: A sociocultural and historical overview with implications for research and practice. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2001;38:1101–1114. doi: 10.1002/tea.10004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Condron D.J. Social class, school and non-school environments, and black/white inequalities in children’s learning. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2009;74:685–708. doi: 10.1177/000312240907400501. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Phillips M., Crouse J., Ralph J. Does the Black–White Test Score Gap Widen after Children Enter School? Brookings Institution Press; Washington, DC, USA: 1998. pp. 229–272. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fryer R.G., Jr., Levitt S.D. The black-white test score gap through third grade. Am. Law Econ. Rev. 2006;8:249–281. doi: 10.1093/aler/ahl003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Farkas G. The Black-White Test Score Gap. Contexts. 2004;3:12–19. doi: 10.1525/ctx.2004.3.2.12. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vanneman A., Hamilton L., Anderson J.B., Rahman T. Achievement Gaps: How Black and White Students in Public Schools Perform in Mathematics and Reading on the National Assessment of Educational Progress. National Center for Education Statistics; Washington, DC, USA: 2009. Statistical Analysis Report NCES 2009-455. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Reardon S.F. The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations. Community Invest. 2011;1:91–116. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fiester L. Early Warning! Why Reading by the End of Third Grade Matters. KIDS COUNT Special Report. Annie E. Casey Foundation. [(accessed on 5 May 2021)];2010 Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED509795.
- 23.Hernandez D.J. Double Jeopardy: How Third-Grade Reading Skills and Poverty Influence High School Graduation. Annie E. Casey Foundation. [(accessed on 5 May 2021)];2011 Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED518818.
- 24.Jacobson J., Olsen C., Rice J.K., Sweetland S., Ralph J. Educational Achievement and Black-White Inequality. Educ. Stat. Q. 2001;3:105–113. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Magnuson K., Waldfogel J. Steady Gains and Stalled Progress: Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap. Russell Sage Foundation; New York, NY, USA: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cunningham A.E., Stanovich K.E. Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Dev. Psychol. 1997;33:934. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McGee R., Prior M., Williams S., Smart D., Sanson A. The long-term significance of teacher-rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: Findings from two longitudinal studies. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2002;43:1004–1017. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Johnson A.S., Flicker L.J., Lichtenberg P.A. Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2006;12:64–71. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Siok W.T., Niu Z., Jin Z., Perfetti C.A., Tan L.H. A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:5561–5566. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801750105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Deutsch G.K., Dougherty R.F., Bammer R., Siok W.T., Gabrieli J.D., Wandell B. Children’s reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex. 2005;41:354–363. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70272-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hosseini S.H., Black J.M., Soriano T., Bugescu N., Martinez R., Raman M.M., Kesler S.R., Hoeft F. Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. NeuroImage. 2013;71:260–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jefferson A.L., Gibbons L.E., Rentz D.M., Carvalho J.O., Manly J., Bennett D.A., Jones R.N. A Life Course Model of Cognitive Activities, Socioeconomic Status, Education, Reading Ability, and Cognition. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2011;59:1403–1411. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03499.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Manly J.J., Jacobs D.M., Touradji P., Small S.A., Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002;8:341–348. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Merz E.C., Maskus E.A., Melvin S.A., He X., Noble K.G. Socioeconomic Disparities in Language Input Are Associated with Children’s Language-Related Brain Structure and Reading Skills. Child. Dev. 2020;91:846–860. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Simos P.G., Breier J.I., Wheless J.W., Maggio W.W., Fletcher J.M., Castillo E.M., Papanicolaou A.C. Brain mechanisms for reading: The role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming. Neuroreport. 2000;11:2443–2446. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gaillard W.D., Pugliese M., Grandin C.B., Braniecki S.H., Kondapaneni P., Hunter K., Xu B., Petrella J.R., Balsamo L., Basso G. Cortical localization of reading in normal children: An fMRI language study. Neurology. 2001;57:47–54. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Turkeltaub P.E., Gareau L., Flowers D.L., Zeffiro T.A., Eden G.F. Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:767–773. doi: 10.1038/nn1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kircher T.T., Brammer M., Andreu N.T., Williams S.C., McGuire P.K. Engagement of right temporal cortex during processing of linguistic context. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:798–809. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Friederici A.D., Rüschemeyer S.-A., Hahne A., Fiebach C.J. The Role of Left Inferior Frontal and Superior Temporal Cortex in Sentence Comprehension: Localizing Syntactic and Semantic Processes. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:170–177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.2.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vandenberghe R., Nobre A.C., Price C.J. The Response of Left Temporal Cortex to Sentences. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:550–560. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Assari S. Parental Education, Household Income, and Cortical Surface Area among 9–10 Years Old Children: Minorities’ Diminished Returns. Brain Sci. 2020;10:956. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Assari S. Family Socioeconomic Status and Exposure to Childhood Trauma: Racial Differences. Children. 2020;7:57. doi: 10.3390/children7060057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Assari S. Unequal Gain of Equal Resources across Racial Groups. Int. J. Health Policy Manag. 2018;7:1–9. doi: 10.15171/ijhpm.2017.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Assari S. Health Disparities due to Diminished Return among Black Americans: Public Policy Solutions. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2018;12:112–145. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mph S.A.M. Understanding America: Unequal Economic Returns of Years of Schooling in Whites and Blacks Race, Years of Schooling, and Economic Wellbeing. World J. Educ. Res. 2020;7:78–92. doi: 10.22158/wjer.v7n2p78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Assari S. Diminished Economic Return of Socioeconomic Status at Birth among Black than White Youth. Soc. Sci. 2018;7:74. doi: 10.3390/socsci7050074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Assari S., Boyce S., Caldwell C.H., Bazargan M. Parent Education and Future Transition to Cigarette Smoking: Latinos’ Diminished Returns. Front. Pediatr. 2020;8:457. doi: 10.3389/fped.2020.00457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Assari S., Caldwell C.H., Bazargan M. Association between Parental Educational Attainment and Youth Outcomes and Role of Race/Ethnicity. JAMA Netw. Open. 2019;2:e1916018. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.16018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Assari S., Caldwell C.H. High Risk of Depression in High-Income African American Boys. J. Racial Ethn. Health Disparities. 2018;5:808–819. doi: 10.1007/s40615-017-0426-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H. African Americans’ Diminished Returns of Parental Education on Adolescents’ Depression and Suicide in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2020;10:656–668. doi: 10.3390/ejihpe10020048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Assari S. Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Brain Sci. 2020;10:312. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Assari S. Youth Social, Emotional, and Behavioral Problems in the ABCD Study: Minorities’ Diminished Returns of Family Income. J. Econ. Public Financ. 2020;6:1–19. doi: 10.22158/jepf.v6n4p1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H. Diminished Returns of Parental Education in Terms of Youth School Performance: Ruling out Regression toward the Mean. Children. 2020;7:74. doi: 10.3390/children7070074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Assari S. Parental Educational Attainment and Academic Performance of American College Students; Blacks’ Diminished Returns. J. Health Econ. Dev. 2019;1:21–31. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Assari S., Caldwell C.H. Parental Educational Attainment Differentially Boosts School Performance of American Adolescents; Minorities’ Diminished Returns. J. Fam. Reprod. Health. 2019;13:7–13. doi: 10.18502/jfrh.v13i1.1607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H. Mathematical Performance of American Youth: Diminished Returns of Educational Attainment of Asian-American Parents. Educ. Sci. 2020;10:32. doi: 10.3390/educsci10020032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H., Zimmerman M.A. Place-Based Diminished Returns of Parental Educational Attainment on School Performance of Non-Hispanic White Youth. Front. Educ. 2020;5:5. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2020.00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Assari S., Caldwell C.H. Family Income at Birth and Risk of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder at Age 15: Racial Differences. Children. 2019;6:10. doi: 10.3390/children6010010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M. Subjective Family Socioeconomic Status and Adolescents’ Attention: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Children. 2020;7:80. doi: 10.3390/children7080080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Akhlaghipour G., Assari S. Parental Education, Household Income, Race, and Children’s Working Memory: Complexity of the Effects. Brain Sci. 2020;10:950. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Assari S. Race, Ethnicity, Family Socioeconomic Status, and Children’s Hippocampus Volume. Res. Health Sci. 2020;5:25–45. doi: 10.22158/rhs.v5n4p25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Assari S. Dimensional Change Card Sorting of American Children: Marginalization-Related Diminished Returns of Age. Child. Teenagers. 2020;3:72–92. doi: 10.22158/ct.v3n2p72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Assari S., Akhlaghipour G. Not Race or Age but Their Interaction Predicts Pre-Adolescents’ Inhibitory Control. Child. Teenagers. 2020;3:50–71. doi: 10.22158/ct.v3n2p50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Assari S., Islam S. Diminished Protective Effects of Household Income on Internalizing Symptoms among African American than European American Pre-Adolescents. J. Econ. Trade Mark. Manag. 2020;2:38–56. doi: 10.22158/jetmm.v2n4p38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Assari S. Income and Mental Well-Being of Middle-Aged and Older Americans: Immigrants’ Diminished Returns. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health. 2020;8:37–43. doi: 10.34172/ijtmgh.2020.06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Assari S. Socioeconomic Status and Current Cigarette Smoking Status: Immigrants’ Diminished Returns. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health. 2020;8:66–72. doi: 10.34172/ijtmgh.2020.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Assari S. Education Attainment and Obesity: Differential Returns Based on Sexual Orientation. Behav. Sci. 2019;9:16. doi: 10.3390/bs9020016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Assari S., Bazargan M. Educational Attainment and Subjective Health and Well-Being; Diminished Returns of Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Individuals. Behav. Sci. 2019;9:90. doi: 10.3390/bs9090090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Card D., Krueger A.B. School Quality and Black-White Relative Earnings: A Direct Assessment. Q. J. Econ. 1992;107:151–200. doi: 10.2307/2118326. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Assari S., Boyce S., Caldwell C.H., Bazargan M., Mincy R. Family Income and Gang Presence in the Neighborhood: Diminished Returns of Black Families. Urban Sci. 2020;4:29. doi: 10.3390/urbansci4020029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hudson D., Sacks T., Irani K., Asher A. The Price of the Ticket: Health Costs of Upward Mobility among African Americans. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:1179. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17041179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Bell C.N., Sacks T.K., Tobin C.S.T., Thorpe R.J. Racial Non-equivalence of Socioeconomic Status and Self-rated Health among African Americans and Whites. SSM Popul. Health. 2020;10:100561. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Williams D.R. Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Health the Added Effects of Racism and Discrimination. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999;896:173–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08114.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Farmer M.M., Ferraro K.F. Are racial disparities in health conditional on socioeconomic status? Soc. Sci. Med. 2005;60:191–204. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Ceci S.J., Papierno P.B. The Rhetoric and Reality of Gap Closing: When the “Have-Nots” Gain but the “Haves” Gain Even More. Am. Psychol. 2005;60:149–160. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Assari S. Parental education better helps white than black families escape poverty: National survey of children’s health. Economies. 2018;6:30. doi: 10.3390/economies6020030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Assari S., Boyce S. Family’s Subjective Economic Status and Children’s Matrix Reasoning: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Res. Health Sci. 2020;6:1–23. doi: 10.22158/rhs.v6n1p1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Bjork J.M., Straub L.K., Provost R.G., Neale M.C. The ABCD Study of Neurodevelopment: Identifying Neurocircuit Targets for Prevention and Treatment of Adolescent Substance Abuse. Curr. Treat. Opt. Psychiatry. 2017;4:196–209. doi: 10.1007/s40501-017-0108-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Research, Alcohol, and Current Reviews Editorial Staff NIH’s Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Alcohol Res. 2018;39:97. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Casey B., Cannonier T., Conley M.I., Cohen A.O., Barch D.M., Heitzeg M.M., Soules M.E., Teslovich T., Dellarco D.V., Garavan H., et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Lisdahl K.M., Sher K.J., Conway K.P., Gonzalez R., Ewing S.W.F., Nixon S.J., Tapert S., Bartsch H., Goldstein R.Z., Heitzeg M. Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:80–96. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Luciana M., Bjork J., Nagel B., Barch D., Gonzalez R., Nixon S., Banich M. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Garavan H., Bartsch H., Conway K., Decastro A., Goldstein R., Heeringa S., Jernigan T., Potter A., Thompson W., Zahs D. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Ewing S.W.F., Bjork J.M., Luciana M. Implications of the ABCD study for developmental neuroscience. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:161–164. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Uban K.A., Horton M.K., Jacobus J., Heyser C., Thompson W.K., Tapert S.F., Madden P.A., Sowell E.R. Biospecimens and the ABCD study: Rationale, methods of collection, measurement and early data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:97–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Muhammad M., De Loney E.H., Brooks C.L., Assari S., Robinson D., Caldwell C.H. “I think that’s all a lie…I think It’s genocide”: Applying a Critical Race Praxis to Youth Perceptions of Flint Water Contamination. Ethn. Dis. 2018;28:241–246. doi: 10.18865/ed.28.S1.241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Curry T.J., Curry G. Critical race theory and the demography of death and dying. Crit. Race Theory Acad. 2020:89–106. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Assari S. Minorities Diminished Returns. MedLine Publications; Bethesda, MD, USA: 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 89.Thompson W.K., Barch D.M., Bjork J.M., Gonzalez R., Nagel B.J., Nixon S.J., Luciana M. The structure of cognition in 9 and 10 year-old children and associations with problem behaviors: Findings from the ABCD study’s baseline neurocognitive battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;36:100606. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Akshoomoff N., Beaumont J.L., Bauer P.J., Dikmen S.S., Gershon R.C., Mungas D., Slotkin J., Tulsky D., Weintraub S., Zelazo P.D., et al. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Composite scores of crystallized, fluid, and overall cognition. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2013;78:119–132. doi: 10.1111/mono.12038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Jovicich J., Czanner S., Greve D., Haley E., van der Kouwe A., Gollub R.L., Kennedy D., Schmitt F., Brown G., MacFall J., et al. Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: Effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. NeuroImage. 2006;30:436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Cardinale F., Chinnici G., Bramerio M., Mai R., Sartori I., Cossu M., Russo G.L., Castana L., Colombo N., Caborni C. Validation of FreeSurfer-estimated brain cortical thickness: Comparison with histologic measurements. Neuroinformatics. 2014;12:535–542. doi: 10.1007/s12021-014-9229-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Fischl B. FreeSurfer. NeuroImage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Fischl B., Liu A., Dale A. Automated manifold surgery: Constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2001;20:70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Segonne F., Pacheco J., Fischl B. Geometrically Accurate Topology-Correction of Cortical Surfaces Using Nonseparating Loops. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2007;26:518–529. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.887364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Dale A.M., Sereno M.I. Improved Localizadon of Cortical Activity by Combining EEG and MEG with MRI Cortical Surface Reconstruction: A Linear Approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1993;5:162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Fischl B., Sereno M.I., Tootell R.B., Dale A.M. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999;8:272–284. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Desikan R.S., Ségonne F., Fischl B., Quinn B.T., Dickerson B.C., Blacker D., Buckner R.L., Dale A.M., Maguire R.P., Hyman B.T., et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Weintraub S., Dikmen S.S., Heaton R.K., Tulsky D.S., Zelazo P.D., Bauer P.J., Carlozzi N.E., Slotkin J., Blitz D., Wallner-Allen K., et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80:S54–S64. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Gershon R.C., Slotkin J., Manly J.J., Blitz D.L., Beaumont J.L., Schnipke D., Wallner-Allen K., Golinkoff R.M., Gleason J.B., Hirsh-Pasek K. IV. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring language (vocabulary comprehension and reading decoding) Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 2013;78:49–69. doi: 10.1111/mono.12034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Mungas D., Widaman K., Zelazo P.D., Tulsky D., Heaton R.K., Slotkin J., Blitz D.L., Gershon R.C. VII. Nih Toolbox Cognition Battery (Cb): Factor Structure for 3 To 15 Year Old. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 2013;78:103–118. doi: 10.1111/mono.12037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Akshoomoff N., Newman E., Thompson W.K., McCabe C., Bloss C.S., Chang L., Amaral D.G., Casey B.J., Ernst T.M., Frazier J.A., et al. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: Results from a large normative developmental sample (PING) Neuropsychology. 2014;28:1–10. doi: 10.1037/neu0000001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Zelazo P.D., Anderson J.E., Richler J., Wallner-Allen K., Beaumont J.L., Weintraub S. National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB): Validation for children between 3 and 15 years: II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monogr. Society Res. Child Dev. 2013;78:16–33. doi: 10.1111/mono.12032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Rigby A.S., Day S.M. The crisis in replicability. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2018;61:504. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Assari S. American Children’s Screen Time: Diminished Returns of Household Income in Black Families. Information. 2020;11:538. doi: 10.3390/info11110538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Assari S., Caldwell C.H., Bazargan M. Parental Educational Attainment and Black-White Adolescents’ Achievement Gap: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Open J. Soc. Sci. 2020;08:282–297. doi: 10.4236/jss.2020.83026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Akhlaghipour G., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H., Assari S. Parental Human Capital and Adolescents’ Executive Function: Immigrants’ Diminished Returns. Med. Res. Arch. 2020;8:1–20. doi: 10.18103/mra.v8i10.2235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Assari S., Caldwell C.H. Teacher Discrimination Reduces School Performance of African American Youth: Role of Gender. Brain Sci. 2018;8:183. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8100183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Fadus M.C., Valadez E.A., Bryant B.E., Garcia A.M., Neelon B., Tomko R.L., Squeglia L.M. Racial Disparities in Elementary School Disciplinary Actions: Findings From the ABCD Study. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Quinn D.M. Kindergarten Black–White test score gaps: Re-examining the roles of socioeconomic status and school quality with new data. Sociol. Educ. 2015;88:120–139. doi: 10.1177/0038040715573027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Roscigno V.J., Ainsworth-Darnell J.W. Race, Cultural Capital, and Educational Resources: Persistent Inequalities and Achievement Returns. Sociol. Educ. 1999;72:158. doi: 10.2307/2673227. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Assari S. Mental Rotation in American Children: Diminished Returns of Parental Education in Black Families. Pediatr. Rep. 2020;12:130–141. doi: 10.3390/pediatric12030028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H., Mincy R. Maternal Education at Birth and Youth Breakfast Consumption at Age 15: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. J. Multidiscip. Sci. J. 2020;3:313–323. doi: 10.3390/j3030024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Assari S., Boyce S., Bazargan M., Mincy R., Caldwell C.H. Unequal Protective Effects of Parental Educational Attainment on the Body Mass Index of Black and White Youth. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2019;16:3641. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Assari S., Caldwell C.H., Mincy R. Family Socioeconomic Status at Birth and Youth Impulsivity at Age 15; Blacks’ Diminished Return. Children. 2018;5:58. doi: 10.3390/children5050058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Assari S., Thomas A., Caldwell C.H., Mincy R.B. Blacks’ Diminished Health Return of Family Structure and Socioeconomic Status; 15 Years of Follow-up of a National Urban Sample of Youth. J. Hered. 2017;95:21–35. doi: 10.1007/s11524-017-0217-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Assari S., Chalian H., Bazargan M. Social Determinants of Hookah Smoking in the United States. J. Ment. Health Clin. Psychol. 2020;4:21–27. doi: 10.29245/2578-2959/2020/1.1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Assari S., Chalian H., Bazargan M. Race, Ethnicity, Socioeconomic Status, and Chronic Lung Disease in the U.S. Res. Health Sci. 2019;5:48–63. doi: 10.22158/rhs.v5n1p48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Assari S., Caldwell C.H., Bazargan M. Parental Educational Attainment and Relatives’ Substance Use of American Youth: Hispanics’ Diminished Returns. J. Biosci. Med. 2020;8:122–134. doi: 10.4236/jbm.2020.82010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Boyce S., Bazargan M., Caldwell C.H., Zimmerman M.A., Assari S. Parental Educational Attainment and Social Environment of Urban Public Schools in the U.S.: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Children. 2020;7:44. doi: 10.3390/children7050044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Assari S., Caldwell C.H., Zimmerman M.A. Family Structure and Subsequent Anxiety Symptoms; Minorities’ Diminished Return. Brain Sci. 2018;8:97. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8060097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Assari S., Bazargan M. Protective Effects of Educational Attainment against Cigarette Smoking; Diminished Returns of American Indians and Alaska Natives in the National Health Interview Survey. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health. 2019;7:105–110. doi: 10.15171/ijtmgh.2019.22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Assari S., Bazargan M. Education Level and Cigarette Smoking: Diminished Returns of Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual Individuals. Behav. Sci. 2019;9:103. doi: 10.3390/bs9100103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Assari S., Perez M.U., Johnson N., Williams N.R., Carrillo E., Garcia L., Hollis X.T. Education Level and Self-rated Health in the United States: Immigrants’ Diminished Returns. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health. 2020;8:116–123. doi: 10.34172/ijtmgh.2020.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Assari S. Socioeconomic status and self-rated oral health; diminished return among hispanic whites. Dent. J. 2018;6:11. doi: 10.3390/dj6020011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Jones N.H. Our Democracy’s Founding Ideals Were False When They Were Written. Black Americans Have Fought to Make Them True. Best Am. Mag. Writ. 2021:359–382. doi: 10.7312/holt19801-018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Feldstein M. Income Inequality and Poverty. National Bureau of Economic Research; Cambridge, MA, USA: 1998. 0898-2937. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Anderson E., Jalles D’Orey M.A., Duvendack M., Esposito L. Does government spending affect income inequality? A meta-regression analysis. J. Econ. Surv. 2017;31:961–987. doi: 10.1111/joes.12173. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Woolard I., Metz R., Inchauste G., Lustig N., Maboshe M., Purfield C. How Much Is Inequality Reduced by Progressive Taxation and Government Spending. [(accessed on 5 May 2021)];2015 Available online: https://www.econ3x3.org/sites/default/files/articles/Woolard%20et%20al%20%202015%20Fiscal%20policy%20progressivity%20FINAL4_0.pdf.
- 130.Goldstein N.D., Palumbo A.J., Bellamy S.L., Purtle J., Locke R. State and Local Government Expenditures and Infant Mortality in the United States. Pediatrics. 2020;146:e20201134. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Gaffney A., McCormick D. The Affordable Care Act: Implications for health-care equity. Lancet. 2017;389:1442–1452. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30786-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Renna F., Kosteas V.D., Dinkar K. Inequality in health insurance coverage before and after the Affordable Care Act. Health Econ. 2021;30:384–402. doi: 10.1002/hec.4195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Sommers B.D., Maylone B., Blendon R.J., Orav E.J., Epstein A.M. Three-Year Impacts Of The Affordable Care Act: Improved Medical Care And Health Among Low-Income Adults. Health Aff. 2017;36:1119–1128. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2017.0293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Griffith K., Evans L., Bor J. The Affordable Care Act Reduced Socioeconomic Disparities In Health Care Access. Health Aff. 2017;36:1503–1510. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2017.0083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Breslau J., Stein B.D., Han B., Shelton S., Yu H. Impact of the Affordable Care Act’s Dependent Coverage Expansion on the Health Care and Health Status of Young Adults: What Do We Know So Far? Med. Care Res. Rev. 2018;75:131–152. doi: 10.1177/1077558716682171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Breslau J., Han B., Stein B.D., Burns R.M., Yu H. Did the Affordable Care Act’s dependent coverage expansion affect race/ethnic disparities in health insurance coverage? Health Serv. Res. 2018;53:1286. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.12728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Breslau J., Han B., Lai J., Yu H. Impact of the Affordable Care Act Medicaid Expansion on Utilization of Mental Health Care. Med. Care. 2020;58:757–762. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0000000000001373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Assari S., Helmi H., Bazargan M. Health Insurance Coverage Better Protects Blacks than Whites against Incident Chronic Disease. Healthcare. 2019;7:40. doi: 10.3390/healthcare7010040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Assari S., Preiser B., Kelly M. Education and Income Predict Future Emotional Well-Being of Whites but Not Blacks: A Ten-Year Cohort. Brain Sci. 2018;8:122. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8070122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Assari S., Bazargan M. Unequal Associations between Educational Attainment and Occupational Stress across Racial and Ethnic Groups. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2019;16:3539. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Assari S., Bazargan M. Unequal Effects of Educational Attainment on Workplace Exposure to Second-Hand Smoke by Race and Ethnicity; Minorities’ Diminished Returns in the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) J. Med. Res. Innov. 2019;3:e000179. doi: 10.32892/jmri.179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Zajacova A., Rogers R.G., Johnson-Lawrence V. Glitch in the gradient: Additional education does not uniformly equal better health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2012;75:2007–2012. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Zajacova A., Lawrence E.M. The Relationship between Education and Health: Reducing Disparities through a Contextual Approach. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2018;39:273–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Parsons C. Explaining sustained inequalities in ethnic minority school exclusions in England—Passive racism in a neoliberal grip. Oxf. Rev. Educ. 2009;35:249–265. doi: 10.1080/03054980902814492. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Shervin A. Parental Education and Spanking of American Children: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. World J. Educ. Res. 2020;8:19. doi: 10.22158/wjer.v7n3p19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Assari S. Does School Racial Composition Explain Why High Income Black Youth Perceive More Discrimination? A Gender Analysis. Brain Sci. 2018;8:140. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8080140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Assari S., Lankarani M.M. Workplace Racial Composition Explains High Perceived Discrimination of High Socioeconomic Status African American Men. Brain Sci. 2018;8:139. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8080139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Straiton M.L., Aambø A.K., Johansen R. Perceived discrimination, health and mental health among immigrants in Norway: The role of moderating factors. BMC Public Health. 2019;19:1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6649-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Oh H., Stickley A., Koyanagi A., Yau R., DeVylder J.E. Discrimination and suicidality among racial and ethnic minorities in the United States. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;245:517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Assari S., Lankarani M.M., Caldwell C.H. Discrimination Increases Suicidal Ideation in Black Adolescents Regardless of Ethnicity and Gender. Behav. Sci. 2017;7:75. doi: 10.3390/bs7040075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Panza G.A., Puhl R.M., Taylor B.A., Zaleski A.L., Livingston J., Pescatello L.S. Links between discrimination and cardiovascular health among socially stigmatized groups: A systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0217623. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Korous K.M., Causadias J.M., Casper D.M. Racial discrimination and cortisol output: A meta-analysis. Soc. Sci. Med. 2017;193:90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Berger M., Sarnyai Z. “More than skin deep”: Stress neurobiology and mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Stress. 2015;18:1–10. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.989204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Hudson D.L., Neighbors H.W., Geronimus A.T., Jackson J.S. Racial Discrimination, John Henryism, and Depression Among African Americans. J. Black Psychol. 2016;42:221–243. doi: 10.1177/0095798414567757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Hudson D.L., Puterman E., Bibbins-Domingo K., Matthews K.A., Adler N.E. Race, life course socioeconomic position, racial discrimination, depressive symptoms and self-rated health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013;97:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Hudson D.L., Bullard K.M., Neighbors H.W., Geronimus A.T., Yang J., Jackson J.S. Are benefits conferred with greater socioeconomic position undermined by racial discrimination among African American men? J. Men’s Health. 2012;9:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jomh.2012.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Assari S. Social Epidemiology of Perceived Discrimination in the United States: Role of Race, Educational Attainment, and Income. Int. J. Epidemiol. Res. 2020;7:136–141. doi: 10.34172/ijer.2020.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Tull E.S., Sheu Y.-T., Butler C., Cornelious K. Relationships between perceived stress, coping behavior and cortisol secretion in women with high and low levels of internalized racism. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 2005;97:206. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Goosby B.J., Heidbrink C. The transgenerational consequences of discrimination on African-American health outcomes. Sociol. Compass. 2013;7:630–643. doi: 10.1111/soc4.12054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Sullivan S. Inheriting racist disparities in health: Epigenetics and the transgenerational effects of white racism. Crit. Philos. Race. 2013;1:190–218. doi: 10.5325/critphilrace.1.2.0190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Wallack L., Thornburg K. Developmental origins, epigenetics, and equity: Moving upstream. Matern. Child. Health J. 2016;20:935–940. doi: 10.1007/s10995-016-1970-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Geronimus A.T., Hicken M., Keene D., Bound J. “Weathering” and age patterns of allostatic load scores among blacks and whites in the United States. Am. J. Public Health. 2006;96:826–833. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Assari S., Hani N. Household income and children’s unmet dental care need; blacks’ diminished return. Dent. J. 2018;6:17. doi: 10.3390/dj6020017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data are available at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd (accessed on 5 May 2021).