Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020 Dec 1;29(7):634–642. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.11.009

Reading the Mind in the Eyes: A Population-Based Study of Social Cognition in Older Adults

Soyoung Lee 1, Erin P Jacobsen 1, Yichen Jia 2, Beth E Snitz 3, Chung-Chou H Chang 2,4, Mary Ganguli 1,3,5
PMCID: PMC8166961  NIHMSID: NIHMS1653393  PMID: 33293250

Abstract

Background

Social cognition indicates the cognitive processes involved in perceiving, interpreting, and processing social information. Although it is one of the six core DSM-5 cognitive domains for diagnosing neurocognitive disorders, it is not routinely assessed in older adults. The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test assesses Theory of Mind, the social cognition mechanism which forms the root of empathy.

Objectives

To describe the distribution of, and factors associated with, scores on a 10-item version of Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test [RMET-10] in older adults.

Design

Population-based cross-sectional study.

Setting

Small-town communities in Pennsylvania.

Participants

Adults aged 66 to 105 years (N=902, mean age=76.6).

Measurements

The assessment included RMET-10, demographics, cognitive screening, literacy, depression symptoms, anxiety symptoms, cognitive composites derived from a neuropsychological test battery, Social Norms Questionnaire, and Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR].

Results

RMET-10 score was normally distributed in our overall study sample. Normative RMET-10 scores among those rated as CDR=0 were calculated by age, sex, and education. RMET-10 score was significantly higher with younger age, higher education, white race, higher cognitive screening scores, literacy, social norms scores, higher scores in all five domains in cognitive composites, and lower CDR. RMET-10 score was also significantly higher with fewer depression and anxiety symptoms after adjusting for demographics.

Conclusions

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test is a potentially useful measure of social cognition for use in the research assessment of older adults. With appropriate calibration it should also have utility in the clinical setting.

Keywords: aging, Theory of Mind, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, community-based, epidemiology, Cognitive empathy

INTRODUCTION

Social cognition indicates the cognitive processes involved in perceiving, interpreting, and processing social information. It integrates perceived social cues with acquired social knowledge and higher-order reasoning to infer the cognitive and emotional status of others, resulting in empathy and ‘Theory of Mind (TOM).’ Social cognition allows individuals to respond to social information appropriately, which is critical to maintain interpersonal relationships, participate in social interactions, and function within society. (1, 2)

Impairment in social cognition can be found in nearly all brain disorders. It is especially recognized in both neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorders with frontal lobe dysfunction, such as autism and frontotemporal dementia, and, increasingly, in other neurocognitive disorders. (37) Most studies in social cognition have focused on these disorders; less is known about the integrity of social cognition in normal aging. (2, 4)

However, unlike other cognitive domains, social cognition is not routinely measured in geriatric evaluations. While a number of objective assessment tools are available to index social cognition, little information is available regarding the performance and utility of those tests in older adults. Further, some social cognition tests that measure social judgment and behavior, such as the Social Norms Questionnaire and the Test of Practical Judgment, are difficult to validate across generational and national/ethnic subgroups because perceptions of normative or appropriate social behavior are heavily based on prevailing culture. (8, 9)

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test [RMET] (10) is a popular measure of individual differences in TOM, the most representative mechanism of social cognition, which refers to “cognitive empathy,” i.e., the ability to discern the mental states of others. In this assessment, participants are presented with images of only the eye regions of single individuals and are asked to choose which of four words best describes each target’s mental state. The relevance of this cognitive function in everyday life has been brought into sharp focus by the global SARS-CoV-2 (COVID-19) pandemic. Near-universal mask-wearing is requiring individuals to ascertain the emotional states and responses of others by looking only at their eyes.

The RMET has been widely used and is one of few TOM tests that have been validated in older adults. (11) Another strength of the test is various abbreviated versions are available (11, 12); brevity is particularly important in the clinical assessment of older patients as well as in large population-based studies, where assessment fatigue is a concern.

The present study aims a) to describe the distribution of the RMET scores in a representative population sample of older adults using an abbreviated 10-item version of the RMET [RMET-10], b) to describe population-based norms on the RMET-10, and c) to examine the associations of RMET-10 scores with age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, literacy, a cognitive screen, clinical dementia rating, individual domains in cognitive composites derived from a neuropsychiatric battery, anxiety and depression screens, and a test of social norms recognition.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were from the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study which is focused on the epidemiology of mild cognitive impairment and dementia. The MYHAT cohort was accrued by age-stratified random selection from the voter registration list an economically distressed, post-industrial region of southwestern Pennsylvania, USA. Participants were initially enrolled in the original MYHAT cohort between 2006–2008 if they met the eligibility criteria of being 65 years and older, lived within select geographically defined areas, and resided independently in the community at the time of recruitment. Individuals were excluded if they met the above criteria but were too ill to participate, had vision or hearing impairment severe enough to preclude neuropsychological testing, or had decisional incapacity. (13, 14) Of 2036 individuals who qualified, the full evaluation described below was administered to 1982 without substantial cognitive impairment, defined as age-education corrected Mini Mental State Exam (15, 16) scores ≥ 21. A new sub-cohort of 709 participants meeting the same criteria, aged of 65–74 during 2016–2019, was enrolled to replenish the original cohort; of these, 703 underwent the full assessment. All participants were invited to undergo annual reassessment, which took place in overlapping data collection cycles.

RMET-10 (10, 12) was added to the MYHAT protocol for all participants in 2017 when the new sub- cohort was beginning its second assessment cycle. Thus, different participants were in different annual cycles when they first completed the RMET-10. All procedures were approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written informed consent.

RMET-10 is an abbreviated version of the full RMET, an advanced test of TOM, originally developed in children with autism, later revised for adults, with shorter versions validated in different populations. (10, 12) The test stimuli consist of 10 grey-scale photos of people that were originally taken from magazines. The photos were cropped and rescaled so that only the area around the eyes is visible. Each photo is surrounded by four words each describing a state of mind, one of which has been predetermined to correctly represent the emotion of the individual in the photo. The participant is asked to select, within 20 seconds, the word which best describes what the person in the photo is thinking or feeling. During pilot testing, we identified five words provided on the test (despondent, contemplative, tentative, imploring, pensive) with which our participants were unfamiliar. For these five words, we found substitutes in the RMET glossary(10) and/or thesaurus (hopeless, reflective, cautious, pleading, thoughtful).

Relevant to this report, the annual MYHAT assessment also included the following items:

Demographics: age, sex, race (white/ non-white), level of education

Cognitive screen: Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE](15)

Literacy: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading [WTAR] (17)

Depression symptoms: modified Center for Epidemiological Studies- Depression scale [mCES- D] (18, 19)

Anxiety symptoms: Generalized Anxiety Disorder brief scale [GAD-7] (20)

Cognitive Composites: composite scores derived for the domains of attention, memory, language, visuospatial function, and executive function, derived from our neuropsychological test battery. (21) A composite score for each domain was calculated for each participant as the average of z- transformed tests in domain. (14)

Social Norms: Social Norms Questionnaire [SNQ-22], (22) measuring a different aspect of social cognition, a test which asks individuals about the appropriateness of specific behaviors in hypothetical scenarios, errors being related either to breaking with norms or to over-adhering to perceived norms.

Dementia Rating: Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR®] Staging Instrument, (23) as previously detailed.(14) On the CDR, ratings of 0, 0.5, and >1 indicate normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment or very mild dementia, and at least mild dementia.

Statistical Analyses.

To visually examine the distribution of scores on RMET-10, we plotted scores on the test among the whole cohort (N=902) and those with CDR scores of 0 (N=756). To generate norms on the test we calculated the mean (SD), median (50th %ile), and 5th %ile RMET scores by age, sex, and educational level (less than or equal to high school [HS] graduate, more than HS graduate) among those with CDR=0. For comparison, we calculated the same values among those with CDR=0.5. We also calculated the Kuder-Richardson coefficient of internal consistency on RMET-10. (24)

In the entire sample, we then calculated mean scores on the RMET among the subgroups determined by each of the following variables: age (65–74, 75–84, ≥85), sex (men, women), education (<HS, HS, >HS), race (white, non-white), MMSE (≤17, 18–23, 24–27, ≥28), WTAR (≤99, 100–108, 109–117, ≥118), SNQ22 (≤19, 20, 21, 22), GAD-7 (0, 1–5, 6–18, ≥11), mCES-D (0, 1–4, ≥5), and CDR (0, 0.5, ≥1). We modeled RMET-10 score as a continuous variable on each of these above-mentioned covariates and cognitive composites domain scores, first in simple linear regression (unadjusted) models and then in multiple linear regression models adjusting for age, sex, and education.

RESULTS

A total of 902 older adults were administered RMET-10. Their ages ranged from 66 to 105 years, with a mean (SD) age of 76.6 (8.06) years; 62.4% were women; 38.9% had high school or less education; 61% had more than high school education; and 93.7% were of European descent.

The RMET-10 scores were roughly normally distributed in our overall study sample [Figure 1.A] with a range of 0–10, a median score of 7, and a mean (SD) score of 6.5 (1.9), and also among those with CDR=0 [Fig 1.B], with range 0–10, median 7 and mean (SD) 6.64 (1.8).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

A. RMET-10 scores of overall study sample (N=902) were roughly normally distributed. The median score was 7, and the mean (SD) score was 6.5 (1.9). B. RMET-10 scores among those rated as CDR=0 (N=756), who are free of dementia, were also roughly normally distributed. The median was 7, and the mean (SD) was 6.644 (1.844).

Normative RMET-10 scores among those with CDR=0, and, for comparison, scores among those with CDR=0.5, are shown as mean (SD), median, and 5th %ile scores by age, sex, and education [Table 1].

Table 1.

Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) scores among MYHAT study participants free of dementia

Age in years Gender Education Level N Mean score SD Median score (50th %ile) 5th %ile score
Clinical Dementia Rating = 0 (Norms)
N=756
65–74 Male ≤ HS 45 6.49 1.87 7 3
> HS 115 6.94 1.46 7 4
Female ≤ HS 62 7.21 1.82 7 4.05
> HS 153 7.24 1.71 8 4
75–84 Male ≤ HS 26 6.23 1.63 6 4
> HS 60 6.38 1.87 6.5 3
Female ≤ HS 81 6.38 1.82 7 4
> HS 92 6.85 1.71 7 4
85+ Male ≤ HS 15 5.13 1.73 5 3.1
> HS 25 5.64 2.18 5 2.2
Female ≤ HS 48 5.81 1.93 6 3
> HS 34 5.59 2.18 5 2
Clinical Dementia Rating = 0.5 N=134
65–74 Male ≤ HS 7 5.43 2.7 6 1.9
> HS 13 6.46 1.61 7 4
Female ≤ HS 17 6.71 2.17 7 3.8
> HS 16 7.12 2.09 7.5 4.25
75–84 Male ≤ HS 9 6 2.18 6 3
> HS 7 7.43 1.9 8 5
Female ≤ HS 14 5.07 1.86 5 2
> HS 9 6.22 2.11 7 3.4
85+ Male ≤ HS 4 5 1.83 5 3.15
> HS 10 5.2 1.87 5 3
Female ≤ HS 15 4.73 1.67 5 2
> HS 3 5.77 1.3 6 4

HS = High school graduate, SD = standard deviation, %ile = percentile

Mean (SD) RMET-10 scores were calculated for each of the categories in age, sex, education, race, MMSE, WTAR, SNQ-22, GAD-7, mCES-D, and CDR [Table 2]. Distribution of RMET-10 scores were statistically significantly different between categories of age, education, race, MMSE, WTAR, and CDR. The Kuder-Richardson coefficient for the RMET-10 was 0.449, which is not unexpected for a short scale where the emphasis is on brevity and content validity.(25)

Table 2.

Factors associated with RMET scores (N = 902)

Participant Characteristics N (%) RMET Score Mean (SD) Multiple linear regression models of RMET score#
Unadjusted Adjusted for Demographics*
Coeff (CI) P Coeff (CI) P
Age 65–74 430 (47.7) 6.99 (1.75) −0.73 (−0.88, −0.57) <0.001 Multiple Values**
75–84 298 (33.0) 6.46 (1.83)
85+ 174 (19.3) 5.45 (1.94)
Sex Male 339 (37.6) 6.39 (1.83) Ref. 0.115
Female 563 (62.4) 6.60 (1.94) 0.21 (−0.05, 0.46)
Education ≤ HS 351 (38.9) 6.22 (1.96) Ref <0.001
> HS 551 (61.1) 6.71 (1.84) 0.49 (0.24, 0.75)
Race Non-White 57 (6.32) 5.60 (2.21) Ref <0.001 1.12 (0.64, 1.60) <0.001
White 845 (93.68) 6.58 (1.86) 0.98 (0.48, 1.49)
Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0 756 (83.8) 6.64 (1.84) Ref.
0.5 134 (14.9) 5.98 (2.04) −0.67 (−1.01, −0.32) <0.001 −0.41 (−0.74, −0.07) 0.017
≥ 1 12 (1.3) 4.58 (1.83) −2.06 (−3.13, −0.99) <0.001 −1.30 (−2.34, −0.26) 0.015
Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) ≤ 17 5 (0.6) 5.20 (0.84) 0.21 (0.16, 0.27) <0.001 0.16 (0.11, 0.21) <0.001
18–23 53 (5.9) 5.55 (2.33)
24–27 287 (31.8) 6.07 (1.90)
≥ 28 557 (61.8) 6.85 (1.78)
Weschler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) ≤ 99 255 (28.6) 5.69 (1.84) 0.05 (0.04, 0.06) <0.001 0.05 (0.04, 0.06) <0.001
100–108 209 (23.5) 6.59 (1.81)
109–117 241 (27.0) 6.77 (1.78)
≥ 118 186 (20.9) 7.35 (1.74)
Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ-22) ≤ 19 304 (34.5) 6.34 (1.93) 0.13 (0.06, 0.21) 0.001 0.10 (0.03, 0.18) 0.007
20 246 (28.0) 6.66 (1.73)
21 209 (23.8) 6.70 (2.00)
22 121 (13.8) 6.52 (1.85)
Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale (GAD-7) 0 361 (48.0) 6.53 (1.89) −0.04 (−0.08, 0.00) 0.081 −0.05 (−0.09, −0.01) 0.013
1–5 290 (38.6) 6.64 (1.89)
6–10 74 (9.8) 6.41 (2.01)
≥ 11 27 (3.6) 5.81 (1.66)
Modified Center for Epidemiologic Studies - Depression Scale (mCES-D) 0 605 (67.5) 6.62 (1.89) −0.04 (−0.09, 0.02) 0.172 −0.07 (−0.12, −0.02) 0.008
1–4 220 (24.6) 6.36 (1.92)
≥ 5 71 (7.9) 6.18 (1.82)
Attention Domain Score N/A 0.58 (0.44, 0.73) <0.001 0.40 (0.25, 0.55) <0.001
Executive Domain Score 0.96 (0.81, 1.11) <0.001 0.78 (0.62, 0.94) <0.001
Language Domain Score 1.02 (0.86, 1.17) <0.001 0.83 (0.65, 1.01) <0.001
Memory Domain Score 0.50 (0.38, 0.63) <0.001 0.37 (0.25, 0.49) <0.001
Visuospatial Domain Score 0.60 (0.46, 0.73) <0.001 0.50 (0.37, 0.64) <0.001

Coeff: coefficient; CI: 95% confidence interval

The p-values are derived from t test, and the df = N - # of tests, where N is the total sample size of each variables (N varies as some variables have missingness). The Bonferroni-corrected alpha level is 0.05/16 =0.003.

#:

In the multiple linear regression model, age, MMSE, WTAR, SNQ-22, GAD-7, and mCES-D are in their continuous format.

Columns5–6

*:

The adjusted model is in the form of RMET~Age+Sex+Education+Covariate (participant characteristic). The base model only contains demographics; each covariate was added to the base model separately in a separate model.

**

The coefficients for demographics in the adjusted models are not shown in the table since there is one coefficient for each demographic in each model.

In unadjusted linear regression models, the RMET-10 score was estimated to be significantly higher among those with younger age, higher education, white race, lower CDR, higher MMSE, WTAR, SNQ-22 scores, and higher cognitive scores in all five domains. [Table 2] Women performed significantly better than men only in the age group 65–74 years, with mean (SD) scores of 7.18 (1.79) vs. 6.72 (1.66), by two-sample t-test, 2.6385, df= 428, P=0.009.

In multivariable regression models adjusted for age, sex, and education, all the above associations remained statistically significant. RMET-10 score was also significantly higher in those with fewer anxiety and depression symptoms after adjusting for demographics. [Table 2]

DISCUSSION

Social cognition is a relatively under-investigated area in geriatric psychiatry despite its being listed in the fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (26) as one of the cognitive domains to be assessed in the diagnosis of neurocognitive disorders. Measurement of social cognition would complement conventional approaches to the study of cognitive aging, and shine a light on the deficits underlying behaviors like disinhibition and loss of empathy that are major sources of distress for patients and families. An effective tool to assess social cognition could also be key to early detection of neurocognitive disorders in subgroups of patients where memory loss is not the first deficit to appear. However, before a measure can be used effectively for diagnosis, it should be evaluated in the appropriate populations, so that effects of normal aging can be described and distinguished from disease effects. Using a large, population-based sample of older adults, we described the distribution of a 10-item version of the RMET in relation to demographics, measures of other cognitive functions, depression and anxiety, and awareness of social norms.

We found that RMET performance was negatively associated with age in this cross-sectional analysis, in line with existing evidence from the literature that overall social cognition, including TOM ability, declines with aging. (2729) Several previous studies focused on the aging effect on the RMET also reported a decline of the RMET performance with aging. (3032) While those studies compared older versus younger adult participants, we were able to show that RMET is negatively associated with greater age even within an older cohort. Continued follow-up of the MYHAT cohort will determine whether we observe further decline in RMET scores over time.

A previous study compared older and younger groups on their performance on 3 social cognition tasks involved in “mentalizing,” while recording their brain activity on fMRI. It showed poor performance and a decrease in neural activation in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in older adults, suggesting that social cognitive deficits involve specific regions of the default network and are associated with aging. (33) Another study that examined the difference in brain activity in younger and older participants completing the RMET reported no difference in RMET scores between young and old groups. However, the two groups had different patterns of brain activity; both groups activated structures in the default mode network, but older adults showed more bilateral activation of frontal areas and stronger involvement of the linguistic components of the mirror neuron system, suggesting functional compensation among successfully aging older individuals with preserved TOM ability. (34, 35)

Age-related decline of TOM ability is prominent in neurocognitive disorders, such as MCI and dementia due to Alzheimer’s Disease, frontotemporal lobar degeneration, Lewy Body disease, and vascular cognitive impairment.(37, 36) The significant negative associations between RMET and CDR, MMSE, WTAR, education and all cognitive domain scores observed here reflect strong relevance to cognitive status and abilities. TOM is a highly complicated mental process that is not restricted to circumscribed brain regions or circuits that represent specific functions. (37) It might be viewed as an independent function from general cognition in aging, but more evidence indicates that some characteristics of social cognition are reliant on other cognitive functions i.e. processing speed, executive function, and working memory. (27, 35) Therefore, aging in social cognition should be assessed in the context of other relevant cognitive functions that are also vulnerable to aging. Longitudinal studies will provide critical information regarding how RMET scores may decline along with cognitive status; whether RMET effectively screens cases of pathologic neurodegenerative or cerebrovascular processes in early stages; and whether RMET predicts the trajectory of cognitive decline.

It has been previously described that depression is related to poor TOM ability including on RMET performance. (38) In our multiple regression models, RMET performance was worse with depressive symptoms as expected, and as well as with anxiety symptoms. While this association with GAD rating appears to diverge from a study reporting better TOM performance associated with worry among individuals with GAD, worry and anxiety are not the same. (39) Depression and anxiety in some but not all older adults may represent underlying neurodegeneration. (13, 40, 41)

The previously reported association between sex and the RMET performance (i.e. female superiority)(10) was not evident in our study except in the age group 65–74 years. Sex differences were also absent in a previous study among adults with autism. (42)

Also, a significant correlation with race was found in our study. A potential explanation is the so-called Other-Race Effect, which has been described in multiple kinds of literature; people consistently display worse recognition memory for other-race faces compared to same-race faces. (43, 44) A more recent study found evidence for both behavioral and neural differences in same-versus other-race mentalizing. (45) Thus, the RMET should be used judiciously in diverse populations. Adapted versions of the RMET test consisting of faces from different races and ethnic groups (i.e. Black RMET (46)) may generate different results.

Our group previously reported the distribution of SNQ-22 in the population-based cohort from the MYHAT study. The RMET score was significantly associated with the SNQ-22, although the two tests examine different components of social cognition. Potentially, a composite of the two measures might serve as a broader representation of social cognition in future studies.

It is increasingly recognized that social cognition is impaired in mild and major neurocognitive disorders due to neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease and Lewy Body disease, as well as vascular cognitive impairment.(37, 26, 36) As the world’s population ages and the prevalence of neurocognitive disorders rises accordingly, early diagnosis is important to provide better support for these patients. Our population-based norms on the RMET could prove useful to clinicians and other researchers in calibrating the scale for their own patients and study participants. Using the scale as a continuous measure, clinicians can assess their patients’ social cognition bearing in mind that test scores will be influenced by age, sex, race, and education, as well as by anxiety and depression. To identify appropriate screening cutoff scores in a given population or setting, the test should be normed and calibrated in that population or setting.

Our study cohort was relatively large, providing sufficient statistical power, and population-based, minimizing selection bias. All data were obtained directly from participants; we had no access to their medical records or neuroimaging data. As only 6% of our cohort was not of European descent, our findings should be replicated in other cohorts with greater minority representation. As with any vision-dependent test, the RMET is not suitable for administration to individuals with severely impaired vision. This was a cross-sectional study and thus no inferences may be drawn regarding the directions of the associations; prospective follow-up of the cohort will allow us to determine whether RMET scores, alone or in combination with SNQ22 scores, predict the incidence of various neurocognitive disorders.

HIGHLIGHTS.

1). What is the primary question addressed by this study?

The distribution in older adults of scores on the 10-item version of Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test of social cognition (Theory of Mind).

2). What is the main finding of this study?

The test score is normally distributed and associated with age, education, race, cognitive screening scores, literacy, dementia rating, depression symptoms, anxiety symptoms, cognitive function measured by neuropsychiatric battery, and awareness of social norms measured by Social Norms Questionnaire.

3). What is the meaning of the finding?

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test is a potentially useful measure of social cognition for use in the research assessment of cognitive decline and dementia. With appropriate calibration it should have utility in the clinical setting.

Acknowledgments

The work reported here was supported in part by grant # R01AG023651 from the National Institute on Aging, NIH, US DHHS.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

DISCLOSURE/CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Dr. Ganguli reports grants from National Institute on Aging during the conduct of the study. Dr. Chang reports grants from National Institute of Health during the conduct of the study. Dr. Lee, Ms. Jia, Ms. Jacobsen, and Dr. Snitz report no conflicts with any product mentioned or concept discussed in this article.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Shany-Ur T,Rankin KP: Cognition, Social, in Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences (Second Edition). Edited by Aminoff MJ,Daroff RB. Oxford, Academic Press, 2014, pp 814–817 [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Henry JD, von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, et al. : Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nature Reviews Neurology 2016; 12:28–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Poletti M, Enrici I,Adenzato M: Cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in neurodegenerative diseases: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 2012; 36:2147–2164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kemp J, Després O, Sellal F, et al. : Theory of Mind in normal ageing and neurodegenerative pathologies. Ageing Res Rev 2012; 11:199–219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Heitz C, Noblet V, Phillipps C, et al. : Cognitive and affective theory of mind in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther 2016; 8:10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ma J, Zhang Y,Guo Q: Comparison of vascular cognitive impairment--no dementia by multiple classification methods. Int J Neurosci 2015; 125:823–830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kynast J, Lampe L, Luck T, et al. : White matter hyperintensities associated with small vessel disease impair social cognition beside attention and memory. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2018; 38:996–1009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Heinrichs N, Rapee RM, Alden LA, et al. : Cultural differences in perceived social norms and social anxiety. Behaviour research and therapy 2006; 44:1187–1197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ganguli M, Sun Z, McDade E, et al. : That’s Inappropriate! Social Norms in an Older Population-based Cohort. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 2018; 32:150–155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, et al. : The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2001; 42:241–251 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chander RJ, Grainger SA, Crawford JD, et al. : Development of a short-form version of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test for assessing theory of mind in older adults. International journal of geriatric psychiatry 2020; 35:1322–1330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Olderbak S, Wilhelm O, Olaru G, et al. : A psychometric analysis of the reading the mind in the eyes test: toward a brief form for research and applied settings. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ganguli M, Snitz B, Vander Bilt J, et al. : How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study. International journal of geriatric psychiatry 2009; 24:1277–1284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ganguli M, Chang CC, Snitz BE, et al. : Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 18:674–683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Folstein MF, Folstein SE,McHugh PR: “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mungas D, Marshall SC, Weldon M, et al. : Age and education correction of Mini-Mental State Examination for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology 1996; 46:700–706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bondon-Guitton E, Perez-Lloret S, Bagheri H, et al. : Drug-induced parkinsonism: a review of 17 years’ experience in a regional pharmacovigilance center in France. Mov Disord 2011; 26:2226–2231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Radloff LS: The CES-D Scale:A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1977; 1:385–401 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ganguli M, Gilby J, Seaberg E, et al. : Depressive Symptoms and Associated Factors in a Rural Elderly Population: The MoVIES Project. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 1995; 3:144–160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, et al. : A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Archives of internal medicine 2006; 166:1092–1097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ganguli M, Bilt JV, Lee CW, et al. : Cognitive test performance predicts change in functional status at the population level: the MYHAT Project. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16:761–770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kramer JH, Mungas D, Possin KL, et al. : NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS 2014; 20:11–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43:2412–2414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kuder GF,Richardson MW: The theory of the estimation of test reliability. Psychometrika 1937; 2:151–160 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ziegler M, Kemper CJ,Kruyen P: Short scales – Five misunderstandings and ways to overcome them. Journal of Individual Differences 2014; 35:185–189 [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - DSM-5, 5th ed, Arlington, VA, US, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., 2013 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Duval C, Piolino P, Bejanin A, et al. : Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Conscious Cogn 2011; 20:627–642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Phillips LH, Bull R, Allen R, et al. : Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding. Cognition 2011; 120:236–247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Henry JD, Phillips LH, Ruffman T, et al. : A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and aging 2013; 28:826–839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bailey PE, Henry JD,Von Hippel W: Empathy and social functioning in late adulthood. Aging & mental health 2008; 12:499–503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Phillips LH, MacLean RDJ,Allen R: Age and the Understanding of Emotions: Neuropsychological and Sociocognitive Perspectives. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 2002; 57:P526–P530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Slessor G, Phillips LH,Bull R: Exploring the specificity of age-related differences in theory of mind tasks. Psychology and aging 2007; 22:639–643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Moran JM, Jolly E,Mitchell JP: Social-Cognitive Deficits in Normal Aging. The Journal of Neuroscience 2012; 32:5553–5561 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Castelli I, Baglio F, Blasi V, et al. : Effects of aging on mindreading ability through the eyes: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:2586–2594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Moran JM: Lifespan development: the effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behav Brain Res 2013; 237:32–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Henry JD, Phillips LH,von Hippel C: A meta-analytic review of theory of mind difficulties in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2014; 56:53–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Frith CD,Frith U: Mechanisms of social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 2012; 63:287–313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Richman MJ,Unoka Z: Mental state decoding impairment in major depression and borderline personality disorder: Meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 2015; 207:483–489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zainal NH,Newman MG: Worry amplifies theory-of-mind reasoning for negatively valenced social stimuli in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of affective disorders 2018; 227:824–833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lyketsos CG, Lopez O, Jones B, et al. : Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: results from the cardiovascular health study. Jama 2002; 288:1475–1483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gatchel JR: Late-Life Neuropsychiatric Symptoms: Windows Into Cognitive Decline? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2020; 28:72–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, et al. : The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test: Complete Absence of Typical Sex Difference in ~400 Men and Women with Autism. PloS one 2015; 10:e0136521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Barkowitz P,Brigham JC: Recognition of Faces: Own-Race Bias, Incentive, and Time Delay1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 1982; 12:255–268 [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lindsay DS, Jack PC,Christian MA: Other-race face perception. Journal of Applied Psychology 1991; 76:587–589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Reginald B Adams J, Rule NO, Robert G. Franklin J, et al. : Cross-cultural Reading the Mind in the Eyes: An fMRI Investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2010; 22:97–108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Handley G, Kubota JT, Li T, et al. : Black “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” task: The development of a task assessing mentalizing from black faces. PloS one 2019; 14:e0221867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES