Abstract
Toxoplasma gondii is hypothesized to manipulate the behavior of warm-blooded hosts to promote trophic transmission into the parasite’s definitive feline hosts. A key prediction of this hypothesis is that T. gondii infections of non-feline hosts are associated with costly behavior toward T. gondii’s definitive hosts; however, this effect has not been documented in any of the parasite’s diverse wild hosts during naturally occurring interactions with felines. Here, three decades of field observations reveal that T. gondii-infected hyena cubs approach lions more closely than uninfected peers and have higher rates of lion mortality. We discuss these results in light of 1) the possibility that hyena boldness represents an extended phenotype of the parasite, and 2) alternative scenarios in which T. gondii has not undergone selection to manipulate behavior in host hyenas. Both cases remain plausible and have important ramifications for T. gondii’s impacts on host behavior and fitness in the wild.
Subject terms: Behavioural ecology, Evolutionary ecology, Coevolution, Animal behaviour
The parasite causing toxoplasmosis can manipulate prey to behave in ways that promote transmission to the parasite’s definitive feline hosts. The first study consistent with this extended phenotype in the wild finds that infected hyena cubs approach lions more closely than uninfected peers and have higher rates of lion mortality.
Introduction
Toxoplasma gondii provides an infamous example of putative host-manipulation by a parasite1. Parasite transmission can occur by ingestion of T. gondii oocysts shed from felids (the definitive host), consumption of infected tissue from intermediate hosts, and congenital infection2,3. Across this protist’s diverse array of warm-blooded hosts, infections are linked to reduced avoidance of, or even attraction to, the odor of feline urine4–6. The parasite’s hypothesized ability to alter host responses to indirect cues of feline presence is thought to have evolved by natural selection on the parasite to increase trophic (prey to predator) transmission. This could benefit T. gondii since the parasite undergoes sexual reproduction within definitive feline hosts to produce recombinant, environmentally stable propagules called oocysts7,8. T. gondii also induces other potentially manipulative behaviors in intermediate hosts, including behavioral boldness9. If this boldness results in lethal contact with felines, it could similarly promote trophic transmission of T. gondii at the expense of intermediate hosts’ fitness in nature10.
While T. gondii is among the best-studied putative host manipulators, and also causes substantial disease burden in human hosts7,11, its effects on host behavior have overwhelmingly been studied in laboratory animals and humans. A smaller body of research from nature, where T. gondii co-evolves with intermediate and definitive hosts, suggests that infection-related behavior might decrease host fitness12,13. For example, wild-caught rodents harboring naturally occurring infections exhibit reduced avoidance of odor cues from local felids14, as well as elevated activity15, reduced neophobia16, and higher rates of capture in human traps in captive and semi-captive settings16. In wild sea otters, infections are also associated with both neuropathy and shark predation17. Yet, no prior study has examined the relationship between T. gondii infections in wild hosts and naturally occurring interactions involving the parasite’s definitive felid hosts.
In this study, we adapt the spotted hyena system to better understand the links between T. gondii infection and fitness-related behavior in free-living hosts towards felids (While the design and objectives for this body of work were presented along with preliminary findings in a non-peer-reviewed Festschrift volume18, the present manuscript includes more rigorous models that include additional candidate covariates to arrive at somewhat modified conclusions. We also present novel syntheses of our research findings in light of existing literature and point to important next steps in studying putative parasitic manipulation in wild hosts). More specifically, we used blood samples and detailed field observations spanning three decades to accomplish three goals: 1) identify demographic, social, and ecological determinants of T. gondii infection in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta); 2) test whether T. gondii-infected hyenas exhibit greater behavioral boldness in the presence of lions, and 3) test whether T. gondii-infected hyenas have higher rates of lion-inflicted mortality. Our data were collected in a natural setting in Kenya where hyenas frequently interact with lions (Panthera leo), which are not only definitive T. gondii hosts11 but are also the leading cause of hyena injuries and mortality19,20. Despite clear risk, hyenas engage with lions to defend territories, protect relatives, and/or compete for food. Tension between the benefits and costs of these interactions likely explain findings of stabilizing selection on hyena boldness toward lions, favoring individuals with intermediate phenotypes21. This study system permits us to characterize relationships among T. gondii infection and naturally occurring behaviors that have fitness consequences.
Here, we show that wild hyena cubs infected with the parasite T. gondii exhibit costly behavioral boldness when interacting with lions and that infected cubs experience a higher probability of lion mortality than their uninfected group mates. Furthermore, our results indicate that T. gondii infection prevalence is age structured with older animals being more likely to be infected.
Results
One hundred and eight (108) of 166 surveyed hyenas (65%) had IgG antibodies to T. gondii, indicating prior exposure to the parasite. Thirty-seven individuals (22%) tested negative, and 21 hyenas (13%) yielded results within the “doubtful” range of the assay (Supplementary Fig. 1). In keeping with prior studies22, we combined individuals with negative and doubtful diagnoses into a single category, treating them as uninfected in the analysis. A subset of 60 plasma samples were also tested using IFAT diagnostics to confirm consistency between ELISA and IFAT (Supplementary Fig. 2; Pearson’s r(58) = 0.70, p < 0.001).
Table 1 shows the distribution of T. gondii infection prevalence across demographic, social and ecological variables (sex, age, dominance rank, and livestock density). We observed no differences in infection prevalence between male vs. female hyenas (61% vs. 68%; P = 0.40). Hyena cubs (35% infected) had lower infection prevalence than subadults (71%) and adults (80%; overall P-difference <0.001). Dominance rank was not associated with the probability of being infected (P = 0.95). Hyenas sampled in areas of high livestock density did not differ in their infection prevalence (76% vs. 62%, P = 0.15) compared to hyenas from areas of low livestock density.
Table 1.
% (N) | OR (95% CI) infected vs. uninfected | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Uninfected | Infected | Unadjusteda | Adjustedb | |
n = 58 | n = 108 | |||
Sex | ||||
Female | 32% (31) | 68% (65) | 1.00 (Reference) | 1.00 (Reference) |
Male | 39% (27) | 61% (43) | 0.76 (0.40, 1.45) | 0.70 (0.33, 1.47) |
Age at diagnosisc | ||||
Cub (<12 mos) | 65% (32) | 35% (17) | 1.00 (Reference) | 1.00 (Reference) |
Subadults (12−24 mos) | 29% (10) | 71% (25) | 4.71 (1.88, 12.49)f | 5.05 (1.80, 15.17)f |
Adult (>24 mos) | 20% (16) | 80% (66) | 7.76 (3.55, 17.80)f | 8.11 (3.59, 19.32)f |
Dominance Rankd | ||||
Standardized rank (−1: 1) | 42% (40) | 58% (56) | 1.02 (0.53, 1.96) | 0.95 (0.43, 2.07) |
Livestock densitye | ||||
High | 24% (8) | 76% (25) | 1.00 (Reference) | 1.00 (Reference) |
Low | 38% (50) | 62% (83) | 0.53 (0.21, 1.22) | 0.56 (0.20, 1.46) |
aFrom a logistic regression model where the explanatory variable of interest is each socioecological characteristic, and the outcome is infection (yes vs. no).
bAdjusted models control for a hyena’s sex, age at diagnosis, and livestock density.
cAge was assessed on the date the hyena was diagnosed (i.e., the darting date).
dAdult female rank or a cub’s maternal rank the year during which the hyena was diagnosed. On the standardized rank scale, −1 corresponds with the lowest rank and 1 with the highest rank.
eBased on illegal livestock grazing in the park during the year in which a hyena was diagnosed. Here, we controlled for continuous age (mon) on the date of diagnosis because all cubs were from low livestock density areas.
fSignificant at P value < 0.05.
Adjusting for potential confounding variables did not change findings (Table 1). Hyena sex, dominance rank, and living in high vs. low livestock density areas were not associated with infection status (odds ratio [OR] for male vs. female hyenas: 0.70 [95% CI: 0.33, 1.47] OR for standardized dominance rank 0.95 [95% CI: 0.43, 2.07]), and OR for high vs. low livestock density 0.56 [95% CI: 0.20, 1.46]). T. gondii was more prevalent in older individuals (OR for subadults vs. cubs: 5.05 [95% CI: 1.80, 15.17]; OR for adults vs. cubs: 8.11 [95% CI: 3.59, 19.32]).
Second, we investigated associations of T. gondii infection with boldness toward lions, as indicated by minimum approach distance to lion(s). Supplementary Table 1 shows bivariate associations between a) hyenas’ minimum approach distances toward lions, and b) candidate confounding variables of the relationship between T. gondii infection and hyena approach distance from lions. At alpha = 0.05, shorter minimum approach distances were seen in female and older hyenas (i.e., subadult or adult), among higher dominance rank hyenas and in areas with high livestock density.
Given the age structure of T. gondii prevalence in hyenas, coupled with our observations that older hyenas consistently approach lions more closely than cubs, we conducted separate analyses of behavioral covariates of infection in cubs vs. older individuals. Here we report all estimates as square root transformed distances in meters from lions, unless otherwise noted. Among cubs, infected individuals had a shorter minimum approach distance from lions (−3.19 [95% CI: −5.57, −0.81]) than their uninfected counterparts after controlling for sex and age in months at the time of interaction (Fig. 1A, Table 2). Among subadults and adults, infection was not related to minimum approach distance (0.27 [95% CI: −0.19, 0.72]; Fig. 1B, Table 2). Limiting our dataset to hyena-lion interactions recorded after the diagnosis dates for seropositive individuals and prior to the diagnosis dates of seronegative individuals, we observed no association (0.48 [95% CI: −0.58, 1.53] between infection status and boldness behaviors. Similarly, excluding subadult and adult hyenas with a “doubtful” T. gondii diagnosis did not materially change the results (0.02 [95% CI: −0.87, 0.92]) related to minimum approach distance between infected vs. uninfected hyenas.
Table 2.
T. gondii infection status | N | β (95% CI) minimum approach distance from lions | |
---|---|---|---|
Unadjusted | Adjusted | ||
Cub hyenas (<12 mos)a | |||
Uninfected | 7 | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.0 (Reference) |
Infected | 8 | −3.35 (−5.52, −1.18)c | −3.19 (−5.57, −0.81)c |
Subadult/adult hyenas (≥12 mos)b | |||
Uninfected | 24 | 0.0 (Reference) | 0.00 (Reference) |
Infected | 85 | 0.30 (−0.18, 0.78) | 0.27 (−0.19, 0.72) |
In all models, distances were square root transformed.
aEstimates are from a linear regression model where infection status is the explanatory variable of interest and minimum approach distance (m) from lions is the outcome. For one study animal, the minimum approach distance was an average of three repeated measures. Adjusted for sex and age (months) on the date of the hyena-lion interaction.
bEstimates are from a mixed linear regression model where the explanatory variable of interest is infection status, a random effect for individual ID, and the outcome is repeated measures of minimum approach distance (m) to lion(s). Adjusted for sex, age group on the date of infection diagnosis (subadult vs. adult), and age group on the date of hyena-lion interaction (subadult vs. adult).
cSignificant at P value < 0.05.
In sensitivity analyses, we assessed the potential effects of known determinants of hyena boldness behaviors in this population among cubs, as this was the subgroup with which we found an effect of T. gondii on approach distance to lions. First, we included the cubs’ maternal rank as a covariate and noted no appreciable change in the estimate nor the interpretation of results (−2.14 [95% CI: −4.08, −0.21]). Next, we adjusted for the presence of a male lion during the hyena-lion interaction (−3.40 [95% CI: −6.03, −0.77]). Finally, we accounted for the presence of food during the interaction (−2.69 [95% CI: −4.91, −0.48]). Addition of the variables in our adjusted models did not markedly change the direction, magnitude, or precision of the estimate for T. gondii infection status in relation to approach distance.
Third, we explored associations of T. gondii infection with lion-related mortality. Among 33 mixed-age hyenas with known mortality causes, infected hyenas were nearly twice as likely to die by lions than by other known causes (52% vs. 25%). Infected hyenas were 3.91 (95% CI: 0.70, 32.78; P = 0.15) times more likely to die by lions than uninfected animals after accounting for sex, though this effect was not significant (Table 3). Among hyenas infected as cubs, 100% of the deaths were caused by lions, while only 17% of the deaths of hyenas not infected as cubs were caused by lions. In this small subsample of 11 hyenas infected as cubs, all of which were sampled between 1990-1999, the probability of dying by lions vs. other known sources of mortality was greater among infected than uninfected individuals (Fisher’s Exact Test P = 0.01).
Table 3.
N | OR (95% CI) of death by lion(s) vs. all other causes | ||
---|---|---|---|
T. gondii serostatus | Unadjusteda | Adjustedb | |
Uninfected | 8 | 1.00 (Reference) | 1.00 (Reference) |
Infected | 25 | 3.23 (0.62, 24.85) | 3.91 (0.70, 32.78) |
aEstimates are from a logistic regression model where the explanatory variable is infection status and the outcome is death by lion (yes vs. no).
bModel is adjusted for hyena sex (male vs. female).
Discussion
We found T. gondii infection was associated with behavioral boldness that brought infected hyena cubs into closer proximity to lions, as well as increased the likelihood of being killed by lions. The fact that we saw null and weaker associations among older (subadult and adult) hyenas suggests that experienced individuals might better assess threats and inhibit risky behavior, though other non-exclusive hypotheses could explain the observed age-dependent relationship between T. gondii infection and host behavior (Supplementary Table 2). Testing these models will require additional data and offer rich opportunities to advance our general insight to host-parasite interactions. Meanwhile, our results provide novel evidence of a widely reported but unproven link between T. gondii infection, boldness toward felids under natural conditions, and fitness in a wild population of non-definitive hosts. This link is mechanistically plausible given that lions readily attack and kill hyenas20,23 and are a leading source of hyena mortality in the wild19. Hyenas with above-average boldness in the presence of lions also have reduced longevity, which may result from injuries and lethal wounds inflicted by lions21.
Determinants of T. gondii prevalence
Our analyses suggest that demographic factors influenced the prevalence of T. gondii in spotted hyenas inhabiting the Mara region. For example, older hyenas were more likely to be infected. A recent survey of carnivores from the Serengeti ecosystem found a similar pattern, suggesting that ingestion of infected prey or carrion—of which older individuals have a longer cumulative exposure—may be an important source of infection24. Counter to our predictions, T. gondii’s prevalence was not significantly higher within Masai Mara sampling localities characterized by relatively high livestock density. We initially predicted that hyenas, through ingestion of contaminated water or consumption of infected meat, would have a higher prevalence of T. gondii infection when living in close proximity to domesticated animals and human commensals since these are known parasite reservoirs7,25. However, we noted that T. gondii prevalence was stable across a gradient of hyena exposures to livestock densities (as well as multiple decades), indicating that this parasite has a widespread distribution throughout the Masai Mara region. Nonetheless, intensification of human activities may have important impacts on other variables (e.g., the timing, genotypes, and outcomes of T. gondii infection).
Possible mechanisms of behavioral alteration in T. gondii-infected hosts
It is tempting to speculate that our findings reflect an underlying mechanism through which T. gondii manipulates hyena boldness to promote transmission to lions. However, several recent studies have also cautioned against over-interpreting observations from animals harboring both experimental and naturally occurring infections26,27. In light of important caveats raised by these other authors, our study was designed solely to test one key prediction of the host manipulation hypothesis: that infected hosts will exhibit costly boldness in the presence of (definitive host) feline species. A second key prediction is that the alteration of host behavior is induced via adaptive parasite traits that have evolved to promote transmission; this prediction can neither be supported or falsified with our current dataset. Below, we discuss competing and plausible explanations for the observed association between T. gondii infection and fitness-related behavior in spotted hyenas.
In one scenario, T. gondii traits that facilitate transmission from a wide range of intermediate hosts (e.g., hyenas) into definitive hosts (e.g., lions) have evolved through natural selection on T. gondii. An ability to manipulate boldness in diverse hosts could be advantageous in biodiverse ecosystems like the Mara; propagation from definitive hosts (i.e., via oocysts) can infect a larger number of hosts over greater spatial and temporal scales in comparison to propagation via asexual stages (i.e., transmission between any two non-definitive hosts). However, an important caveat within the focal system is that lions rarely consume hyenas after killing them. Still, exchanges of blood and tissue during lethal conflicts may transmit T. gondii from an infected hyena to a lion where the parasite can sexually reproduce28 (c.f. Supplementary Movie 1). Additionally, an infected hyena that is killed by a lion may also enhance parasite transmission because hyena carcasses are consumed by highly mobile carrion-feeding animals (e.g., vultures)29 that can widely disperse the parasite (Supplementary Figure 4).
In a second scenario, the behavioral phenotypes of infected hyenas may simply represent “collateral manipulation” via traits that evolved to influence other host species like rodents. For instance, T. gondii-infected humans exhibit riskier behavior7 despite being dead-end hosts. This possibility is further supported by findings that homologous neural and hormonal regulators of behavior are similarly altered by T. gondii infections in human and non-human hosts30. However, the concept of collateral manipulation has not been rigorously tested in wild animals, where researchers can directly assess its ecological and evolutionary significance. Only a small body of work examines T. gondii’s relationship to behavior outside of laboratory rodents and human hosts (e.g., sea otters and chimpanzees5,13); this work also examines a very narrow range of taxa despite the fact that a much wider array of mammals and birds are susceptible7.
A final scenario is that symptoms of T. gondii infection, such as encephalitis, coincidentally alter infected hyenas’ behaviors but did not evolve through natural selection on T. gondii transmission. We cannot rule this out, though we note that if T. gondii caused severe neuropathy we would predict infections to elevate mortality involving other forms of behavioral maladaptation (e.g., lethal conflicts with Masai herders or vehicle collisions) and not just interactions with lions specifically.
This study is not without limitations. First, because we were only able to diagnose infection status at the timepoint when a hyena was darted and blood sampled, we were unable to assess how behaviors differed within individuals following T. gondii infection onset. Second, given the social nature of this species, the focal hyenas’ behaviors towards lions during observation sessions were likely influenced by groupmates’ behaviors, or the proximity of their mothers during these interactions. An ideal test of these predictions would involve hierarchical modeling that accounts for group composition and behavior during each observation session—an effort that is currently not possible with the available data. Finally, more sophisticated assessments of potential modes of transmission among livestock, pastoralists and hyenas are required to better understand how infection risks and outcomes are modified where humans and wildlife live in close proximity. These future directions will mandate heavier sampling and more sophisticated analyses that leverage longitudinal data to pinpoint the timing of T. gondii infections within hosts.
Our results suggest that behavioral boldness toward felines associated with T. gondii infection is likely deleterious to hyenas, at least when contracted early in life. We encourage further explorations of fitness-related behavior in natural settings—both for infections of T. gondii, which involve a substantial proportion of mammals and birds, and for other parasites suspected to alter host behavior to serve their own evolutionary interests.
Methods
The Mara Hyena Project
This study uses data and samples from the Mara Hyena Project (approved by MSU IACUC and KWS), a long-term field study of individually known spotted hyenas that have been observed since May 1979. Study hyenas are monitored daily and behavioral, demographic, and ecological data are systematically collected and entered into a database. Here, we used data from four different hyena groups, called clans, as well as historic information about ecological conditions in the Masai Mara National Reserve. We maintained detailed records on the demographics of our study population, including sex, age, and the dates of key life-history milestones such as birth, weaning, dispersal and death. In the ensuing sections, we describe data collection and data processing procedures for assessment of T. gondii infection diagnosis, quantification of demographic and ecological determinants of infection status, and assessment of behavioral (boldness) and fitness (cause of mortality) characteristics hypothesized to be a consequence of positive T. gondii infection. The present analysis includes 168 hyenas, but specific subsamples vary depending on the particular hypothesis being tested.
Biospecimen collection and assessment of Toxoplasma gondii exposure
As part of our long-term data collection, we routinely darted study animals in order to collect biological samples and morphological measurements. Of special relevance to this study is our blood collection procedure. We immobilized hyenas using 6.5 mg/kg of tiletamine-zolazepam (Telazol ®) in a pressurized dart fired from a CO2 powered rifle. We then drew blood from the jugular vein into sodium heparin-coated vacuum tubes. After the hyena was secured in a safe place to recover from the anesthesia, we took the samples back to camp where a portion of the collected blood was spun in a centrifuge at 1000 × g for 10 min to separate red and white blood cells from plasma. Plasma was aliquoted into multiple cryogenic vials. Immediately, the blood derivatives, including plasma, were flash frozen in liquid nitrogen where they remained until they were transported on dry ice to a −80 °C freezer in the U.S. All samples remained frozen until time of laboratory analysis for the T. gondii assays.
Using archived plasma, we diagnosed individual hyenas using the multi-species ID Screen® Toxoplasmosis Indirect kit (IDVET, Montpellier). This ELISA-based assay tests for serological (IgG) reactivity to T. gondii’s P-30 antigen and has been used in many prior studies of T. gondii in diverse mammals22. The output of the assay is an SP ratio, which is calculated as colorimetric signal of immunoreactivity for a tested blood sample (S) divided by that of a positive control (P), after subtracting the background signal for the ELISA plate (i.e., a negative control) from both S and P. We tested 168 plasma samples from 168 individual spotted hyenas and determined infection status based on the kit manufacturer’s criteria for interpreting S/P: ≤ 40% = negative result, 40% < S/P < 50% = doubtful result, S/P ≥ 50% = positive result (Supplementary Fig. 1). Only 21 hyenas (13%) fell within the “doubtful” range of ELISA S/P values. We treated these individuals as negative in our analyses, following protocols from other recent studies22. Although ELISA-based assays performed relatively well in prior studies of T. gondii in both hyenas31 and other mammals22, the method can also be sensitive to cross-reactivity with antibodies to other, related parasites32. We therefore retested 60 randomly chosen plasma samples using an alternative, indirect fluorescence agglutination test (IFAT), which is less sensitive to cross reactivity31, in order to confirm that the two methods yielded similar results using our plasma samples (Supplementary Fig. 2). Samples were submitted to the Michigan State University Veterinary Diagnostic Laboratory for a standard diagnostic IFAT procedure that used reagents supplied by VMRD, Pullman, WA. In brief, the IFAT measures the maximum dilution of a plasma sample at which immunoreactivity to T. gondii antigen is visible by microscopy. Two samples were excluded from our main analyses because of suspected assay error (e.g., one negative SP ratio and one additional IFAT vs. SP ratio discrepancy) making our final diagnostic sample size, N = 166 hyenas. However, it should be noted that inclusion of these two questionable data points did not substantively change our results and had no effect on our analyses of hyena boldness or fitness as these two hyenas lacked data required for those analyses. To rule out the possibility of misclassification of T. gondii infection due to titer decay over time, we plotted the SP ratio for infected and uninfected animals separately with respect to age and did not observe any decline in SP ratio as a function of age (Supplementary Fig. 3). Diagnostic assays were performed by people who were blind to the individual hyena’s demographic, ecological, and behavioral data.
Demographic, social, and ecological characteristics
The first aim of this analysis was to identify demographic and ecological correlates and determinants of T. gondii infection. The key characteristics of interest include sex and age, two demographic traits that have previously been implicated in health and behavioral outcomes, as well as exposure to livestock density.
We determined the sex of each hyena based on the glans morphology of its erect phallus during field observations; this is reliable starting at 3 months of age33.
We aged each hyena by back-calculating its birthdate based on its physical appearance when first observed in infancy. Based on its pelage, morphology and behavior, we are able to determine a birthdate with an accuracy of ±7 days34. We used this method to determine each hyena’s age in months at the time of blood collection. In the analysis, we assessed age continuously in months, as well as in distinct age groups divided into cubs (<12 months), subadults (≥12 to ≤24 months), and adults (>24 months of age). The age cut-offs were determined based on the timing of major life history milestones; weaning occurs at approximately 12 months of age and hyenas of both sexes achieve reproductive competence at around 24 months of age34,35.
As part of our routine data collection, all aggressive interactions between hyenas are recorded, such that we can calculate rates of threat displays, chases, and bites between clan members. Agonistic interaction data are used to calculate each hyena’s dominance rank each year via a matrix of dyadic wins and losses in fights36–38. Values from each rank matrix are normalized as a continuous variable from −1 (lowest) to 1 (highest) and are updated annually to account for demographic change. We use maternal rank as a proxy of each cubs rank until they learn their own rank.
To quantify exposure to livestock density, we took advantage of naturally occurring variation in exposure to human activity across distinct regions of the Masai Mara National Reserve. We classified all hyenas from the western region of the Reserve (also known as the Mara Triangle) as “low livestock density” due to strict bans on livestock grazing and travel on foot in this area. The eastern side of the Reserve borders pastoralist villages that have experienced an increase in human population growth in recent years, especially around the burgeoning Talek community39. Additionally, assessment of trends in livestock counts within the Reserve indicates a marked increase in illegal livestock grazing on the eastern side of the Reserve starting in 2000, followed by another increase between 2009 and 2013. These changes coincided with parallel shifts in hyena demography and wildlife community composition40. To improve discriminatory power in our analyses that reflect these changes in livestock density and shifts in ecology, we enriched our sample selection to include hyenas from areas of low and higher livestock density. For “low livestock density,” we selected animals from the eastern side of the Reserve sampled before 2000 along with animals from the western side of the Reserve (any time period). For “high livestock density,” we selected animals from the eastern side of the Reserve from 2012 onward.
Boldness behaviors and fitness
In addition to identifying determinants and correlates of T. gondii infection, we also sought to explore the effects of infection status on hyena behavior and fitness. Over the duration of our study, we documented all observed hyena-lion encounters i.e., all instances where at least one hyena and at least one lion approached to within ~200 m of one another. In 731 observation sessions we recorded 3791 minimum distance estimates between individual hyenas and one or more lions along with the date, location, and identities of all hyenas present, as well as whether food (a dead prey animal or its components) or a male lion was present during the encounter, as both these factors are known to influence hyena behaviors. All boldness behaviors were extracted by four individuals blinded to infection status with 83% agreement across seven metrics recorded during hyena-lion interactions41.
Based on previous findings in this study population that minimum distance from lions is a measure of boldness that shows inter-individual consistency and corresponds with fitness21, we used this metric as an indicator for behavioral boldness. During each hyena-lion encounter, we recorded the distances between lions and individual hyenas in meters using 20-min scan sampling of individual hyena distances from the nearest lion, as well as all-occurrence sampling of close behavioral interactions between lions and hyenas (e.g., a hyena comes within 10 m of a lion). Because the body length of an adult hyena is ~1 m, we are able to accurately estimate approach distances at this scale. Due to the inherent frenetic activity at some hyena-lion encounters, some of the minimum approach distances were recorded as ranges (e.g., 10–15 m) or inequalities (e.g., <10 m). For ranges, we calculated the mid-point and used this value in the analysis (e.g., if the range was 10–15 m, then we used 12.5 m in our calculations). If the distance range was large (>25 m) and therefore highly uncertain (i.e., if the range exceeded ½ a standard deviation as estimated from the minimum approach distance data set), we removed it from the dataset. Of the 529 approach distance ranges in our data set, 225 were removed because the range exceeded 25 m. We retained inequality distances by using the ‘less than’ distance if the recorded distance was smaller than 25 m (approximately the mean [mean = 45 m] minus ½ a standard deviation [sd = 50 m] of all hyena minimum approach distances) and by including the ‘greater than’ distances if the recorded distance was greater than 75 m (approximately the mean plus ½ a standard deviation of hyena minimum approach distances). For example, a distance recorded as <50 m would be removed from the data set as it could include a wide range of actual distances (0–50 m), while a recorded distance of <15 m was retained in the data set as 15 m. As a result of filtering inequality distances with large uncertainty, we removed 67 of 72 approach distances recorded as inequalities. Finally, we filtered the hyena approach distance to lions by removing instances when the minimum approach distance exceeded 100 m, given that at this range hyenas and lions pose little threat to one another. After filtering, our final data set included 2725 minimum approach distance estimates. It should be noted that during any particular hyena-lion interaction, we retained a single minimum approach distance for each hyena, but over their lifetime hyenas interact with lions on multiple occasions, thus the repeated minimum distance measures for individual hyenas.
Our longstanding behavioral database also documents the source of mortality for each hyena whenever known. Deaths attributed to lions included cases in which lions were observed killing hyenas and when fresh corpses of hyenas were found with puncture wounds in each corpse made by canine teeth that were too far apart to have been inflicted by anything but a lion. In our analysis, we dichotomized cause of mortality as death by lion vs. all other known causes of mortality and evaluated this as a binary outcome in the statistical analysis. We did not include data in which a hyena’s cause of death was unknown.
Statistical analyses
In the analysis, we tested three hypotheses: (H1) higher livestock density is associated with higher risk of T. gondii infection in spotted hyenas; (H2) infected hyenas behave more boldly towards lions than uninfected hyenas, as indicated by a shorter minimum approach distance; (H3) T. gondii infection imposes fitness costs on the host, as indicated by greater odds of death by lion(s). We describe methods for testing each hypothesis below, following a description of our general approach to data analysis.
Prior to formal analyses, we assessed the distributions of all variables. This included viewing the distributions and calculating descriptive statistics for continuous variables (e.g., minimum approach distance towards lions) to check for deviations from normality and missing values. We also assessed frequency distributions for all categorical variables (infection status, sex, age group, food presence, and livestock density). Finally, as part of our data exploration, we conducted bivariate analyses of associations between demographic and ecological characteristics and infection status, as well as associations of demographic or ecological characteristics with behavioral outcomes to identify covariates for inclusion in multiple variable analysis. In all models, we considered an estimate to be statistically significant at a nominal cut-off of alpha = 0.05. Data cleaning and analyses were performed in program R version 4.0.242. Linear mixed models were conducted using the lme4 package43, version 1.1.21.
H1: Greater livestock density is associated with higher risk of T. gondii infection. In this portion of the analysis, we used univariable logistic regression to investigate the relationship between livestock density (high vs. low) as the primary explanatory variable of interest, and T. gondii infection (positive vs. negative) as the outcome. In addition, we also explored associations of other key demographic characteristics as determinants of infection, namely sex, age at diagnosis and social dominance rank. Following the simple regression models that contained one single explanatory variable (unadjusted analysis), we also examined multiple-variable (mutually-adjusted) associations among the above variables. In models where dominance rank was not the primary variable of interest, we did not include rank as covariate due to missing data.
H2: Infected hyenas behave more boldly towards lions, as indicated by shorter minimum approach distances. To investigate the extent to which infection status is related to boldness behaviors, we used simple (unadjusted) and multiple-variable (adjusted) linear regression models in which T. gondii diagnosis (infected vs. uninfected) was the explanatory variable of interest, and the hyenas’ square root transformed minimum approach distance (m) was the outcome. We transformed the distances to improve assumptions of normality. We stratified all models by age group such that cubs were analyzed separately from subadults and adults. We made this decision based on bivariate associations that revealed a significant age structuring of infection status (i.e., much higher prevalence of infection in subadults and adults than in cubs), as well as significant effects of age on hyena approach distances towards lions (i.e., older hyenas were much more likely to approach lions closer than younger hyenas). The cub models included individual hyenas that had both infection diagnosis and hyena-lion interaction data during their first year of life. Similarly, the subadult and adult models were restricted to include only infection diagnosis and hyena-lion interactions collected from hyenas 12 months of age and older.
When exploring associations among cubs, we first examined the unadjusted association of T. gondii diagnosis with minimum approach distance to lions. Next, we controlled for sex and age in months on the date of the interaction with lions. The age distributions of hyena cubs during observed interactions with lions were 2.7–8.5 months for uninfected cubs and 3.2–11.8 months for infected cubs. We did not need to account for livestock density because all cubs were sampled in low livestock density areas. Additionally, for all but one cub, we only had a single minimum approach distance from lions. For the cub with multiple measures (N = 3), we took the average of its minimum approach distances for use in the analysis.
When exploring associations among subadults and adults, we used a similar modeling strategy to that used for cubs, except rather than using conventional linear regression, we employed mixed linear regression models to account for the multiple assessments of minimum approach distance to lions for hyenas in these two age groups (median = 5 measurements per hyena) via a random intercept for the hyena’s ID. After examining unadjusted associations, we implemented a multiple variable model that adjusted for age group (subadult vs. adult) both at the time of the diagnosis and at the time of the hyena lion interaction, and sex (male vs. female). Nota bene: in the subadult and adult model age was not parametrized as a continuous measure (e.g., age in months) because for some adult female hyenas, who we began observing as adults, and for some immigrant males, whose natal clan is not known, we do not know the exact birth date of these hyenas.
In addition to the above analysis for subadult and adult hyenas, which leveraged all available hyena-lion interaction data, we also conducted sensitivity analyses on a restricted data set wherein we only considered approach distances from lions that occurred prior to the diagnostic date among hyenas who tested negative for T. gondii infection, thus ensuring these represented behaviors of uninfected hyenas. Similarly, we only considered hyena-lion interaction data that occurred after the diagnostic date for individuals who tested positive for T. gondii infection. The rationale for this approach is rooted in achieving temporal separation to avoid erroneously examining hyena-lion interactions for negative diagnosis hyenas who subsequently became infected and vice versa (nota bene: we did not do this for cubs given our small sample size in this age group and because the small age range limited the possibility that a hyena’s approach from lions did not reflect its infection diagnosis). Using this restricted dataset, we modeled the associations between infection status and each hyena’s closest approach distance to lions following the previously described modeling strategy. We also modeled the hyena approach distance from lions as function of T. gondii infection among hyenas diagnosed as either positive or negative but excluding the doubtful diagnosis category. This second sensitivity analysis aimed to rule out any potential variable misclassification bias.
H3: T. gondii infection imposes fitness costs on the host, as indicated by greater odds of death by lion(s). Here, we assessed the probability that T. gondii infection in hyenas was associated with lion-induced mortality. To do this, we used logistic regression models to compare the odds of mortality due to lions vs. all other known causes of mortality for infected vs. uninfected hyenas. Following unadjusted analysis, we controlled for sex in a multiple-variable logistic regression analysis. Due to small sample sizes (i.e., cells in cross tabulations with N = 0) we were not able to adjust for hyenas’ ages and livestock density levels. However, we were able to use a two-by-two table and Fisher’s exact test to determine whether the probability of dying by lions vs. other sources of mortality differed between infected and uninfected cubs.
Additional sensitivity analyses. Dominance rank, presence of food, and presence of male lions are key determinants of boldness behaviors in hyenas. Therefore, in age-stratified subgroup analyses where T. gondii infection was a significant determinant of approach distance to lions (i.e., in cubs only), we included maternal rank, presence of food, and presence of male lions to rule out the possibility of extraneous causes of boldness behavior. We then assessed the extent to which inclusion of each of these variables, singly, changed the direction, magnitude, and precision of the estimate for T. gondii infection in relation to approach distance to lions. Similar to cubs, we ran sensitivity analyses that included food present during the interaction (yes vs. no), and livestock density during the year of the interaction (high vs. low).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank the Kenyan National Commission for Science, Technology, and Innovation, the Naboisho Conservancy, the Narok County Government, the Kenya Wildlife Service (KWS) and Brian Heath for permission to conduct research in the Mara ecosystem. We also thank Chiara Bowen and Nichole Grosjean for lab assistance, Samantha Gregg, Katie Keyser, Leah McTigue, and Abigail Thiemkey for assistance extracting distances between lions and hyenas, the NSF-BEACON center for the Study of Evolution in Action for funding, the Mara Hyena Project field crew, and the residents of the Mara ecosystem for their direct and indirect support of long-term field research in their backyard. We thank Joy Baldwin for providing the lion illustration and Mark Bellncula and Page E. Van Meter for providing the hyena silhouettes seen in Fig. 1. We thank members of the Getty and Holekamp labs for their feedback on this project and paper. This material is based in part upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Cooperative Agreement No. DBI-0939454. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. Sample collection and behavioral data collection were supported by NSF grants IOS1755089 and OISE1853934. Z.M.L. was funded by the Morris Animal Foundation grant D19ZO-411.
Source data
Author contributions
Conceptualization, E.G., Z.M.L.; data generation and curation, E.G., Z.M.L., P.W., G.S.H., K.D.S.L., T.M.M., J.W.T., M.O.P.; formal analysis, Z.M.L., W.P.; funding acquisition, E.G., Z.M.L., K.E.H.; investigation, E.G., Z.M.L.; methodology, E.G., Z.M.L., P.W., G.H., K.D.S.L., T.M.M., W.P.; resources, E.G., P.W., G.S.H., K.E.H.; supervision, K.E.H., T.G.; visualization, E.G., Z.M.L.; writing—original draft, E.G., Z.M.L.; writing—review & editing, E.G., Z.M.L., P.W., G.S.H., K.D.S.L., T.M.M., J.W.T., W.P., M.O.P., K.E.H., T.G.
Data availability
The full data used in this paper are available at https://zenodo.org/badge/latestdoi/245864615 (10.5281/zenod.4699720)44. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
Source code is provided with this paper at https://zenodo.org/badge/latestdoi/245864615 (10.5281/zenod.4699720)44.
Competing interests
Authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Eben Gering, Zachary M. Laubach.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-021-24092-x.
References
- 1.Poulin, R. Parasite Manipulation of Host Behavior: An Update and Frequently Asked Questions. Advances in the Study of Behavior41, (Elsevier Inc., 2010).
- 2.Dubey J. Advances in the life cycle of Toxoplasma gondii. Int. J. Parasitol. 1998;28:1019–1024. doi: 10.1016/S0020-7519(98)00023-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Elmore SA, et al. Toxoplasma gondii: epidemiology, feline clinical aspects, and prevention. Trends Parasitol. 2010;26:190–196. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Flegr, J., Lenochová, P., Hodný, Z. & Vondrová, M. Fatal attraction phenomenon in humans—cat odour attractiveness increased for Toxoplasma-infected men while decreased for infected women. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 5, e1389 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 5.Poirotte C, et al. Morbid attraction to leopard urine in toxoplasma-infected chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:R98–R99. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Vyas A, Kim S-K, Giacomini N, Boothroyd JC, Sapolsky RM. Behavioral changes induced by Toxoplasma infection of rodents are highly specific to aversion of cat odors. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:6442–6447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608310104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dubey, J. P. Toxoplasmosis of Animals and Humans. Toxoplasmosis of Animals and Humans (CRC Press, 2016). 10.1201/9781420092370.
- 8.Di Genova BM, Wilson SK, Dubey JP, Knoll LJ. Intestinal delta-6-desaturase activity determines host range for Toxoplasma sexual reproduction. PLoS Biol. 2019;17:1–19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Boillat M, et al. Neuroinflammation-associated aspecific manipulation of mouse predator fear by Toxoplasma gondii. Cell Rep. 2020;30:320–334. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Webster JP. The effect of Toxoplasma gondii on animal behavior: playing cat and mouse. Schizophr. Bull. 2007;33:752–756. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tenter AM, Heckeroth AR, Weiss LM. Toxoplasma gondii: from animals to humans. Int. J. Parasitol. 2000;30:1217–1258. doi: 10.1016/S0020-7519(00)00124-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Worth AR, Lymbery AJ, Thompson RCA. Adaptive host manipulation by Toxoplasma gondii: fact or fiction? Trends Parasitol. 2013;29:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.pt.2013.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Shapiro, K. et al. Type X strains of toxoplasma gondii are virulent for southern sea otters (Enhydra lutris nereis) and present in felids from nearby watersheds. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 286, 1–10 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 14.Berdoy M, Webster JP, Macdonald DW. Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2000;267:1591–1594. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Webster JP. The effect of Toxoplasma gondii and other parasites on activity levels in wild and hybrid Rattus norvegicus. Parasitology. 1994;109:583–589. doi: 10.1017/S0031182000076460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Webster JP, Brunton CFA, Macdonald DW. Effect of Toxoplasma gondii upon neophobic behaviour in wild brown rats, Rattus norvegicus. Parasitology. 1994;109:37–43. doi: 10.1017/S003118200007774X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Miller MA, et al. An unusual genotype of Toxoplasma gondii is common in California sea otters (Enhydra lutris nereis) and is a cause of mortality. Int. J. Parasitol. 2004;34:275–284. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpara.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gering, E. et al. Time makes you older, parasites make you bolder: Toxoplasma gondii infections predict hyena boldness toward definitive lion hosts. In Evolution in Action: Past, Present and Future: A Festschrift in Honor of Erik D. Goodman (eds. Banzhaf, W. et al.) 205–224 (Springer International Publishing, 2020). 10.1007/978-3-030-39831-6_16.
- 19.Watts HE, Holekamp KE. Ecological determinants of survival and reproduction in the spotted Hyena. J. Mammal. 2009;90:461–471. doi: 10.1644/08-MAMM-A-136.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Périquet S, Fritz H, Revilla E. The Lion King and the Hyaena Queen: Large carnivore interactions and coexistence. Biol. Rev. 2015;90:1197–1214. doi: 10.1111/brv.12152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Yoshida, K. C. S., Van Meter, P. E. & Holekamp, K. E. Variation among free-living spotted hyenas in three personality traits. Behaviour10.1163/1568539X-00003367 (2016).
- 22.Laforet CK, et al. Toxoplasma gondii seroprevalence in extensively farmed wild boars (Sus scrofa) in Denmark. Acta Vet. Scand. 2019;61:4. doi: 10.1186/s13028-019-0440-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kruuk, H. The spotted hyena: A study of predation and social behavior (University of Chicago Press, 1972).
- 24.Ferreira SCM, et al. Evidence of high exposure to Toxoplasma gondii in free-ranging and captive African carnivores. Int. J. Parasitol. Parasites Wildl. 2019;8:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.ijppaw.2018.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dubey JP, et al. Sources and reservoirs of Toxoplasma gondii infection on 47 swine farms in Illinois. J. Parasitol. 1995;81:723–729. doi: 10.2307/3283961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Johnson HJ, Koshy AA. Latent toxoplasmosis effects on rodents and humans: How much is real and how much is media hype? MBio. 2020;11:1–10. doi: 10.1128/mBio.02164-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Doherty J-F. When fiction becomes fact: exaggerating host manipulation by parasites practices. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2020;287:1–7. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.1081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Siegel SE, et al. Transmission of toxoplasmosis by leukocyte transfusion. Blood. 1971;37:388–394. doi: 10.1182/blood.V37.4.388.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jones, S., Strauss, E. & Holekamp, K. Ecology of African carrion. In Carrion ecology, evolution, and their applications (eds. Benbow, M., Tomberlin, J. & Tarone, A.) 461–491 (CRC Press, 2015).
- 30.Webster JP, McConkey GA. Toxoplasma gondii-altered host behaviour: Clues as to mechanism of action. Folia Parasitol. (Praha) 2010;57:95–104. doi: 10.14411/fp.2010.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wait LF, et al. A Comparison of antiserum and protein A as secondary reagents to assess Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers in cats and spotted hyenas. J. Parasitol. 2015;101:390–392. doi: 10.1645/14-705.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gondim LFP, Mineo JR, Schares G. Importance of serological cross-reactivity among Toxoplasma gondii, Hammondia spp., Neospora spp., Sarcocystis spp. and Besnoitia besnoiti. Parasitology. 2017;144:851–868. doi: 10.1017/S0031182017000063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Frank LG, Glickman SE, Powch I. Sexual dimorphism in the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta) J. Zool. 1990;221:308–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1990.tb04001.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Holekamp K, Smale L, Szykman M. Rank and reproduction in the female spotted hyaena. J. Reprod. Fertil. 1996;108:229–237. doi: 10.1530/jrf.0.1080229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Holekamp KE, Smale L. Behavioral development in the spotted hyena. Bioscience. 1998;48:997–1005. doi: 10.2307/1313456. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Smale L, Frank LG, Holekamp KE. Ontogeny of dominance in free-living spotted hyaenas: juvenile rank relations with adult females and immigrant males. Anim. Behav. 1993;46:467–477. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1215. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Holekamp KE, Smale L. Ontogeny of dominance in free-living spotted hyaenas: juvenile rank relations with other immature individuals. Anim. Behav. 1993;46:451–466. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1214. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Engh AL, Esch K, Smale L, Holekamp KE. Mechanisms of maternal rank ‘inheritance’ in the spotted hyaena, Crocuta crocuta. Anim. Behav. 2000;60:323–332. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Green, D. S., Zipkin, E. F., Incorvaia, D. C. & Holekamp, K. E. Long-term ecological change inside a protected area in the Mara-Serengeti ecosystem. Glob Ecol Conserv. 20, e00697, 10.1007/s10531-017-1469-7(2019).
- 40.Green DS, Johnson-Ulrich L, Couraud HE, Holekamp KE. Anthropogenic disturbance induces opposing population trends in spotted hyenas and African lions. Biodivers. Conserv. 2018;27:871–889. doi: 10.1007/s10531-017-1469-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lehmann KDS, et al. Lions, hyenas and mobs (Oh my!) Curr. Zool. 2017;63:313–322. doi: 10.1093/cz/zow073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Team, R. C. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Team, R. C., 2021).
- 43.Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 67, 1–48, 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 (2015).
- 44.Gering, E. et al. Toxoplasma gondii infections are associated with costly boldness toward felids in a wild host. 10.5281/zenod.4699720 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The full data used in this paper are available at https://zenodo.org/badge/latestdoi/245864615 (10.5281/zenod.4699720)44. Source data are provided with this paper.
Source code is provided with this paper at https://zenodo.org/badge/latestdoi/245864615 (10.5281/zenod.4699720)44.