Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jun 8.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019 Jul 5;30:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.05.005

Color, Pattern, and the Retinal Cone Mosaic

David H Brainard 1
PMCID: PMC8186451  NIHMSID: NIHMS1586340  PMID: 34109261

Abstract

Textbook trichromacy accounts for human color vision in terms of spectral sampling by three classes of cone photoreceptors. This account neglects entangling of color and pattern information created by wavelength-dependent optical blur (chromatic aberrations) and interleaved spatial sampling of the retinal image by the three classes of cones. Recent experimental, computational, and neurophysiological work is now considering color and pattern vision at the elementary scale of daylight vison, that is at the scale of individual cones. The results provide insight about rich interactions between color and pattern vision as well as the role of the statistical structure of natural scenes in shaping visual processing.

Keywords: color perception, spatial vision, cones, cone mosaic, psychophysics, computational models

Introduction

Trichromacy provides the foundation for understanding of human color vision, and the standard textbook treatment may be easily summarized. Light entering the eye is encoded by the excitations of three classes of cone photoreceptors, each with its characteristic spectral sensitivity (Figure 1A). The nature of this encoding places strong constraints on our color experience, since any information about spectra not encoded by the cone excitations will not be available to human experience. Indeed, two physically-different spectra can produce the same cone excitations, and these two lights must then necessarily appear the same to us – no subsequent processing can restore spectral distinctions that are not available at the first stage of light encoding [1]. This observation underlies our theoretical and practical understanding of color mixing, where mixtures of three primary lights may be used to elicit essentially the full gamut of color sensations. Indeed, this understanding is the basis of color recording and color reproduction technologies [e.g., the colors on televisions and computer displays are produced by mixtures of red, green and blue primaries, rather than be recreating the actual spectra we encounter in daily viewing; 2].

Figure 1. Trichromacy and the Interleaved Cone Mosaic.

Figure 1.

A) The human retina contains three classes of cone photoreceptors, referred to as the long-wavelength-sensitive (L), middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) and short-wavelength-sensitive (S) cones. Each class of cones has a distinct spectral sensitivity, so that the relative excitations of the three classes depends on the spectrum of the incident light. B) Schematic of human foveal cone mosaic. There are approximately 2 L cones (red) for every M cone (green), while S cones make up only a small fraction of the mosaic (about 5%). The arrangement of S cones depicted is semi-regular; the actual S-cone mosaic may be less regular than shown. There are no S cones in the central portion (here 0.3 deg) of the fovea. The algorithm used to generate this model mosaic is described in [37]. Figure courtesy of Nicolas Cottaris.

Although the textbook account is deep and correct, it also embodies an important simplification. In particular, it encourages the view that color vision may be understood without consideration of the spatial pattern of the images that reach the retina, that is that color vision and pattern vision are in some sense independent processes. Under this view, it would be appropriate to study color vision for spatially uniform patches and pattern vision for grayscale (luminance) images, with any interactions between the two relegated as effects of interest only to specialists. Although there is no denying that we have learned much about both color and pattern vision through separate consideration of the two, there is also no doubt that there can be large and striking interactions between color and pattern vision (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Interactions between space and color.

Figure 2.

A) The panel shows an example of color assimilation. The two reddish bars have the same RGB values, but appear quite different. This difference is a consequence of where the reddish bars are inserted into the blue-yellow grating. B) Color assimilation depends on spatial pattern. The reddish bars again have the same RGB values as each other (and the same RGB values as the reddish bars in A), and the RGB values of the blue-yellow grating are also matched those in A. The effect of assimilation is reduced in B, as compared to A. See [48] for discussion of possible mechanisms underlying color assimilation. Comparison of the panels also reveals a difference in the color appearance of the blue and yellow bars.

This short review argues that there are fundamental reasons for interactions between color and pattern vision; that is, these interactions begin with a confounding of color and pattern information that occurs at the initial stage of visual encoding that unavoidably leads to tradeoffs between the acuity of color and pattern vision. Consideration of the implications of color-pattern encoding may yield important general insights about color vision and its evolution.

The Interleaved Cone Mosaic and Color-Pattern Information Loss

Figure 1B illustrates the arrangement of cones in a patch of retinal mosaic typical of the high-resolution human fovea. Several observations are noteworthy. First, although the mosaic as a whole is trichromatic, at each retinal location there is only one cone. That is, at its elementary spatial scale the retina is locally monochromatic rather than trichromatic: the trichromacy we experience for spatially-coarse stimuli must arise by neural comparisons that operate not only across cones of different classes but also across cones at different spatial locations.

Second, the L and M cones vastly outnumber the S cones; in a typical retina over 90% of cones will be L or M, with only a small fraction of S cones [36]. The small fraction of S cones is likely related to chromatic aberration in the eye’s optics, which lead to a much blurrier short wavelength retinal image relative to that at longer wavelengths [7]. Chromatic aberration is another form of color-pattern interaction in early vision, which we do not expand on in detail here. There are also typically more L cones than M cones (about 2:1 L to M), but this varies considerably across individuals [4].

Third, the arrangement of the L and M cones is quite irregular, close to random [4]. This leads to a retinal patchiness, with some local regions L dominated and others M dominated. This patchiness is not readily apparent in our everyday experience of color. This, as well as the variation across individuals in L:M cone ratio, suggests that neural processing must be tailored to the local arrangement of the cone mosaic to account for the local pattern of spatial sampling [810, see also 11].

It is worth emphasizing that although the interleaving of cones in the retina occurs at a fine spatial scale, the consequences of such interleaving can propagate to coarser scales. One way to appreciate this is to consider artifacts that occur in digital imaging, as digital cameras also generally employ a sensor design that interleaves elements with three distinct (R, G and B) spectral sensitivities. Figure 3A shows salient chromatic fringing artifacts that can occur in images acquired with such sensors. The reason is that interleaved sampling loses the distinction between fine grayscale patterns (e.g. the herringbone pattern of the jacket) and coarse color patterns (e.g. the artifactual red-green pattern seen over portions of the jacket). The information loss can be understood by considering a simple example with just two sensor elements, one R and one G (Figure 3B,C top). A fine dark to light pattern across these two elements will produce a low response in the R sensor and a high response in the G element. The same low-to-high response pair will also be produced by a spatially-uniform green pattern, which will excite the G element more than its neighboring R 3B, bottom). Similarly, a light-to-dark grayscale pattern produces the same pair of sensor responses as a spatially-uniform red pattern (3C, bottom). Thus, fine grayscale patterns can be indistinguishable from coarse color patterns in the encoding of images by interleaved mosaics. In the image shown, this effect plays out as the jacket is imaged by the camera’s interleaved mosaic and the image is rendered by the camera’s software. Although a different camera image-rendering algorithm might have avoided the artifact shown, such an algorithm would then have been subject to rendering actual coarse color patterns as fine grayscale patterns, an equally salient error.

Figure 3. Color-pattern artifacts.

Figure 3.

A) A digital color image taken with a camera that employed an interleaved RGB sensor design. Note the green-red color artifacts on the jacket. The subject’s face has been intentionally distorted to protect identity; that distortion is not what is being illustrated here. [Panel A reproduced with permission from 9.] B) A high-spatial-frequency grayscale pattern can produce the same low/high alternation of sensor responses as a low-spatial-frequency green pattern, for a simple two-pixel RG interleaved sensor. C) When the phase of the high-spatial-frequency pattern is shifted relative to the sensor, the grayscale pattern produces the same high/low alternation of sensor responses as a low-spatial-frequency red pattern. The type of effects illustrated in panels B and C lead to the red-green artifacts shown in A. [Panels B and C reproduced with permission from 38.]

Implications for Perception

When we look at the world, we do not typically experience salient artifacts of the sort illustrated in Figure 3, even though our retinas consist of an interleaved cone mosaic. That said, there is a perceptual phenomenon known as Brewster’s colors [12], in which subtle colored splotches are sometimes seen when we look at fine black and white stripes. Experiments that explore Brewster’s colors suggest that they indeed have their origin in interleaved sampling by the cone mosaic, and that their subtlety is a consequence of sophisticated neural processing that minimizes our exposure to the information lost to us through interleaved sampling [13]. The exact nature of this processing remains mysterious, but recent theoretical and experimental approaches are now advancing our understanding and promise to inform us about the degree to which some color-pattern interactions may be ascribed to such processing.

Bayesian approach to reconstruction from samples.

At a broad level, an interleaved cone mosaic collects less information about the retinal image than a hypothetical “ideal” mosaic where all cone classes sample the retinal image at each location. The consequences of this depend strongly on the nature of the images that the visual system will view. If it were known, for example, that all environmental images were spatially uniform and varied only in color then the consequences of interleaved sampling would be negligible. Similarly, if it were known that there was no variation in image color so that all environmental images were grayscale, again there would be little consequence of interleaved sampling (and indeed, little need for more than one type of cone). In the actual case, where images vary in both color and pattern, the consequences of interleaved sampling depend on the spatio-chromatic regularities in natural images. These may be examined by using Bayesian methods [14], together with explicit models of natural image statistics and retinal image formation, to reconstruct the external image from the cone excitations [15]. Distortions in the reconstructions then provide hypotheses about the sorts of color-pattern interactions we might expect to find in human vision.

An analysis along these lines [9] incorporated the known strong correlations between natural image values at nearby locations [16] and between the excitations of cones of different classes [17]. The results indicated, among other observations, that the human visual system’s well-known [18, 19] lack of sensitivity to high-spatial frequency chromatic patterns (both blue-yellow and red-green) may have its origins in the combined effect of optical chromatic aberrations and interleaved cone sampling.

Experiments with small spots.

Studying directly consequences of interleaved cone sampling has historically been difficult. The primary reason is that under normal circumstances, blurring of the retinal image as well as fixational eye movements prevent precise experimental control of the excitations of individual cones. It has also historically been difficult to determine the spectral class of any individual cone in the living retina.

Early insights were gained by study of the S-cone submosaic, whose distinct spectral sensitivity and spatial sparseness within the mosaic (Figure 1) make it more amenable to cone-specific selective stimulation [20, 21]. Another fruitful approach was the use of interferometric stimuli, which allowed presentation of high-contrast high-spatial frequency sinusoidal gratings on the retina. Resulting distortions of perceived spatial patterns revealed consequences of spatial sampling by the mosaic as a whole [22], but interactions between color and pattern remained difficult to assess [but see 19, 23].

Recently, however, techniques have been developed that promise much richer experimental understanding. Adaptive compensation of aberrations in the eye [24, 25], combined in some cases with real-time tracking of fixational eye movements [26, 27], now allow targeted stimulation of individual cones and groups of cones for psychophysical study [28, 29]. Wavelength-selective imaging using adaptive optics also enables determining the spectral class of individual cones in an individual’s eye [36].

Experiments using these techniques are now providing a wealth of fundamental information about color and spatial vision at the elementary spatial scale of the visual system. We now know that signals from individual cones can result in a reliable perceptual signal [28, 30], at least in the central visual field; that signals from multiple cones can be combined by neural processes to improve detection performance of small spots [31, see also 32]; and that there is variation in the color percept associated with stimulating different single cones of the same class [30]. For example, stimulation of one L cone in an observer’s retina results in reliable reports of perceived red, while stimulation of a different L cone leads to reliable reports of perceived white [33, see also 34].

This latter effect, which at first seems counter-intuitive, may be understood as a rather direct characterization of color-pattern interactions at the finest spatial scale available to the visual system, that of the cone mosaic. The general form of the interaction is consistent with the Bayesian image reconstruction analyses described above [9]. To provide intuition, Figure 4 shows Bayesian reconstructions obtained from two model cone mosaics where incremental stimulation was provided to a single L cone. The figure shows an expanded view of two model mosaics, each with an L cone near its center. For each mosaic, we simulated the cone excitations that would have occurred with incremental stimulation of the central L cone using adaptive optics, against an otherwise spatially uniform gray background. The cone excitations were then processed using a Bayesian image reconstruction algorithm that learned its prior from a dataset of natural images. Note that although the central L cone is excited in the same way in each case, the resulting reconstructions differ, particularly in their color appearance. The intuition for this is that different information about the stimulus is provided by the cones surrounding the single stimulated cone, and this information differs depending on the type of the cones in the local neighborhood of the stimulated cone. In one case, there are only L cones in the immediate neighborhood, and little direct information is available about stimulus color. In the other, the mix of L and M cones provides different information, leading the algorithm to a redder reconstruction, based on the low response of the nearby M cones. To implement the Bayesian reconstructions shown here, the prior was based on an independent-components analysis [35] of the CIFAR image dataset (https://www.cs.toronto.edu/~kriz/cifar.html), and the distribution of the coefficients for each independent component was assumed to be exponential. The computation was implemented using cross-validated lasso regression [36] and by taking advantage of the ISETBio [37] software to determine the relation between training image data and the mosaic’s cone excitations; it will be described in detail in a future publication. See also Figures 7 and 8 in [9] and associated discussion. It is not yet clear the extent to which a Bayesian computational model is consistent in detail with current experimental results [see also 38]; this remains an exciting direction for future experimental and modeling work.

Figure 4. Bayesian small spot reconstructions.

Figure 4.

Expanded view of two cone mosaics with an L cone near the center (left column). Corresponding Bayesian image reconstructions based on the cone excitations of each mosaic. The reconstructions for incremental stimulation of a single L cone (circled in black in each case) are quite different, as they depend on the responses of the whole mosaic and the information carried by the two mosaics differ. See main text for additional description. Figure courtesy Lingqi Zhang.

Mechanisms.

In work that parallels targeted single-cone psychophysics, it is now possible to measure individual cone inputs to single neurons in the visual pathways [3941, see also 42]. This work seems likely to enable strong links between psychophysical results obtained with single cone stimulation and the neural mechanisms that mediate those results [see 43].

Evolution.

The selective advantages conveyed by color vision have received considerable treatment in the literature, particularly in the context of how color vision evolved and why it differs across species [4446]. Less considered in this literature are the tradeoffs between color and pattern vision mandated when cones of different classes sample the retinal image in an interleaved fashion. As we understand more about how visual systems can and do optimize post-receptoral processing to handle the information loss imposed by interleaved sampling, and how this processing depends on the statistical structure of any given species’ visual ecology (both in terms of the statistical structure of scenes and in terms of what features of those scenes are behaviorally relevant), there should be an opportunity to bring consideration of interleaved sampling into the discussion. Such consideration may clarify when increased dimensionality of the color sensory apparatus (i.e. number of distinct receptor classes) is beneficial and when it is not [see 38, 47].

Highlights.

  1. Color and pattern information are entangled by the initial stages of visual encoding.

  2. Experiments at vision’s elementary spatial scale can now probe color-pattern interactions.

  3. Computational models predict limits of vision imposed by optics and the interleaved cone mosaic.

Acknowledgments

Supported by NIH RO1 EY10016 and a gift from Facebook Reality Labs. I thank Nicolas Cottaris for Figure 1 and Lingqi Zhang for Figure 4.

References

  • [1].Stockman A, Brainard DH. Fundamentals of color vision I: Color processing in the eye. In: Handbook of Color Psychology. Edited by: Elliot AJ, Fairchild MD. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Bodrogi P, Khanh TQ. Illumination, Color and Imaging. Weinheim, Germany: Wiley-VCH Verlag & Co.; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Roorda A, Williams DR. The arrangement of the three cone classes in the living human eye. Nature 1999; 397:520–522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Hofer HJ, Carroll J, Neitz J, Neitz M, Williams DR. Organization of the human trichromatic cone mosaic. Journal of Neuroscience 2005; 25:9669–9679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Sabesan R, Hofer H, Roorda A. Characterizing the human cone photoreceptor mosaic via dynamic photopigment densitometry. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0144891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Zhang F, Kurokawa K, Lassoued A, Crowell JA, Miller DT. Cone photoreceptor classification in the living human eye from photostimulation-induced phase dynamics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; **High-precision classification of cone types in living human retina, using visual stimulation in conjunction with phase-sensitivie adaptive-optics optical coherence tomoagraphy (OCT).
  • [7].Autrusseau F, Thibos L, Shevell SK. Chromatic and wavefront aberrations: L-, M- and S-cone stimulation with typical and extreme retinal image quality. Vision Res 2011; 51:2282–2294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Wachtler T, Doi E, Lee T- W, Sejnowski TJ. Cone selectivity derived from the responses of the retinal cone mosaic to natural scenes. Journal of vision 2007; 7:1–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Brainard DH, Williams DR, Hofer H. Trichromatic reconstruction from the interleaved cone mosaic: Bayesian model and the color appearance of small spots. Journal of vision 2008; 8:15 11–23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Benson NC, Manning JR, Brainard DH. Unsupervised learning of cone spectral classes from natural images. PLoS Comput Biol 2014; 10:e1003652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Brainard DH, Roorda A, Yamauchi Y, Calderone JB, Metha A, Neitz M, Neitz J, Williams DR, Jacobs GH. Functional consequences of the relative numbers of L and M cones. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 2000; 17:607–614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Brewster D On the undulations excited in the retina by the action of luminous points and lines. London and Edinburgh Philosoph. Mag. and J. Sci 1832:1–169 [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Williams DR, Sekiguchi N, Haake W, Brainard DH, Packer O. The cost of trichromacy for spatial vision. In: From Pigments to Perception. New York: Plenum Press; 1991. pp. 11–22. [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Bishop CM. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning. New York: Springer Science + Business Media LLC; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Brainard DH. Bayesian method for reconstructing color images from trichromatic samples. In: IS&T 47th Annual Meeting. Rochester, NY: 1994. pp. 375–379. [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Simoncelli EP, Olshausen BA. Natural image statistics and neural representation. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001; 24:1193–1216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Garrigan P, Ratliff CP, Klein JM, Sterling P, Brainard DH, Balasubramanian V. Design of a trichromatic cone array. PLoS Computational Biology 2010; 6:e1000677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Mullen KT. The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red-green and blue-yellow gratings. Journal of Physiology 1985; 359:381–400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Sekiguchi N, Williams DR, Brainard DH. Aberration-free measurements of the visibility of isoluminant gratings. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 1993; 10:2105–2117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Williams DR, MacLeod DIA, Hayhoe MM. Punctate sensitivity of the blue-sensitive mechanism. Vision Research 1981; 19:1357–1375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Williams DR, Collier RJ, Thompson BJ. Spatial resolution of the short-wavelength mechanism. In: Colour Vision. Edited by: Mollon JD, Sharpe LT. London: Academic Press; 1983. pp. 487–503. [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Williams DR. Aliasing in human foveal vision. Vision Research 1985; 25:195–205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Brainard DH, Williams DR. Spatial reconstruction of signals from short-wavelength cones. Vision Research 1993; 33:105–116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Liang J, Williams DR, Miller DT. Supernormal vision and high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 1997; 14:2884–2992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Williams DR. Imaging single cells in the living retina. Vision Res 2011; 51:1379–1396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Yang Q, Arathorn DW, Tiruveedhula P, Vogel CR, Roorda A. Design of an integrated hardware interface for AOSLO image capture and cone-targeted stimulus delivery. Opt Express 2010; 18:17841–17858 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Roorda A Adaptive optics for studying visual function: A comprehensive review. Journal of vision 2011; 11:6.1–21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Harmening WM, Tuten WS, Roorda A, Sincich LC. Mapping the perceptual grain of the human retina. Journal of Neuroscience 2014; 34:5667–5677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *Describes and validates method for non-invasive and targeted psychophysical stimulation of individual cone photoreceptors in humans.
  • [29].Sincich LC, Sabesan R, Tuten WS, Roorda A, Harmening WM. Functional imaging of cone photoreceptors. In: Human Color Vision. Edited by: Kremers J, Baraas RC, Marshall NJ. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing; 2016. pp. 71–104. [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Hofer H, Singer B, Williams DR. Different sensations from cones with the same photopigment. Journal of vision 2005; 5:444–454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Tuten WS, Cooper RF, Tiruveedhula P, Dubra A, Roorda A, Cottaris NP, Brainard DH, Morgan JIW. Spatial summation in the human fovea: Do normal optical aberrations and fixational eye movements have an effect? Journal of vision 2018; 18:6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Tuten WS, Harmening WM, Sabesan R, Roorda A, Sincich LC. Spatiochromatic interactions between individual cone photoreceptors in the human retina. Journal of Neuroscience 2017; 37:9498–9509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Sabesan R, Schmidt BP, Tuten WS, Roorda A. The elementary representation of spatial and color vision in the human retina. Sci Adv 2016; 2:e1600797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; **Reports psychophysical measurment of color sensations evoked by targeted individual cones of known spectral type.
  • [34].Schmidt BP, Sabesan R, Tuten WS, Neitz J, Roorda A. Sensations from a single M-cone depend on the activity of surrounding S-cones. Scientific Reports 2018; 8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Hyvarinen A, Karhunen J, Oja E. Independent Component Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Hastie T, Tibshirani R, Friedman J. The Elements of Statistical Learning. New York: Springer; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Cottaris NP, Jiang H, Ding X, Wandell BA, Brainard DH. A computational observer model of spatial contrast sensitivity: Effects of wavefront-based optics, cone mosaic structure, and inference engine. Journal of vision 2019; 19:8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *Describes an open-source image-computable model for computing mosaic cone excitations.
  • [38].Brainard DH. Color and the cone mosaic. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2015; 1:519–546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Sincich LC, Zhang Y, Tiruveedhula P, Horton JC, Roorda A. Resolving single cone inputs to visual receptive fields. Nature neuroscience 2009; 12:967–969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Field GD, Gauthier JL, Sher A, Greschner M, Machado TA, Jepson LH, Shlens J, Gunning DE, Mathieson K, Dabrowski W, Paninski L, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Functional connectivity in the retina at the resolution of photoreceptors. Nature 2010; 467:673–677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Li PH, Field GD, Greschner M, Ahn D, Gunning DE, Mathieson K, Sher A, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Retinal representation of the elementary visual signal. Neuron 2014; 81:130–139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [42].Yin L, Masella B, Dalkara D, Zhang J, Flannery JG, Schaffer DV, Williams DR, Merigan WH. Imaging light responses of foveal ganglion cells in the living macaque eye. J Neurosci 2014; 34:6596–6605 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [43].Kling A, Field GD, Brainard DH, Chichilnisky EJ. Probing computation in the primate visual system at single-cone resolution. Annual Review Of Neuroscience 2019; 42, epub ahead of print [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [44].Regan BC, Julliot C, Simmen B, Vienot F, Charles-Dominique P, Mollon JD. Fruits, foliage and the evolution of primate color vision. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences 2001; 356:229–283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [45].Jacobs GH. Evolution of colour vision in mammals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 2009; 364:2957–2967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [46].Baden T, Osorio D. The retinal basis of vertebrate color vision. Annual Review of Vision Science in press; 19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [47].Manning JR, Brainard DH. Optimal design of photoreceptor mosaics: why we do not see color at night. Vis Neurosci 2009; 26:5–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [48].Cao D, Shevell SK. Chromatic assimilation: spread light or neural mechanism? Vision Res 2005; 45:1031–1045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES