Abstract
Studies in children have reported associations of screen time and background TV on language skills as measured by their parents. However, few large, longitudinal studies have examined language skills assessed by trained psychologists, which is less prone to social desirability. We assessed screen time and exposure to TV during family meals at ages 2, 3 and 5–6 years in 1562 children from the French EDEN cohort. Language skills were evaluated by parents at 2 years (Communicative Development Inventory, CDI) and by trained psychologists at 3 (NEPSY and ELOLA batteries) and 5–6 years (verbal IQ). Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations were assessed by linear regression adjusted for important confounders. Overall, daily screen time was not associated with language scores, except in cross-sectional at age 2 years, where higher CDI scores were observed for intermediate screen time. Exposure to TV during family meals was consistently associated with lower language scores: TV always on (vs never) at age 2 years was associated with lower verbal IQ (− 3.2 [95% IC: − 6.0, − 0.3] points), independent of daily screen time and baseline language score. In conclusion, public health policies should better account for the context of screen watching, not only its amount.
Subject terms: Risk factors, Epidemiology, Paediatric research
Introduction
Language is a thoroughly social phenomenon, and human interaction plays an important role in language acquisition1. Children’s language development is greatly influenced by their immediate environment (i.e., parents, siblings, and peers)2. Over the last decades, screens have become an additional part of the children’s environment, and preschool-aged children spend considerable time watching screens, especially TV3,4. Research has provided convincing evidence that it may lead to poorer cognitive and behavioral outcomes5–7, however, the observed effects largely depend on the age, the media content and the social and family context of viewing5,7. For example, high-quality media content and co-viewing with an adult may provide benefits for children’s learning and language skills7,8.
Although children are exposed to language via screen media, adult–child verbal interaction (questioning and commenting) is strongly associated with better children’s language development, which suggests that without such interaction or not using language in daily life to express themselves, children will not acquire a specific language9,10. Even when media programs are not intended for the child to watch, child–adult interaction and games are diminished when the TV is on11. Waldman et al. described the parents’ media behavior as an environmental trigger that creates hurdles in cognitive development with possible long-term developmental consequences12. Indeed, background TV was shown to reduce the amount and quality of interaction between parents and infants and the number of utterances produced by the parent–child dyad, with the direct effect of distracting a child and the indirect effect of taking a parent’s attention away from the child9,11,13.
However, there is limited evidence from large epidemiological studies on the role of the context of TV exposure on language development of pre-school children. In addition, daily screen time has been frequently, although not consistently, associated with language delays in infants and toddlers2,6,9,14–18. Disparate findings in the literature may be attributed to sampling differences due to small sample sizes, type of screen time measurement, and use of different language assessments. Moreover, most of these studies were cross-sectional and focused on children younger than age 3 years, which limits drawing conclusions on potential reverse causation. Furthermore, there is compelling evidence that screen time increases during early childhood4,19, and this change needs to be accounted for when analyzing longitudinal data. Lastly, few longitudinal studies have used specific psychological tests to assess language development and most studies rely on parental evaluation of the child’s language skills.
Analyzing data from the French mother–child EDEN cohort, we used both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses to explore associations of daily screen time and exposure to TV during family meals with children’s language development at ages 2, 3 and 5–6 years while accounting for a wide range of established confounders and covariates.
Methods
Study design and population
Data were obtained from the EDEN mother–child study, a cohort aiming to identify prenatal and early postnatal nutritional, environmental, and social determinants of children's health and development. The study design and protocol have been published20. Briefly, 2002 pregnant women were enrolled between 2003 and 2006 in public maternity units of Poitiers and Nancy, France. A total of 1907 children were included and followed up. Exclusion criteria included a history of diabetes, twin pregnancies, intention to deliver outside the maternity unit or to move out of the study region within the next 3 years, and an inability to speak French. Children born at < 33 weeks’ gestation (n = 23) and children without any language assessment or assessed out of the assessment age window (n = 322) were excluded from the present work (Fig. 1). The study was approved by the Ethical Research Committee of Bicêtre Hospital and the French Data Protection Authority. Informed written consent was obtained from parents at the time of enrollment for themselves and for the newborn after delivery. All research was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Exposure to screens
Questionnaires were completed at ages 2, 3 and 5–6 years. Parents reported the time spent daily by their child watching TV or playing video or computer games on typical weekdays, Wednesdays (day off from school), and weekend days. Daily screen time was calculated as (4 × weekday + Wednesday + 2 × weekend day)/7. As the frequency distribution of daily screen time was multimodal (greater number of round values), we categorized it as 0, 1–30, 31–60, 61–120, and > 120 min at age 2 years. At ages 3 and 5–6 years, the categories 0 and 1–30 min were grouped because of insufficient number of children not watching screens at all. Exposure to TV during family meals was repeatedly assessed at the three ages with the following question: “How often is the television on in the dining room while the child is eating at home?” with four response items: never, sometimes, often, or always.
Language assessment
When children were 2 years old, parents completed the French version of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI), an assessment of expressive vocabulary with high test–retest reliability and strong validity against the full version21. From a list of 100 words, parents indicated those their child could say spontaneously; and the sum was used as a score. At age 3 years, two trained psychologists (one per study center) assessed language development by using five neuropsychological subtests from the Evaluation du Langage Oral de L’enfant Aphasique22 and A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment23 batteries: semantic fluency, word and nonword repetition, sentence comprehension task, sentence repetition, and picture naming. From these five subtest scores, a standardized composite language score was derived by principal component analysis with oblique rotation, as previously published24. In the present work, we re-scaled this variable (mean = 100, standard deviation = 15) so that subsequent effect sizes were more comparable with the verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) scale described below. At age 5–6 years (mean [± SD]: 67.9 [± 1.8] months), trained psychologists administered the French version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition25. The core subtests were assessed (information, vocabulary, word reasoning) to derive an age-adjusted verbal IQ.
Covariates
Child sex, gestational age at birth, study center and birthweight were collected from obstetric records. Mothers completed questionnaires on their pre-pregnancy weight and tobacco and alcohol consumption during pregnancy. Maternal height was measured during pregnancy, allowing to calculate pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI) as pre-pregnancy weight divided by height squared (in kg/m2) and classified according to the World Health Organization classification. Data on partial and exclusive breastfeeding duration were collected. Symptoms of postpartum depression were assessed with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale at 4, 8 and 12 months and were dichotomized with ≥ 12 as the threshold to define women at risk of depression.
Mothers and fathers completed questionnaires on their speech and language delay histories during childhood and on the frequency of shared cognitive activities with their child (e.g., reading stories, singing songs), both well-known predictors of language development26. At age 5–6 years, the quality of children's cognitive stimulation and emotional support in their home environment was assessed by the parents with items from the Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME) scale27. Parents reported the date when their child entered preschool. In France, children can enter school from age 2 years, but most enter school within the year of their third birthday. Additional information on potential confounders collected included maternal age, household income, bilingual household, both parents’ education level, main caretaker, and number of older siblings aged < 14 years who were living at home. Parents reported children’s night sleep duration per 24 h, nap durations at ages 2 and 3 years, and sleep quality (night waking frequency per week). Frequent night waking was defined as ≥ 3 times per week.
Statistical analyses
We describe the characteristics of participants with means (± SDs) and numbers (percentages) for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. We assessed the differences between excluded and included samples with Student t and chi-square tests.
We analyzed the associations of exposure to screens with language scores by multivariable linear regression. Analyzes were conducted both cross-sectionally (i.e., models with exposure to screens and outcome measured at concurrent time points) and longitudinally (i.e., models with exposure to screens measured at age 2 years and outcome measured at age 5–6 years). Longitudinal models were adjusted for language score at age 2 years (baseline) to estimate the effect of the exposure to screens at age 2 years on language at age 5–6 years while keeping baseline language constant. We adjusted regression models for the above-described confounders (except for sleep variables, see below). To gain precision in the prediction of the outcomes, we adjusted for the child’s exact age at language assessment (except for verbal IQ, which is already age-adjusted). We adjusted for schooling duration only in models at age 3 years, because, in our sample, almost no children had entered school at age 2 years, and almost all were in school at age 5–6 years. We evaluated the presence of multicollinearity between predictors of the models by calculating their variance inflation factor; as all were below 3, we considered multicollinearity not to be an issue in our models. We also examined the correlation between the two screen variables (Daily screen time and TV during family meals) and ran covariate-adjusted regression models with and without mutual adjustment for the two exposure variables to evaluate confounding by each other. We tested the interactions between daily screen time and TV during family meals but found none (data not shown).
To explore potential mediation by sleep28, we performed sensitivity analyses with further adjustment for sleep variables (night waking frequency, night sleep and nap durations) and observed whether it affected the magnitude of our estimates.
Missing data for exposures, outcomes and covariates were imputed by using multiple imputation techniques among the sample of participants with at least one measure of screen exposure and one measure of language (n = 1562) (Supplementary Table S4). We imputed 10 datasets by using the fully conditional specification method and combined the estimates following Rubin’s rules. We performed sensitivity analyses on the complete-case samples. Analyses were performed with SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA).
Results
As compared with children excluded from our analyses, those included had a lower number of older siblings, lived in households of higher income and education level, and were born to mothers with lower pre-pregnancy BMI and less frequently smoked during pregnancy (Supplementary Table S1). The maternal, child and household characteristics of the 1,562 participants analyzed are shown in Table 1. Mean (± SD) daily screen time increased with age: 46 (± 47), 66 (± 50) and 84 (± 52) min at ages 2, 3 and 5–6 years, respectively (Table 2). The proportion of children never exposed to TV during family meals decreased from 40.5% to 37.8% and 34.1% from ages 2 to 3 and 5–6 years, respectively. Daily screen time and frequency of TV on during family meals were positively correlated with each other (Spearman correlations: 0.24, 0.31, 0.40 at ages 2, 3 and 5–6 years, respectively). Mean language scores were 60.7 (± 29.5) for CDI (age 2 years), 100.0 (± 15.0) (by construct) for composite language (3 years) and 106.7 (± 14.2) for verbal IQ (age 5–6 years) (Table 2).
Table 1.
Observed sample (n = 1,562)a,b | |
---|---|
Maternal characteristics | |
Study center | |
Poitiers | 783 (50.1) |
Nancy | 779 (49.9) |
Age at delivery, mean (SD), y | 29.7 (4.8) |
Pre-pregnancy body mass index | |
< 18.5 kg/m2 | 128 (8.4) |
18.5–24.9 kg/m2 | 1013 (66.2) |
25.0–29.9 kg/m2 | 269 (17.6) |
≥ 30.0 kg/m2 | 121 (7.9) |
Tobacco consumption during pregnancy | 373 (24.0) |
Alcohol consumption during pregnancy | |
None | 868 (55.8) |
< 2 glasses/week | 573 (36.8) |
≥ 2 glasses/week | 117 (7.5) |
Symptoms of postpartum depression | 282 (21.7) |
Child characteristics | |
Sex | |
Male | 812 (52.0) |
Female | 750 (48.0) |
Gestational age at birth, mean (SD), wk | 39.4 (1.5) |
Birthweight, mean (SD), kg | 3.30 (0.47) |
Breastfeeding duration, mean (SD), month | 3.3 (3.7) |
Household characteristics | |
Older siblings | |
0 | 718 (46.1) |
1 | 566 (36.3) |
≥ 2 | 275 (17.6) |
Parental level of education, mean (SD), y | 13.5 (2.3) |
Bilingual household | 132 (8.5) |
Mother's language difficulties during childhood | 93 (6.0) |
Father's language difficulties during childhood | 125 (8.7) |
Monthly household income | |
< 1,500 EUR | 207 (13.3) |
1,501–3,000 EUR | 902 (57.7) |
> 3,000 EUR | 453 (29.0) |
aUnless otherwise indicated, data are number (percentage) of participants.
bData were missing for 31 participants (2%) for pre-pregnancy body mass index, 5 (0.3%) for tobacco consumption during pregnancy, 6 (0.4%) for alcohol consumption during pregnancy, 16 (1.0%) for symptoms of postpartum depression, 1 (0.1%) for birthweight, 1 (0.1%) for duration of breastfeeding, 3 (%) for older siblings, 22 (1.4%) for mother’s language difficulties, 131 (8.4%) for father’s language difficulties, 9 (0.6%) for household income.
Table 2.
Sample at age 2 years (n = 1,413)b | Sample at age 3 years (n = 1,216) | Sample at age 5–6 years (n = 1,095) | |
---|---|---|---|
Exposure to screens | |||
Daily screen time, mean (SD), min | 46 (47) | 66 (50) | 84 (52) |
Daily screen time | |||
0 min | 160 (12.2) | 25 (2.2) | 5 (0.5) |
1–30 min | 543 (41.3) | 272 (23.8) | 113 (11.3) |
31–60 min | 320 (24.3) | 402 (35.2) | 292 (29.1) |
61–120 min | 226 (17.2) | 338 (29.6) | 422 (42.0) |
> 120 min | 66 (5.0) | 105 (9.2) | 172 (17.1) |
TV on during family meals | |||
Never | 569 (40.5) | 439 (37.8) | 353 (34.1) |
Sometimes | 383 (27.3) | 343 (29.5) | 347 (33.5) |
Often | 284 (20.2) | 238 (20.5) | 225 (21.7) |
Always | 168 (12.0) | 141 (12.1) | 111 (10.7) |
Covariates | |||
Main caretaker | |||
Nursery | 314 (22.2) | 229 (21.7) | 351 (41.4) |
Childminder | 602 (42.6) | 178 (16.9) | 39 (4.6) |
Family, neighbors | 154 (10.9) | 424 (40.2) | 243 (28.7) |
Mother | 343 (24.3) | 224 (21.2) | 214 (25.3) |
Cognitive stimulating activities shared with child | |||
< 1 time per week | 14 (1.0) | 13 (1.1) | 122 (11.2) |
1–2 times per week | 467 (33.1) | 470 (40.2) | 201 (18.5) |
3–5 times per week | 207 (14.6) | 215 (18.4) | 424 (39.0) |
Every day | 725 (51.3) | 471 (40.3) | 185 (17.0) |
HOME score | n.a | n.a | 17.3 (2.3) |
Schooling duration, mean (SD), month | n.a | 2.9 (3.3) | n.a |
Night sleep duration, mean (SD), hour | 11.1 (0.8) | 10.9 (0.7) | 10.9 (0.5) |
Nap duration, mean (SD), hour | 2.1 (0.5) | 1.9 (0.5) | n.a |
Frequent night awakenings (≥ 3 times per week) | 314 (22.3) | 300 (25.8) | 92 (9.0) |
Language development | |||
Assessment tool | MacArthur-Bates CDI | Composite language | Verbal IQ |
Exact age, mean (SD), month | 23.8 (1.2) | 37.3 (0.92) | 67.2 (1.8) |
Language score | |||
Mean (SD) | 60.7 (29.5) | 100.0 (15.0) | 106.7 (14.2) |
Median (interquartile range) | 64 (35‒88) | 102.6 (91.6‒110.5) | 107 (98‒116) |
Min‒max | 1‒100 | 39.5‒133.7 | 44‒147 |
CDI Communicative Development Inventory, HOME Home Observation Measurement of the Environment, IQ intelligence quotient.
aUnless otherwise indicated, data are expressed number (percentage) of participants.
bSample sizes indicated in column headers reflect largest sample size (i.e., the number of children with data for language development); data for exposure to TV and covariates were calculated without imputation for missing data and may be based on smaller sample sizes.
Table 3 shows the unadjusted and adjusted mean differences (and their 95% confidence interval) of the linear regression models assessing the associations between exposure to screens and language scores at concurrent time points (cross-sectional analyses). Of note, we observed an inverted U-shaped association between daily screen time and CDI score (age 2 years): the CDI score was 8.7 (95% CI: 3.4, 13.9) points higher on average for children watching screens for 31–60 min than never watching screens; CDI scores were intermediate for children watching screens for > 120 min daily. Although the shape of these associations also seemed nonlinear in cross-sectional analyses at ages 3 and 5–6 years (Table 3) and in the longitudinal analysis (ages 2 to 5–6 years; Fig. 2, Panel A), the associations were not statistically significant. Models with or without mutual adjustment for the two variables of exposure to screens carried out similar estimates (Supplementary Table S3).
Table 3.
Cross-sectional models at age 2 years (MacArthur-Bates CDI as outcome) |
Cross-sectional models at age 3 years (Composite language as outcome) |
Cross-sectional models at age 5–6 years (Verbal IQ as outcome) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Unadjusted | Adjusted | Unadjusted | Adjusted | Unadjusted | Adjusted | |
Daily screen time | ||||||
0 min | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | – | – | – | – |
1–30 min | 6.8 (1.5, 12.2) | 6.2 (1.2, 11.2) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) |
31–60 min | 7.6 (1.8, 13.3) | 8.7 (3.4, 13.9) | 0.6 (− 1.6, 2.9) | 1.5 (− 0.5, 3.5) | 0.2 (− 2.8, 3.2) | 1.0 (− 1.8, 3.8) |
61–120 min | 6.8 (0.6, 13.1) | 6.4 (0.7, 12.0) | − 0.6 (− 3.1, 2.0) | 0.8 (− 1.5, 3.2) | − 0.0 (− 2.5, 2.5) | 2.0 (− 0.6, 4.6) |
> 120 min | 1.7 (− 6.3, 9.8) | 3.2 (− 4.8, 11.2) | − 2.6 (− 6.5, 1.3) | 0.1 (− 3.6, 3.7) | − 1.6 (− 4.8, 1.6) | 0.8 (− 2.4, 4.1) |
TV on during family meals | ||||||
Never | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) |
Sometimes | − 2.4 (− 6.1, 1.4) | − 1.4 (− 4.9, 2.2) | − 4.8 (− 7.1, − 2.6) | − 2.8 (− 4.9, − 0.8) | − 5.0 (− 7.3, − 2.8) | − 3.3 (− 5.6, − 1.0) |
Often | − 2.8 (− 7.0, 1.4) | − 0.7 (− 4.8, 3.4) | − 4.4 (− 6.8, − 2.1) | − 2.9 (− 5.2, − 0.6) | − 8.3 (− 11, − 6.0) | − 5.0 (− 7.4, − 2.7) |
Always | − 13.7 (− 18.6, − 8.7) | − 6.7 (− 11.8, − 1.6) | − 9.8 (− 13, − 7.0) | − 4.6 (− 7.4, − 1.8) | − 6.9 (− 10, − 3.8) | − 2.5 (− 5.7, 0.7) |
CDI Communicative Development Inventory, IQ intelligence quotient, HOME Home Observation Measurement of the Environment.
aValues are adjusted mean differences (vs the reference group) from linear regression models conducted on multiply imputed datasets (n = 1,562). Models are of cross-sectional design, i.e., outcomes are being predicted by exposure to screens as measured at concomitant age. Exposure to screens variables (Daily screen time and TV on during family meals) were mutually adjusted for each other. Adjusted models were further adjusted for the following covariates: study center, maternal age at delivery, pre-pregnancy body mass index, tobacco and alcohol consumption during pregnancy, symptoms of postpartum depression, child sex, gestational age at birth, birthweight, breastfeeding duration, number of older siblings, parental education level, bilingual household, maternal and paternal language difficulties during childhood, household income, main caretaker, cognitive stimulating activities, HOME score. Models at ages 2 and 3 years were further adjusted for the child’s exact age at language assessment (verbal IQ scoring accounts for age). Models at age 3 years were further adjusted for schooling duration.
Higher frequency of TV on during family meals was associated with poorer language scores in all cross-sectional (Table 3) and longitudinal (Fig. 2, Panel B) models. At age 2 years, the CDI score was lower for children always (vs never) exposed to TV during family meals (mean difference [95% CI]: − 5.9 [− 11.7, − 0.1] CDI points). In cross-sectional analyses at ages 3 and 5–6 years, language composite score and verbal IQ were higher for children never (vs sometimes or more frequently) exposed to TV during family meals (Table 3). In the longitudinal analysis, verbal IQ at age 5–6 years was lower for children always (vs never) exposed to TV during family meals at age 2 years (mean difference [95% CI]: − 3.2 [− 6.0, − 0.3] points). A dose–response relationship was observed for intermediate frequencies of TV during family meals (p for trend = 0.002) (Fig. 2, Panel B). Overall, multivariable adjustment did not substantially reduce the strengths of the associations seen in unadjusted models. Results with the complete-case analysis did not differ from those with multiple imputation (Supplementary Table S2). Furthermore, adjusting for children’s sleep quantity and quality did not significantly change the estimates (Supplementary Table S3).
Discussion
In this cohort study of 1,562 children aged 2 to 5–6 years old, we found a cross-sectional inverted U-shaped association between children’s daily screen time at age 2 years and language development (with increased language scores for children watching screens for intermediate durations), but no associations at 3 or 5–6 years of age. In contrast, exposure to TV during family meals was negatively associated with language scores at every age, including when analyzing data longitudinally with adjustment for language skills at baseline.
Overall, we found no linear associations between daily screen time and language development, and this finding does not agree with the literature7,16–18,29. Rather, we found poorer language at age 2 years among children never watching screens, which may puzzle the readers. Previous studies have reported disparate results, which could be explained by residual confounding, and variations in study setting and language assessment methods. It is noteworthy that we found an association with the only language score that was evaluated by the parents, but not with those assessed by psychologists. However and in agreement with our results, another study found that infants exposed to no media actually had lower levels of language development than infants with some exposure9. Indeed, previous studies also suggested that parental characteristics and home environment (e.g., socioeconomic status and parental support) mattered more and were stronger predictors of children’s neurodevelopment than the amount of screen media use per se10,30,31. Another possible explanation for the inverted U-shape relationship could be that children watching moderate amount of TV watch programs of higher quality, and this may be beneficial for their language compared to non-watchers32,33.
Several studies have identified that human interaction, especially the frequency and quality of adults’ exchanges with their children, is crucial to children’s language development7,34–37. In agreement with other studies on TV exposure, the importance of child–adult interaction with regard to TV was reinforced when we explored the association between exposure to TV during family meals and language development11,13,38,39. Frequent exposure to TV during family meals was negatively associated with language scores at every age. Our main explanations for these findings, in line with previous work, are that TV during family meals may have both a direct effect of distracting a child and an indirect effect by taking a parent’s attention away from their child. In several studies, less verbal interaction with children when the TV was on were noted, as was less verbal production by children7,10,13,15. Also, auditive and visual stimulations may increase children’s and parents’ distractions in their family environment and increase the difficulties for a child to extract phonological and syntactical sounds from the background noise at home40,41. In agreement with these elements, we found that increased exposure to TV during family meals at age 2 years was strongly associated with poorer language at age 5–6 years.
Our study has several strengths, including its longitudinal design, large sample size and the availability of a wide range of confounding factors that few past studies were able to account for. Another important strength relies on the use of language tests assessed by trained psychologists; therefore, our findings are unlikely to be affected by social desirability bias arising from parental reporting only. Also, we performed longitudinal analyses of the associations between TV exposure in early childhood and later verbal IQ at age 5–6 years, a method ensuring that a potential cause precedes its potential effect, unlike cross-sectional analyses that have been the most frequent in the literature so far. Finally, we performed a sensitivity analysis with factors identified as potential mediators between TV exposure and language development: the results remained unchanged when including sleep characteristics, which agrees with recent results identifying a main action of sleep quality and quantity on executive functions rather than language development42.
Our study must be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, we could not examine screens other than TV and video games, such as smartphones and tablets, which have become increasingly widespread over the last decade. EDEN children turned 5–6 years between 2008 and 2012, when the market of handheld devices was only emerging and targeted adult users. Research in more recent cohorts is warranted. Second, we were not able to account for the content of children’s TV programs in our analysis. Lacking these data, we attempted to control for program content indirectly by including variables likely to be associated with the types of program a child watches (e.g., parental education, family income, parent–child interaction). Without direct adjustment for TV content programs, residual confounding may remain, however. Third, we measured children’s exposure to screens with parent-reported questionnaires; this method is relatively inaccurate and suffers from social desirability. Future studies need more objective and comprehensive methods for measuring screen use to tackle this limitation43. Finally, the three language assessments we conducted were not directly comparable, which limits our ability to implement models with repeated measures.
Despite 2016 American Academy of Pediatrics’ recommendations5, as well as European scientific academic reports suggesting thresholds on age limits or TV time for children, we lack evidence-based consensus. Families need to be better informed about what activities really promote their children’s healthy neurodevelopment. This work consolidates previous results and adds new elements to support recommendations, especially with regard to the context of TV viewing.
In this analysis, we found no relationship between daily screen time and language development, except cross-sectionally at age 2 years with a U-shaped relationship where children exposed to TV for intermediate times had greater scores. We found, however, consistent negative dose–response associations between frequency of exposure to TV during family meals and language development. Our findings encourage scientists and decision-makers to better consider contextual traits of screen viewing.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives and psychologists who recruited and followed them, and the whole EDEN team, including research scientists, engineers, technicians and managers and especially Josiane Sahuquillo and Edith Lesieux for their commitment and their role in the success of the study. We also acknowledge the commitment of the members of the EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study Group: I Annesi-Maesano, JY Bernard, J Botton, M-A Charles, P Dargent-Molina, B de Lauzon-Guillain, P Ducimetière, M De Agostini, B Foliguet, A Forhan, X Fritel, A Germa, V Goua, R Hankard, B Heude, M Kaminski, B Larroque, N Lelong, J Lepeule, G Magnin, L Marchand, C Nabet, F Pierre, R Slama, M-J Saurel-Cubizolles, M Schweitzer, O Thiebaugeorges. We thank Laura Smales (BioMedEditing) for the English review of the manuscript.
Abbreviations
- CDI
Communicative Development Inventory
- EDEN
Etude des Déterminants pré et postnatals précoces du développement de la santé de l’Enfant [Study of the Early Pre and Postnatal Determinants of Childhood Health Development]
- IQ
Intelligence quotient
Author contributions
M.D.A., M.A.C., S.P. and B.H. designed the study, M.D.A., M.A.C. and B.H. conceptualized the data collection instruments, P.M., J.Y.B. and H.P. contributed to data acquisition under the coordination of A.F., P.M. analysed the data under the supervision of J.Y.B., S.P. and B.H., P.M., J.Y.B., H.P., M.D.A., M.A.C., S.P. and B.H. interpreted the results, P.M. drafted and revised the manuscript under the supervision of J.Y.B., S.P. and B.H. All authors reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content and approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Funding
The EDEN cohort study was supported by: National Institute for Research in Public Health (IRESP TGIR Cohorte Santé 2008 Program); National Agency for Research (ANR nonthematic program); French Speaking Association for the Study of Diabetes and Metabolism (Alfediam); Mutuelle Générale de l’Éducation Nationale; Nestlé; French National Institute for Health Education (INPES); Paris‐Sud University; French National Institute for Population Health Surveillance (InVS); French Agency for Environment Security (AFFSET); French Ministry of Health Perinatal Program; Inserm Nutrition Research Program; Institut Fédératif de Recherche and Cohort Program; French Ministry of Research; EURIP and FIRE doctoral school – Programme Bettencourt; Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (FRM). The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the cohort study, nor in the present work should it be statistical analysis, manuscript preparation and decision to submit it for publication.
Data availability
The datasets generated or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions related to protecting patient confidentiality and legal restrictions imposed by the French National Commission on Data Processing and Liberties (CNIL). Investigators who wish to access the data reported in this article must address a reasonable request to the EDEN steering committee at etude.eden@inserm.fr.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Pauline Martinot and Jonathan Y. Bernard.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Sabine Plancoulaine and Barbara Heude.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-90867-3.
References
- 1.Zimmerman FJ, et al. Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics. 2009;124:342–349. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McKean C, et al. Levers for language growth: Characteristics and predictors of language trajectories between 4 and 7 years. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0134251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rideout, V. The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight, 263–283 (Common Sense Media, San Francisco, CA, 2017).
- 4.Anses. Étude individuelle nationale des consommations alimentaires 3 (INCA 3). Evolution des habitudes et modes de consommation, de nouveaux enjeux en matière de sécurité sanitaire et de nutrition. 566 (Maisons-Alfort, France, 2017).
- 5.Council On Communications and Media Media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20162591–e20162591. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children's performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173:244–250. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174:665–675. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mares M-L, Pan Z. Effects of sesame street: A meta-analysis of children's learning in 15 countries. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2013;34:140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2013.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mendelsohn AL, et al. Do verbal interactions with infants during electronic media exposure mitigate adverse impacts on their language development as toddlers? Infant Child Dev. 2010;19:577–593. doi: 10.1002/icd.711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mendelsohn AL, et al. Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2008;162:411–417. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.5.411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pempek TA, Kirkorian HL, Anderson DR. The effects of background television on the quantity and quality of child-directed speech by parents. J. Child. Media. 2014;8:211–222. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2014.920715. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Waldman, M., Nicholson, S. & Adilov, N. Does television cause autism? (No. w12632). National Bureau of Economic Research (2006).
- 13.Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Murphy LA, Schmidt ME, Anderson DR. The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009;80:1350–1359. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yang-Huang J, et al. Socioeconomic differences in children's television viewing trajectory: A population-based prospective cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0188363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aishworiya R, et al. Television viewing and child cognition in a longitudinal birth cohort in Singapore: The role of maternal factors. BMC Pediatr. 2019;19:286. doi: 10.1186/s12887-019-1651-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97:977–982. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00831.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Anderson DR, Subrahmanyam K, Impacts C, of Digital Media, W. Digital screen media and cognitive development. Pediatrics. 2017;140:57–S61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. J. Pediatr. 2007;151:364–368. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Biddle SJ, Pearson N, Ross GM, Braithwaite R. Tracking of sedentary behaviours of young people: a systematic review. Prev. Med. 2010;51:345–351. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Heude B, et al. Cohort Profile: The EDEN mother-child cohort on the prenatal and early postnatal determinants of child health and development. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2016;45:353–363. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kern, S., Langue, J., Zesiger, P. & Bovet, F. Adaptations françaises des versions courtes des inventaires du développement communicatif de MacArthur-Bates. ANAE22, 217–228 (2010).
- 22.De Agostini M, et al. Batterie d'évaluation du langage oral de l'enfant aphasique (ELOLA): standardisation franqaise (4–12 ans) Rev. Neuropsychol. 1998;8:319–367. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Davis JL, Matthews RN. NEPSY-II review. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2010;28:175–182. doi: 10.1177/0734282909346716. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Peyre H, et al. Predicting changes in language skills between 2 and 3 years in the EDEN mother-child cohort. PeerJ. 2014;2:e335. doi: 10.7717/peerj.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wechsler, D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—fourth edition (The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, 2012).
- 26.Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cogn. Psychol. 2010;61:343–365. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Caldwell, B. M. & Bradley, R. H. Home observation for measurement of the environment (University of Arkansas at Little Rock, Little Rock, 1984).
- 28.Janssen X, et al. Associations of screen time, sedentary time and physical activity with sleep in under 5s: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2020;49:101226. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.101226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Children's television viewing and cognitive outcomes: A longitudinal analysis of national data. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2005;159:619–625. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.7.619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ribner A, Fitzpatrick C, Blair C. Family socioeconomic status moderates associations between television viewing and school readiness skills. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2017;38:233–239. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Khan KS, Purtell KM, Logan J, Ansari A, Justice LM. Association between television viewing and parent-child reading in the early home environment. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2017;38:521–527. doi: 10.1097/dbp.0000000000000465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Taylor G, Monaghan P, Westermann G. Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J. Child. Media. 2017;12:51–65. doi: 10.1080/17482798.2017.1365737. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dore RA, Logan J, Lin TJ, Purtell KM, Justice LM. Associations between children's media use and language and literacy skills. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1734. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Barr R. Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Dev. Perspect. 2013;7:205–210. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:2143–2152. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Branigan HP, McLean JF. What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult–child dialogue. J. Mem. Lang. 2016;91:141–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kostyrka-Allchorne K, Cooper NR, Simpson A. The relationship between television exposure and children’s cognition and behaviour: A systematic review. Dev. Rev. 2017;44:19–58. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Christakis DA, et al. Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2009;163:554–558. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Milteer RM, et al. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond: Focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics. 2012;129:e204–213. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.McMillan BTM, Saffran JR. Learning in complex environments: The effects of background speech on early word learning. Child Dev. 2016;87:1841–1855. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Riley KG, McGregor KK. Noise hampers children's expressive word learning. Lang. Speech Hear Serv. Sch. 2012;43:325–337. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/11-0053). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Plancoulaine S, et al. Early features associated with the neurocognitive development at 36 months of age: The AuBE study. Sleep Med. 2017;30:222–228. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barr R, et al. Beyond screen time: A synergistic approach to a more comprehensive assessment of family media exposure during early childhood. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions related to protecting patient confidentiality and legal restrictions imposed by the French National Commission on Data Processing and Liberties (CNIL). Investigators who wish to access the data reported in this article must address a reasonable request to the EDEN steering committee at etude.eden@inserm.fr.