Abstract
Managing age-related decrease of cognitive function is an important public health challenge, especially in the context of the global aging of the population. Over the last years several Cognitive Mobile Games (CMG) have been developed to train and challenge the brain. However, currently the level of evidence supporting the benefits of using CMG in real-life use is limited in older adults, especially at a late age. In this study we analyzed game scores and the processing speed obtained over the course of 100 sessions in 12,000 subjects aged 60 to over 80 years. Users who trained with the games improved regardless of age in terms of scores and processing speed throughout the 100 sessions, suggesting that old and very old adults can improve their cognitive performance using CMG in real-life use.
Subject terms: Geriatrics, Dementia
Introduction
According to the World Health Organization (WHO), the world population aged over 60 years will have doubled in number by 2050, with an estimated total of 2 billion people1. Clinically, normal healthy aging is associated with some progressive decline in cognitive domains, such as processing speed and executive function. A significant decline in cognitive function, particularly memory, which is an early symptom of dementia, can lead to mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Currently it is estimated that 50 million are living with dementia worldwide and nearly 10 million new cases occur every year, representing a serious public health problem2. As such, the WHO has suggested that preventing cognitive decline and dementia is a global mental health priority. In addition to impacting the patient, dementia also has a significant impact on the family and society in general. The economic cost has been estimated at €232 billion for European countries in 2015 and is expected to double by 20403. Age is the biggest risk factor for the development of dementia4, and aging is associated with a decline of cognitive function5. However, dementia is not considered a normal sequela of aging and prevention should be the key strategy to mitigate the identified risk factors6. Non-pharmacological interventions such as physical exercise and cognitive interventions7,8 may offer an alternative to pharmacological intervention in delaying dementia-related functional decline.
Over the last decade, the accessibility and use of smartphones and mobile internet has quickly expanded around the globe. In parallel to this rapid growth, the industry of mobile apps is exploding. Health-related apps make up an important part of this market, and numerous apps have been developed to ‘train’ cognition and challenge the brain, such as the ‘How Old Is Your Brain’ games developed by Dr Kawashima9 in 2006 which pioneered the arrival of this type of application. Since its release, many studies have been carried out to evaluate the efficacy of cognitive training using commercial or specially-developed applications. In cognitively healthy people aged 65 or older, there was some evidence from the included studies to suggest that 12 or more weeks of computerized cognitive training may improve cognition10, Similar results were found in recent meta-analysis summarizing the efficacy of commercially available cognitive training in the healthy elderly11,12. Finally, in people with MCI, the currently available evidence could not determine whether or not cognitive training would prevent clinical dementia or improve or maintain cognitive function13. The results of these studies suggest that the use of cognitive games could be effective in training cognition if used prior to the onset of dementia. These results were confirmed in a 10-year longitudinal study following 2802 healthy older adults to assess the efficacy of three cognitive training programs (training memory, reasoning, or speed of processing) relative to a control condition. Processing speed training resulted in reduced dementia risk (hazard ratio [HR] 0.71 (95% CI 0.50–0.99), p = 0.049) compared to control, but memory and reasoning training generated no significant risk reduction (HR 0.79 (0.57–1.11), p = 0.18 and HR 0.79 (0.56–1.10), p = 0.16, respectively)14.
Previous studies have suggested a beneficial effect of structured cognitive training using commercially available applications in healthy older adults11,12 however, those were not real-life use, but instead well-controlled studies with standardized training programs (i.e., number of sessions, duration, frequency). Currently, the literature in older adults supporting the benefits of Cognitive Mobile Games (CMG) when used in areal-life use context is still limited. Therefore, the main objective of our study is to add to the current knowledge by evaluating the efficacy of 100 sessions of CMG used in real-life (independently and without specific guidelines on training frequency) in older adults and how the effect may vary as a function of age. Based on the results of one previous study15, we hypothesized that the rate of improvement in CMG performance would be slower in the oldest subjects. Since training frequency was not imposed, the time to perform the 100 sessions may vary between subjects and type of CMG. Therefore, we investigated whether there was an impact of the time needed to perform all the sessions on the performance.
Results
Time needed to perform all sessions
Since no particular guidelines are given in the app regarding the frequency of the training sessions, we first analyzed the number of days needed to reach the 100 sessions for each CMG. Results were right-skewed so we presented the median, p25 and p75 (results according to the age groups are presented in Supplementary Table S1): 503 (230; 750) days for Square Numbers, 614 (407; 810) days for Memory Sweep, 410 (226; 646) days for Word Pairs, 200 (79; 424) days for Babble Bots, 411 (237; 610) days for Must Sort, 411 (233; 616) days for Unique and 472 (297; 657) days for Rush Back. Statistically significant differences were observed for the different CMG (p < 0.001) and within the CMG for the different age groups for Memory Sweep (p = 0.013), Babble Bots (p < 0.001) and Unique (p < 0.001).
We then performed linear regression to determine if the duration of the training has an impact on the progress (expressed in percentage of the progress obtained between the first and the last session, negative coefficient indicates that the progress are lower when the duration of the training increases). For Square Numbers: β = − 0.03%, SE = 0.005, p < 0.001, for Memory Sweep: β = 1.12e−5%, SE = 4.08e−3, p = 0.95, for Word Pairs: β = − 0.17%, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001, for Babble Bots: β = − 0.11%, SE = 0.05, p = 0.016, for Must Sort: β = 1.09%, SE = 0.71, p = 0.12, for Unique: β = − 0.28%, SE = 0.09, p < 0.001 and for Rush Back: β = − 0.14%, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001. Complete results are presented in Supplementary Table S2 and in Supplementary Figs. S1–S7. Since we observed statistically significant differences between age group for the duration and that the duration may influence the progress, we adjusted the analysis of the changes of scores and processing speed by the total duration of the training for each participants and CMG.
CMG scores
First, we analyzed the results of the first session of training to evaluate the influence of age on initial CMG scores. Results are presented in Table 1. We observed a statistically significant linear decrease in scores with increasing participant age in all CMG: Square Numbers (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.016), Memory Sweep (plinear trend < 0.001, ω2 = 0.02), Word Pairs (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.005), Babble Bots (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.012), Must Sort (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.009), Unique (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.007) and Rush Back (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.003).
Table 1.
Age | Cognitive Mobile Games | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Square Numbers | Memory Sweep | Word Pair | Babble Bots | Must Sort | Unique | Rush Back | ||||||||
n | Score | n | Score | n | Score | n | Score | n | Score | n | Score | n | Score | |
60–64 | 4863 | 16,735 [13,910] | 3157 | 31,687 (6260) | 3543 | 2460 [2310] | 1006 | 4530 [7600] | 3538 | 3392 [2019] | 3558 | 3330 [3010] | 3553 | 11,350 [10,550] |
65–69 | 3591 | 16,600 [13,975] | 2833 | 30,872 (6101) | 3559 | 2460 [2095] | 1015 | 4110 [8170] | 3543 | 3205 [2160] | 3569 | 3240 [3560] | 3556 | 10,750 [11,200] |
70–74 | 3312 | 14,485 [11,730] | 1885 | 29,931 (5439) | 3537 | 2460 [2360] | 1012 | 3695 [6738] | 3565 | 3005 [2280] | 3048 | 2930 [3890] | 3549 | 10,300 [11,050] |
75–79 | 1034 | 14,442 [10,968] | 726 | 29,246 (5408) | 1345 | 1960 [2480] | 1004 | 3020 [5772] | 1421 | 2865 [2445] | 1449 | 2910 [4740] | 1330 | 9850 [10,600] |
≥ 80 | 527 | 13,340 [12,205] | 368 | 28,576 (5880) | 723 | 1960 [2680] | 1005 | 3530 [6010] | 848 | 2758 [2742] | 802 | 2610 [4150] | 734 | 10,300 [10,738] |
To analyze the time course of scores during the analysis, the results of the mixed models are presented in Table 2 and Fig. 1. We used likelihood ratio (LR) tests to determine if we needed to use models with or without interaction for each CMG and found significant results for all of them (p < 0.001). Therefore, an important outcome of this analysis is the interaction between training session and participant age group (results of the interactions in Table 2—complete results are available in Supplementary Table S3). As for the initial scores, we observed an interesting linear trend between age and session, indicating that all participants improved in all CMG but that the progress was slower in older participants (p < 0.001 for the 7 CMG).
Table 2.
Age | Cognitive Mobile Games | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Square Numbers | Memory Sweep | Word Pair | Babble Bots | Must Sort | Unique | Rush Back | |
60–64 | 108 (0.6) | 76 (0.3) | 143 (0.5) | 61 (0.9) | 172 (0.4) | 148 (0.4) | 101 (0.3) |
65–69 | 89 (0.7) | 63 (0.3) | 130 (0.5) | 61 (0.9) | 131 (0.4) | 123 (0.4) | 87 (0.2) |
70–74 | 75 (0.9) | 54 (0.4) | 114 (0.5) | 53 (0.9) | 96 (0.5) | 100 (0.4) | 78 (0.3) |
75–79 | 63 (1.3) | 45 (0.6) | 93 (0.8) | 48 (0.9) | 69 (0.8) | 80 (0.6) | 73 (0.4) |
≥ 80 | 61 (1.8) | 38 (0.8) | 88 (1.1) | 50 (0.9) | 87 (1) | 81 (0.8) | 68 (0.6) |
p-value | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Models are adjusted for the total duration of the training (Supplementary Table S1).
Processing speed
As for the scores, first, we analyzed the results of the first session of training to evaluate the influence of age on initial CMG scores. Results are presented in Table 3. We observed a statistically significant linear increase in processing speed for the different CMG: Square Numbers (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.16), Word Pairs (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.09), Must Sort (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.32), Unique (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.17) and Rush Back (plinear trend < 0.001, ε2 = 0.16).
Table 3.
Age | Cognitive Mobile Games | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Square Numbers | Word Pair | Must Sort | Unique | Rush Back | |
60–64 | 4371 [2391] | 7478 [2059] | 571 [240] | 4121 [1472] | 1175 [650] |
65–69 | 4478 [2404] | 7592 [2489] | 609 [264] | 4238 [1662] | 1182 [685] |
70–74 | 4450 [2158] | 7775 [2852] | 637 [283] | 4396 [1685] | 1226 [700] |
75–79 | 5144 [2460] | 8076 [2892] | 665 [309] | 4547 [2103] | 1249 [695] |
≥ 80 | 5811 [2505] | 8086 [2880] | 669 [380] | 4643 [2459] | 1215 [742] |
When then evaluated the progress of the processing speed, the increase in game difficulty throughout the sessions must be taken into account. The time course of the processing speed over the 100 sessions, adjusted for difficulty levels and the total duration of the training, are presented in Fig. 2. The results of the mixed models are presented in Table 4. For Word Pairs we observed an decrease in the processing speed in all age groups. For Square Numbers, Unique and Rush Back there was a statistically significant increase for all participant age groups, however as for the score, the increase of the processing speed is more marked for younger participants. For Must Sort, only participants aged 70 and over presented a decrease in processing speed while the other age groups presented a slight increase, but in both cases the magnitude was relatively small (between + 0.09 ms [95% CI 0.07; 0.10]/session for the 60–64 age group and − 0.10 ms [95% CI − 0.14; − 0.06]/session for the 80+ group).
Table 4.
Age | Cognitive Mobile Games | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Square Numbers | Word Pair | Must Sort | Unique | Rush Back | |
60–64 | − 3.12 [− 3.29; − 2.95] | 3.60 [3.53; 3.65] | 0.09 [0.07; 0.10] | − 7.0 [− 7.11; − 6.97] | − 2.51 [− 2.53; − 2.48 |
65–69 | − 2.71 [− 2.90; − 2.52] | 3.46 [3.41; 3.51] | 0.04 [0.03; 0.05] | − 5.93 [− 6.01; − 5.87] | − 2.22 [− 2.24; − 2.20] |
70–74 | − 2.81 [− 3.20; − 2.41] | 3.11 [3.06; 3.16] | − 0.05 [− 0.06; − 0.04] | − 4.98 [− 5.06; − 4.91] | − 2.07 [− 2.09; − 2.05] |
75–79 | − 2.59 [− 2.89; − 2.29] | 2.38 [2.29; 2.47] | − 0.06 [− 0.08 ; − 0.04] | − 4.31 [− 4.42; − 4.20] | − 1.96 [− 1.99; − 1.92] |
≥ 80 | − 0.68 [− 0.89; − 0.48] | 2.49 [2.36; 2.63] | − 0.10 [− 0.14 ; − 0.06] | − 3.68 [− 3.84; − 3.51] | − 1.77 [− 1.82; − 1.71] |
p-value | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Models are adjusted for the difficulty levels and the total duration of the training (Supplementary Table S1).
Discussion
This study aimed to determine the efficacy of a cognitive training performed using CMG in real-life use on cognitive performance in older adults. First, we compared the baseline game scores per age group and observed that outcomes are sensitive to age-related cognitive changes, which is in line with the results of a previous study, where we showed that CMG scores are correlated with the cognitive abilities of older adults with and without cognitive impairments16.
When investigating the scores of the CMG, we observed statistically significant linear decreases with the increasing age of the participants, and conversely, a significant decrease in processing speed. These results are in accordance with neuropsychological and physiological data: aging is indeed related to a decrease in cognitive function17 and an increase in reaction time18. This observation supports that our outcomes are sensitive to age-related changes in cognitive function.
The literature also supports that basic numerical skills are preserved in healthy aging19 and that deficits may be associated with MCI20. The age-related differences in baseline scores we observed in Square Numbers are therefore probably not related to a decrease of numerical skills but may be explained by slowed reaction times and inhibiting abilities, both of which are known to be affected by aging21. Our study did not measure inhibitory processes directly, but Must Sort may be considered an indirect measure of inhibitory response. In Must Sort, we observed a linear decrease in scores as well as an decrease in processing speed with increasing participant age, both results are consistent with the aforementioned study21 and could explain why we observed age-dependent differences in baseline Square Numbers scores.
Though the changes in different cognitive abilities over the lifespan are relatively well-documented22,23, there is less evidence on the plasticity of these different cognitive functions across the lifespan23–25. Furthermore, it has not yet been established whether all cognitive functions can be trained or the extent to which progress can be achieved in healthy subjects of different ages26. These are both important questions in the field of cognitive training. Neuroplasticity is the ability of the brain to modify its structure and function for example under conditions of learning or compensation. We studied a healthy population and therefore the observed improvements are most likely due to training-induced plasticity rather than compensation. Previous studies have shown neuroimaging and neurotransmitter changes after cognitive training of working memory in healthy people27–29, that could ultimately lead to an increase of cognitive reserve30. However, it is possible given the age of the subjects that this may be a compensatory mechanism. For example the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition provides a theoretical model for the causes and the consequences of age-related compensatory neural activity31. According to this theory, scaffolding is conceptualized as the recruitment of additional circuitry that shores up declining brain function that has become inefficient. Despite the age-related alteration in different important brain structures (i.e., declining activity in the hippocampus, poor modulation of default network activity, amyloid deposition)11,32. Cognitive training or sustained engagement in challenging novel tasks like CMGs could enhance the development of scaffolding and as a result, confer protection and improvement in cognitive functions33.
We observed a clear linear trend for the analysis of the initial score, the same tendency was found for the time course of the scores, where all progress were smaller with increasing age. Those results confirm that even if the age-related cognitive decline is inevitable, lifelong trajectories of brain and cognitive functions are variable and stay plastic throughout the lifespan34.
For the next part of discussion, we will address the effect of training on each cognitive domain (see Table 5 for the different cognitive abilities trained by the CMG) in turn. Note that each CMG may train different cognitive abilities but for the sake of this discussion, we define the main component of cognition for each CMG.
Table 5.
CMG | Instruction | Main cognitive abilities trained | Time per CMG (s) | Total training time (min) | Scoring system | Processing speed |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Square Numbers | Match the target shown on top by adding two or more number blocks |
Quantitative reasoning Arithmetic Working memory |
70 | 117 |
Base score for each correct answer with a speed related bonus (50 − (elapsed seconds × 5)) Streak up after 4 correct rounds, down after 5 incorrect |
The processing speed is measured as the inverse of the average time to perform the different calculations |
Memory Sweep | Memorize the positions of the highlighted tiles and remember their positions when gone |
Attention Spatial memory Working memory |
90 | 150 |
Points for each correct square (250) plus a base score for complete round Streak up (bonus) after 1 correct answer, down after 1 incorrect move |
NA |
Word Pair | Pair words according to a specific rule presented (similar, opposite) |
Semantic access Vocabulary |
90 | 150 | Base score for correct round, streak up after 2 correct answers, down after 2 incorrect | The processing speed is measured as the inverse of the average time to pair the different words |
Babble Bots | Create the maximal number of words of at least 3 letters with the 6 available letters |
Word fluency Vocabulary |
60 | 100 | Points for letters in word multiplied by the word length, as the streak multiplier. Letter scores are localised to the region based on the Scrabble scoring system | NA |
Must Sort | Sort the items correctly by tapping on the correct side |
Response control Task shifting |
45 | 75 | Base score multiplied by streak multiplier, streak is incremented by correct answers and is reduced to 1 on incorrect or more than 5 s between answers | The processing speed is measured as the inverse of the average time between when the card appears and taping on the screen |
Unique | Find the odd one out and tap on it |
Visual attention Visual recognition |
70 | 117 |
Baseline score per correct answer based on difficulty level. Delta is added to the baseline and becomes larger with consecutive correct answers (Baseline + (streak × delta)). Streak of 8 correct up, 6 down |
The processing speed is measured as the inverse of the average time between when the objects appear and the discovery of the unique object |
Rush Back | Memorize a shape, then decide if the next shape matches the one memorized |
Sustained attention Visual recognition Working memory |
45 | 75 |
One base score per difficulty level with a multiplier which goes up and down based on streak Streak up of 4 correct in a row but not changed during game play Bonus for end of game, current streak multiplier × bonus |
The processing speed is measured as the inverse of the average time between when the card appears and the classification |
Arithmetic ability: Square Numbers
We observed a small but significant decrease in processing speed in Square Numbers over the course of the 100 sessions, even in the 80+ age group. The processing speed increases during the first 50 sessions then remain stable while the score of the games is continuously increasing, this seems to indicate that the speed is no longer decreasing but the participants are able to perform more complex tasks. There are, to the authors’ knowledge, no existing studies assessing the evolution of processing speed during arithmetic training in the older adults, with previous studies only investigating these outcomes in primary school students35 or young adults36. In both studies, the authors observed an improvement in subjects’ arithmetic abilities as well as processing speed. The results of the present study are consistent with these results and extend them to older adults.
Word processing: Word Pairs
We observed an increase in Word Pairs scores throughout the sessions in every age group. This increase was greater for the younger participants. Word processing and literacy engagement along adulthood enable to maintain an efficient lexical processing37, which is reflected by the evolution of the scores observed in the current study indicating that semantic learning abilities are preserved even at advanced ages. However, concerning the processing speed, even after adjusting for the difficulty level, we observed an increase in all age groups during the training. Word Pairs and Babble Bot are the only two CMG using retrieval from long-term memory. Participants tended to recall common, more easily accessible items before unique, less accessible items, and this pattern was more prominent in older adults38. The words to pair become more difficult and less common as the training progresses, which may explain why, despite the adjustment, the time needed to associate these words increases significantly in the different age groups.
Response control and task-shifting: Must Sort
It has been demonstrated that older adults experience more difficulties in task switching, coupled with infrequent and unexpected transitions from one task set to another39. Despite the highest costs to task shifting performance40, we observed that older participants were able to train this function, as exhibited by their significant improvements in processing speed. One potential mechanism that could explain this is a shift in cognitive control. Previous neuroimaging studies have indeed shown that older adults may switch from a proactive (e.g., anticipation) to reactive cognitive control strategy (e.g., late-correction mechanism) as a means of retaining relatively preserved behavioral performance in the face of age-related neurocognitive changes41. In the Must Sort, reactive control strategy is the most used mechanism.
Visual attention: Unique
With regard to visual attention, it is widely accepted that aging is associated with the deterioration of vision and field of view42, and with a decrease in selective attention43. We observed that the time needed to find the unique object decreased in all age groups over time, which may indicate that this CMG is able to improve selective attention in older adults, or at least improve response speed, which is a good indicator of cognitive function44. These results are in line with a previous study that showed that processing speed training improves selective attention in older adults45.
Working memory: Rush Back
Similarly to other CMGs, scores and reaction time of Rush Back, which mainly trains working memory, were improved in all age groups with a slower progression in the older groups. It has been demonstrated that older adults can improve their working memory after a specific training46. In another study the investigators analyzed the effect of a 20-session training program using an n-back task program (same principle as the Rush Back where the subjects must remember the previous card) in younger, middle-aged and older adults47. The authors found that age exerted independent effects on training gains and asymptotic performance: older adults tended to show less improvement in scores than younger adults47, which is also consistent with our findings.
There are three main limitations in this study: the first is that we did not have access to any information about the background of the participants: it is well-known that several factors influence cognitive function and the risk of dementia such as genetic risk factors48, as well as non-genetic risk factors including lifestyle-related factors49, for example education level, smoking history, history of hypertension, dyslipidemia, physical activity, body mass index, or concomitant pathologies such as stroke50, cardiovascular disease51, diabetes52, or chronic respiratory disease53. Gender is also postulated to influence some cognitive functions such as vocabulary capacity54. Due to the fact that we did not have access to this background information, we cannot establish whether the effects observed in the current study were influenced by any of these factors. Most probably, subjects playing with this kind of app are cognitively healthy and quite comfortable with mobile devices.
The second limitation is the choice of the outcomes, namely, the scores of the CMG and the processing speed data obtained within the games. It could be argued that traditional clinical scales or scores would have been more effective in evaluating subjects’ cognitive functioning; however, using the scores of the games to assess the course of the performance has been used in a previous study55. Furthermore, both of the scores of the CMG and processing speed have been shown to be good indicators of cognitive function16,19. In a recent study examining the effects of cognitive training on cognitive performance of healthy adults, the authors found that there was a transfer effect between the trained abilities and the instruments used only when the tests were similar to the trained situation (near effects). If the tests differed too much from the training tasks (far effects) no training effect was observed56. Therefore, using the scores of the games and the reaction times could be considered as a near-effect instrument/measure and quantification of the real transfer to daily activities is still needed. However, some studies did show a transfer to general cognitive function as tested byneuropsychological batteries for multiple cognitive domains11,16 and also demonstrated a protective effect in patients with MCI57. Those beneficial effects could be related to the multi-domains, novel and continuously challenging (self-adaptative) stimulation provided by most cognitive training apps, which has been shown to be superior to the routine mental activities of everyday life11. These challenging and unusual stimuli induce changes in brain activity and connectivity in areas that are known to be affected by aging and neurodegenerative diseases. Those changes may help counteract age- and disease-related alterations and help to explain cognitive benefits and transfers, once their link with cognitive improvements has been clearly established33,57.
Finally, the study suffers a selection bias, since the participants were all users of this app and were therefore most probably familiar with the use of smartphones and current technology. This has two consequences: first, older people who are less familiar with mobile technology might find this app less usable and therefore the adherence may be lower. Secondly, a recent study underlined the importance of digital devices use in delaying cognitive decline in the older adults58, thus the participants of this study may have already been benefiting from this phenomenon and thus functioning at a higher cognitive level than those who do not regularly use mobile technology. Despite these limitations, the results of this study support that even at old age (above 80 years old), participants are able to use CMG and to train and improve cognition through CMG.
Although technological devices and medical-related apps cannot single-handedly improve cognitive decline, in the absence of effective, low-cost, and accessible treatments for cognitive and motivational deficits, these brain training apps could be greatly beneficial to public health. One salient aspect of the games is that they could be combined with automated evaluation and assessment of cognitive function16,59. Therefore, we strongly suggest that the evaluation of cognitive function for long-term follow-up should not be restricted to cross-sectional measurement (typically done only once per year) but should also include longitudinal measurement to evaluate subjects’ learning abilities or cognitive abilities in general if there is no training in between the assessments15. In this context, the presented method could be an interesting complementary tool due to its potential to become widely available thanks to the growing use of mobile technology. Another positive aspect is that the cognitive training and follow-up with games on mobile can be also proposed to patients with limited mobility, or living to far to come on a regular basis to specialized centers60, and in lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic61,62.
While cognitive training app games have been shown to improve memory in older people with mild cognitive impairment63, further studies are needed to determine if technologies, such as apps, can decrease dementia risk in healthy subjects or slow down the progression of the disease in patients suffering from cognitive impairment and if there is a transfer to the activities of daily living. We can, also, speculate that since psychomotor slowing associated with aging has an important negative effect on multi-tasking activities of daily living, improving the processing speed could have a positive effect on the quality of life of the participants64.
Methods
Study design and participants
We carried out a retrospective observational study in which we obtained anonymized CMG results of healthy participants. This study was approved by The Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics Committee (Pre.2020.28) and research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines, and informed consent was obtained from the participants and they agreed that their data could be used for research purposes when installing the app. The scores of the CMG, automatically recorded by the application, were then analysed anonymously for each of the five age groups provided: 60–64, 65–69, 70–74, 75–79, and 80 years or older. The number of participants varied in each CMG and in the different age groups (Table 1).
Procedures
In this study, we used a set of seven individual short CMG provided by Peak brain training (www.peak.net, London—UK) to analyze changes in-game scores and processing speed over the course of 100 sessions of CMG (one session is defined as the completion of one level of the CMG). The games are organized by categories based on the main cognitive functions on which they focus. The seven CMG were selected based on a previous study that identified correlations between CMG scores and scores in two clinically-established cognitive assessments (the Mini-Mental State Examination and Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Evaluation) in older subjects with and without cognitive impairments16.
Screenshots of the games are presented in Fig. 3, and games description and main cognitive abilities trained in Table 5. The difficulty level of each CMG is adapted automatically according to the previous performance of the participant (i.e., rate of correct responses and reaction time). The number of stimuli and the intersimulus intervals depend on the CMG and the difficulty level The CMG were played on smartphones or tablets and the scores of 100 training sessions were analyzed. No particular instructions were given to the participants about the frequency or the duration of each training session, the total duration needed to achieve the 100 sessions of training for the different CMG is presented in Supplementary Table S1.
Outcomes
The primary outcome was the scores obtained in the seven CMG for the different age groups. Several cognitive sub-functions are usually assessed during standard cognitive evaluations: attention, memory, fluency, language, and visuospatial abilities (Table 5)65. To have a complete overview of the cognition, those different sub-functions need to be assessed individually; the scores of the CMG are used as a proxy of the main sub-cognitive abilities challenged in each game.
As a second primary outcome, we computed the processing speed based on the reaction time for the speed-dependent CMG (exceptions were Memory Sweep and Word Pairs)66. Details of the computations are presented in Table 4. Processing speed is considered as a good indicator of general cognitive performance19 and has been proposed as a predictor of frailty risk among people in old age67,68.
Statistical analysis
Two different kinds of analyses were performed using the CMG score data:
Firstly, the first session scores of the different age groups were compared using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Kruskal–Wallis tests, depending the distribution of the data, to determine if age had an influence on the initial scores. Omega-squared analyses or epsilon-squared (non-normally distributed) tests were computed to estimate the effect size69. Post-hoc tests for linear trends were performed last.
We then analysed each CMG using a separate mixed model with random slope (age) and intercept with the scores from each session treated as repeated measures adjusted for the total duration of the training for each participant. Fixed effects of age group, session (1 to 100), and the interaction between age group and session were specified, and the estimated baseline measures were constrained to be identical in the age groups by subtracting the mean values of the first session for each age group in all the sessions.
This approach is equivalent to adjusting for baseline and permitting the relationship between baseline and follow-up scores to differ at each session.
with α and β representing fixed effect, random error and the measure of the random effect. Likelihood-ratio tests were used to test the significance of the random effects model and linear mixed model with interaction.
For the processing speed, we applied a separate mixed model for the different CMG with random slope (age) and intercept with the processing speed from each session treated as repeated measures, adjusted for the difficulty levels reached and the total duration of the training for each participant.
Statistical analyses were performed at an overall significance level of 0.05, carried out in RStudio (version 1.1.442), using R version 3.4.470.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Prof. Bruno Bonnechère was funded by the Fondation Wiener-Anspach ; Dr. Christelle Langley was funded by the Wellcome Trust Collaborative Award 200181/Z/15/Z; Prof. Barbara J Sahakian receives funding from the Wallitt Foundation and Eton College and research is conducted within the NIHR MedTech and in vitro diagnostic Co-operative (MIC) and the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) Mental Health and Neurodegeneration Themes.
Author contributions
The study was conceived by B.B. B.B. performed the analysis. B.B., M.K., C.L. and B.J.S. did the data interpretation and contributed to the writing.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
Prof. Bonnechère, Dr. Langley and Prof. Klass have nothing to disclose. Prof. Sahakian consults for Cambridge Cognition, Greenfield BioVentures and Cassava Sciences. She receives funds from Cambridge Enterprise for Technology Transfer of Wizard and Decoder to Brainbow and Peak.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-91867-z.
References
- 1.WHO . Ageing and Health. WHO; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 2.World Health Organization . Dementia. WHO; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cimler R, Maresova P, Kuhnova J, Kuca K. Predictions of Alzheimer’s disease treatment and care costs in European countries. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0210958. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cleveland ML. Preserving cognition, preventing dementia. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2020;36:585–599. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2020.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lamar M, Boots EA, Arfanakis K, Barnes LL, Schneider JA. Common brain structural alterations associated with cardiovascular disease risk factors and Alzheimer’s dementia: Future directions and implications. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09460-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Livingston G, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. The Lancet. 2020;396:413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dyer SM, Harrison SL, Laver K, Whitehead C, Crotty M. An overview of systematic reviews of pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions for the treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2018;30:295–309. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217002344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Abraha I, et al. Systematic review of systematic reviews of non-pharmacological interventions to treat behavioural disturbances in older patients with dementia. The SENATOR-OnTop series. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e012759. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nouchi R, et al. Brain training game boosts executive functions, working memory and processing speed in the young adults: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e55518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gates NJ, et al. Computerised cognitive training for 12 or more weeks for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2020 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Shah TM, Weinborn M, Verdile G, Sohrabi HR, Martins RN. Enhancing cognitive functioning in healthly older adults: A systematic review of the clinical significance of commercially available computerized cognitive training in preventing cognitive decline. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2017;27:62–80. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9338-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bonnechère B, Langley C, Sahakian BJ. The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: A meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72281-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gates NJ, et al. Computerised cognitive training for preventing dementia in people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019;3:CD012279. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012279.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Edwards JD, et al. Speed of processing training results in lower risk of dementia. Alzheimers Dement (N. Y.) 2017;3:603–611. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bonnechère B, et al. Age-associated capacity to progress when playing Cognitive Mobile Games: Ecological retrospective observational study. JMIR Serious Games. 2020;8:e17121. doi: 10.2196/17121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bonnechère B, et al. The use of mobile games to assess cognitive function of elderly with and without cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2018;64:1285–1293. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bettio LEB, Rajendran L, Gil-Mohapel J. The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;79:66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Norris JE, McGeown WJ, Guerrini C, Castronovo J. Aging and the number sense: Preserved basic non-symbolic numerical processing and enhanced basic symbolic processing. Front. Psychol. 2015 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Johari K, den Ouden D-B, Behroozmand R. Effects of aging on temporal predictive mechanisms of speech and hand motor reaction time. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2018;30:1195–1202. doi: 10.1007/s40520-018-0902-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Martin RC, et al. Loss of calculation abilities in patients with mild and moderate Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2003;60:1585. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.11.1585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cappelletti M, Didino D, Stoianov I, Zorzi M. Number skills are maintained in healthy ageing. Cogn. Psychol. 2014;69:25–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Vogel A, Salem LC, Andersen BB, Waldemar G. Differences in quantitative methods for measuring subjective cognitive decline—Results from a prospective memory clinic study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2016;28:1513–1520. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216000272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rizeq J, Flora DB, Toplak ME. Changing relations among cognitive abilities across development: Implications for measurement and research. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2017;31:1353–1374. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2017.1317034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Li T, et al. Cognitive training can reduce the rate of cognitive aging: A neuroimaging cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2016;16:12. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0194-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.van de Vijver I, Ridderinkhof KR, de Wit S. Age-related changes in deterministic learning from positive versus negative performance feedback. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2015;22:595–619. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1020917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Raz N. Decline and compensation in aging brain and cognition: Promises and constraints. Preface. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2009;19:411–414. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9122-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McNab F, et al. Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science. 2009;323:800–802. doi: 10.1126/science.1166102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:75–79. doi: 10.1038/nn1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2010;14:317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Orrell M, Sahakian B. Education and dementia. BMJ. 1995;310:951–952. doi: 10.1136/bmj.310.6985.951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009;60:173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Park DC, Bischof GN. The aging mind: Neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2013;15:109–119. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.1/dpark. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.van Balkom TD, van den Heuvel OA, Berendse HW, van der Werf YD, Vriend C. The effects of cognitive training on brain network activity and connectivity in aging and neurodegenerative diseases: A systematic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020;30:267–286. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09440-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mitchell MB, et al. Cognitively stimulating activities: Effects on cognition across four studies with up to 21 years of longitudinal data. J. Aging Res. 2012;2012:461592. doi: 10.1155/2012/461592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Caviola S, Gerotto G, Mammarella IC. Computer-based training for improving mental calculation in third- and fifth-graders. Acta Physiol. (Oxf) 2016;171:118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Takeuchi H, et al. Working Memory training using mental calculation impacts regional gray matter of the frontal and parietal regions. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e23175. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Steen-Baker AA, et al. The effects of context on processing words during sentence reading among adults varying in age and literacy skill. Psychol. Aging. 2017;32:460–472. doi: 10.1037/pag0000184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Murphy DH, Castel AD. Age-related similarities and differences in the components of semantic fluency: Analyzing the originality and organization of retrieval from long-term memory. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2020 doi: 10.1080/13825585.2020.1817844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Eich TS, MacKay-Brandt A, Stern Y, Gopher D. Age-based differences in task switching are moderated by executive control demands. GERONB. 2016 doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Eich TS, et al. Functional brain and age-related changes associated with congruency in task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2016;91:211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jimura K, Braver TS. Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:1420–1431. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Matthews K, Nazroo J, Whillans J. The consequences of self-reported vision change in later-life: Evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Public Health. 2017;142:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2016.09.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Malavita MS, Vidyasagar TR, McKendrick AM. The effect of aging and attention on visual crowding and surround suppression of perceived contrast threshold. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2017;58:860. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-20632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nyberg L, et al. Forecasting memory function in aging: Pattern-completion ability and hippocampal activity relate to visuospatial functioning over 25 years. Neurobiol. Aging. 2020;94:217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.O’Brien JL, et al. Cognitive training and selective attention in the aging brain: An electrophysiological study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013;124:2198–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mishra J, Rolle C, Gazzaley A. Neural plasticity underlying visual perceptual learning in aging. Brain Res. 2015;1612:140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rhodes RE, Katz B. Working memory plasticity and aging. Psychol. Aging. 2017;32:51–59. doi: 10.1037/pag0000135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.van der Lee SJ, et al. The effect of APOE and other common genetic variants on the onset of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: A community-based cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:434–444. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30053-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Livingston G, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390:2673–2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Zheng F, Yan L, Zhong B, Yang Z, Xie W. Progression of cognitive decline before and after incident stroke. Neurology. 2019;93:e20–e28. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stefanidis KB, Askew CD, Greaves K, Summers MJ. The effect of non-stroke cardiovascular disease states on risk for cognitive decline and dementia: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2018;28:1–15. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9359-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Li C-I, et al. Risk score prediction model for dementia in patients with type 2 diabetes. Eur. J. Neurol. 2018;25:976–983. doi: 10.1111/ene.13642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Zhang X, et al. Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease as a risk factor for cognitive dysfunction: A meta-analysis of current studies. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52:101–111. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ardila A, Rosselli M. Spontaneous language production and aging: Sex and educational effects. Int. J. Neurosci. 1996;87:71–78. doi: 10.3109/00207459608990754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Anguera JA, et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature. 2013;501:97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Golino MTS, Flores Mendoza C, Golino HF. Effects of cognitive training on cognitive performance of healthy older adults. Span. J. Psychol. 2017;20:E39. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Li B-Y, et al. Computerized cognitive training for Chinese mild cognitive impairment patients: A neuropsychological and fMRI study. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101691. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jin Y, Jing M, Ma X. Effects of digital device ownership on cognitive decline in a middle-aged and elderly population: Longitudinal observational study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2019;21:e14210. doi: 10.2196/14210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bonnechère B, et al. Automated functional upper limb evaluation of patients with Friedreich ataxia using serious games rehabilitation exercises. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2018;15:87. doi: 10.1186/s12984-018-0430-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.van der Kolk NM, et al. Effectiveness of home-based and remotely supervised aerobic exercise in Parkinson’s disease: A double-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:998–1008. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30285-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Geddes MR, et al. Remote cognitive and behavioral assessment: Report of the Alzheimer Society of Canada Task Force on dementia care best practices for COVID-19. Alzheimer’s Dementia (Amsterdam) 2020;12:e12111. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Vatansever D, Wang S, Sahakian BJ. Covid-19 and promising solutions to combat symptoms of stress, anxiety and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00791-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Savulich G, et al. Cognitive training using a novel Memory Game on an iPad in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;20:624–633. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wolinsky FD, Jones MP, Dotson MM. Does visual speed of processing training improve health-related quality of life in assisted and independent living communities?: A randomized controlled trial. Innov. Aging. 2020;4:029. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igaa029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Koo BM, Vizer LM. Mobile technology for cognitive assessment of older adults: A scoping review. Innov. Aging. 2019;3:038. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igy038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wainer H. Speed vs reaction time as a measure of cognitive performance. Mem. Cognit. 1977;5:278–280. doi: 10.3758/BF03197375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kochan NA, et al. Is intraindividual reaction time variability an independent cognitive predictor of mortality in old age? Findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0181719. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Amieva H, Meillon C, Proust-Lima C, Dartigues JF. Is low psychomotor speed a marker of brain vulnerability in late life? Digit symbol substitution test in the prediction of Alzheimer, Parkinson, stroke, disability, and depression. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2019;47:297–305. doi: 10.1159/000500597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Albers C, Lakens D. When power analyses based on pilot data are biased: Inaccurate effect size estimators and follow-up bias. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018;74:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (2019).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.