Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2021 Jun 7;175(9):968–970. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.1338

Association of Polygenic Liability for Autism With Face-Sensitive Cortical Responses From Infancy

Anna Gui 1,, Emma L Meaburn 2, Charlotte Tye 3,4,5, Tony Charman 6, Mark H Johnson 7, Emily J H Jones 1
PMCID: PMC8185629  PMID: 34096992

Abstract

This cohort study investigates whether N290 latency to faces vs nonfaces is associated with autism polygenic scores and cross-disorder polygenic scores in infants with and without a family history of autism.


Autism is a heritable condition affecting 1% of people worldwide. Despite a pressing need for early intervention, the developmental paths through which genetic variants are associated with emerging behavioral symptoms in infancy remain opaque. The latency of the N170 event-related potential response to faces is replicably altered in individuals with autism1 and has potential as a stratification biomarker for prognostic social functioning.2 The N170 precursor (N290) to faces vs nonfaces is also altered prior to symptom emergence in infants subsequently diagnosed with autism.3 These early differences in brain processing represent a plausible developmental mechanism linking genetic liability and behavioral autism symptoms. We investigated whether N290 latency to faces vs nonfaces is associated with autism polygenic scores and cross-disorder polygenic scores in infants with and without a family history of autism.

Methods

In this cohort study, 104 infants with and without a family history of autism provided DNA and participated in an electroencephalography (EEG) task3 presenting face and nonface images as part of a longitudinal prospective study (the British Autism Study of Infant Siblings [BASIS]). Diagnostic assessments at age 3 years determined whether infants with a family history of autism were diagnosed with autism, showed typical development, or showed other signs of atypical development (Table). Ethical approval was obtained from the Health Research Authority of the English National Health Service. Parents gave written informed consent.

Table. Characteristics of Participants in the Study Sample and Group Comparisons for the Continuous Measures.

Characteristic Mean (SD) Levene test Group difference
Total sample Lost to follow-up No FH FH-TD FH-Other FH-Aut P value η 2 a
Total, No. 104 3 22 45 20 14 NA NA NA
Female, No. 53 2 11 28 11 1 NA NA NA
Age, mo 8.3 (1.2) 8.7 (0.6) 8.2 (1.1) 8.2 (1.2) 8.5 (1.2) 8.1 (1.2) >.99 .78 0.01
F-N N290 latency 6.68 (19.46) −10.23 (5.89) 17.16 (20.53)b 4.83 (17.79) 7.42 (17.81) −1.32 (21.12)b .89 .03 0.09
Autism PGS (threshold P = .01) 0.11 (0.98) 0.62 (0.77) 0.31 (1.02) −0.15 (0.94)b −0.07 (0.73)c 0.82 (1.02)b,c .49 .006 0.12
Cross disorder PGS (threshold P = .50) 0.09 (1.03) 0.32 (0.48) −0.20 (1.20) 0.13 (1.04) 0.14 (0.83) 0.27 (1.07) .59 .54 0.02
Mullen Scales of Early Learning Composite standard score at 8 mo 104.29 (15.74) 92.67 (15.50) 111.32 (13.47)b 105.22 (15.96) 101.60 (15.08) 96.57 (15.64)b .89 .03 0.09
Mullen Scales of Early Learning Composite standard score at 3 y 107.94 (21.15) NA 121.40 (13.11)d,e 112.18 (14.51)f 98.60 (24.77)d 88.71 (24.91)e,f .009 <.001 η2[H] = 0.19g

Abbreviations: FH-Aut, infants with a family history of autism and a later diagnosis of autism; FH-Other, infants with a family history of autism and with neurodevelopmental difficulties at age 3 years who did not meet criteria for a clinical diagnosis of autism; FH-TD, infants with a family history of autism and typical development at age 3 years; F-N N290 latency, N290 latency difference between the face and the nonface conditions; No FH, infants with no family history of autism; PGS, polygenic score.

a

η2 Indicates eta-squared as a measure of the analysis of variance effect size.

b

Significant differences based on Tukey honest significant difference post hoc test.

c

Significant differences based on Tukey honest significant difference post hoc test.

d

Significant differences based on Dunn post hoc test.

e

Significant differences based on Dunn post hoc test.

f

Significant differences based on Dunn post hoc test.

g

η2[H], eta squared based on the H statistic obtained in the Kruskal-Wallis test

Infants viewed face or nonface (scrambled pixels of the face) images while brain electrical activity was measured continuously with a 128-channel Hydrocel Sensor Net System (Electrical Geodesics Inc). N290 latency was extracted for each condition (220 to 319 milliseconds; more than 10 good-quality EEG trials; mean of 19 occipitotemporal electrodes), and the difference in N290 latency between face and nonface stimuli was computed (face-nonface [F-N] N290 latency).

Genome-wide genotype data were obtained from saliva and buccal cheek-swab DNA.4 Standardized polygenic scores were calculated using PRSice-2 software in R version 3.6.3 (The R Foundation) for 234 unrelated infants of European ancestry, assigned by the investigators based on principal component analysis on a combined sample of infants’ and Hapmap3 genotypes. Autism polygenic scores and cross-disorder polygenic scores were generated using the Autism5 and Cross-Disorder6 European-based genome-wide association studies (GWAS) at a range of P value thresholds (.001 < threshold P ≤ 1). Linkage disequilibrium estimation for clumping (r2 < 0.1; 250-kilobase distance from index variant) was based on the 1000 Genomes Project reference panel. Five ancestry principal components were included as covariates.

Regression analyses tested the association between F-N N290 latency and autism polygenic scores and cross-disorder polygenic scores at the GWAS P value thresholds that explained the highest variance (Nagelkerke R2) in infants with autism and a family history of autism and those with atypical development (whether autism or other). Model fit improvement was tested using χ2 when adding autism polygenic scores to the logistic model that tested the association of F-N N290 latency with autism. Tests were 2-tailed and significance was set at P < .05. Details on diagnostic assessment, EEG, and genetic data preprocessing are available in the eMethods in the Supplement and online at https://github.com/annagui/PGS_EEG.

Results

Of 104 infants included in this study, 53 were female (51.0%). The mean (SD) age was 8.3 (1.2) months. As previously reported,2 infants later diagnosed with autism showed diminished differentiation between N290 latency to face and nonface stimuli relative to infants without a family history of autism (Table; Figure, A). Higher autism polygenic scores (threshold P = .01; number of single nucleotide variants = 4806; Nagelkerke R2 = 0.054; P = .01) (Figure, B) was associated with shorter N290 latency to face vs nonface stimuli (β = −3.89; SE = 1.94; P = .047) (Figure, C). Cross-disorder polygenic score (threshold P = .50; number of single nucleotide variants = 59 669; Nagelkerke R2 = 0.015; P = .15) was even more strongly associated with F-N N290 latency (β = −5.05; SE = 1.81; P = .006) (Figure, D). Testing the association between these precursors and autism (dependent variable), the model fit significantly improved when adding polygenic scores to F-N N290 latency as an independent variable (McFadden R2 = 0.121; P = .008).

Figure. N290 Latency Face-Nonface Difference and Polygenic Scores by Group and Their Association.

Figure.

Shaded area indicates standard errors; error bars, standard error of the mean. FH indicates a family history of autism.

Discussion

Altered cortical responses to social vs nonsocial stimuli in infancy may be one brain processing pathway through which genetic liability leads to behavioral autism symptoms and may suggest a suitable target for early identification. This study has limitations. The relatively small size and composition of the sample somewhat limit the generalizability of findings. Future studies should leverage larger GWAS and population infant samples, including those of non-European ancestry.

Supplement.

eMethods

eReferences

References

  • 1.Kang E, Keifer CM, Levy EJ, Foss-Feig JH, McPartland JC, Lerner MD. Atypicality of the N170 event-related potential in autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018;3(8):657-666. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.European Medicines Agency . Letter of support for N170 ERP as a prognostic biomarker for adaptive social functioning and its potential to stratify study populations in people with Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) without intellectual disability. Accessed February 24, 2021. https://www.ema.europa.eu/en/documents/other/letter-support-n170-erp-prognostic-biomarker-adaptive-social-functioning-its-potential-stratify_en.pdf.
  • 3.Tye C, Bussu G, Gliga T, et al. ; BASIS Team . Understanding the nature of face processing in early autism: a prospective study. medRxiv. Preprint posted online May 11, 2020. doi: 10.1101/2020.05.06.20092619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gui A, Mason L, Gliga T, et al. ; BASIS-STAARS Team . Look duration at the face as a developmental endophenotype: elucidating pathways to autism and ADHD. Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32(4):1303-1322. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, et al. ; Autism Spectrum Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; BUPGEN; Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; 23andMe Research Team . Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):431-444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schork AJ, Won H, Appadurai V, et al. A genome-wide association study of shared risk across psychiatric disorders implicates gene regulation during fetal neurodevelopment. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(3):353-361. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0320-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eMethods

eReferences


Articles from JAMA Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES