Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Jun 4;16(6):e0252043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252043

Attentional and executive functions in children and adolescents with developmental coordination disorder and the influence of comorbid disorders: A systematic review of the literature

Catherine Lachambre 1,2, Mélodie Proteau-Lemieux 3, Jean-François Lepage 3, Eve-Line Bussières 4, Sarah Lippé 1,2,*
Editor: Celestino Rodríguez5
PMCID: PMC8177544  PMID: 34086687

Abstract

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting primarily motor skills, but attentional and executive impairments are common in affected individuals. Moreover, the presence of neurodevelopmental comorbidities is frequent in this population, which certainly influences the cognitive profile of the children concerned. Previous studies have reported deficits in visuospatial/nonverbal and planning tasks. This systematic review of the literature aims to determine if impairments can be found in other attentional and executive functions as well. The type of cognitive tasks, the tasks’ modality (verbal/nonverbal), and the influence of comorbid disorders on attentional and executive profiles are systematically considered. Forty-one studies were identified through the PubMed/Medline and PsycINFO databases according to pre-established eligibility criteria. The results reveal weaknesses in inhibitory control, working memory, planning, nonverbal fluency, and general executive functioning in children with DCD. The presence of comorbid disorders seemingly contributes to the verbal working memory difficulties findings. This review contributes to a better understanding of the cognitive impairments in DCD and of the needs of children with this disorder, allowing to optimize practitioners’ therapeutic interventions.

1. Introduction

1.1. Developmental coordination disorder

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects motor skills development in children and influences their ability to perform multiple activities of daily living [1, 2]. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition [3], DCD is characterized by difficulties in movement acquisition and execution. Diagnostic criteria include motor abilities that are significantly inferior to those expected given the individual chronological age and learning opportunities, and impairments significantly interfering with daily activities, academic and/or professional accomplishments and hobbies, which emerge during a child’s early development and are not better explained by intellectual disability, neurological condition affecting movement or visual impairment. Motor difficulties can be manifested as clumsiness, slowness and inaccuracy in movement execution.

Significant signs of DCD usually emerge during school years, but motor difficulties associated with DCD are developmental and tend to persist into adolescence [4] and adulthood [5]. Its prevalence is estimated between 5 and 6% in children aged 5 to 11 years old [3]. More boys than girls are affected, with reported ratios of men to women ranging from 1.9:1 to 7.3:1 [6, 7]. Great variability exists within countries, with prevalence among school-age children ranging from 1.8% in the United-Kingdom [7], 8% in Canada and 19% in Greece [8]. DCD is present across all cultural, ethnic and socioeconomic groups, but daily living activities and functional impacts of the disorder may vary within these different groups. Thus, these variables should be considered at the time of diagnosis.

DCD has been formerly referred to as clumsy child syndrome, and today the terms specific developmental disorder of motor function and dyspraxia can also be used to describe DCD [3, 9]. The terms DCD and dyspraxia (or developmental dyspraxia) are the most frequently used terms in the literature. DCD focusses on the observable symptoms, on their functional impacts and manifestations in daily living, and its diagnosis often belongs to occupational therapists or to physiotherapists [10, 11]. As for dyspraxia, the term is used interchangeably with DCD, and is widely used and diagnosed in neuropsychology [1013]. Dyspraxia is often defined as difficulties with organizing, planning, executing and coordinating movement, which leads to impairments in acquisition of complex movements and of movement sequences [10, 12, 14]. It relies on a developmental conception of the brain, implying a motor cognition disorder that includes visual and spatial processing problems [11]. Children with dyspraxia and DCD typically show visuoperceptual, visuospatial, visuoconstructives and visuomotor deficits [12, 15, 16], further impacting school achievement and daily living.

Several theoretical models of dyspraxia have been proposed over the years to provide a coherent view of its core elements. The one proposed by Vaivre-Douret and her team [13] offers a thorough definition as it stipulates that dyspraxia involves a deficit of both execution of voluntary gesture and planning/programming of movement. According to this model, when a child intends to perform an action, he first has to formulate a plan to execute that movement, that considers perceptual information, then to build a mental representation of the movement by coding spatiotemporal parameters, and finally to execute the movement while correcting it according to sensorimotor feedback [13, 14]. By conducting neuropsychological, neuro-psychomotor and neuro-visual assessments in children with dyspraxia, the authors showed that the planning and programming processes were the core problems in this disorder, and that the execution mechanisms were disturbed only when dyspraxia was comorbid with other neuropsychological disorders [13]. Therefore, the question arises as to whether children with dyspraxia (or DCD) present only planning impairments on motor tasks, or broader planning and executive functioning deficits.

1.2. Neurodevelopmental comorbidities

It seems that children who only have DCD are the exception rather than the rule, comorbid disorders being present in most of the cases [1719]. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most frequent comorbid condition, occurring approximately in 50% of the cases [3, 4, 6, 20, 21]. Besides, some authors report that DCD and ADHD overlap in their symptoms, as children with ADHD frequently demonstrate motor difficulties [22], and that executive dysfunctions and slow processing speed, which are typical features of ADHD, are often present in children with DCD [20]. It is thus unclear which difficulties are inherent to which disorder, but ADHD and DCD must still be considered as separate disorders since their core deficits are distinct [23]. Learning disorders, including developmental dyslexia (DDL) [24, 25], specific language impairment (SLI) [17, 26, 27], behavioral problems [17, 28] and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [6, 29] are also common in children with DCD, concomitance between these disorders and DCD reaching up to 30 to 50%. Since deficits in executive and attentional functioning are generally part of these disorders [3032], it seems essential to consider their presence and potential influence on executive and attentional capacities in children with DCD.

1.3. Executive and attentional functions

Executive functions are generally conceptualized as a set of general high order control processes [33] working together [34] to direct and manage cognitive, emotional and behavioral functions, especially during active problem solving [35]. A variety of cognitive abilities falls under the umbrella of executive functions, including working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility/shifting, goal-setting, planning, organization, self-regulation and fluency [3337]. To this day, however, it is still unclear exactly how many components define executive functions and under which terms they should be grouped [38]. Indeed, several models of executive functions have been proposed, integrating different components and establishing interactions between them. One of the most integrative and complete models was proposed by Diamond [36]. According to this model, working memory is defined as the ability to hold information in mind for a short period of time while mentally manipulating it to execute a task, and it includes verbal and visuospatial subcomponents. Inhibitory control is described as the ability to control attention, behavior, thoughts and emotions to override a dominant, automatic or prepotent response. The author also explains that inhibitory control is divided into two subcomponents: interference control, combining cognitive and attentional inhibition, and response inhibition, defined as behavioral inhibition. It also encompasses self-regulation, which includes attentional and response inhibition, while focusing primarily on emotional control and regulation. According to Diamond [36], working memory and inhibitory control support each other: working memory allows one to maintain their goals in their mind along with what they should or should not do, and inhibitory control allows one to stay focused on the important working memory content by inhibiting distractors. When working together, these two executive functions allow cognitive flexibility, defined as one’s ability to see things from another perspective and to shift between tasks. According to this model, fluency skills are part of cognitive flexibility, since one must be able to shift between different mind sets to be fluent in generating various ideas. Higher-level executive functions are also underpinned by working memory, inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility in this model: reasoning and problem-solving, which are synonymous with fluid intelligence, and planning skills [36].

Attentional functions can be divided into three central components [39, 40]: alertness, selective attention, and divided attention. Alertness allows individuals to adopt and sustain a state of vigilance and is typically involved in long, monotonous tasks. It contrasts with the second component, selective attention, which is the ability to selectively process a specific kind of information while ignoring distractors and involves filtering mechanisms. The third subfunction of attention is divided attention and concerns the notion of sharing mental processing capacities, which appears necessary when performing two or more tasks simultaneously [39, 40]. These attentional functions are also closely related to executive functions, particularly to inhibitory control and working memory; inhibition capacities allow attentional orienting and control [36], while the allocation of attentional resources to specific information stream will dictate the ones accessing working memory [41].

Executive dysfunction leads to resistance to change, incapacity to modify non-optimal behaviors, impulsivity and risk-taking, social difficulties, avolition and learning difficulties [34, 36, 42]. For this reason, executive functions are essential to children’s optimal daily functioning and their future quality of life, in their affective, social as well as academic spheres [11, 36].

1.4. State of knowledge

Several studies have reported impairments in executive and attentional functions in children with DCD [21, 4345]. While some authors report a generalized executive dysfunction in this population [20], others report deficits in specific domains of attentional or executive functioning [45, 46], which can be more severe in nonverbal modalities [47]. Consequently, and according to the theoretical model of dyspraxia previously discussed, the question arises as to whether the impairments are only present in visuospatial/nonverbal modalities and on planning tasks, and thus are more linked to the primary deficits found in DCD, or whether difficulties can be found across a broader range of executive functions. This systematic review of the literature aims to answer this question, while considering the influence of comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders combined with DCD on attentional and executive profiles found in these children. This review is an important step in the acquisition and gathering of supplemental evidence regarding the attentional and executive deficits found in DCD. Indeed, consolidating the results found in previous research appears relevant, especially given that several studies based their conclusions on small samples.

On a clinical level, the purpose of this review is to shed light on impairments that can be a part of DCD rather than being explained by the comorbid disorders often observed in this disorder, and to better identify the needs of children with DCD considering their potentially complex clinical picture, in order to optimize their assessment and the interventions carried out. This systematic review is the first to focus exclusively on the cognitive profile associated with DCD, and more specifically on the attentional and executive functions in young individuals with this disorder, while explicitly considering the possible influence of co-occurring disorders.

2. Method

This systematic review was performed based on the guidance outlined in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) [48].

2.1. Search strategy and selection criteria

A systematic literature research was performed in PubMed/Medline and PsycINFO databases, including articles published between January 1980 and May 2020. Considering that pediatrics neuropsychology was poorly developed before 1980, it was chosen as the earliest publication date. Due to the multiple ways of conceptualizing attentional and executive functions in terms of their components, and to be as inclusive as possible, our research was made using all terms previously mentioned that are used to describe these functions. Thereby, research was conducted in English with the following keywords: (1) “developmental coordination disorder” OR “dyspraxi*” OR “motor skills disorder” OR “specific developmental disorder of motor function” OR “clumsiness” OR “clumsy child syndrome”; (2) AND “executive function*” OR “goal setting” OR “set-shifting” OR “shifting” OR “switching” OR “flexibility” OR “planning” OR “inhibit*” OR “working memory” OR “organis*” OR “organiz*” OR “self-regulation” OR “fluency” OR “attention*”; (3) AND “child*” OR “adolescen*” OR “teen*” OR “youth” OR “schoolchild*” OR “preschool*”. Publications referenced in the included articles were also screened to find additional articles.

Studies were included if (1) their participants were children or adolescents (17 years of age or younger; studies including older participants were excluded), (2) they had a group of participants with a diagnosis of DCD made by a health care professional, by a score at or below the 5th percentile on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC first or second edition), as this score indicates a significant movement difficulty [49], or by DSM (IV, IV-TR or 5) criteria for DCD combined with a movement ability measure, in which case a total score at or below the 15th percentile on the MABC was accepted, (3) their participants did not explicitly have any medical condition that could affect their motor or cognitive abilities, (4) they measured one or more attentional or executive functions using performance tests, (5) they used normative data of standardized measures or a control group comprising healthy individuals for results comparison, (6) they were published in French or English in a peer-reviewed journal and (7) they had an empirical research design.

Studies were first selected according to their title. Second, abstracts were read by two authors (CL and MPL) and studies that did not meet the eligibility criteria listed above were excluded. Then the same two authors screened full articles independently to ensure that all eligibility criteria were met. Their disagreements were discussed to reach a consensus and, whenever necessary, another author (SL) settled. The remaining articles were entirely read by the first author.

2.2. Data extraction

Data extraction from articles that met the selection criteria was made by the two first authors. Information was organized in an spreadsheet and included: title of the article, authors, year of publication, journal in which it was published, aims of the study, groups, their origin and samples size, gender and age of the subjects assessed (mean, standard deviation and range, when available), country in which the study took place, inclusion and exclusion criteria, presence of comorbid disorders in the sample and their nature, information about assessment of motor functions and confounding variables, cognitive functions assessed and tasks used, statistical analysis, results, limitations and commentaries about the paper. Subsequently, the relevant information was analyzed and summarized in Table 1: the authors and year of publication, the sample size, the gender of the participants and the number of males, the mean age of the participants, the presence of comorbid disorders, the attentional or executive function studied, the task(s) used to evaluate the function, and a summary of the results. Components of attentional and/or executive functioning assessed in each included study were determined according to what the study purported to measure. When the same score on a task was reported to assess more than one component of attentional or executive functioning, or to measure different components in different studies, results were reported only in relation to the component it was the most associated with, according to the Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests [50] or the task’s reference. When several scores were available on the same task and associated with different components of attentional or executive functioning, information provided by each score was considered as a measure of its respective component.

Table 1. Studies included in the systematic review.

Studies Authors (year) Groups (sample size) Origin Gender Number of males (%) Age (years) M (SD) Presence of a comorbid disorder to DCD Attentional or executive function studied Task(s) used to assess attentional or executive functions Summary of results
Alesi et al. (2019) DCD (18)
TD (18)
Schools
9 (50.0%)
9 (50.0%)
4.6 (0.9)
4.6 (0.9)
Exclusion of children with intellectual disability, visual or neurological impairment and neurodevelopment-tal disorders. Fluency
(verbal)
Response inhibition
(verbal)
Working memory (verbal)


Working memory (visuospatial)
BVN-5-11

Stroop Task,
Gift Wrap Task,
Snack Delay Task
Working Memory Tasks:
Forward word recall, Selective word recall, Verbal dual task
Pathway recall, Selective pathway recall, Visuospatial dual task
Children with DCD performed significantly more poorly than TD children on both the forward (p = .022) and selective word recalls (p = .05), but the results did not differ significantly for the verbal dual task. They also had lower scores on the three visuo-spatial tasks (p < .05). Children with DCD also performed more poorly than TD children on the BVN-5-11(p = .00). Children with DCD obtained lower scores for both congruent and incongruent stimuli on the Stroop task (p < .01). However, there was no significant difference between DCD and TD children on emotionally-valenced tasks
Alloway (2007) DCD (55; same sample as Alloway, 2011 and Alloway et al., 2009)
Schools, referred by health care professio-nals
44 (80.0%) 8.8 (1.6) No mention of exclusion.
Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
AWMA:
Listening Recall, Counting Recall, Backwards Digit Recall
Odd-One-Out, Mr. X, Spatial Span
49% of sample obtained standard scores of less than 85 on the verbal WM measures. 60% of sample obtained scores of less than 85 on the visuospatial WM measures.
The difference between performance on visuospatial and verbal WM measures was not significant.
Alloway (2011) DCD (55; same sample as Alloway, 2007 and Alloway et al., 2009)
ADHD (50)
TD (50)
Schools, referred by health care professio-nals
44 (80.0%)



43 (86.0%)
30 (60.0%)
8.8 (1.6)



9.8 (1.0)
9.9 (1.0)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with behavioral or attentional problems.
Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
AWMA:
Listening Recall, Counting Recall, Backwards Digit Recall
Odd-One-Out, Mr. X, Spatial Span
The DCD and ADHD groups performed more poorly than the TD group on both verbal and visuospatial WM measures (p < .05). Children with DCD showed deficits on both modalities compared to TD controls. Their WM profile did not differ significantly compared to that of children with ADHD.
Alloway & Archibald (2008) DCD only (11)
DCD unselected (12)
SLI (11)
Schools, referred by health care professio-nals
8 (72.7%)
8 (66.7%)
7 (63.6%)
8.9 (1.4)
8.5 (1.6)
8.8 (1.4)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with ADHD or ASD.
Inclusion of children with language or nonverbal reasoning difficulties (DCD unselected group).

Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
AWMA:
Listening Recall, Counting Recall, Backwards Digit Recall
Odd-One-Out, Mr. X, Spatial Span
The two DCD groups did not significantly differ on their WM profiles. Their performance was more than 1.25 SD below the standardized mean on most tasks. Children in the DCD only group performed significantly worse than children with SLI on the visuospatial WM measures, even when the contribution of receptive language skills was accounted for. Their verbal WM skills were similar. Thus, children in DCD groups had deficits in both WM modalities, whereas those in SLI group only had verbal WM deficits.
Alloway et al. (2009) DCD (55; same sample as Alloway, 2007, 2011)
SLI (15)
ADHD (83)
AS (10)
Schools, referred by health care professio-nals
44 (80.0%)



9 (60.0%)
71 (85.5%)
8 (80.0%)
8.8 (1.6)



9.2 (1.7)
9.1 (1.1)
8.8 (1.5)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with behavioral problems.
Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
AWMA:
Listening Recall, Counting Recall, Backwards Digit Recall
Odd-One-Out, Mr. X, Spatial Span
Children with DCD had significantly lower scores than children with AS on visuospatial WM measures (p < .05), but the difference was attributable to the motor component of the tests. There were no significant differences when comparing with the other clinical groups. On verbal WM measures, there were no differences between groups.
Alloway & Temple (2007) DCD (20)
MLD (20)
Schools, referred by health care professio-nals
14 (70.0%)
15 (75.0%)
9.8 (1.4)
9.8 (1.4)
No mention of exclusion.
Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
AWMA:
Listening Recall, Counting Recall, Backwards Digit Recall
Odd-One-Out, Mr. X, Spatial Span
Mean standard scores of children with DCD on all WM tasks were less than 85. Performances of children with DCD were poorer than that of children with MLD on all WM measures (p < .01). Children with DCD showed significant deficits in visuospatial WM, while MLD group performed within age-expected level in this domain. On measures of verbal WM, both groups were impaired. Thus, impaired visuospatial WM seems to be more specific to children with DCD when compared to children with MLD.
Asonitou & Koutsouki (2016) DCD (54)
TD (54)
General population
36 (66.7%)
36 (66.7%)
Total of 42 5-year-olds and 66 6-year-olds:
5.5 (0.4)
Exclusion of children with any other medical or neurological condition, or identified intellectual disability.
Selective attention (visual)


Planning
CAS:
Expressive Attention, Number Detection, Receptive Attention
Matching Numbers, Planned Codes, Planned Connections
A significant proportion of children with DCD were impaired on planning and attention measures (p < .01). Globally, children with DCD had more difficulties than TD children in all measured domains, and difficulties in planning were greater than those in attention.
Asonitou et al. (2012) DCD (54)
5-year-old (24)
6-year-old (30)
TD (54)
5-year-old (18)
6-year-old (36)
General population
36 (66.7%)
13 (54.2%)
23 (76.7%)
37 (68.5%)
15 (83.3%)
22 (61.1%)
42 5-year-olds:
5.2 (0.3)
66 6-year-olds:
5.8 (0.2)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with emotional or behavioral disorder, with a history of pre- or existing developmental disorder (such as ADHD), or with an intellectual disability (IQ < 70).
Selective attention (visual)


Planning
CAS:
Expressive Attention, Number Detection, Receptive Attention
Matching Numbers, Planned Codes, Planned Connections
Children with DCD performed more poorly than TD children on all tasks (p < .05), indicating difficulties in terms of both attention and planning when compared to healthy controls. Also, more severe motor impairment seems to be associated with poorer planning abilities.
Barray et al. (2008) DD (32)
AP (16)
Learning disorders centers
24 (75.0%)
6 (37.5%)
9.1 (2.3)
9.0 (2.1)
Exclusion of children with probable personality disorder, social conduct disorder, or verbal IQ < 80.
Selective attention (visual)
Selective attention (auditive)
Planning
Fluency (nonverbal)
NEPSY:
Visual Attention


Auditory Attention and Response Set

Tower task
Design Fluency
Children with DD performed significantly better than those with AP only on the Visual attention task (p = .022). All children had difficulties in the Design Fluency task that seemed to be the most difficult task for them. Performances on the other tasks were globally within the normative range.
Bernardi et al. (2017) DCD (17)
MD (17)
TD (17)
General population
6 (35.3%)
8 (47.1%)
13 (76.5%)
12.0 (1.2)
10.5 (0.6)
11.3 (1.0)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with ADHD, ASD, reading, language and IQ < -2 SD, or any medical condition. Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (nonverbal)

Fluency (verbal)
Fluency (nonverbal)
Response inhibition (verbal)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Planning (verbal)
Planning (nonverbal)

Flexibility (verbal)
Flexibility (nonverbal)
WMTBC: Listening Recall
Odd-One-Out test
D-KEFS: Verbal Fluency
D-KEFS: Design Fluency
Verbal VIMI (total errors)
Motor VIMI (total errors)
D-KEFS: Sorting test (verbal sorts)
D-KEFS: Sorting test (nonverbal sorts)
D-KEFS: Trail Making test
CANTAB: Intra-/Extra-Dimensional Shift
Children with DCD performed significantly more poorly than TD children on all nonverbal measures (p < .001) and on the verbal fluency task (p = .001) at both time points (2-year follow-up). Children with MD also had poorer performance on all nonverbal measures, except the flexibility one. At time 2, only nonverbal WM (p = .005) and nonverbal fluency (p = .047) differences remained between MD and TD groups.
Improvement over time was significant for verbal (p < .001) and nonverbal (p = .002) WM, fluency and flexibility(p < .001), and for nonverbal planning (p = .013). It was not significant for verbal and nonverbal inhibition and verbal planning. Changes over time were similar between each group.
Bernardi et al. (2016) DCD (23)
MD (30)
TD (38)
(Same sample as Leonard et al., 2015)
General population
16 (69.6%)
17 (56.7%)
17 (44.7%)
10.0 (1.1)
8.9 (1.2)
9.3 (1.0)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with any other neurodevelopment-tal disorder, including ADHD. Response inhibition (verbal)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Verbal VIMI (total errors and total completion time)
Motor VIMI (total errors and total completion time)
On the motor inhibition task, children with DCD or MD had difficulties performing accurately. On the verbal inhibition task, they took significantly more time than TD children (p = .002), but the accuracy between groups was similar. Inhibition impairments in DCD and MD groups thus appear to affect accuracy when a motor response is required and completion time when the response is given verbally. There were no significant differences between DCD and MD groups.
Biotteau et al. (2017) DCD (22)
DCD+ DDL (23)
DDL (20)
Referred by a learning disability center or health care profession-nals
16 (72.7%)
16 (69.6%)

12 (60.0%)
9.7 (1.6)
9.9 (1.2)

10.2 (1.3)
Exclusion of children with intellectual disability, SLI or ADHD, according to DSM-IV-TR criteria.
Presence of DDL, excluding surface dyslexia, in DCD+DDL group.
Sustained attention (visual)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Working memory (verbal)
CPT-II (omission errors)

CPT-II (commission errors)
WISC-IV: Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing
All children had mean scores within or slightly above the normal range on the CPT-II. Variations between groups were not significant. WM index was within the normal range in the three groups.
Having a dual diagnosis did not lead to a cumulative impact on cognitive abilities.
Blais et al. (2017) DCD (10)
TD (10)
Hospital
7 (70.0%)
3 (30.0%)
13.5 (1.4)
13.5 (1.8)
Exclusion of children with a history of head trauma or epilepsy, an intellectual disability or ADHD according to DSM-5 criteria. Sustained attention (visual)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
CPT-II (omission errors)

CPT-II (commission errors)
Children with DCD performed significantly worse than TD children. Their average percentages of commission, omission and perseveration errors were significantly greater than those of control individuals (p < .05). There was no difference between groups for reaction times.
Chen et al. (2012) SDCD (20)
MDCD (46)
TD (36)
Research database
9 (45.0%)
16 (34.8%)
24 (66.7%)
9.5 (0.3)
9.6 (0.3)
9.7 (0.3)
Exclusion of children with any signs of neurolo-gical of physical impairments, developmental disorders or intellectual disability, accor-ding to a rehabi-litation physician. Attentional inhibition (endogenous mode of orienting attention) COVAT The capacity of children with DCD, both severe and moderate, to intentionally disengage their attention from invalid cued location when the delay between precue and target stimulus was longer was reduced. This suggests a deficit of attentional control in DCD, more specific to the endogenous mode of orienting attention and referring to the process of disengagement inhibition.
de Castelnau et al. (2007) DCD (24)
TD (60)
Hospital
18 (75.0%)
30 (50.0%)
3 age groups:
8–9 years, 10–11 years and 12–13 years.
8 children with DCD and 20 TD children in each of them.
Exclusion of children with ADHD, according to a brief neuropsychological examination and DSM-IV criteria, and with an IQ < 80. Sustained attention (visual)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
CPT double version (correct responses)
CPT double version (commission errors)
Children with DCD had significantly less correct responses than TD children (p < .0001), but it increased with age (p < .05). They omitted significantly more responses than controls. However, there was no significant difference in number of commission errors between groups, and these decreased with age. In conclusion, children with DCD had poorer attentional capacities than TD children, but they were not more impulsive, and their capacities improved with age.
Dyck & Piek (2010) DCD (20)
RELD (21)
PLA (22)
PMC (28)
Referred by health care profession-nals
13 (65.0%)
16 (76.2%)
14 (63.6%)
17 (60.7%)
8.4 (2.1)
7.1 (1.8)
7.5 (1.6)
8.8 (3.1)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with any other comorbid disorders. Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Working memory (verbal)
Go/No Go task (commission errors)
Trailmaking/Me-mory Updating task, Goal Neglect task
Children with DCD had scores within the normal range on the response inhibition task. Their performances were within the low normal range for WM tasks. Differences with children with PMC were not significant. Scores of children in the RELD group were lower on the response inhibition and WM tasks (p < .05), indicating greater difficulties in this group.
Gonzalez et al. (2016) DCD (10)
TD (12)
Specialized clinic
7 (70.0%)
8 (66.7%)
10.1 (1.0)
10.0 (1.1)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with ADHD. Attentional inhibition (exogenous mode of orienting attention) Visuospatial attention task cued and non-cued conditions Children with DCD made more inhibition errors (saccades to the cue) than TD children (p = .002), indicating poor inhibitory control. These inhibition difficulties resulted in inappropriate allocation of visual attention.
Kaiser & Albaret (2016) DCD (7)
ADHD (9)
TD (15)
Specialized school, referred by health care profession-nals
Not mentioned 9.9 (0.9)
10.2 (1.3)
9.6 (1.3)
Exclusion of children with a dual diagnosis of DCD and ADHD, and with an IQ < 70. Selective attention (visual)
Alertness (visual)
Divided attention
KITAP:
Distractibility

Alerting

Divided Attention
Results of children with DCD on KITAP tasks were not significantly different from results of children with ADHD or of TD children. Thus, the three groups cannot be discriminated based on their performance on the KITAP.
Kirby et al. (2010) DCD (11)
TD (28)
Research database
8 (72.7%)
13 (46.4%)
9.9
10.2
(SDs not available)
Exclusion of children with severe learning difficulties or low cognitive ability, according to teachers.
Inclusion of children with ADHD symptoms.
Planning River Crossing task Significant differences between groups were seen in children’s planning strategies. Children with DCD tended to add mats (p < .05), but not modify their placement of mats, whereas TD children were more likely to change spacing of mats. This suggests that initial mat placement plan of TD children was more effective than that of children with DCD. This indicates that children with DCD may have difficulty to elaborate a plan as efficiently as TD children and may also not know how to improve it.
Leonard et al. (2015) DCD (23)
MD (30)
TD (38)
(Same sample as Bernardi et al., 2016)
General population
16 (69.6%)
17 (56.7%)
17 (44.7%)
10.0 (1.1)
8.9 (1.2)
9.3 (1.0)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with ASD or ADHD, and of children with IQ, language or reading skills more than two SD below the mean. Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (nonverbal)
Fluency (verbal)

Fluency (nonverbal)
Response inhibition (verbal)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Planning (verbal)
Planning (nonverbal)
Flexibility (verbal)
Flexibility (nonverbal)
WMTBC: Listening Recall

Odd-One-Out test


D-KEFS: Verbal Fluency
D-KEFS: Design Fluency

Verbal VIMI (total errors)

Motor VIMI (total errors)
D-KEFS: Sorting test (verbal sorts)
D-KEFS: Sorting test (nonverbal sorts)
D-KEFS: Trail Making test
CANTAB: Intra-/Extra-Dimensional Shift
For nonverbal WM, fluency and inhibition tasks, children with MD or DCD scored significantly lower than TD children (p < .001). For nonverbal planning, only the MD group (not the DCD group) differed significantly from the TD group (p < .001). There were no significant differences between groups on flexibility tasks or on any of the verbal measures. In summary, children with DCD or MD had more difficulties on nonverbal tasks compared to TD children.
Mandich et al. (2002) DCD younger + older (20)
TD younger + older (20)
Specialized clinic
Not mentioned Younger: 8.6 (0.8)
Older: 10.6 (0.7)
Younger: 8.5 (0.7)
Older: 10.8 (0.8)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with ADD. Response inhibition
(nonverbal)
Attentional inhibition (exogenous mode of orienting attention)
Visual Simon task (failure-to-inhibit errors)
Visual Simon task (reaction times)
Children with DCD seem to have no deficit regarding the time needed to suppress an incorrect response and this ability appears to develop at the same rate in both groups. However, children with DCD had a reliably smaller rate of correct responses in the incompatible trials compared to TD children (p = .011). Overall, children with DCD exhibited an inhibitory dysfunction regarding manual response inhibition, manifesting itself in terms of error rate.
Mandich et al. (2003) DCD (18)
TD (18)
Specialized clinic
Not mentioned 9.9 (1.5)
9.8 (1.4)
No mention of exclusion. Response inhibition (nonverbal)



Attentional inhibition (endogenous mode of orienting attention)
Go/No Go task with both informative and uninformative precue conditions (anticipation and failure-to-inhibit errors)
Go/No Go task with both informative and uninformative precue conditions (reaction times)
Children with DCD did not commit more anticipation errors than TD children, but they produced about twice as many failure-to-inhibit errors (p = .007). Children with DCD may have impaired inhibitory control, since they needed more time to intentionally disengage their attention from the cued position (disengagement inhibition). They also exhibited difficulty to inhibit the unwanted movement of attention urged by precues. Thus, they exhibited more difficulties than controls in restraining both their manual and attentional movements.
Piek et al. (2007) DCD (18)
ADHD-I (20)
ADHD-C (19)
TD (138)
Referred by health care profession-nals
12 (66.7%)
16 (80.0%)
15 (78.9%)
59 (42.8%)
8.8 (2.0)
10.8 (1.8)
10.7 (2.3)
10.3 (2.2)
Exclusion of children with an estimated IQ < 80. Working memory (verbal)
Flexibility
Trailmaking/Memory Updating task, Goal Neglect task
Visual Inspection Time task
On the Goal Neglect task, children with DCD made more errors than the three other groups (p < .05). On the Trailma-king/Memory Updating task, children in the DCD group were significantly slower than those in other groups (p < .01) but did not commit more errors. They were also slower than the other groups on the set-shifting task. Thus, performance of children with DCD was significantly poorer than that of ADHD and TD children on all measures of EF, and there was no significant difference between ADHD and TD children.
Pratt et al. (2014) DCD (26)
TD (24)
General population
22 (84.6%)
13 (54.2%)
9.9 (2.5)
9.6 (2.0)
Exclusion of children with any other disorder, such as ADHD, ASD or dyslexia. Planning (reduced motor-load)
Planning (high motor-load)
Response inhibition (reduced motor-load/verbal)
Response inhibition (high motor-load/nonverbal)
NEPSY: Tower task

Rotational Bar task
Stroop task


NEPSY: Knock-Tap task
Children with DCD performed significantly more poorly than TD children on both the high motor-load and reduced motor-load planning tasks. They also had significantly lower scores than TD children on the reduced motor-load inhibition task, but differences in performance on the high motor-load inhibition task were not significant. Results might have been influenced by the tasks’ complexity.
Querne et al. (2008) DCD (9)
TD (10)
Hospital
7 (77.8%)
7 (70.0%)
9.9 (1.8)
10.0 (1.1)
Exclusion of children with a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Sustained attention (visual)
Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Go/No Go task (omission errors)

Go/No Go task (commission errors)
Children with DCD did not make more commission errors than TD children, but they made significantly more omission errors (p = .011). Thus, children with DCD seem to be as effective as TD children in inhibiting a prepotent motor response, but to have more difficulties with sustained attention.
Rahimi-Golkhan-dan et al. (2016) DCD (12)
TD (24)
Schools, general population
4 (33.3%)
10 (41.7%)
9.8 (1.4)
10.3 (1.6)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with intellectual disability, or neurological or psychiatric disorders such as ADHD. Sustained attention (visual)


Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Go/No Go task with positively- and negatively-valenced stimuli (omission errors)
Go/No Go task with positively- and negatively-valenced stimuli (commission errors)
Children with DCD were more impulsive than TD children with happy faces stimuli, as they made more commission errors (p = .017). The difference between groups for sad faces was not significant. There was no significant difference between the two groups for omission errors. Overall, results showed a deficit of inhibitory control in the DCD group when the no-go stimulus was a compelling, positively-valenced cue, which shows a deficit in ‘hot’ executive functions.
Ruddock et al. (2016) DCD (62)
6 years (7)
7 (13)
8 (11)
9 (11)
10 (15)
11 (5)
TD (109)
6 years (28)
7 (21)
8 (21)
9 (18)
10 (15)
11 (5)
12 (1)
Schools, general population
35 (56.5%)
3 (42.8%)
9 (69.2%)
9 (81.8%)
7 (63.6%)
6 (40.0%)
1 (20.0%)
48 (44.0%)
9 (32.1%)
7 (33.3%)
10 (47.6%)
12 (66.7%)
7 (46.7%)
2 (40.0%)
1 (100.0%)

6.4 (0.6)
7.5 (0.3)
8.6 (0.4)
9.5 (0.3)
10.6 (0.3)
11.3 (0.2)
6.4 (0.4)
7.5 (0.3)
8.4 (0.3)
9.5 (0.3)
10.3 (0.3)
11.4 (0.2)
12.3 (0.0)
Exclusion of children with a history of developmental (ASD, ADHD), physical and/or neurological condition reported in a parent/teacher pre-screening questionnaire. Response inhibition (nonverbal) Double-Jump Reaching task–Modified version Until 10–11 years old, children with DCD performed slower and with more variability than TD children. Therefore, in general, they were less efficient than controls, but the developmental lag in their performance seemed to lessen in later childhood.
Performance of both groups improved with age, but the growth curve was different.
Ruddock et al. (2015) DCD (42)
6–7 years (10)
8–9 (16)
10–12 (16)
TD (87)
6–7 years (26)
8–9 (38)
10–12 (23)
Schools, general population
22 (52.4%)
5 (50.0%)
11 (68.8%)
6 (37.5%)
34 (39.1%)
9 (34.6%)
15 (39.5%)
10 (43.5%)

7.3 (0.7)
8.9 (0.6)
11.1 (0.4)
7.2 (0.5)
8.9 (0.6)
10.7 (0.5)
Exclusion of children for whom any developmental, neurological and/or physical condition was reported, which was confirmed by the child’s school health officer. Response inhibition (nonverbal) Double-Jump Reaching task–Modified version Children with DCD were generally slower than TD children (p = .002) and they made significantly more anticipation errors, indicating difficulties in inhibitory control. Moreover, only the differences between younger children and the two other groups were significant (p = .005); mid-aged and older children did not significantly differ in their anticipation error rate. Thereby, initiation inhibition capacities are poorer in children with DCD than in TD children, but the gap between them lessen when they get older.
Sartori et al. (2020) DCD (63)
MD (31)
TD (63)
Schools
39 (62.0%)
20 (65.0%)
39 (62.0%)
8.7 (0.64)
8.9 (0.74)
8.7 (0.63)
Exclusion of children with a neurological condition or with ADHD. Flexibility

Response inhibition
(nonverbal)
Response inhibition
(verbal)
Working memory (verbal)
Working memory (visuospatial)
Five Digits Test, Trail Making Test
Go/No Go app (motor response)

Go/No Go app
(verbal response)

Hayling Test,
Oral Word Span in Sentences
Odd-One-Out
DCD and MD children were slower on the Oral Word Span and the Odd-One-Out than TD children (p < .001). Children with DCD performed more poorly than TD children on both tasks (verbal and motor responses) of the Go/No Go app (p < .005) and on the Hayling test (p < .001). DCD children made more mistakes and had longer completion times on parts 2, 3 and 4 of the Five Digits Test (p < .001), and MD children made more mistakes than TD children on parts 3 and 4 of the Five Digit Test (p = .01). Children with DCD also performed more poorly than TD children on both parts of the Trail Making Test. No statistical differences were found between DCD and MD children.
Sumner, Pratt, & Hill (2016) DCD (52)
TD (52)
General population
36 (69.2%)
36 (69.2%)
9.2 (2.2)
9.3 (1.5)
Exclusion of children diagnosed with any other developmental disorder (ADHD, ASD, dyslexia), neurological condition, or other medical condition that could explain motor impairment. Working memory (verbal) WISC-IV: Digit Span, Letter-Number Sequencing Scores of children with DCD were significantly lower than TD children on the WM index (p = .02). Children with DCD performed significantly worse than TD children on the Digit span task (p < .05), but not on the Letter-number sequencing task. Almost 30% of children with DCD had scores of more than 1 SD below the population mean on the Digit span. Thus, the results showed some indication of difficulties in WM.
Thornton et al. (2018) DCD (9)
DCD+
ADHD (18)
ADHD (20)
TD (20)
Schools,advertising in hospitals and physicians’ offices
6 (67.0%)
15 (83.0%)
18 (90.0%)
8 (40.0%)
13.6 (2.74)
10.9 (2.62)
12.4 (2.8)
10.6 (1.67)
Exclusion of children with a visual or intellectual impairment and a neurological or medical disorder. Response inhibition
(nonverbal)
Go/No Go Task
(commission errors)
Children with co-occurring DCD and ADHD made more commission and total errors than TD children (p < .05). However, the reaction times and omission errors did not differ between groups.
Toussaint-Thorin et al. (2013) DD (13)
TD (14)
Hospital
11 (84.6%)
8 (57.1%)
10.1 (1.4)
10.4 (1.3)
Exclusion of children with possible associated dyslexia and with a verbal IQ < 70.
Inclusion of children with ADHD.
Selective attention (auditive)
Flexibility

Planning

Response inhibition (nonverbal)
Fluency (verbal)

General executive functioning
NEPSY: Auditory Attention and Response Set
Trail Making test A and B for Children
NEPSY: Tower task, BADS-C: 6-Part test
The Paired Images test

NEPSY: Verbal Fluency
Ecological assessment: cooking task
No deficits were found on measures of attention, flexibility and fluency. Almost half of children with DD had a score in the clinical range on the inhibition task. Three children with DD had a pathological score on the tower task, but planning appeared to be more impaired on the 6-Part test, the strategy score being particularly affected. Thus, children with DD exhibited deficits of nonverbal planning and inhibition (p < .05). On the cooking task, children with DD made significantly more errors than TD children (p < .001) and they were not able to inhibit their verbalizing behavior, despite the task’s guidelines. They were also significantly more dependent than TD children (p = .034), suggesting a limited capacity to find strategies or solutions to face a problem.
Tsai et al. (2012) DCD (24)
TD (30)
General population
12 (50.0%)
15 (50.0%)
11.6 (0.3)
11.7 (0.4)
Exclusion of children with ADHD according to DSM-IV criteria, with any known neurological disorder, behavioral problems, pervasive developmental disorders or special educational needs, or an IQ < 85 or > 115. Alertness (visual)


Working memory (visuospatial)
Visuospatial Working Memory Paradigm (non-delay condition)
Visuospatial Working Memory Paradigm (delay condition)
Children with DCD performed less accurately than TD children in the conditions with delays (p < .001), but not in the non-delay condition. This indicates difficulty in WM tasks, but not in the attentional task. EEG results showed that children with DCD allocated fewer resources to compare spatial locations (p < .001). Overall, a deficit in retrieval of spatial information was found in children with DCD through EEG measures, which seemed to impair their WM performance.
Tsai et al. (2010) DCD (30)
TD (30)
General population
15 (50.0%)
15 (50.0%)
9.5 (0.3)
9.6 (0.2)
Exclusion of children with ADHD according to DSM-IV, any defi-nite signs of neuro-logical disorders, behavioral pro-blems or pervasive development disor-ders, special needs in education, or an IQ < 85 or > 125. Attentional inhibition (exogenous mode of orienting attention) Visuospatial attention (eye-gaze cueing) paradigm There were no significant differences between groups on error rates. Children with DCD responded significantly slower than TD children in all conditions (p < .001). This suggests a reduced alertness to the imminent appearance of the target. Results showed a deficit in reflexive/automatic (exogenous) orienting of visual attention.
1Tsai et al. (2009) DCD (28)
TD (26)
General population
12 (42.9%)
12 (46.2%)
9.5 (0.3)
9.5 (0.3)
Exclusion of children with ADHD according to DSM-IV, any defi-nite signs of neuro-logical disorders or behavioral problems, special needs in education, or an IQ < 85 or > 125. Attentional inhibition (endogenous mode of orienting attention) Endogenous Posner Paradigm Children with DCD responded more slowly than TD children (p < .0001), but there was no significant difference on the error rate. They had more difficulties than TD controls to move their attention when it has been primed to a falsely indicated location. Thus, they exhibited an inhibition deficit in the endogenous mode of orienting attention.
2Tsai et al. (2009) DCD-LEs (36)
TD (36)
General population
19 (52.8%)
19 (52.8%)
9.9 (0.5)
9.8 (0.5)
Exclusion of children with ADHD according to DSM-IV, special educational needs, physical or behavioral problems, or evident neurological damage. Attentional inhibition (endogenous mode of orienting attention)
Attentional inhibition (exogenous mode of orienting attention)
COVAT



Visual Simon task (reaction times)
Children with DCD took longer than TD children to respond to neutral trials, indicating a reduced alertness to the imminent appearance of the target. Results showed a deficit in DCD children associated with only the intentional disengagement of attention (endogenous mode; p < .001) but not the automatic/reflexive dislocation of attention (exogenous mode).
Wang et al. (2015) DCD (23)
TD (23)
General population
12 (52.2%)
12 (52.2%)
9.4 (0.5)
9.3 (0.5)
Exclusion of children with ADHD according to DSM-IV criteria, with any definite signs of neurolo-gical disorders, behavioral problems or special educational needs, or an IQ < 85 or > 125. Attentional inhibition (exogenous mode of orienting attention) Visuospatial attention (eye-gaze cueing) paradigm There were no significant differences between groups on error rates. Children with DCD responded significantly slower than TD children in all conditions (p < .001). They had a poorer general attentional orienting capacity and a difficulty to effectively alter a planned action after their attention had been incorrectly oriented.
Williams et al. (2013) DCD (10)
DCD+ ADHD (16)
ADHD (14)
TD (18)
Schools, hospital or referred by health care professio-nals
6 (60.0%)
14 (87.5%)
8 (57.1%)
10 (55.6%)
8.5 (1.2)
9.1 (1.7)
10.1 (1.4)
10.2 (1.3)
Exclusion of children with ADHD or DCD in other groups than the ones they are supposed to be in, with any physical or neurological condition that could contribute to motor impairment, or with an IQ < 70. Sustained attention (auditive) TEA-Ch: Score! Children in the DCD+ADHD group had significantly more difficulty in the sustained attention task compared to the TD children. Scores in the DCD group were also lower than in the ADHD group, but the differences were not significant. There were no other differences between other groups.
Wilson & Maruff (1999) DCD (20)
TD (20)
General population
10 (50.0%)
10 (50.0%)
10.3
10.6
Exclusion of children with current or history of neurological diseases, including head injury, psychiatric disorders, including ADHD, or an estimated IQ < 80. Attentional inhibition (endogenous and exogenous modes of orienting attention) COVAT Within each group, responses were significantly faster for valid than invalid cues, but the effect was much greater for children with DCD (p < .001). Even when the delay between the cue and the target was longer, these children still had difficulty to shift their attention as efficiently as TD children. Results showed a deficit in the endogenous disengagement of attention in children with DCD, while the exogenous orienting mode was not affected.
Wilson et al. (1997) DCD (20)
TD (20)
General population
18 (90.0%)
18 (90.0%)
9.8
9.7
Exclusion of children with current or past history of neurological diseases, including head injury, psychiatric disorders, including ADHD, or as estimated IQ < 80. Attentional inhibition (endogenous and exogenous modes of orienting attention) COVAT Children with DCD responded slower than TD children (p = .001) and increasing delay between the cue and the stimulus did not help them as it did for TD children. They were able to complete attentional orienting as efficiently as TD children when the delay was brief. Reaction time was also significantly greater in the DCD group for invalid cues, compared with the TD group (p = .001). These results showed a deficit in the disengagement of attention in children with DCD. Thus, they have a deficit in the endogenous mode of orienting attention, but not in the exogenous mode.
Zhu et al. (2012) DCD (39)
TD (39)
Hospital
28 (71.8%)
28 (71.8%)
8.1 (0.5)
8.0 (0.7)
Exclusion of children with any comorbid neurological diseases, mental and neurodevelop-mental disorders, according to detailed physical and psychiatric examinations. General executive functioning Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Children with DCD committed significantly more errors, perseverative responses and perseverative errors than TD children (p < .001). They also needed more trials to complete the first category (p < .001). Results indicated difficulties in executive functioning in children with DCD.

Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; DCD = developmental coordination disorder; TD = typically developing; BVN-5-11 = Development Neurological Assessment Battery; AWMA = Automated Working Memory Assessment; WM = working memory; ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (-I = impulsive; -C = combined); SLI = specific language impairment; ASD = autism spectrum disorder; AS = Asperger syndrome; IQ = intellectual quotient; MLD = mild learning difficulties; CAS = Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System; DD = developmental dyspraxia; AP = dyspraxia following preterm birth; NEPSY = Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; MD = motor difficulties (without DCD); WMTBC = Working Memory Test Battery for Children; D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; VIMI = Verbal Inhibition Motor Inhibition test; CANTAB = Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery; DDL = developmental dyslexia; CPT-II = Continuous Performance Test, Second Edition; WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition; SDCD = severe developmental coordination disorder; MDCD = moderate developmental coordination disorder; COVAT = Covert Orienting of Visuospatial Attention Task; RELD = mixed receptive expressive language disorder; PLA = relatively poor language ability; PMC = relatively poor motor coordination; KITAP = computerized test battery of attention for children; MAND = McCarron Assessment of Neuromuscular Development; BADS-C = Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome in Children; BOT-2-SF = Short Form Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, second edition; DCD-LEs = DCD on lower extremities; TEA-Ch = Test of Everyday Attention for Children.

2.3. Quality assessment of included studies

Quality assessment of studies included in this review was conducted using a checklist we developed based on the Newcastle-Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale (NOS) [51]. The NOS consists of three domains: “selection of subjects”, “comparability of subjects” and “outcome”, each domain including two to three items. Although the use of the NOS has been established in meta-analyses [52], it was first developed for case-control and cohort studies. Therefore, its use was not suitable for the present study. Since we could not find a tool giving standardized criteria for assessing the quality of neuropsychological and behavioral studies, we adapted the NOS based on the methods used by Wu et al. [5355] and Caçola et al. [56], that were inspired by the NOS and the PRISMA standards. We developed three to four quality items for each domain of the NOS (selection, comparability and outcome). The result is a 10-item checklist, including: inclusion/exclusion criteria and samples source (for the selection domain), comparability of samples regarding age, gender and IQ (for the comparability domain), and description of outcome measures, adequacy of outcome analysis and discussion (for the outcome domain; see Appendix 1 for the complete checklist). Items could be answered by “yes” or “no”, and quality level of evidence was rated as high (8 “yes” or more), medium (6–7 “yes”) or low (5 “yes” or less). The quality assessment was carried out by the first two authors independently and any discrepancy was discussed until they reached a decision by consensus.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of included studies

A total of 1088 articles were identified through databases; 708 remained after removing duplicates. Of these, twenty articles (2.8%) had to be discussed between two authors to reach a consensus regarding their inclusion or exclusion, and a third had to settle for five (0.7%) of them. As a result, 41 studies were included in this systematic literature review (see flow diagram in Fig 1), representing 1,097 children and adolescents with DCD (or developmental dyspraxia; DD) with or without comorbid disorders, 16 with dyspraxia following preterm birth, 106 with motor difficulties (MD) without DCD, 51 comparison subjects with neurodevelopmental conditions other than DCD, and 1,213 typically developing (TD) controls, for a total of 2,766 subjects. MD groups were composed of children that had been identified with motor difficulties by scoring below the 16th percentile on the MABC-2 [47, 5759] or had a standardized score under 80 on the McCarron Assessment of Neuromuscular Developmental [60], without having a diagnosis of DCD [61]. Some articles used the same tasks in the same sample of participants with DCD, and therefore, whether it was mentioned in the article [47, 58] or obvious (common authors, close in time, same sample size, characteristics and inclusion/exclusion criteria) [6264], their samples were considered as one and their participants were counted only once in the reported total participants. The age range for all 41 studies combined is 3 to 17 years old and the mean is 9.46 years old. Results and characteristics of studies included in this review are presented in Table 1. The quality ratings ranged between 5 and 10 out of 10. Table 2 provides the details regarding quality assessment of all included studies. Seven studies (17.1%) needed to be discussed between two authors to reach a consensus about their quality level of evidence. It was finally rated as high in 18 studies, medium in 21 studies and low in two studies. The two studies that were rated as low quality were nevertheless included, because they still met the eligibility criteria established by authors. However, they are identified in the sections in which they are discussed in order to nuance their contribution.

Fig 1. PRISMA flow chart illustrating articles identification and selection process.

Fig 1

Table 2. Quality assessment of studies included in the systematic review.

Studies Authors (year) Quality items
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. # of “yes” Quality level
Diagnosis and inclusion criteria mentioned Exclusion criteria mentioned Samples source mentioned Groups come from same community Groups are age matched Groups are gender matched Study controls for IQ Measures of outcome clearly described Adequacy of outcome analysis Implications and limitations discussed
Alesi et al. (2019) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No 8 High
Alloway (2007) Yes No Yes NA NA NA Yes Yes Yes No 8 High
Alloway (2011) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 8 High
Alloway & Archibald (2008) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes No 7 Medium
Alloway et al. (2009) Yes Yes No No No No Yes Yes Yes Yes 6 Medium
Alloway & Temple (2007) Yes No Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 7 Medium
Asonitou & Koutsouki (2016) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes 9 High
Asonitou et al. (2012) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes 8 High
Barray et al. (2008) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 8 High
Bernardi et al. (2017) Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes Yes Yes 7 Medium
Bernardi et al. (2016) Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Biotteau et al. (2017) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes 8 High
Blais et al. (2017) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No No Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Chen et al. (2012) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No No Yes Yes No 6 Medium
de Castelnau et al. (2007) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No No Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Dyck & Piek (2010) Yes Yes No No No No No Yes Yes Yes 5 Low
Gonzalez et al. (2016) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No No Yes Yes Yes 7 Medium
Kaiser & Albaret (2016) Yes Yes Yes No No No No Yes Yes Yes 6 Medium
Kirby et al. (2010) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes 7 Medium
Leonard et al. (2015) Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Mandich et al. (2002) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No No Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Mandich et al. (2003) Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Piek et al. (2007) Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes Yes No 6 Medium
Pratt et al. (2014) Yes Yes Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 7 Medium
Querne et al. (2008) Yes Yes No No Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes 7 Medium
Rahimi-Golkhan-dan et al. (2016) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes 8 High
Ruddock et al. (2016) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes 7 Medium
Ruddock et al. (2015) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes 7 Medium
Sartori et al. (2020) Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No 7 Medium
Sumner et al. (2016) Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes 8 High
Thrornton et al. (2018) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 8 High
Toussaint-Thorin et al. (2013) Yes Yes No No Yes No No Yes No Yes 5 Low
Tsai et al. (2012) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 10 High
Tsai et al. (2010) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 9 High
1Tsai et al. (2009) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 8 High
2Tsai et al. (2009) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 9 High
Wang et al. (2015) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 9 High
Williams et al. (2013) Yes Yes Yes No No No Yes Yes Yes Yes 7 Medium
Wilson & Maruff (1999) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 9 High
Wilson et al. (1997) Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 9 High
Zhu et al. (2012) Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes 8 High

Note: NA = Non-applicable; was counted as “yes” to avoid penalizing studies for criteria that did not apply to them.

No study explicitly assessing organization, self-regulation or goal-setting processes and meeting eligibility criteria established in this review was found. Overall, included studies explored nine dimensions of cognition: three attentional functions (alertness and sustained attention, selective attention, and divided attention) and six executive functions (inhibitory control, working memory, planning, cognitive flexibility, fluency and general executive functioning). A study was considered to discuss general executive functioning when the task they used intentionally assessed multiple executive components, by using an ecological task or a standardized task that did not allow isolating one component of executive functioning. Results are presented in terms of these nine cognitive domains. When several components of attentional and executive functioning were assessed in the same study, the results for each component were reported in their respective section. When it was mentioned in the study, the different modalities (auditory/verbal or visual/nonverbal) used to measure each cognitive function are discussed separately (when available, the modality is identified in Table 1). When it was not possible to identify the modalities, the general cognitive function of interest is discussed. An exhaustive summary of attentional and executive functions assessed and their modalities, tasks used, sample size and age group in which they were respectively assessed, along with studies providing results about each cognitive domain, is provided in Appendix 2.

3.2. Attentional functions

3.2.1. Alertness and sustained attention

Among the seven studies that assessed alertness and sustained attention in the visual modality, four found no significant difference between performances of children with DCD and their TD peers or normative data [6568], while three studies reported that children with DCD had significantly more difficulty than TD children in maintaining their alertness during a long, boring visual task [44, 69, 70]. In the auditory modality, children with DCD did not differ from their TD peers [71].

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders, (for more details about exclusions and comorbid disorders present in the samples, see Table 1), the three studies that compared the performance of children in different clinical groups on tasks of alertness or sustained attention found that children with DCD, DDL or both DCD and DDL did not differ in their sustained attention capacities [65], nor did children with a single diagnosis of either DCD or ADHD [66]. However, the study by Williams et al. [71] revealed that children with a single diagnosis of DCD do not exhibit a deficit in auditory sustained attention compared to TD controls, but that participants with comorbid DCD and ADHD did have greater difficulty than the normative group in this domain. This suggests that overall, DCD in itself does not seem to be associated with deficits in alertness or sustained attention, but rather the co-occurrence of ADHD, which is common in this population, negatively influences these abilities.

3.2.2. Selective attention

Among the four articles that discussed selective attention in the visual modality, two reported impairment on this attentional domain in children with DCD [72, 73]. The two other studies did not find any significant difference between performance of children with DCD or DD and TD controls or normative data [66, 74]. However, it seems that children with dyspraxia following preterm birth had significantly poorer scores on a visual attention task than children with DD, a difference that may be due to sequalae related to prematurity rather than dyspraxia [74]. In addition, both studies that assessed auditive selective attention in children with DCD reported no deficit [46, 74]. Even though the results of both studies are consistent, note that the study by Toussaint-Thorin et al. [46] was the one qualified as low quality and, for this reason, their results should be considered cautiously.

Regarding the management of possible comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders in children composing the DCD groups, the two studies that excluded children with any other medical, neurological or developmental conditions [72, 73] were the only studies to find deficits in selective attention in children with DCD. Thus, these impairments do not appear to be linked to the presence of comorbid conditions. The reasons why studies including children with co-occurring disorders in their DCD sample did not find deficits in this domain may include methodological weaknesses, since the study by Toussaint-Thorin et al. [46] was rated as low quality, but further research is needed to elucidate this point. Finally, Kaiser and Albaret [66] compared children with DCD and children with ADHD on this attentional domain, and they found no significant difference between their performances, suggesting that these clinical groups cannot be distinguished based on their selective attention capacities.

3.2.3. Divided attention

Results showed no impairment on this attentional component in participants with DCD when compared to TD controls, and there was no significant difference between DCD and ADHD groups [66]. The authors only mention excluding children with ADHD from their DCD sample and thus, it is possible that at least some children presented other comorbid neurodevelopmental conditions or learning difficulties. However, if present, it does not seem that these concomitant disorders had a negative influence on the children’s divided attention capacities, since no deficits were found.

3.3. Executive functions

3.3.1. Inhibitory control

Two components of inhibitory control were assessed: the response inhibition component and the attentional inhibition component.

3.3.1.1. Response inhibition. Overall, 17 studies measured nonverbal response inhibition, while verbal response inhibition was assessed in five different samples.

3.3.1.1.1. Nonverbal response inhibition. Difficulties in children with DCD regarding the ability to inhibit a nonverbal prepotent response when compared to their TD peers were found in ten different studies, especially in terms of correct responses [46, 47, 5759, 67, 69, 7578]. Rahimi-Golkhandan et al. [67] specified that the response inhibition deficit in DCD children is only present when stimuli are positively-valenced, and thus more compelling. However, another study using emotionally-valenced tasks found no significant difference between children with DCD and TD controls [79]. Finally, five studies using independent samples found no deficit on this ability in participants with DCD or MD compared to normative data or a control group [44, 61, 65, 70, 80].

Regarding comorbid disorders, Thornton et al. [81] found that only children with co-occurring DCD and ADHD made significantly more errors than their TD peers, while there was no significant difference between performance of children with a single diagnosis of DCD or ADHD and TD controls.

3.3.1.1.2. Verbal response inhibition. In one of the five samples, the difference between DCD and TD groups was significant on the reduced motor-load task (verbal task), while they were not on the high motor-load task (nonverbal task) [80]. However, the authors propose that these results might be better explained by the tasks’ complexity rather than by their modality. Two other samples found a significant impairment as children with DCD made more errors than TD children on tasks requiring a verbal response [59, 79]. In the two other samples, no impairment was found on verbal inhibition measures in terms of accuracy [57, 58], but Bernardi et al. [58] found that participants with DCD were significantly slower than their TD peers on the verbal tasks. Longer completion time on verbal response inhibition tasks has also been found in two other studies [59, 79].

To summarize, children with DCD exhibit response inhibition difficulties in the nonverbal modality, as well as in the verbal modality of response inhibition.

Furthermore, response inhibition difficulties were found in children with DCD, whether they had comorbid conditions or not. In addition, Biotteau et al. [65] compared the results of children with DCD with those of children with DDL and found no significant difference between children with DCD only, DCD with comorbid DDL, or DDL only on these capacities. Hence, impairments that were found in most of the studies included in this subsection do not appear to be due to the presence of comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders.

3.3.1.2. Attentional inhibition. Two different modes of orienting attention were studied: the endogenous and the exogenous mode. The former is defined as a controlled and volitional allocation of attentional resources requiring a cognitive interpretation of stimuli, while the latter refers to an automatic and reflexive allocation of attention that has an alerting utility [82, 83].

All six articles that assessed the endogenous mode of orienting attention reported a deficit of endogenous attentional inhibitory control or “disengagement inhibition” of attention in children with DCD, when compared to TD controls [75, 8286]. Among the seven studies discussing the exogenous mode, four found no deficit [76, 82, 83, 85], while three reported this mode to be impaired in children with DCD [8789]. The lack of consistency between studies could be related to the nature of the precues used in certain tasks (eyes) compared to the ones previously used (arrows), which presumably trigger different brain areas and neural networks, may be more alerting and involve volitional components of orienting attention as well as automatic components [88, 89]. In summary, the endogenous aspects of attentional inhibition seem to be more severely affected in children with DCD than exogenous aspects, which means these children have more difficulty to voluntarily direct and shift their attention toward a stimulus or a task while inhibiting distractors than to automatically shift their attention to peripheral alerting stimuli.

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders in the DCD samples, deficits in attentional inhibition were found in children with pure DCD as well as in subjects with possible co-occurring disorders, suggesting that impairments in this domain seem to be an inherent part of DCD and do not seem to be attributable to other disorders associated with DCD.

3.3.2. Working memory

The visuospatial modality of working memory and its verbal modality were assessed.

3.3.2.1. Visuospatial working memory. All eight samples of children with DCD showed impairments of visuospatial working memory compared to normative data or control groups [26, 47, 57, 59, 63, 68, 79, 90]. Children with MD without DCD did not differ from those with DCD: both groups performed more poorly than TD children [47, 59].

As for the comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders, Alloway [63] and Alloway et al. [64] found no significant difference between children with DCD and children with ADHD, SLI or ASD. In fact, children with ASD performed better than those with DCD, but the difference was not significant once the nonverbal IQ test’s shared motor component was accounted for [64]. Furthermore, a study that compared two groups of children with DCD, one in which children had language or nonverbal reasoning difficulties and one in which their disorder was purer, found no difference on visuospatial working memory capacities between the two groups. However, children with a purer DCD performed significantly worse than children with SLI in this study, even when the contribution of receptive language skills was accounted for [26]. Also, children with DCD were significantly more impaired than children with general learning difficulties on the visuospatial working memory modality [90]. Considering these results, visuospatial working memory seems to be a deficit in children with DCD whether their disorder is pure or not, and it appears to be more specific to children with DCD than to children with mild learning impairments or SLI. However, this deficit is similar in children with DCD, ADHD or ASD.

3.2.2.2. Verbal working memory. Three samples showed no impairment of verbal working memory in children with DCD when compared to TD controls or normative data [47, 57, 65]. Children with MD without DCD did not differ from children with DCD nor TD children [47, 59]. However, eight different samples showed difficulties in verbal working memory in children with DCD according to comparisons with control groups or normative data [20, 26, 59, 61, 63, 79, 90, 91]. Again, children with poor motor coordination without DCD did not differ significantly from children with DCD [61]. However, the study by Dyck and Piek [61] was rated as low quality because of the non-comparability of their groups, and thus these results might be explained by confounding variables. Moreover, two research teams put their results in perspective: Sumner et al. [91] mentioned they could not conclude to a primary deficit in verbal working memory in children with DCD since their results showed a great heterogeneity across all intelligence domains in these children, while Piek et al. [20] explained that their results could be attributable to a slower processing speed in children with DCD, although further analysis would be required to confirm this hypothesis.

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders, among the eight different samples in which verbal working memory difficulties were found, all comorbid conditions were excluded from four samples [59, 61, 79, 91]. Consequently, it seems possible that the impairments found in some studies are, at least in part, influenced by comorbid disorders. Furthermore, among the 11 articles included in this section, nine compared the results of children with DCD on verbal working memory measures with those of children with non-motor neurodevelopmental or learning problems. While the five studies finding at least some difficulties in children with DCD on verbal working memory measures reported no difference between children with DCD and children with ADHD, SLI, ASD, mild learning difficulties, mixed receptive expressive language disorder (RELD) or relatively poor language ability [26, 61, 63, 64, 90], only one found that children with DCD performed significantly poorer than children with ADHD [20]. Among studies that did not report any impairment in verbal working memory, one specified that there was no difference between their two groups of DCD subjects, one in which children had a diagnosis of comorbid DDL and one purer group, nor with a group of children with a single diagnosis of DDL [65]. In summary, impairment of verbal working memory in children with DCD is less clear than the deficit observed in visuospatial working memory, which, however, does not seem to be specific to DCD.

3.3.3 Planning

Two studies compared children’s performance on nonverbal and verbal measures [47, 65], while six considered general planning abilities [46, 7274, 80, 92]. Among the six articles that discussed planning irrespective of the task’s modality, only one reported no deficit on this ability compared to normative data [74], while the other five reported significantly poorer performance in children with DCD in comparison to TD children [46, 72, 73, 80, 92]. One of these five studies was however rated as low quality [46]. Pratt et al. [80] specify that the task’s motor-load did not affect performance on the planning measures, since subjects with DCD were impaired in both tasks, even when the effect of perceptual reasoning was accounted for. Also, the two studies that compared performance on nonverbal and verbal measures reported diverging results: Leonard et al. [47] found no difference between children with DCD and TD controls on both nonverbal and verbal measures, while Bernardi et al. [57] reported that children with DCD, as well as children with MD, performed more poorly than TD controls on the nonverbal measure of planning, but observed no significant difference on the verbal measure. Thus, in all children with motor problems, planning might be impaired on a nonverbal measure, but not on a verbal one.

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders in DCD samples of studies included in this section, among the six articles that reported at least some impairment of planning in children with DCD four used pure samples [57, 72, 73, 80]. Therefore, impairments found do not seem to be attributable to the presence of comorbid disorders, and these do not appear to influence planning abilities in children with DCD.

3.3.4. Cognitive flexibility

Among the three articles that addressed general cognitive flexibility, one found no deficit [46], while the other two reported significantly worse performance in children with DCD than in controls [20, 59]. Furthermore, Sartori et al. [59] found no difference between DCD children and children with MD without DCD. Note, however, that the study reporting no deficit was rated as low quality, and thus more weight should be given to the studies by Piek et al. [20] and Sartori et al. [59]. In regard to the two studies that compared performance on nonverbal and verbal measures, Leonard et al. [47] found no difference between groups of participants with DCD, MD without DCD, and TD controls on both nonverbal and verbal measures, while Bernardi et al. [57] reported that children with DCD performed more poorly than TD controls on the nonverbal measure of cognitive flexibility, without any difference between the MD and the TD groups. Cognitive flexibility was thus impaired specifically in children with DCD, but only on the nonverbal measure. However, it is important to note that both studies used a Trail Making test as a measure of cognitive flexibility, which is a graphomotor task. It is thus possible that the impairment reported by Bernardi et al. [57] is at least partly attributable to the primary motor deficits of children with DCD.

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders among DCD samples, two out of three studies that used a pure sample found impairments in cognitive flexibility [57, 59], specifying that difficulties are only present on a nonverbal measure. One study that included children with comorbid conditions did not find any difficulties [46], while the other found some [20]. Furthermore, one study compared the performance of children with DCD with that of children with ADHD and found that the firsts were more impaired than the seconds on a general cognitive flexibility measure [20]. Since results are heterogeneous, no specific cognitive flexibility profile seems to be associated with DCD, although weaker flexibility in the nonverbal domain might be present. In addition, comorbid disorders do not seem to have a notable influence on this ability in this clinical group.

3.3.5. Fluency

The nonverbal and verbal modalities of fluency were assessed.

3.3.5.1. Nonverbal fluency. All three studies reported difficulties in children with DCD compared to normative data or control groups [47, 57, 74]. Children with MD without DCD were at par with children with DCD, both groups showing poorer performance than TD children [47]. Furthermore, there seems to be no difference between children with DCD and children with dyspraxia following preterm birth on nonverbal fluency, but Barray et al. [74] pointed out that the difficulties observed in these clinical groups on design fluency tasks could be attributable to graphomotor difficulties rather than fluency weaknesses per se [74].

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders among the DCD samples, whether neurodevelopmental comorbidities are excluded [47, 57] or not [74], nonverbal fluency capacities appear to be impaired in children with DCD.

3.3.5.2 Verbal fluency. Two studies found no deficit on verbal fluency in children with DCD or DD compared to control group [46, 47], while the other two found that children with DCD performed more poorly than controls [57, 79]. However, since the study by Toussaint-Thorin et al. [46] was rated as low quality, their results should be considered with caution.

Regarding the presence of comorbid disorders among the children with DCD or DD, exclusion [47, 57, 79] or inclusion [46] of ADHD did not seem to influence the results about verbal fluency capacities.

3.3.6. General executive functioning

Both studies reported that children with DCD had significantly lower performance than a control group composed of healthy individuals [46, 93]. Children with DCD were impaired on most measures of the WCST [93], and they made more errors, exhibited difficulties in respecting the task’s guidelines and were more dependent on the cooking task, suggesting impairments in problem solving abilities [46]. However, since this study is considered of low-quality, more research using ecological tasks is needed to confirm its conclusions.

Regarding the presence of comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders, Zhu et al. [93] excluded participants with any neurodevelopmental or medical conditions, while Toussaint-Thorin et al. [46] included children with ADHD in their DCD sample. Thus, children with DCD seem to exhibit difficulties in tasks integrating multiple components of executive functioning, whether they also have ADHD or not.

3.4. Developmental considerations

The effect of age on cognitive functions has been studied in visual sustained attention, response inhibition, working memory, planning, cognitive flexibility and fluency abilities.

Regarding visual sustained attention, in a cross-sectional study, de Castelnau et al. [70] found that this capacity improved with age in both their sample of children with DCD and their TD group, but the discrepancy between the two groups remained, from 8–9 years old to 12–13 years old. These results suggest that difficulties in sustained attention persist with age in children with DCD.

Four studies considered the effect of years passing on response inhibition capacities. Three studies using a cross-sectional design found that nonverbal response inhibition capacities were better in older participants with DCD than in younger children with the disorder [70, 77, 78], so that these capacities in children with DCD approached those of TD children as they got older [77, 78]. The other study, using a longitudinal design, found no improvement in nonverbal response inhibition in a DCD sample after a two-year follow-up, and the differences originally found between their DCD group and their TD group persisted two years later [57]. These results indicate that the impairment found in nonverbal response inhibition in children might be due to a delay in the development of this cognitive function rather than a primary deficit, but given the heterogeneity of results across studies, more research is necessary to confirm this hypothesis. Regarding verbal inhibition capacities, Bernardi et al. [57] reported no significant change after two years in their DCD sample, although there was no deficit in DCD at both time points.

Additionally, among the functions that they found to be impaired in children with DCD in their longitudinal study, Bernardi et al. [57] reported a significant improvement of visuospatial working memory, nonverbal planning, nonverbal cognitive flexibility and nonverbal and verbal fluency over time in children with DCD. They also mention that improvements in their TD group were similar, so that the deficit found in these domains in children with DCD, in comparison to TD children, persisted after a two-year follow-up [57].

In summary, attentional and executive functions seem to improve with age in children with DCD, as well as in TD controls, but studies found that discrepancies between performance of children with DCD and that of TD children persist over time on most executive tasks.

4. Discussion

4.1. Summary of findings

The 41 articles included in this systematic review, totalizing 1,097 children and adolescents with DCD, repeatedly reported significant impairment of nonverbal response inhibition, attentional inhibition, visuospatial and verbal working memory, planning, nonverbal fluency and general executive functioning in children with DCD. Studies assessing cognitive flexibility are divided on the presence or magnitude of impairments: one study, with a relatively small sample size (n = 17 DCD), found a deficit only on a nonverbal measure, while another study, with 23 subjects with DCD, did not find any significant impairment. Similarly, half of the studies measuring verbal response inhibition found some impairment in this area, although completion time was the only aspect affected in one study. Regarding studies exploring alertness and sustained attention, selective attention, divided attention and verbal fluency, most found no deficit in these areas in children with DCD. No studies investigating organization and self-regulation skills were included in this systematic review as none met the inclusion criteria.

In light of these results, there is no evidence of an obvious attentional deficit in children with DCD, but executive functions are widely impaired. Namely, difficulties are not limited to domains directly associated with core impairments of the disorder (e.g. nonverbal planning), but extend to other areas of executive processes, including nonverbal inhibitory control, nonverbal and verbal working memory, and general planning abilities, as well as on ecological tasks integrating multiple components of executive functions. In summary, executive functions are more impaired on a nonverbal/visuospatial modality than on a verbal modality, and results suggest that a relatively broad executive deficit is present in children with DCD.

A few studies also explored the possible differences between a group of children with DCD and a group of children with MD without DCD. As previously described, these groups were composed of children whose motor difficulties have been objectified, but who did not meet formal diagnostic criteria for DCD. It is interesting to note that in most studies, no significant difference was noted between MD and DCD groups in terms of executive impairments. This raises the possibility that executive difficulties are tightly linked with motor impairments, irrespective of severity. Consequently, the findings and conclusions about DCD highlighted in this review could also apply to a large portion of children with measurable motor difficulties that do not have a clinical diagnosis of DCD.

4.2. Influence of comorbid disorders

Among the cognitive functions that were noted as being impaired in most studies, the presence of comorbid disorders did not seem to have a determinant influence on inhibitory control (both response and attentional inhibition), visuospatial working memory, planning, cognitive flexibility, nonverbal fluency and general executive functioning capacities. Thus, the weaknesses described in these domains do not appear to be attributable to co-occurring disorders, but rather suggest that cognitive impairments are integrant elements of DCD. This is unexpected since DCD is primarily a motor disorder, and the definition does not include executive functioning problems [13]. However, not all studies reviewed here addressed the issue of comorbidities in the same way: some studies based their exclusion criteria only on comorbid disorders that had been diagnosed in children, while others measured and confirmed their presence in children composing their sample. In most studies in which response inhibition, visuospatial working memory, planning, cognitive flexibility, nonverbal fluency and general executive functioning were assessed, children with co-occurring disorders were excluded based on the presence of previously diagnosed disorders, without these diagnoses being confirmed by the research teams. However, the presence of children’s difficulties linked to comorbid disorders was confirmed by authors in most studies investigating attentional inhibition. Therefore, it is particularly clear that deficits in attentional inhibition are part of DCD. However, since most studies did not confirm or refute the presence of co-occurring disorders, it is possible that some children in DCD samples nonetheless suffered from additional neurodevelopmental disorders that were not documented. Considering this, additional studies using large sample size and thorough screening methods to assess the presence of comorbid disorders are needed to conclude on the definite influence of comorbid conditions on the cognitive profile of children with DCD.

Despite the imprecision of screening and sometimes inclusions and exclusion criteria in some studies, it seems likely that comorbid conditions exert at least some influence on verbal working memory capacities. Indeed, among the eight samples in which verbal working memory difficulties were found, all comorbid disorders were excluded from four samples [59, 61, 79, 91], and two other samples were free of ADHD, behavioral problems or ASD [26, 63]. It is thus unlikely that the impairments reported in these studies are due to these co-occurring neurodevelopmental disorders, but it does not exclude the possible influence of language or learning difficulties in half of the studies [20, 26, 63, 90]. Interestingly, among the three samples in which verbal working memory abilities were preserved, two were pure DCD samples and one was free of comorbid ADHD or SLI. In summary, when at least the co-occurrence of SLI is excluded, impairments in verbal working memory are less probable in children with DCD. Therefore, deficits in this area might not be part of the DCD profile per se, but may appear when co-occurring difficulties, especially in terms of language, are present.

It appears that difficulties observed on measures of visuospatial and verbal working memory are frequent but not specific to children with DCD, since they are not significantly different than those found in children with neurodevelopmental disorders that do not necessarily involve a motoric component, including ADHD, SLI, RELD, ASD, and learning difficulties. For this reason, it is possible that impairments in these domains could reflect a general sign of an atypical cognitive development, without being specific to a given disorder, nor systematically present in children with neurodevelopmental conditions.

4.3. Brain correlates

Studies on the neural correlates of DCD contribute to our understanding of the results included in the present review. Several neuroimaging studies have revealed implications of corpus callosum, basal ganglia, inferior parietal cortex, thalamus and cerebellum in DCD, and of connections between these structures [44, 65, 94, 95]. Abnormalities have also been noted in frontal cortex functioning and in white matter maturation and composition in individuals with DCD [44, 9597]. Furthermore, deficits in functional activation during a motor response inhibition task have been found in the right pre- and postcentral gyri, as well as in the left superior and middle frontal gyri [81]. However, the authors specified that these deficits were only present in children with co-occurring DCD and ADHD. Given the plurality of brain areas involved in DCD, the hypothesis of an “Atypical Brain Development” (ABD) has been proposed. This view stipulates that brain abnormalities underlying the cognitive and motor deficits observed in various neurodevelopmental disorders are rather diffuse than localized [19, 98]. ABD is not a disorder per se, but rather a suboptimal developmental process that manifests itself in different forms, such as motor, attentional and/or reading disorders [19, 99]. Given the strong overlap between symptoms of DCD and ADHD [19], the umbrella term “Deficit in Attention, Motor control and Perception” (DAMP) was formerly proposed to define a combination of DCD and ADHD [6, 100]. The authors insisted on the importance of a term acknowledging both attentional and motor control problems in clinical practice, stipulating that these symptoms have strong common background factors and that the prognosis of children with DAMP was poorer than that of children with either DCD or ADHD [100].

Several authors have also hypothesized that children with DCD could have an automatization deficit, related to a cerebellar dysfunction [19]. Although possibly greater and more obvious in children with DCD than in children with ADHD and/or DDL, this deficit does not appear to be specific to the DCD population, but is on the contrary common to most developmental disorders [19, 101]. It could therefore explain the high prevalence of comorbidities among neurodevelopmental disorders and the non-specificity of the cognitive impairments found amongst them [98, 102].

The fact that the frontal cortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum and parietal cortex are especially implicated in attentional and executive functioning [4, 38, 41], and that these brain structures do not operate optimally in individuals with DCD, as previously described, might explain some of the attentional and executive impairments compiled in the present review. Additionally, an atypical hemispheric lateralization for attention and inhibitory functions has been shown in children with DCD, along with a reduced efficiency of cerebral network involved in inhibitory control [44]. Evoked potential studies have also reported that children with DCD allow fewer resources than TD controls for spatial locations comparison and response retrieval and selection [68], and that they may have less mature anticipatory and executive processes, reduced interhemispheric and cognitive-to-motor transfer speeds as well as an atypical neural activity associated with attentional control [85, 88, 89]. Without providing correlational or causal evidence, the set of cerebral abnormalities observed in DCD, which involves key structures robustly involved in the corticostriatal function and the frontal cortex, the presence of impairments in the executive and attentional domain in DCD is not unexpected.

5. Conclusion

This systematic review of the literature revealed that children and adolescents with DCD show impairments mostly on tasks of inhibitory control, working memory, planning, nonverbal fluency and general executive functioning. Alertness and sustained attention, selective and divided attention and verbal fluency capacities appear more intact, whereas results regarding cognitive flexibility are divided. More evidence supports the presence of a deficit in nonverbal executive tasks, without the impairments being exclusive to this modality. Co-occurring disorders might influence impairments found on verbal working memory capacities in children with DCD.

These results contribute to a better understanding of the cognitive profile associated with DCD and have several implications. On a conceptual level, we must consider that executive impairments are common in DCD and, although they may be partly explainable by the underlying visuospatial and motor deficits, they are not entirely so, especially in areas of working memory, planning abilities and general executive functioning. When evaluating executive functioning in children with DCD, clinicians should still be aware of the possible contribution of visuospatial and motor deficits in their results, and impairments on executive tasks that do not require visuospatial or motor skills should be found before concluding to an executive deficit in a child with DCD. Since a few studies have identified similar difficulties in children with DCD to children with less severe motor difficulties that did not have a diagnosis of DCD, clinicians must keep in mind that children with motor difficulties that have not been diagnosed with DCD may present the same set of executive difficulties. Furthermore, when clinicians do conclude to executive deficits in a child with DCD, they must keep in mind that attentional and executive impairments may be part of the disorder itself. While it is important that children benefit from appropriate interventions to help them with their difficulties, professionals should also be parsimonious in diagnosing concomitant disorders. Indeed, it appears possible that the high prevalence of comorbid disorders found in children with DCD is due to cognitive dysfunction related to DCD itself, without necessarily being specific to this disorder, rather than to an additional disorder. In all cases, professionals intervening with children and adolescents with DCD should expect them to be impulsive and to be easily distracted by task-irrelevant stimuli. They should also adapt their interventions to try to avoid overloading their working memory and support them in developing their planning skills.

5.1. Strengths and limitations of the current review

This systematic review of the literature has several strengths. On a methodological level, the facts that we used a broad variety of research terms for executive functions and that we included studies using different types of performance tasks, from experimental tasks to standardized neuropsychological tests, allow our review to be exhaustive, inclusive, and thus more representative of the attentional and executive functions in children and adolescents with DCD. The inclusion of studies using more ecological tasks is also a strength since our results contribute to a better understanding and representation of these children’s daily difficulties. In addition, this review is the first to explicitly discuss the influence of concomitant disorders on attentional and executive processes in DCD. The fact that we included studies that used DCD samples with comorbid disorders, as well as the fact that the included studies recruited their DCD sample from various sources, increases the likelihood that the conclusions drawn are ecologically valid and generalizable to the population of children with DCD.

Regarding methodological limitations, the lack of search and inclusion of grey literature and nonpublished studies may bring a publication bias. Furthermore, a standardized quality assessment tool for neuropsychological studies could not be found, so we developed our own quality assessment checklist based on the NOS and the PRISMA standards. By this mean, this review raised the risk of including studies that provide inadequate quality of evidence. Although we explicitly mentioned in the relevant sections when the quality of a study was low, a common and important limitation of several studies was the small sample size, which entails that even when grouped together, the number of participants remained relatively limited. This is especially relevant for results regarding auditive alertness/sustained attention, divided attention, comparisons between verbal and nonverbal planning and between verbal and nonverbal cognitive flexibility, and verbal fluency. Similarly, some of the attentional and executive components discussed in this review have only been the topic of a small number of studies, particularly selective attention, divided attention, cognitive flexibility, fluency and general executive functioning. Thereby, we must be cautious when considering the conclusions drawn regarding these domains, and further studies are necessary to confirm the interpretations made of these findings. Another important limitation of studies included in this review is the inconsistency regarding the clarity of exclusion criteria of several DCD samples. As the presence of comorbid disorders was not always rigorously documented nor confirmed, it is possible that comorbid disorders may play a larger role in the results highlighted in this review than we believe.

Other factors are to be considered when interpreting the conclusions of this systematic review. Firstly, we presented our results according to nine attentional and executive components to reduce the heterogeneity of the construct evaluated in each section. However, on a conceptual level, attentional and executive functions, although dissociable, are also united and interrelated [33, 36, 41, 103]. Similarly, all tasks require the implication of more than a single cognitive function, and in certain cases, it might be difficult to isolate one attentional or executive component. As a matter of fact, some studies using the same assessment tools reported their results in relation to different cognitive domains. Hence, the results described might be partially due to impairment in other cognitive areas than those purportedly assessed. Also, several tasks used to measure cognitive functions require a motor component. Children with DCD could perform more poorly on these tasks not because of an attentional or executive impairment, but because of their primary motor deficit associated with their disorder. Secondly, no study explicitly assessing organization, self-regulation or goal-setting processes were included since none met the eligibility criteria established in this review and because few tasks assessing these components of executive functions exist. Therefore, this systematic review does not allow discussing the integrity of these executive functioning components. Thirdly, even though eligibility criteria allowed including studies with subjects up to 17 years old, the most often studied ages were 8 to 12 years old. Given the considerable development of executive functions through childhood and adolescence [36, 38, 104], it would be incorrect to generalize the results of this review to younger children and older adolescents. Fourthly, it was previously mentioned that impairments found in the domains of visuospatial and verbal working memory were not specific to children with DCD, since children with ADHD or ASD shared these difficulties. Yet children with ADHD and ASD also often share sensorimotor deficits with children with DCD [23, 24, 105]. Given the overlap between symptoms that seems to be present, there is a limitation to the comparisons that can be made between these clinical groups. Finally, as in most neurodevelopmental disorders, there is great heterogeneity among children with DCD [19, 26, 65, 91]. The lack of details regarding the severity and predominant type of impairments could contribute to the variability of results noted in certain cognitive areas.

5.2. Directions for future research

Additional studies using larger samples and replicating the results of these studies are needed, especially in domains of alertness/sustained attention, divided attention, verbal and nonverbal planning, verbal and nonverbal cognitive flexibility, and verbal fluency. Likewise, a small number of studies have explored selective attention, divided attention, cognitive flexibility, fluency and general executive functioning in children in DCD, and more research on these cognitive functions is also needed to confirm the results highlighted in this review. Since no studies discussing organization, self-regulation and goal-setting skills and meeting our eligibility criteria were found, more rigorous studies exploring these executive functions are needed. Future studies should also focus on children under 8 years old and over 12 years old. Considering the development of attentional and executive functions during childhood and adolescence, and the variability of developmental trajectories among the different components of executive functioning [104], cross-sectional and longitudinal studies comparing different age groups and following children through several years would be important in order to determine whether children with DCD present a lasting executive deficit or whether their difficulties are due to a developmental delay that catches up with time. In addition, to improve results generalization and comparison between studies, future research should avoid selecting their sample only in specialized clinical settings in order to obtain samples that are more representative of the population of children with DCD. Furthermore, given the fact that co-occurring disorders may have an influence on some executive functions, especially on verbal working memory, future studies should document and verify the presence of comorbid conditions in their sample. More studies should also compare attentional and executive functioning profiles of children with DCD without any other developmental or neurological problem, with that of children with other neurodevelopmental disorders and of children with multiple diagnoses to explore the possible distinction between these groups. Lastly, future research should allow better understanding of the overlap between executive functions and motor skills. To do so, the choice of tasks should be judicious and make possible a better comparison of children’s performance on tasks requiring motor and visuospatial skills with their performance on tasks requiring no such abilities. In addition, given the overlap between DCD, ADHD and ASD previously discussed, a dimensional approach could be useful to go beyond group comparisons and allow a better understanding of co-occurring motor and cognitive difficulties. With more studies using larger samples, a meta-analytic approach would be relevant and could control for such confounding variables.

Supporting information

S1 Data. Minimal data set.

(DOCX)

S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

(DOC)

S1 Appendix. Quality assessment: Adaptation of the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale cohort studies.

(DOCX)

S2 Appendix. Summary of attentional and executive functions assessed, age ranges, sample sizes and tasks used among studies.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Missiuna C., Gaines R., Mclean J., DeLaat D., Egan M., & Soucie H. (2008). Description of children identified by physicians as having developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol, 50(11), 839–844. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03140.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zwicker J. G., Missiuna C., Harris S. R., & Boyd L. A. (2012). Developmental coordination disorder: a review and update. Eur J Paediatr Neurol, 16(6), 573–581. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2012.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Miller L. T., Missiuna C. A., Macnab J. J., Malloy-Miller T., & Polatajko H. J. (2001). Clinical description of children with developmental coordination disorder. Can J Occup Ther, 68(1), 5–15. doi: 10.1177/000841740106800101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kirby A. (2011). Developmental co-ordination disorder and emerging adulthood: Not just a motor disorder. J Adult Dev, 18(3), 105–106. doi: 10.1007/s10804-011-9124-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kadesjö B., & Gillberg C. (1999). Developmental coordination disorder in Swedish 7-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 38(7), 820–828. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199907000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lingam R., Hunt L., Golding J., Jongmans M., & Emond A. (2009). Prevalence of developmental coordination disorder using the DSM-IV at 7 years of age: A UK population–based study. Pediatrics, 123(4), e693–e700. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tsiotra G. D., Flouris A. D., Koutedakis Y., Faught B. E., Nevill A. M., Lane A. M., et al. (2006). A comparison of developmental coordination disorder prevalence rates in Canadian and Greek children. J Adolesc Health, 39(1), 125–127. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kirby A., Sugden D., & Purcell C. (2014). Diagnosing developmental coordination disorders. Arch Dis Child, 99(3), 292–296. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-303569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gibbs J., Appleton J., & Appleton R. (2007). Dyspraxia or developmental coordination disorder? Unravelling the enigma. Arch Dis Child, 92(6), 534–539. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.088054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lussier F., Chevrier E., & Gascon L. (2017). Neuropsychologie de l’enfant et de l’adolescent. (3e ed.). Paris: Dunod. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dewey D. (1995). What Is Developmental Dyspraxia. Brain and Cognition, 29(3), 254–274. 10.1006/brcg.1995.1281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Vaivre-Douret L., Lalanne C., Ingster-Moati I., Boddaert N., Cabrol D., Dufier J. L., et al. (2011). Subtypes of developmental coordination disorder: research on their nature and etiology. Dev Neuropsychol, 36(5), 614–643. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.560696 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Vaivre-Douret L. (2007). Troubles d’apprentissage non verbal: les dyspraxies développementales. Archives de Pédiatrie, 14(11), 1341–1349. 10.1016/j.arcped.2007.06.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chaix Y. & Albaret J. (2013). Trouble de l’Acquisition de la Coordination et déficits visuo-spatiaux. Développements, 15(2), 32–43. doi: 10.3917/devel.015.0032 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Polatajko H. J., & Cantin N. (2005). Developmental coordination disorder (dyspraxia): an overview of the state of the art. Semin Pediatr Neurol, 12(4), 250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2005.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.King-Dowling S., Missiuna C., Rodriguez M. C., Greenway M., & Cairney J. (2015). Co-occurring motor, language and emotional-behavioral problems in children 3–6 years of age. Hum Mov Sci, 39, 101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Martin N. C., Piek J., Baynam G., Levy F., & Hay D. (2010). An examination of the relationship between movement problems and four common developmental disorders. Hum Mov Sci, 29(5), 799–808. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Visser J. (2003). Developmental coordination disorder: A review of research on subtypes and comorbidities. Hum Mov Sci, 22(4–5), 479–493. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Piek J. P., Dyck M. J., Francis M., & Conwell A. (2007). Working memory, processing speed, and set-shifting in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol, 49(9), 678–683. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00678.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tal Saban M., Ornoy A., & Parush S. (2014). Executive function and attention in young adults with and without developmental coordination disorder—a comparative study. Res Dev Disabil, 35(11), 2644–2650. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pitcher T. M., Piek J. P., & Hay D. A. (2003). Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol, 45(8), 525–535. doi: 10.1017/s0012162203000975 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goulardins J. B., Rigoli D., Licari M., Piek J. P., Hasue R. H., Oosterlaan J., et al. (2015). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder: Two separate disorders or do they share a common etiology. Behav Brain Res, 292, 484–492. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Iversen S., Berg K., Ellertsen B., & Tønnessen F. E. (2005). Motor coordination difficulties in a municipality group and in a clinical sample of poor readers. Dyslexia, 11(3), 217–231. doi: 10.1002/dys.297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jongmans M. J., Smits-Engelsman B. C. M., & Schoemaker M. M. (2003). Consequences of Comorbidity of Developmental Coordination Disorders and Learning Disabilities for Severity and Pattern of Perceptual-Motor Dysfunction. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(6), 528–537. doi: 10.1177/00222194030360060401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Alloway T. P., & Archibald L. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. J Learn Disabil, 41(3), 251–262. doi: 10.1177/0022219408315815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Flapper B. C., & Schoemaker M. M. (2013). Developmental coordination disorder in children with specific language impairment: co-morbidity and impact on quality of life. Res Dev Disabil, 34(2), 756–763. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dewey D., Kaplan B. J., Crawford S. G., & Wilson B. N. (2002). Developmental coordination disorder: Associated problems in attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Hum Mov Sci, 21(5–6), 905–918. doi: 10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00163-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Green D., Baird G., Barnett A. L., Henderson L., Huber J., & Henderson S. E. (2002). The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger’s syndrome: a comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 43(5), 655–668. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Castellanos F. X., Sonuga-Barke E. J., Milham M. P., & Tannock R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci, 10(3), 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Henry L. A., Messer D. J., & Nash G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hill E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogn Sci, 8(1), 26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A., & Wager T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn Psychol, 41(1), 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Anderson P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol, 8(2), 71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gioia G. A., Isquith P. K., Guy S. C., & Kenworthy L. (2000). BRIEF–Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Diamond A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol, 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pennington B. F., & Ozonoff S. (1996). Executive Functions and Developmental Psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jurado M. B., & Rosselli M. (2007). The Elusive Nature of Executive Functions: A Review of our Current Understanding. Neuropsychol Rev, 17(3), 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Posner M. I., & Boies S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychol Rev, 78(5), 391–408. doi: 10.1037/h0031333 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Posner M. I., & Petersen S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci, 13(1), 25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Knudsen E. I. (2007). Fundamental Components of Attention. Annu Rev Neurosci, 30(1), 57–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hofmann W., Schmeichel B. J., & Baddeley A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci, 16(3), 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Piek J. P., Dyck M. J., Nieman A., Anderson M., Hay D., Smith L. M., et al. (2004). The relationship between motor coordination, executive functioning and attention in school aged children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol, 19(8), 1063–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Querne L., Berquin P., Vernier-Hauvette M. P., Fall S., Deltour L., Meyer M. E., et al. (2008). Dysfunction of the attentional brain network in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: a fMRI study. Brain Res, 1244, 89–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wilson P. H., Ruddock S., Smits-Engelsman B., Polatajko H., & Blank R. (2013). Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research. Dev Med Child Neurol, 55(3), 217–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04436.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Toussaint-Thorin M., Marchal F., Benkhaled O., Pradat-Diehl P., Boyer F. C., & Chevignard M. (2013). Executive functions of children with developmental dyspraxia: assessment combining neuropsychological and ecological tests. Ann Phys Rehabil Med, 56(4), 268–287. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2013.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Leonard H. C., Bernardi M., Hill E. L., & Henry L. A. (2015). Executive functioning, motor difficulties, and developmental coordination disorder. Dev Neuropsychol, 40(4), 201–215. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.997933 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D. G., & Prisma Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med, 6(7), e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Brown T., & Lalor A. (2009). The Movement Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition (MABC-2): a review and critique. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr, 29(1), 86–103. doi: 10.1080/01942630802574908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Strauss E., Sherman E. M., & Spreen O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wells G. A., Shea B., O’Connell D., Peterson J., Welch V., Losos M., et al. (2014). Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale; Cohort studies. Available from http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed April 15, 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Stang A. (2010). Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol, 25(9), 603–605. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wu M., Brockmeyer T., Hartmann M., Skunde M., Herzog W., & Friederich H. C. (2016). Reward-related decision making in eating and weight disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence from neuropsychological studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 61, 177–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wu M., Hartmann M., Skunde M., Herzog W., & Friederich H. C. (2013). Inhibitory control in bulimic-type eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS One, 8(12), e83412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wu M., Brockmeyer T., Hartmann M., Skunde M., Herzog W., & Friederich H. C. (2014). Set-shifting ability across the spectrum of eating disorders and in overweight and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med, 44(16), 3365–3385. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Caçola P., Miller H. L., & Williamson P. O. (2017). Behavioral comparisons in autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordination disorder: a systematic literature review. Res Autism Spectr Disord, 38, 6–18. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bernardi M., Leonard H. C., Hill E. L., Botting N., & Henry L. A. (2017). Executive functions in children with developmental coordination disorder: A 2-year follow-up study. Dev Med Child Neurol, No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13640 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bernardi M., Leonard H. C., Hill E. L., & Henry L. A. (2016). Brief report: Response inhibition and processing speed in children with motor difficulties and developmental coordination disorder. Child Neuropsychol, 22(5), 627–634. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1014898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sartori R.F., Valentini S.C., & Fonseca R.P. (2020). Executive function in children with and without developmental coordination disorder: A comparative study. Child Care Health Dev, 46, 294–302. doi: 10.1111/cch.12734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.McCarron L. T. (1997). McCarron Assessment of NeuroMuscular Development: Fine and Gross Motor Abilities (Rev. Ed.). Dallas, TX: Common Market Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dyck M., & Piek J. (2010). How to distinguish normal from disordered children with poor language or motor skills. Int J Lang Commun Disord, 45(3), 336–344. doi: 10.3109/13682820903009503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Alloway T. P. (2007). Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. J Exp Child Psychol, 96(1), 20–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Alloway T. P. (2011). A comparison of working memory profiles in children with ADHD and DCD. Child Neuropsychol, 17(5), 483–494. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.553590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Alloway T. P., Rajendran G., & Archibald L. M. (2009). Working memory in children with developmental disorders. J Learn Disabil, 42(4), 372–382. doi: 10.1177/0022219409335214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Biotteau M., Albaret J.-M., Lelong S., & Chaix Y. (2017). Neuropsychological status of French children with developmental dyslexia and/or developmental coordination disorder: Are both necessarily worse than one? Child Neuropsychol, 23(4), 422–441. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1127339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kaiser M. L., & Albaret J. M. (2016). Troubles moteurs chez les enfants présentant un trouble du déficit de l’attention et/ou hyperactivité (TDAH). [Motor disorders in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADD/ADHD).]. A.N.A.E., 28(1[140]), 79–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rahimi-Golkhandan S., Steenbergen B., Piek J. P., Caeyenberghs K., & Wilson P. H. (2016). Revealing hot executive function in children with motor coordination problems: What’s the go? Brain Cogn, 106, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tsai C.-L., Chang Y.-K., Hung T.-M., Tseng Y.-T., & Chen T.-C. (2012). The neurophysiological performance of visuospatial working memory in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol, 54(12), 1114–1120. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04408.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Blais M., Baly C., Biotteau M., Albaret J.-M., Chaix Y., & Tallet J. (2017). Lack of motor inhibition as a marker of learning difficulties of bimanual coordination in teenagers with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Neuropsychol, 42(3), 207–219. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2017.1306526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.de Castelnau P., Albaret J. M., Chaix Y., & Zanone P. G. (2007). Developmental coordination disorder pertains to a deficit in perceptuo-motor synchronization independent of attentional capacities. Hum Mov Sci, 26(3), 477–490. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Williams J., Omizzolo C., Galea M. P., & Vance A. (2013). Motor imagery skills of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci, 32(1), 121–135. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2012.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Asonitou K., & Koutsouki D. (2016). Cognitive process-based subtypes of developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Hum Mov Sci, 47, 121–134. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Asonitou K., Koutsouki D., Kourtessis T., & Charitou S. (2012). Motor and cognitive performance differences between children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Res Dev Disabil, 33(4), 996–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Barray V., Picard A., & Camos V. (2008). Comparative study of dyspraxia: neuropsychological evaluation of children with developmental dyspraxia or consecutive to a preterm birth. Ann de Readapt Med Phys, 51(3), 161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.annrmp.2008.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Mandich A., Buckolz E., & Polatajko H. (2003). Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and their ability to disengage ongoing attentional focus: More on inhibitory function. Brain Cogn, 51(3), 346–356. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00039-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Mandich A., Buckolz E., & Polatajko H. (2002). On the ability of children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) to inhibit response initiation: The Simon effect. Brain Cogn, 50(1), 150–162. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(02)00020-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Ruddock S., Caeyenberghs K., Piek J., Sugden D., Hyde C., Morris S., et al. (2016). Coupling of online control and inhibitory systems in children with atypical motor development: A growth curve modelling study. Brain Cogn, 109, 84–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Ruddock S., Piek J., Sugden D., Morris S., Hyde C., Caeyenberghs K., et al. (2015). Coupling online control and inhibitory systems in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: Goal-directed reaching. Res Dev Disabil, 36, 244–255. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Alesi M., Pecoraro D., & Pepi A. (2019). Executive functions in kindergarten children at risk for developmental coordination. Eur J Spec Needs Educ, 34(3), 285–296. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2018.1468635 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Pratt M. L., Leonard H. C., Adeyinka H., & Hill E. L. (2014). The effect of motor load on planning and inhibition in developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil, 35(7), 1579–1587. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Thornton S., Bray S., Langevin L.M., & Dewey D. (2018). Functional brain correlates of motor response inhibition in children with developmental coordination disorderand attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Human Movement Science, 59, 134–142. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Wilson P. H., & Maruff P. (1999). Deficits in the endogenous control of covert visuospatial attention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci, 18(2–3), 421–442. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(99)00017-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Wilson P. H., Maruff P., & McKenzie B. E. (1997). Covert orienting of visuospatial attention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol, 39(11), 736–745. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07375.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Tsai C. L., Pan C. Y., Cherng R. J., Hsu Y. W., & Chiu H. H. (2009). Mechanisms of deficit of visuospatial attention shift in children with developmental coordination disorder: a neurophysiological measure of the endogenous Posner paradigm. Brain Cogn, 71(3), 246–258. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Tsai C. L., Yu Y. K., Chen Y. J., & Wu S. K. (2009). Inhibitory response capacities of bilateral lower and upper extremities in children with developmental coordination disorder in endogenous and exogenous orienting modes. Brain Cogn, 69(2), 236–244. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Chen W. Y., Wilson P. H., & Wu S. K. (2012). Deficits in the covert orienting of attention in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: does severity of DCD count? Res Dev Disabil, 33(5), 1516–1522. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Gonzalez C. C., Mon-Williams M., Burke S., & Burke M. R. (2016). Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements in children with developmental coordination disorder. PLoS One, 11(11). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Tsai C. L., Pan C. Y., Chang Y. K., Wang C. H., & Tseng K. D. (2010). Deficits of visuospatial attention with reflexive orienting induced by eye-gazed cues in children with developmental coordination disorder in the lower extremities: an event-related potential study. Res Dev Disabil, 31(3), 642–655. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Wang C. H., Lo Y. H., Pan C. Y., Chen F. C., Liang W. K., & Tsai C. L. (2015). Frontal midline theta as a neurophysiological correlate for deficits of attentional orienting in children with developmental coordination disorder. Psychophysiology, 52(6), 801–812. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Alloway T. P., & Temple K. J. (2007). A comparison of working memory skills and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Appl Cogn Psychol, 21(4), 473–487. doi: 10.1002/acp.1284 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Sumner E., Pratt M. L., & Hill E. L. (2016). Examining the cognitive profile of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Res Dev Disabil, 56, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.05.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Kirby A., Sugden D., & Edwards L. (2010). Developmental co-ordination disorder (DCD): More than just a movement difficulty. J Res Spec Educ Needs, 10(3), 206–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-3802.2010.01158.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Zhu S., Tang G., & Shi J. (2012). Cross-sectional study of executive functioning in children with developmental coordination disorders. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry, 24(4), 217–221. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2012.04.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Lundy-Ekman L., Ivry R., Keele S., & Woollacott M. (1991). Timing and Force Control Deficits in Clumsy Children. J Cogn Neurosci, 3(4), 367–376. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Zwicker J. G., Missiuna C., & Boyd L. A. (2009). Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: A review of hypotheses. J Child Neurol, 24(10), 1273–1281. doi: 10.1177/0883073809333537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.de Castelnau P., Albaret J.-M., Chaix Y., & Zanone P.-G. (2008). A study of EEG coherence in DCD children during motor synchronization task. Human Movement Science, 27(2), 230–241. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2008.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Peters L. H. J., Maathuis C. G. B., & Hadders-Algra M. (2013). Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol, 55, 59–64. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Gilger J. W., & Kaplan B. J. (2001). Atypical brain development: a conceptual framework for understanding developmental learning disabilities. Dev Neuropsychol, 20(2), 465–481. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2002_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Kaplan B. J., Wilson B. N., Dewey D., & Crawford S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Hum Mov Sci, 17(4–5), 471–490. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00010-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gillberg C., & Kadesjö B. (2003). Why bother about clumsiness? The implications of having developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Neural Plast, 10(1–2), 59–68. doi: 10.1155/NP.2003.59 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Puyjarinet F. (2018). Trouble du déficit de l’attention/hyperactivité avec ou sans troubles associés: Mise en évidence de différences attentionnelles et exécutives. Neuropsychi Enf Adolesc, 66(1), 13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neurenf.2017.06.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Nicolson R. I., & Fawcett A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47(1), 117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Lehto J. E., Juujärvi P., Kooistra L., & Pulkkinen L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br J Dev Psychol, 21(1), 59–80. doi: 10.1348/026151003321164627 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Best J. R., & Miller P. H. (2010). A Developmental Perspective on Executive Function. Child Dev, 81(6), 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Wisdom S. N., Dyck M. J., Piek J. P., Hay D., & Hallmayer J. (2007). Can autism, language and coordination disorders be differentiated based on ability profiles? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 16(3), 178–186. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0586-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Celestino Rodríguez

10 Feb 2021

PONE-D-20-33814

Attentional and Executive Functions in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Coordination Disorder and the Influence of Comorbid Disorders: A Systematic Review of the Literature

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lippé,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

In view of the criticism of the reviewers found at the bottom of the page, your manuscript has been denied publication.  I too have read your manuscript and agree with the reviewers’ observations that is necessary to improve:

The research question should be added clearly in the introduction. Also, the objective that appears in the summary does not match (or is misleading) with what appears as a research question in the objectives. It would be interesting to improve this aspect.

It is recommended to relate the content of the discussion with what was previously stated in the introduction and avoid explaining future prospects in this section.

In this sense, it is desirable that the conclusions section appears before Strenghts and Limitations and Directions for Future Research.

Please ensure that your decision is justified on PLOS ONE’s publication criteria and not, for example, on novelty or perceived impact.

For Lab, Study and Registered Report Protocols: These article types are not expected to include results but may include pilot data. 

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 27 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Celestino Rodríguez, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1) Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2) Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

[JFL is supported by a Junior I Research Scholar Award from the Fonds de la Recherche

du Québec – Santé. MLP is supported by a scholarship from the Faculté de Médecine

et des Sciences de la Santé of Université de Sherbrooke]

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

 [The author(s) received no specific funding for this work]

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3) PLOS requires an ORCID iD for the corresponding author in Editorial Manager on papers submitted after December 6th, 2016. Please ensure that you have an ORCID iD and that it is validated in Editorial Manager. To do this, go to ‘Update my Information’ (in the upper left-hand corner of the main menu), and click on the Fetch/Validate link next to the ORCID field. This will take you to the ORCID site and allow you to create a new iD or authenticate a pre-existing iD in Editorial Manager. Please see the following video for instructions on linking an ORCID iD to your Editorial Manager account: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xcclfuvtxQ

4) Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Title: Attentional and Executive Functions in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Coordination Disorder and the Influence of Comorbid Disorders: A Systematic Review of the Literature.

The research justification and aim are clear and relevant; the article is good and useful to improve the future intervention. The article seems solid and the language helps keep track. No important or relevant references appear in the abstract, it has to improve.

- It must be verified that the set of Citations used in the text are correctly cited in the References section (and vice versa) and that, in each case, the DOI or URL corresponding to each cited work is located. The references inserted in the text must also be verified according to APA regulations.

INTRODUCTION. The theoretical review that begins in the introduction of the article reflects a deep exploration but it would be interesting to use recent studies (last 5 years).

Perhaps, expressions such as "most recent" should be reviewed if there are other subsequent references later. Regarding what is already known about the subject of the article, it is recommended to specify why it is an important study and how it differs from previous publications.

The research question should be added clearly in the introduction. Also, the objective that appears in the summary does not match (or is misleading) with what appears as a research question in the objectives. It would be interesting to improve this aspect.

Methods. The methodology is appropriate in terms of the selection criteria. The variables involved are clearly defined. They should review the description of the content in table 1, since what appears in table 1 partially coincides with what is discussed in the methodology section. There are enough details to replicate the study but it would be interesting to add the two or three elements that the NOS includes in each domain and why it was not suitable for the present study.

Results. The results are arranged by categories, which makes it easier to read and understand. The text is not repetitive with respect to the data in the table.

Statistically significant results are clear in some categories, it is recommended to do this in all categories.

In the sustained attention category, the relationship between DCD and ADHD is sometimes confusing. It would be convenient to clarify this.

Although the data is presented clearly and the table format is correct, the table is presented in alphabetical order according to authors, the results are arranged according to categories.

The quality items should be specified in table 2 to facilitate reading.

Discussion and conclusions. It is recommended to relate the content of the discussion with what was previously stated in the introduction and avoid explaining future prospects in this section.

Future research adds that future studies should also focus on children under 8 and over 12 years old, why?

Reviewer #2: The present work shows the results obtained from a systematic review of the literature whose main objective is to identify the attentional and executive deficits present in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD), for which the type of cognitive tasks, the modality of the tasks (verbal and non-verbal) and the influence of comorbid disorders on the attentional and executive profiles were systematically considered.

The presented results contribute to a better understanding of the cognitive profile associated with CDD, revealing as one of the main findings to be considered that executive impairments are common in CDD (especially in certain areas). Therefore, one of the main implications to be taken into account, and which emerges from the review conducted in this paper, is the consideration of possible attentional and executive impairments in CDD children as inherent to the disorder itself, which, in itself, is one of the strengths of this review.

Another of the main strengths of this review is at the methodological level, as it is a comprehensive review that includes a wide variety of research terms and different types of experimental tasks, as well as standardized neuropsychological tests for the selection of studies. Another strength is the consideration of studies using samples of DCD children with comorbid disorders.

A possible limitation arising from the above consideration is that at certain moments and, assuming the importance of presenting the information in the exhaustive way in which it is done, the reading of the document becomes certainly cumbersome and complex. It would be desirable to review this issue.

Other possible limitations that may be found in this work are assumed by the authors themselves in the section described for this purpose, such as the creation of their own quality assessment tool or the inclusion of studies that, basically due to the number of samples, cannot be considered relative. In this case, the authors warn the reader to take the conclusions drawn with sufficient caution. Furthermore, in the section on Directions for Future Research, lines of research are suggested that also respond to another series of limitations that can be found in the review carried out, such as including studies of self-regulation, extending the age of the sample, verifying whether the executive deficit is prolonged or whether it is a developmental delay, or verifying the overlap between executive functions and motor skills. In this sense, it is desirable that the conclusions section appears before Strenghts and Limitations and Directions for Future Research.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Zaira Santana Amador

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jun 4;16(6):e0252043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252043.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 Mar 2021

The authors have revised the comments in the Decision Letter and have changed the manuscript accordingly. The "Response to Reviewers" document contains all the answers/changes that the authors have made.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_reviewers_Round1.docx

Decision Letter 1

Celestino Rodríguez

10 May 2021

Attentional and Executive Functions in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Coordination Disorder and the Influence of Comorbid Disorders: A Systematic Review of the Literature

PONE-D-20-33814R1

Dear Dr. Lippé,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Celestino Rodríguez, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Most of the changes initially proposed to the authors of the manuscript have been implemented, although it is true that some of them have not been implemented favourably.

I believe that it was not necessary to change the name of the Objectives section to State of the Knowledge. The only request was that the objective that should appear in the Introduction section should be clearer and, above all, consistent with what was stated in the initial summary.

The clarification included in the Methodology section makes it easier to understand the content of table 1.

Similarly, the changes made in the Results section make the results clearer to understand. Even so, it is still a complicated section.

Finally, the changes in the presentation of the Conclusions, Strengths and Limitations of the current review and Directions for Future Research make the manuscript more consistent and coherent.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Dra. Zaira Santana Amador

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Celestino Rodríguez

20 May 2021

PONE-D-20-33814R1

Attentional and executive functions in children and adolescents with developmental coordination disorder and the influence of comorbid disorders: A systematic review of the literature

Dear Dr. Lippé:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Celestino Rodríguez

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data. Minimal data set.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

    (DOC)

    S1 Appendix. Quality assessment: Adaptation of the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale cohort studies.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Appendix. Summary of attentional and executive functions assessed, age ranges, sample sizes and tasks used among studies.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_reviewers_Round1.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES