Abstract
Music is characterized by acoustical forms that are predictive of its behavioral functions. For example, adult listeners accurately identify unfamiliar lullabies as infant-directed on the basis of their musical features alone. This property could reflect a function of listeners’ experiences, the basic design of the human mind, or both. Here, we show that American infants (N = 144) relax in response to 8 unfamiliar foreign lullabies, relative to matched non-lullaby songs from other foreign societies, as indexed by heart rate, pupillometry, and electrodermal activity. They do so consistently throughout the first year of life, suggesting the response is not a function of their musical experiences, which are limited relative to those of adults. The infants’ parents overwhelmingly chose lullabies as the songs that they would use to calm their fussy infant, despite their unfamiliarity. Together, these findings suggest that infants may be predisposed to respond to common features of lullabies found in different cultures.
Introduction
Music is a human universal1–4 that appears often in the lives of infants and their families5–9. Infants demonstrate a remarkable variety of responses to music as they develop: in the first few days of life, newborns remember melodies heard in the womb10; distinguish consonant from dissonant intervals11; and detect musical beats12. Older infants differentiate synchronous movement from asynchronous movement in response to music13; become attuned to the rhythms of their native culture’s music by their first birthday14; garner social information from the songs they hear15,16; and recall music in impressive detail17,18 after long delays15.
Why are infants so interested in music? One possibility centers on the dynamics of parent-offspring interactions. Relative to other animals, human infants are helpless; to survive, they rely on resources provided by parents and alloparents19. Such resources, whether material (like food) or not (like attention) constitute parental investment20. Human parental investment is routinely provided to infants in response to their elicitations, which often take the form of fussiness and crying21.
Infant-directed songs may credibly signal parental attention to infants, conveying information to infants that an adult is nearby, attending to them, and keeping them safe22,23. Singing indicates the location, proximity, and orientation of the singer (even when the singer is not visible, as at night); and it is also costly, in that the singer could be expending their energy on some other activity. Because parental attention is a key resource for helpless infants, they likely are predisposed to attend to signals of it: infants should be particularly interested in and reassured by vocal music with features suggesting that it is directed toward them.
Studies of people with genomic imprinting disorders provide a unique test of this hypothesis because these disorders are characterized by divergent behaviors related to parental investment24,25. For example, infants with Prader-Willi syndrome elicit less parental investment than do typically developing infants: they have feeding difficulties, nursing less often; and they tend to be lethargic26. Children with Angelman syndrome show the opposite pattern: they elicit more parental investment, with frequent drooling and chewing, uncoordinated overfeeding, and high degrees of social engagement27.
Genomic imprinting disorders also alter the psychology of music, in a fashion consistent with the idea that infant-directed song signals parental investment. Compared to the relaxation response that typically developing people display during passive music listening, Prader-Willi syndrome is associated with an increased relaxation response28, and Angelman syndrome is associated with a reduced relaxation response29. These effects are specific to music; they were not elicited by listening to pleasant speech, suggesting that singing is a particularly effective means of satisfying parental investment elicitations in Prader-Willi syndrome, and a particularly ineffective means of doing so in Angelman syndrome.
Credible signals have evolved repeatedly in many species with similar patterns across senders and receivers23,30. The resulting innate links between the forms and functions of vocal signals31–33 explain why, for example, hostile vocalizations across species — from growling tigers to shrieking eagles — are recognized as hostile by human listeners34. Because these signals are shaped by natural selection, they are expected to show consistency across members of a species.
Infant-directed vocalizations appear to fit this pattern. Infant-directed speech is acoustically distinct from adult-directed speech across cultures35–40. Lullabies, a common form of infant-directed song, are reliably distinguishable from other songs41; in a representative sample of music from small-scale societies, adult naïve listeners considered foreign lullabies likely to be “used to soothe a baby”, relative to dance, healing, and love songs42. This result, which has also been supported by a massive conceptual replication (N = 29,357), is explained in large part by the striking musical consistency of lullabies found across cultures: their slow tempos and smooth, minimally-accented melodic contours1. Strikingly, these same musical features appear in infant-directed or low-arousal Western music43–46.
If infant-directed song indeed functions as a credible signal of parental attention, then the universal features of the signal should produce reliable relaxation effects in the receiver: singing should satisfy infants’ fussy demands for parental investment, calming them. Common sense does suggest that infants are calmed by infant-directed song, but typically, this question has been tested in the context of songs that are known to the infant and/or are sung in a familiar language. This makes it difficult to measure the specific soothing effects of infant-directed song, independently of the soothing effects of familiar sounds, more generally. Adults’ ratings of the familiarity and perceived relaxation of music are positively correlated47, and parents produce music for their children often5–9, so familiar music may produce mere-exposure effects48 on infant relaxation.
Indeed, infant arousal, as indexed by electrodermal activity, decreased in response to maternal singing in a “soothing” style, relative to a “playful” style; but both styles were produced in familiar songs49. Listening to live or recorded lullabies reduced heart rate in pre-term infants, more so than a silent control, but the songs were well-known and produced in a familiar language50. Singing reduced distress after a still-face procedure, as indexed by increased smiling and decreased ratings of negative affect, but the effects were driven by the familiarity of the songs51. Infants attended longer to singing than speech before becoming fussy, when both were produced in a foreign language52, but whether this effect reflects increased attention to songs or increased relaxation as a result of listening to music is unknown. In sum, while there is some evidence that infant-directed songs produce relaxation effects in infants, the effects in prior studies may be attributable to infants’ familiarity with the songs, rather than the songs’ acoustic properties (as would be predicted by a credible signaling account22,23).
Here, we ask whether infants relax in response to infant-directed songs produced in unfamiliar languages from foreign societies. We studied 144 infants’ responses to videos of animated characters who lip-synched to songs drawn from the Natural History of Song Discography1, a collection of lullabies, dance songs, healing songs, and love songs recorded in 86 world cultures. The songs were either infant-directed (the lullabies) or not (the other song types) (see Fig. 1 and Table 1). We measured infants’ heart rate, pupil dilation, electrodermal activity, frequency of blinking, and gaze direction. Based on prior results in a similar listening paradigm28,29, we preregistered a hypothesis that infants would show decreased heart rate (i.e., a relaxation response) during the lullabies, relative to the non-lullabies. We report a test of that hypothesis, a series of planned exploratory analyses of other measures of infants’ responses, and a measure of parents’ intuitions about the songs.
Table 1 |. The songs infants heard.
Lullaby | Paired non-lullaby | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Society | Region | Language | Type | Society | Region | Language |
Female | Saami | Scandinavia | Luk Saami | Love | Nenets | North Asia | Tundra Nenets |
Nahua | Maya Area | Western Nahuatl | Love | Serbs | Southeastern Europe | Serbian Standard | |
Igulik Inuit | Arctic and Subarctic | Western Canadian Inuktitut | Dance | Chachi | Northwestern South America | Cha’palaa | |
Kuna | Central America | Border Kuna | Love | Highland Scots | British Isles | Scottish Gaelic | |
Male | Iroquois | Eastern Woodlands | Cherokee | Love | Kurds | Middle East | Central Kurdish |
Hopi | Southwest and Basin | Hopi | Healing | Hawaiians | Polynesia | Hawaiian | |
Ona | Southern South America | Selk’nam | Love | Chuuk | Micronesia | Chuukese | |
Highland Scots | British Isles | Scottish Gaelic | Healing | Seri | Northern Mexico | Seri |
Results
Confirmatory analysis
We preregistered the prediction that infants’ heart rate would decrease more substantially as a result of listening to foreign lullabies than non-lullabies (the preregistration is available at https://osf.io/f69mn). To this end, we normalized heart rate values during singing trials relative to the previous trial (where the previous trial was either a singing trial or a silent preference trial, where the animated characters were silent but still visible; see Fig. 1), such that z-scores are interpretable as immediate changes in heart rate, indexing moment-to-moment relaxation (n.b., this normalization procedure was also preregistered): positive z-scores thus indicate an increase in heart rate from the previous trial, and negative scores a decrease. We obtained heart rate data with a non-invasive physiology monitor (Empatica E4), which also measured electrodermal activity (see Exploratory Analyses, below).
In the main analyses, we analyzed trial-wise mean z-scores for each infant, split by song type. As in previous work28,29, we trimmed (a) all values on trials for which there were fewer than 5 heart rate observations during the normalization period (the previous trial), as this would produce uninterpretable standard deviation values with which to compute z-scores; and (b) extreme values, defined as |z| > 5. These trimming rules dropped 2.19% and 0.31% of the heart rate observations, respectively, and 2 of the 144 participants. These decisions did not substantively affect any of the results. For this and all following analyses, assumptions for the relevant statistical tests were met.
Mean normalized heart rate during lullabies (Fig. 2a) differed significantly from 0, indicating a decrease in heart rate relative to the previous trial (in z-scores, M = −0.15, SD = 0.43, 95% CI [−0.23, −0.08]; t(140) = −4.28, < 0.001, d = 0.36, one-sample two-tailed t-test). In contrast, heart rate during non-lullabies was comparable to 0, indicating no change in heart rate relative to the previous trial (M = −0.01, SD = 0.4, 95% CI [−0.07, 0.06]; t(139) = −0.21, p = 0.83, one-sample two-tailed t-test).
The within-subjects difference between mean heart rates (i.e., the main pre-registered analysis) showed a clear difference between song types, such that lullabies decreased heart rates significantly more than non-lullabies (Fig. 2a; t(138) = −2.75, p = 0.007, paired two-tailed t-test). These findings confirm the preregistered prediction of reduced heart rate in response to unfamiliar foreign lullabies.
We conducted three planned follow-up analyses. First, to determine what drove the mean difference in heart rate across lullabies and non-lullabies, we visualized the trajectory of heart rate within singing trials in a time-series analysis (Fig. 2b). While heart rates dropped almost immediately following the onset of singing, regardless of song type, this drop was more pronounced during lullabies. Because time-wise heart rate trends were nonlinear, and in the absence of any a priori predictions about those trends, we elected not to model them directly.
Second, we tested whether the heart rate effects were driven by any particular age range of infants. They were not: a regression of the within-subjects difference between mean heart rate during lullabies vs. non-lullabies on infant age found no significant effect (Supplementary Fig. 3; F(1, 137) = 1.22, p = 0.27, R2 = 0.01, omnibus test).
Third, we tested whether a match between the gender of the infant’s primary caregiver (as specified by the parent who attended the experiment with the infant) and the perceived gender of the singers predicted any difference in within-subjects main effects, because, for instance, when hearing male-sounding lullabies, those infants who have male primary caregivers may be likely to relax more than those infants with female primary caregivers, since male singers may sound more familiar to them. We found no evidence for such an effect: the within-subjects main effect was of comparable size across infants (main effect when gender of singer was matched to primary caregiver: M = −0.16, SD = 0.68, 95% CI [−0.32, 0.01]; main effect when gender of singer was not matched to primary caregiver: M = −0.14, SD = 0.57, 95% CI [−0.27, 0]; t(131.28) = 0.18, p = 0.85, independent samples two-tailed t-test).
Exploratory analyses
We conducted a series of exploratory analyses to test for convergent evidence supporting the preregistered result reported above, and to examine an alternate interpretation of the heart rate findings suggested by an anonymous reviewer: that rather than relaxing infants, the lullabies simply captured their attention more so than the other songs. Indeed, in some contexts, heart rate decreases can indicate increased attention to a stimulus53, and music is known to attract infants’ attention52. Additional measures can arbitrate between these interpretations.
First, we analyzed infants’ pupil dilation, an indicator of both attention to a stimulus54 and emotional arousal in response to it55, including during music listening56,57. If the lullabies relaxed infants, then pupil size should decrease during lullabies, relative to non-lullabies — contrasting sharply with an attention account for the heart rate findings, which would predict increases in pupil size.
Second, we analyzed infants’ electrodermal activity, an indicator of arousal used in prior studies of relaxation responses to music49,51. If the lullabies relaxed infants, then electrodermal activity should decrease during lullabies, relative to non-lullabies. Increased attention, however, does not imply a directional effect on electrodermal activity.
Third, we analyzed infants’ gaze towards the animated characters, and rate of blinking, as measures of interest in the songs. These measures do not bear on the relaxation hypothesis, but rather, they test the degree to which infants’ attention to the animated characters varied as a function of whether they were singing lullabies or non-lullabies.
Last, in two additional analyses (unrelated to the relaxation and attention accounts described above), we explored the degree to which the perceived infant-directedness of the songs was predictive of infants’ heart rates; and the degree to which parents made inferences about the different song types.
Relaxation response as indexed by pupillometry
Using videos of infants’ faces during the experiment, we developed a procedure for manual annotation of pupil size and obtained pupil size annotations for the singing trials (see Methods and Supplementary Figures 1 and 2). We normalized these annotations across all available data from each infant, after binning observations by second to reduce noise. We analyzed changes in pupil dilation over the course of a singing trial, collapsing across all trials; and tested for differences between lullabies and non-lullabies.
Consistent with a relaxation account, and in contrast to an attention account, pupils were smaller during lullabies than during non-lullabies (Fig. 3). We fit a random-effects linear model to the z-scored observations, predicted from the time course of each trial, with a random effect of trial (N = 3,096 binned relative pupil size observations from 30 infants, mean 103.2 observations per infant; likelihood ratio χ2 = 7.682, p = 0.021). The model showed that pupil size was smaller during lullabies than non-lullabies, on average (t(3086) = 2.507, p = 0.012, β = 0.089). We found no time-by-trial-type interaction; this is likely because pupil size appeared to regress to the mean by the end of each trial (see Fig. 3).
Relaxation response as indexed by electrodermal activity
We used the same normalization approach as the pupillometry analysis, because normalizing to the previous trial, as in the heart rate analyses, produced a distribution with unacceptably long tails (zs > 100). This is likely because the short trial length (14 s) affords only minimal variability in electrodermal activity, which generally changes much more slowly than does heart rate, inflating z-scored values. Normalization to the full experiment period produced a more acceptably narrow range of z-scores, such that applying the same trimming criterion as we used for heart rate (|z| > 5) resulted in the removal of only 4 observations of nearly 100,000.
First, we noted an overall positive trend in electrodermal activity throughout the study, irrespective of the songs the infant was listening to. We fit a random-effects linear model to all z-scored observations (N = 25,938 from 144 infants, mean 180 observations per infant), which showed that electrodermal activity steadily increased throughout the experiment, on average (t(25819) = 38.5, p < 0.001, β = 0.002).
Note that this result contrasts sharply with infants’ responses during a distress induction procedure, as in previous research on the calming effects of singing51. In that type of study, arousal and fussiness increase during a negative interaction (e.g., a still-face procedure), and subsequently decrease during a positive “recovery phase”. It is unsurprising, however, given the structure of this experiment: infants often become bored and fussy during repetitive experiments, increasing arousal.
As such, we measured the rate of increase in electrodermal activity, and analyzed changes in electrodermal activity as a function of lullaby or non-lullaby listening relative to this increase. This required centering the z-scores infant- and trial-wise. The key question is thus whether listening to a lullaby yields lower electrodermal activity than the predicted overall trial-wise increase, all else equal.
The results supported the relaxation account (Fig. 4). We fit a random-effects linear model of electrodermal activity change scores over time, trial-wise, so as to test for a time by song type interaction. The model fit was acceptable (likelihood ratio χ2 = 443, p < 0.001), the interaction term was significant (t(61174) = −10.3, p < 0.001, β = −0.006), and a general linear hypothesis test showed an expected difference in electrodermal activity between lullabies and non-lullabies at the end of the trail (time = 14 s; β = −0.075, 95% CI [−0.098, −0.052], χ2 = 289.2, p < 0.001, d = −0.25) These results indicate that lullabies attenuated increases in electrodermal activity.
Visual attention to singers
We ran two sets of exploratory analyses concerning infants’ visual attention to the animated characters, using manual annotations of infants’ duration of gaze toward them (see Methods). In previous research, infants demonstrated social preferences for a person who had previously sung a song familiar to the infant15,16; as such, we explored whether such a preference could be elicited purely on the basis of a difference in the types of songs a singer produced.
We found no evidence for such an effect. Infants looked for comparable durations to the two characters during singing trials (Supplementary Fig. 4; in seconds, lullabies: M = 8.1, SD = 2.57, 95% CI [7.68, 8.53]; non-lullabies: M = 7.92, SD = 2.77, 95% CI [7.46, 8.37]; t(144) = 0.68, p = 0.5, two-tailed paired t-test). The two one-sided test procedure for equivalence testing58 confirmed that these rates of attention were statistically equivalent (Δ = 1 s; ΔL : t(144) = 4.335,p < 0.001; ΔU : t(144) = −2.972, p = 0.002).
The same pattern was observed during the preference trials: attention to the two characters in silence, and after they had each sung a lullaby or non-lullaby, did not differ (Supplementary Fig. 4; attention in seconds to lullaby singer: M = 4.42, SD = 2.45, 95% CI [4.02, 4.82]; non-lullabies: M = 4.62, SD = 2.66, 95% CI [4.18, 5.05]; t(145) = −0.53, p = 0.6, two-tailed paired t-test). These rates were statistically equivalent (Δ = 1 s; ΔL : t(145) = 2.202, p = 0.015; ΔU : t(145) = −3.264, p < 0.001). Note that these analyses include a few more infants than the heart rate analyses do; this is because some infants completed the study and were subsequently excluded from the heart rate analyses due to a poor physiology monitor signal, but had usable gaze data.
As an additional exploratory measure, we counted the number of eye blinks during the singing trials, as blinks may index perceived stimulus salience59. Infants blinked slightly less during lullabies (number of blinks per trial: median = 1, interquartile range: 0.5–1.5) than non-lullabies (median = 1, interquartile range: 0.5–2), suggesting that they were more interested in the singers during lullabies than during non-lullabies (z = −2.25, p = 0.02, Wilcoxon two-tailed signed-rank test). But blinking was rare, so this exploratory result should be interpreted with caution, as it may be an artifact of restricted range.
Relation between songs’ infant-directedness and relaxation effects
The lullabies we studied differ acoustically from non-lullabies in a number of ways: they tend to be less accented, slower in tempo, have smaller pitch ranges, and have more variable macrometers than the other songs1. These features are reflected in naïve listeners’ ratings: the lullabies are perceived to have lower melodic and rhythmic complexity, slower tempo, less steady beat, lower arousal, lower valence, and lower pleasantness42. Together, these features predict the degree to which listeners perceive a song as infant-directed35,42.
Infants’ relaxation responses to lullabies should be explicable by their responses to these acoustic features. To test this, we asked whether we could predict variability in infant physiological responses as a function of the degree of infant-directedness of each song, using the adult ratings from prior work42. Modeling trials and participants as random effects in a linear regression, we predicted infant heart rate from songs’ perceived infant-directedness. We found a significant negative relationship, of modest size, such that the more infant-directed a song was, the larger the expected reduction in infant heart rate (t(15239) = −6, p < 0.001, β = −0.052). This result confirms that the acoustical features of the songs drove the relaxation effects on infants.
Parent intuitions about foreign lullabies
After each infant had participated in the experiment, we showed a parent the same animated characters that the infant had seen. The characters sang the 8 songs that were not presented during the experiment, such that the songs were unfamiliar to the parent.
We asked the parent, for each pair of songs, to choose the character whose song they would prefer to sing to soothe their infant, if the infant were fussy and the parent knew how to sing the songs. Given previous findings that adults are sensitive to the soothing functions of foreign lullabies1,42, we expected parents to choose the lullaby singers more often than the non-lullaby singers.
They did (Fig. 5). For the 4 pairs of songs, the median number of choices for the lullaby singer was 4 (all of them), a rate higher than the chance level of 2 choices (interquartile range: 3–4; z = −9.89, p < 0.001, Wilcoxon two-tailed signed-rank test).
Discussion
We found that infants relax in response to unfamiliar foreign lullabies. Relative to non-lullabies, infants’ heart rates slowed while listening to lullabies; this effect did not merely reflect attention-related heart rate deceleration, as it was accompanied by decreased pupil dilation and attenuated electrodermal activity. Moreover, the size of the heart rate effect remained steady across all ages of infants in the age range 2 – 14 months, suggesting that it was not altered by infants’ rapidly growing experience with music. And the effect was predictable as a function of the degree of infant-directedness of the songs, suggesting that a core set of acoustic features associated with infant-directedness across cultures produced the psychophysiological effects in the infants.
Infants were also highly attentive to the simple animated characters who produced the songs: they reliably attended to the characters for the majority of each singing trial, rarely blinking, and blinking modestly less during lullabies than non-lullabies. Moreover, parents uniformly chose lullabies over non-lullabies as the songs that they themselves would prefer to use to calm a fussy infant.
Infants and parents demonstrated all these behaviors without having previously learned anything about the music in question: they were given no cues as to the original behavioral contexts of the songs, as all the music was produced by solo voices without accompanying instruments; and they were unfamiliar with the songs they heard, unfamiliar with the languages they were sung in, and unfamiliar with the musical styles of the societies that originally produced the songs.
These findings support a hypothesized role for infant-directed song in the ecosystem of parental investment22,23 — including the proclivity of parents to sing to their infants6–8; the acoustic features that characterize infant-directed songs worldwide1,35,41–46; infants’ ability to perceive them and motivation to engage with them60,61; and their calming effects49–52 — that is both universal and innately specified.
Note, however, that we only studied infants and parents from a single Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) society62, who in this experiment heard music from many other societies. We expect that the findings will repeat with infants and parents in any society and are eager to find out whether they do.
The findings reported here may also be compatible with an alternative account. Over the course of early infancy, infants likely learn associations between soothing, sleep-inducing contexts and lullabies produced by their caregivers. Perhaps the infants we studied listened to the unfamiliar, foreign lullabies; found that they sounded somewhat similar to the lullabies that their caregivers produce; and subsequently relaxed. Such an account would not explain cross-cultural consistency in lullaby features, but these could have arisen via mechanisms other than innate specifications of infant responses (e.g., convergent cultural evolution63). The consistency of the relaxation effects across a full year of infancy (see Supplementary Fig. 3), a time when infant music perception is actively shaped by musical experience14, weighs against an experience-dependent interpretation, but we cannot rule it out.
Whether the relaxation effects reflect infants’ predispositions, early learning, or both, two aspects of infants’ responses to music are surprising. First, whereas prior work has demonstrated effects of music on infant arousal50,51, they were likely bolstered by mere-familiarity effects48, as infants have robust preferences for familiar, positive experiences. Here, because infants were unfamiliar with all aspects of the lullabies (including the languages in which they were sung and the societies in which they were recorded) the results imply a specific soothing effect of music, over and above any potential effects of familiarity. Second, despite the fact that lullabies are characterized by a universal set of acoustical features, there is nevertheless a great deal of variability in the lullabies infants heard (see stimuli at https://osf.io/2t6cy). This implies that infants’ responses are robust to a degree of musical variability, providing further support for the idea that infant-directed song induces relaxation in infants.
We leave open at least three series of questions about how and why infant-directed music works the way it does.
First, what is it about lullabies that makes them relaxing for infants? The acoustic features of lullabies differ from those of other songs in systematic ways, universally1: which of these features drive the relaxing effect of lullabies, and how? Do those features reflect evolved predispositions that are specific to music22,23? Or might they reflect general form-function principles of animal vocal signals, such as those that lead alarm signals to be consistently loud and harsh across species31,32,64? Future studies could test these questions by comparing infants’ physiological responses across different song types and across different acoustic signals; or by systematically manipulating the acoustic features present in songs to measure their relaxation effects on listeners. Prior research has outlined some musical features that parents in Western cultures exaggerate while singing lullabies43,65, which correspond with acoustic differences between infant- and adult-directed song across many societies35; these are good candidates for possible features that may drive relaxation effects.
Experiments in adults might also inform whether soothing effects of particular acoustic features in music could underlie later responses to music in adulthood. While adults no longer seek out parental investment, perhaps the musical features that soothe fussy infants (e.g., slower tempos, fewer rhythmic accents) correspond with musical features that shape adults’ emotional responses to music66,67, which could influence physiological responses to music listening.
Second, while infants in our studies listened to songs produced by simple animated characters in isolation, their real-world musical experiences are far richer, obviously. Parents sing to their infants in a multitude of environments (before a nap, in the car, during a bath) and as part of complex multimodal experiences including other actions (rocking, bouncing) and other content (stories, instructions). The relaxing properties of lullabies demonstrated here likely interact with all of these other features. Experiments that manipulate them — for example, by comparing the relaxation effects of music listening in isolation to those of music listening while being rocked — would more fully lay out the feature space of the infant’s musical experience, specifying what it is about lullabies that infants find satisfying in natural contexts of parental investment. Moreover, lullabies are but one of many forms of infant-directed songs; songs directed toward infants in the context of play68, which also appear universally1, likely have their own unique, contrasting features, and corresponding effects on infants.
Third, at present we know very little about what inferences infants make about the songs they hear or the people who sing them. In previous work, infants preferred the singer of a song that infants had previously learned in the context of live social interactions15,16. Here, although infants relaxed more when listening to lullabies, they showed no preference for the lullaby singer over the other singer during the silent preference trials or during singing. This suggests that infants’ physiological responses to music may be dissociated from their musical and/or social preferences. One possibility is that the lullabies are more relaxing than the other songs for infants, but that they do not necessarily prefer hearing the lullabies. Another is that infants do prefer listening to the lullabies over the other songs, but that this does not translate to a social preference for their singers. Infants’ social preferences for singers may rely instead on the context in which they learned the songs they know, such that infants consider singers of songs which they previously learned in a social interaction to be particularly good social partners or sources of parental investment15,16.
Taking this possibility a step further, might infants expect caregiving characters to produce infant-directed music for other distressed babies, just as they expect different adults who soothe the same baby to affiliate with one another69? Such results are plausible, given the known links between musical experience and infant social cognition15,16,70,71, but have not yet been tested.
However these lines of research play out, the present findings immediately suggest that singing is an effective means by which caregivers can relax infants, and raise the possibility of cumulative positive effects of music on infant and parent well-being. Live and recorded music have shown promise in improving a variety of clinical outcomes72,73, including in parents and infants74–78. Music may also play an everyday role in improving health in infants — a role it has taken on across cultures and across human history1.
Methods
Participants
This research was approved by the Committee on the Use of Human Subjects, Harvard University’s Institutional Review Board and complies with all relevant ethical regulations. Parents provided informed consent prior to their and their infant’s participation.
We recruited 144 typically-developing infants from the greater Boston area (72 females, mean age = 7.2 months, SD = 3.1, range: 2.1–14.3). Data from an additional 21 infants were collected but excluded due to infant fussiness (n = 11); lack of attention (n = 1); technical error (n = 8); or experimenter error (n = 1). Nearly all infants were born full-term. Information about language exposure was available from 98 of the participants; of these, none of the languages spoken at home matched those used in the stimuli of this study (see Table 1).
Infants who became fussy and ended their participation partway through the study were included in the analyses if they attended to the first pair of songs and the subsequent preference trial (see Stimuli, below). Most infants (n = 125) contributed data for all four song pairs and preference trials. For compensation, parents received a $5 gift card and infants were given a prize. All testing took place at the Music Lab at Harvard University.
Stimuli
We chose 16 songs from the Natural History of Song Discography1 that were originally produced in 15 different societies and languages (Table 1). Eight of the songs were infant-directed, having been used as lullabies (i.e., they were originally used to soothe, calm, or put an infant/child to sleep) in the societies where they were recorded, according to the anthropologist or ethnomusicologist who collected each recording. The other 8 songs were originally produced in the context of expressing love (5); healing the sick (2); or dancing (1).
We chose this particular subset of 16 songs by first limiting the corpus to those songs produced by a single singer with no instrumental accompaniment; then, using adults’ ratings of the songs from a previous study42, we chose a set of lullabies rated as likely to be “used to soothe a baby” and a set of non-lullabies with low ratings on that item.
We paired the lullabies and non-lullabies from these sets so as to match the perceived gender of the singer as closely as possible, because infants are sensitive to the gender of voices79. We ordered the pairs such that those with larger differences on the rating “used to soothe a baby” were presented first, so as to maximize the measurable differences in responses to lullabies vs. non-lullabies in each infant, even if they became inattentive or fussy partway through the study. All recordings were normalized to approximately balance their perceived loudness and were also manually edited to remove background noise and microphone artifacts, using noise reduction filters and equalization.
We generated animations of two characters who lip-synced to each song, giving the impression that they were singing (Fig. 1; videos are available at https://osf.io/2t6cy). Each character sang four songs, such that one exclusively sang lullabies while the other exclusively sang non-lullabies. The videos were counterbalanced on four dimensions: which was the first song heard (lullaby or non-lullaby), which character was the lullaby singer (red or blue), which side the lullaby singer appeared on (left or right, to match character placement during silent preference trials; see Procedure), and the perceived gender of the singer (male or female). This yielded 16 conditions, which we balanced across ages, such that each counterbalancing condition included infants across the full range of ages tested.
Regardless of the counterbalancing condition, we varied the presentation order of lullabies vs. non-lullabies, so that they did not appear in strict alternation, which could introduce order effects. This yielded trial orderings that were either P-L-N-P-N-L-P-L-N-P-N-L-P or P-N-L-P-L-N-P-N-L-P-L-N-P, where L denotes a lullaby singing trial, N denotes a non-lullaby singing trial, and P denotes a preference trial. Because there were two characters, and each character sang four songs, each infant in the experiment listened to 8 of the 16 songs.
Procedure
Infants sat in a high chair (n = 62), recliner (n = 57), or a parent’s lap (n = 25) approximately 150 cm away from a 107.5 × 60.5 cm television screen; parents chose the seat based on the physical size of the infant and whether the infant was comfortable sitting in it. When infants sat in a high chair or recliner the parent sat behind them. When infants sat on their parent’s lap, the parent listened to masking music through passive noise-canceling headphones throughout the experiment; we also asked parents to keep their eyes closed. We recorded videos of the infants at ultra high definition (8-bit 4K at 150Mbps; Panasonic Lumix GH5S and Lumix G Vario 14–140mm lens).
Fig. 1 depicts the order of events. The experiment began with a 14 s baseline preference trial, in which the two animated characters were presented simultaneously in silence. Four sets of three trials followed, with each set consisting of two singing trials and one preference trial. On the singing trials, one of the animated characters sang a song, appearing alone on the screen next to a screen-saver-like animation (to reduce the likelihood that infants would look only at the singer). Each singing trial was 14 s long. The preference trials were identical to the baseline preference trial. Attention-grabbing animations appeared at the center of the screen before each preference trial. The experiment lasted about five minutes. Experimenters were unaware of the order of presentation of stimuli and thus were unable to influence the infants’ behavior or responses in relation to the music they heard.
Characters on the screen were 25 cm wide. They were presented 45 cm apart when appearing simultaneously during the preference trials. Videos were presented at 4K resolution and audio played from two speakers (Neumann KH80 DSP) at approximately the height of the infants’ ears, 125 cm apart, placed such that the infant was seated at the apex of an equilateral triangle formed with the two speakers. The songs had a maximum volume of approximately 60 dB.
Psychophysiology
We recorded infant heart rate and electrodermal activity with a physiological monitor (Empatica E4) attached to the infant’s thigh or calf, depending on the size of the infant, and usually on the left side. The monitor records heart rate via a photoplethysmograph at the site of the device and electrodermal activity via electrodes attached to the side or bottom of the infant’s foot (with BIOPAC isotonic gel); it has been successfully validated in adults80.
Pupillometry
We developed a procedure to manually annotate pupil dilation and applied it to still images from 30 of the infants. We extracted still images of the infant’s face from the videos and used the dlib face recognition library81 to automatically rotate the frame, levelling the eye horizontally; and to isolate one of the infant’s eyes (we randomly selected either the left or right eye for each infant). Workers on Amazon Mechanical Turk then viewed each eye image (see Supplementary Fig. 1) and were asked (1) to adjust its brightness and contrast, so as to optimize visibility of the pupil; (2) to rate how visible the pupil was (from one of six options: Pupil is clearly visible; Pupil is visible, but it’s difficult to see; Pupil is NOT visible, but I could see enough of it to make a guess about its outline; Pupil is NOT visible but the eye is still open; Pupil is NOT visible because the eye is closed; Other); and (3) to draw a superimposed ellipse on the image, surrounding the visible area of the pupil.
We set two qualification criteria for workers based on their performance in 10 eye images: (1) a correlation of at least r = .8 between their annotations (i.e., width and height of their ellipses) and the mean annotations from a pilot study (N = 46 workers); and (2) in at least 7 of the 10 images, a matching visibility rating with the option selected by at least 15% of pilot participants. Workers were not aware of whether or not an image being annotated was counted toward the qualification, but they were told that their performance was being evaluated in real time.
The pool of qualified workers then annotated 3 images per second of infant video, drawn from the singing trials only and presented in a random order. Each worker annotated approximately 263 images and spent 19.5 s per image, on average (trimming top and bottom 1 percent). Four images per trial were “validation images” that were presented more than once to the same worker, providing a measure of internal reliability of the annotations. Reliability was high, as measured in two ways. First, visibility ratings were internally consistent (that is, validation images were generally classified repeatedly in the same fashion by annotators; see confusion matrix in Supplementary Fig. 2). Second, the annotated pupil sizes were internally consistent: validation annotations correlated with the original annotations at r = 0.88 (using total pupil area). The degree of reliability varied as a function of how visible the pupil was; validation images marked Pupil is clearly visible correlated at r = 0.95, whereas those marked Pupil is NOT visible, but I could see enough of it to make a guess about its outline correlated less strongly, at r = 0.86. Because the workers were unaware of what songs the infants were listening to at the time of each still image (or even that the infants were listening to music at all) they always remained blinded to the conditions of the study.
To produce the data used in analyses, we computed a relative pupil size measure by dividing the pupil area by the full eye area, in pixels and within-participants, so as to adjust for increases in visible pupil size due to motion toward or away from the camera (which would erroneously increase or decrease the visible area of the pupil, respectively). Last, we removed all observations above the 99th percentile and below the 1st percentile; these appeared to be impossibly large or small values due to face recognition errors in the automated image extraction.
Gaze and blinking
We manually annotated infant gaze and blinks, frame-by-frame at 60 fps using Datavyu82. Annotators always worked with the audio muted, so that they remained unaware of the songs each character sang, and thus were blinded to the conditions of the study.
For gaze, we randomly selected 20% of the videos, which a second person then annotated, independently of the first set of annotations. We assessed reliability by correlating trial-wise durations of gaze toward the two locations on the screen across pairs of annotators for each infant. Reliability was high (median r = .98, interquartile range: .90-.99).
For blinks, which are more difficult to annotate, and, given their sparsity, are more likely to produce internally unreliable annotations, we used a slightly different procedure. Two annotators independently annotated all the videos, and we assessed the reliability of each video’s annotations by correlating the two annotators’ trial-wise counts of blinks for each infant. The distribution of correlations was strongly left-skewed, with approximately ten low outliers (rs < .6). The annotators revisited those 10 videos and corrected any evident errors, or elected to drop these infants from analyses, because they disagreed about the timing and frequency of the blinking. The decision to drop these participants was made blind to the results of any analyses. Among the remaining participants (n = 140) reliability was high (median r = .94, interquartile range: .85–1).
Parent measures
After the infant completed the experiment, parents viewed videos of singing trials for the 8 songs which their infant had not heard during the study, using a tablet. For each pair, we asked parents to choose the song they would prefer to sing if their baby were fussy (and assuming the parent already knew how to sing both songs). We analyzed all available data, regardless of whether or not the parent’s infant had completed the experiment. Parents also completed a survey concerning their infant’s home musical environment, for use in a separate study.
Statistical power
We used data from a similar listening experiment in typically-developing adults28 to compute a plausible within-subjects effect size, based on the difference in mean heart rate during speech vs. song in people with Prader-Willi syndrome (d = 0.36). We chose a target sample size of N = 144 prior to running the experiment to provide power greater than .99 for the main planned comparison (i.e., mean heart rate during lullaby trials relative to non-lullaby trials). We also chose this sample size to facilitate even counterbalancing of stimuli across a wide range of infant ages, maximizing our ability to measure age effects while avoiding effects of stimulus ordering.
Data availability
All data reported here are available at https://github.com/themusiclab/infant-relax. Stimuli are available at https://osf.io/2t6cy. Audio excerpts from the Natural History of Song Discography are available at https://osf.io/vcybz; the full corpus can be explored interactively at https://themusiclab.org/nhsplots. For assistance with data or materials, please contact M.B., C.M.B., and S.A.M.
Code availability
A reproducible version of this manuscript, including all analysis and visualization code, is available at https://github.com/themusiclab/infant-relax. For assistance with code, please contact M.B., C.M.B., and S.A.M.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the infants and parents who participated in this research; J. Kominsky, N. Soja, W. Pepe, E. Spelke, and S. Carey for their support with participant recruitment; H. Alton, A. Bergson, A. Bitran, G. Jessani, A. Keomurjian, and B. Milosh for research assistance; and C. Payne and three anonymous reviewers for detailed and constructive feedback on the manuscript. This research was supported by the NIH Director’s Early Independence Award DP5OD024566, the Harvard University Department of Psychology, and the Harvard Data Science Initiative. The funders had no role in the conceptualization, design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Mehr SA et al. Universality and diversity in human song. Science 366, 957–970 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jacoby N. & McDermott JH Integer ratio priors on musical rhythm revealed cross-culturally by iterated reproduction. Curr. Biol. 27, 359–370 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jacoby N. et al. Universal and non-universal features of musical pitch perception revealed by singing. Curr. Biol. 29, 3229–3243 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ravignani A, Delgado T. & Kirby S. Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0007 (2016). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mendoza JK & Fausey CM Everyday music in infancy. PsyArXiv (2019) doi: 10.31234/osf.io/sqatb. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Custodero LA, Rebello Britto P. & Brooks-Gunn J. Musical lives: A collective portrait of American parents and their young children. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 24, 553–572 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- 7.Custodero LA & Johnson-Green EA Passing the cultural torch: Musical experience and musical parenting of infants. J. Res. Music Educ. 51, 102–114 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mehr SA Music in the home: New evidence for an intergenerational link. J. Res. Music Educ. 62, 78–88 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 9.Trehub SE, Hill DS & Kamenetsky SB Parents’ sung performances for infants. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 385–396 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Granier-Deferre C, Bassereau S, Ribeiro A, Jacquet A-Y & DeCasper AJ A melodic contour repeatedly experienced by human near-term fetuses elicits a profound cardiac reaction one month after birth. PLoS ONE 6, e17304 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zentner MR & Kagan J. Perception of music by infants. Nature 383, 29 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Winkler I, Háden GP, Ladinig O, Sziller I. & Honing H. Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 2468–2471 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hannon EE, Schachner A. & Nave-Blodgett JE Babies know bad dancing when they see it: Older but not younger infants discriminate between synchronous and asynchronous audiovisual musical displays. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 159, 159–174 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hannon EE & Trehub SE Tuning in to musical rhythms: Infants learn more readily than adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 12639–12643 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mehr SA, Song LA & Spelke ES For 5-month-old infants, melodies are social. Psychol. Sci. 27, 486–501 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mehr SA & Spelke ES Shared musical knowledge in 11-month-old infants. Dev. Sci. 21, (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Trainor LJ, Wu L. & Tsang CD Long-term memory for music: Infants remember tempo and timbre. Dev. Sci. 7, 289–296 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Volkova A, Trehub SE & Schellenberg EG Infants’ memory for musical performances. Dev. Sci. 9, 583–589 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hrdy SB Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. (Harvard University Press, 2009). [Google Scholar]
- 20.Trivers RL Parental investment and sexual selection. in Sexual selection and the descent of man (ed. Campbell BG) 136–179 (Aldine, 1972). [Google Scholar]
- 21.Soltis J. The signal functions of early infant crying. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 443–458 (2004). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mehr SA & Krasnow MM Parent-o spring conflict and the evolution of infant-directed song. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 674–684 (2017). [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mehr SA, Krasnow M, Bryant GA & Hagen EH Origins of music in credible signaling. PsyArXiv (2020) doi: 10.31234/osf.io/nrqb3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Haig D. & Wharton R. Prader-Willi syndrome and the evolution of human childhood. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 15, 320–329 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ubeda F. Evolution of genomic imprinting with biparental care: Implications for Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes. PLoS Biol. 6, 1678–1692 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cassidy SB & Driscoll DJ Prader-Willi syndrome. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 17, 3–13 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Williams CA et al. Angelman syndrome 2005: Updated consensus for diagnostic criteria. Am. J. Med. Genet. 140, 413–418 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mehr SA, Kotler J, Howard RM, Haig D. & Krasnow MM Genomic imprinting is implicated in the psychology of music. Psychol. Sci. 28, 1455–1467 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kotler J, Mehr SA, Egner A, Haig D. & Krasnow MM Response to vocal music in Angelman syndrome contrasts with Prader-Willi syndrome. Evol. Hum. Behav. 40, 420–426 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Maynard Smith J. & Harper D. Animal signals. (Oxford University Press, 2003). [Google Scholar]
- 31.Morton ES On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am. Nat. 111, 855–869 (1977). [Google Scholar]
- 32.Owren MJ & Rendall D. Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evol. Anthropol 10, 58–71 (2001). [Google Scholar]
- 33.Endler JA Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 340, 215–225 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Filippi P. et al. Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: Evidence for acoustic universals. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 284, (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Moser CJ et al. Acoustic regularities in infant-directed vocalizations across cultures. bioRxiv (2020) doi: 10.1101/2020.04.09.032995v1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fernald A. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Dev. 60, 1497–1510 (1989). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kuhl PK et al. Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277, 684–686 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Piazza EA, Iordan MC & Lew-Williams C. Mothers consistently alter their unique vocal fingerprints when communicating with infants. Curr. Biol. 27, 3162–3167 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Broesch T. & Bryant GA Fathers’ infant-directed speech in a small-scale society. Child Dev. 89, e29–e41 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bryant GA & Barrett HC Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech: Evidence for universals. Psychol. Sci. 18, 746–751 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Trehub SE, Unyk AM & Trainor LJ Adults identify infant-directed music across cultures. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 193–211 (1993). [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mehr SA, Singh M, York H, Glowacki L. & Krasnow MM Form and function in human song. Curr. Biol. 28, 356–368 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Trainor LJ, Clark ED, Huntley A. & Adams BA The acoustic basis of preferences for infant-directed singing. Infant Behav. Dev. 20, 383–396 (1997). [Google Scholar]
- 44.Trehub SE et al. Mothers’ and fathers’ singing to infants. Dev. Psychol. 33, 500–507 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gomez P. & Danuser B. Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion 7, 377–387 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rock A, Trainor L. & Addison T. Distinctive messages in infant-directed lullabies and play songs. Dev. Psychol. 35, 527–534 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tan X, Yowler CJ, Super DM & Fratianne RB The interplay of preference, familiarity and psychophysical properties in defining relaxation music. J. Music Ther. 49, 150–179 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zajonc RB Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 224–228 (2001). [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cirelli LK, Jurewicz ZB & Trehub SE Effects of maternal singing style on mother–infant arousal and behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. (2019) doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Garunkstiene R, Buinauskiene J, Uloziene I. & Markuniene E. Controlled trial of live versus recorded lullabies in preterm infants. Nord. J. Music Ther. 23, 71–88 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 51.Cirelli LK & Trehub SE Familiar songs reduce infant distress. Dev. Psychol. (2020) doi: 10.1037/dev0000917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Corbeil M, Trehub SE & Peretz I. Singing delays the onset of infant distress. Infancy 21, 373–391 (2016). [Google Scholar]
- 53.Richards JE & Cronise K. Extended visual fixation in the early preschool years: Look duration, heart rate changes, and attentional inertia. Child Dev. 71, 602–620 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Laeng B, Sirois S. & Gredebäck G. Pupillometry: A window to the preconscious? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 18–27 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA & Lang PJ The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45, 602–607 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Laeng B, Eidet LM, Sulutvedt U. & Panksepp J. Music chills: The eye pupil as a mirror to music’s soul. Conscious. Cogn. 44, 161–178 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Widmann A, Schröger E. & Wetzel N. Emotion lies in the eye of the listener: Emotional arousal to novel sounds is reflected in the sympathetic contribution to the pupil dilation response and the P3. Biol. Psychol 133, 10–17 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lakens D, Scheel AM & Isager PM Equivalence testing for psychological research: A tutorial. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 1, 259–269 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shultz S, Klin A. & Jones W. Inhibition of eye blinking reveals subjective perceptions of stimulus salience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 21270–21275 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Trehub SE Musical predispositions in infancy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 1–16 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hannon EE & Trainor LJ Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 466–72 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Henrich J, Heine SJ & Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Sperber D. & Hirschfeld LA The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 40–46 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Blumstein DT, Bryant GA & Kaye P. The sound of arousal in music is context-dependent. Biol. Lett. 8, 744–747 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Trainor LJ Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant-directed playsongs and lullabies. Infant Behav. Dev. 19, 83–92 (1996). [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sievers B, Polansky L, Casey M. & Wheatley T. Music and movement share a dynamic structure that supports universal expressions of emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 70–75 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Cowen AS, Fang X, Sauter D. & Keltner D. What music makes us feel: At least 13 dimensions organize subjective experiences associated with music across different cultures. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. (2020) doi: 10.1073/pnas.1910704117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Trehub SE & Trainor L. Singing to infants: Lullabies and play songs. Adv. Infancy Res. 12, 43–78 (1998). [Google Scholar]
- 69.Spokes AC & Spelke ES The cradle of social knowledge: Infants’ reasoning about caregiving and a liation. Cognition 159, 102–116 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Soley G. & Sebastián-Gallés N. Infants prefer tunes previously introduced by speakers of their native language. Child Dev. 86, 1685–1692 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tsang CD, Falk S. & Hessel A. Infants prefer infant-directed song over speech. Child Dev. 88, 1207–1215 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Hole J, Hirsch M, Ball E. & Meads C. Music as an aid for postoperative recovery in adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet 386, 1659–1671 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Richard-Lalonde M. et al. The effect of music on pain in the adult intensive care unit: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. J. Pain Symptom Manage. (2019) doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2019.12.359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bieleninik £, Ghetti C. & Gold C. Music therapy for preterm infants and their parents: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics e20160971 (2016) doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Bo LK, Mn B. & Callaghan P. Soothing pain-elicited distress in Chinese neonates. Pediatrics 105, e49–e49 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Keith DR, Russell K. & Weaver BS The effects of music listening on inconsolable crying in premature infants. J. Music Ther. 46, 191–203 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Filippa M, Devouche E, Arioni C, Imberty M. & Gratier M. Live maternal speech and singing have beneficial effects on hospitalized preterm infants. Acta Paediatr. 102, 1017–1020 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Fancourt D. & Perkins R. Could listening to music during pregnancy be protective against postnatal depression and poor wellbeing post birth? Longitudinal associations from a preliminary prospective cohort study. BMJ Open 8, e021251 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Miller CL Developmental changes in male/female voice classification by infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 6, 313–330 (1983). [Google Scholar]
- 80.van Lier HG et al. A standardized validity assessment protocol for physiological signals from wearable technology: Methodological underpinnings and an application to the E4 biosensor. Behav. Res. Methods 52, 607–629 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.King DE Dlib-ml: A machine learning toolkit. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 10, 1755–1758 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- 82.Datavyu Team. Datavyu: A video coding tool. (Databrary Project, New York University, 2014). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data reported here are available at https://github.com/themusiclab/infant-relax. Stimuli are available at https://osf.io/2t6cy. Audio excerpts from the Natural History of Song Discography are available at https://osf.io/vcybz; the full corpus can be explored interactively at https://themusiclab.org/nhsplots. For assistance with data or materials, please contact M.B., C.M.B., and S.A.M.