Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 May 14;10:e59437. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59437

Do wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society?

Adrian V Jaeggi 1,2,†,, Aaron D Blackwell 3,†,, Christopher von Rueden 4, Benjamin C Trumble 5,6, Jonathan Stieglitz 7, Angela R Garcia 2,5,6, Thomas S Kraft 8, Bret A Beheim 9, Paul L Hooper 10,11, Hillard Kaplan 10, Michael Gurven 8
Editors: Milagros Ruiz12, George H Perry13
PMCID: PMC8225390  PMID: 33988506

Abstract

In high-income countries, one’s relative socio-economic position and economic inequality may affect health and well-being, arguably via psychosocial stress. We tested this in a small-scale subsistence society, the Tsimane, by associating relative household wealth (n = 871) and community-level wealth inequality (n = 40, Gini = 0.15–0.53) with a range of psychological variables, stressors, and health outcomes (depressive symptoms [n = 670], social conflicts [n = 401], non-social problems [n = 398], social support [n = 399], cortisol [n = 811], body mass index [n = 9,926], blood pressure [n = 3,195], self-rated health [n = 2523], morbidities [n = 1542]) controlling for community-average wealth, age, sex, household size, community size, and distance to markets. Wealthier people largely had better outcomes while inequality associated with more respiratory disease, a leading cause of mortality. Greater inequality and lower wealth were associated with higher blood pressure. Psychosocial factors did not mediate wealth-health associations. Thus, relative socio-economic position and inequality may affect health across diverse societies, though this is likely exacerbated in high-income countries.

Research organism: Human

eLife digest

Poverty is bad for health. People living in poverty are more likely to struggle to afford nutritious food, lack access to health care, or be overworked or stressed. This may make them susceptible to chronic diseases, contribute to faster aging, and shorten their lifespans. In high-income countries, there is growing evidence to suggest that a person’s ‘rank’ in society also impacts their health. For example, individuals who have a lower position in the social hierarchy report worse health outcomes, regardless of their incomes. But it is unclear why living in an unequal society or having a lower social status contributes to poorer health.

One possibility is that inequalities in society are creating a stressful environment that leads to worse physical and mental outcomes. It is thought that this stress largely comes from how humans evolved to prioritize reaching a higher social status over having a long and healthy life. If this is the case, this would mean that the link between social status and health would also be present in non-industrialized communities where social hierarchies tend to be less pronounced.

To test this, Jaeggi, Blackwell et al. studied the Indigenous Tsimane population in Bolivia who live in small communities and forage and farm their own food. The income and relative wealth of 870 households from 40 Tsimane communities were compared against various outcomes, including symptoms associated with depression, stress hormone levels, blood pressure, self-rated health and several diseases.

Jaeggi, Blackwell et al. found poverty and inequality did not negatively impact all of the health outcomes measured as has been previously reported for industrialized societies. However, blood pressure was higher among people with lower incomes or those who lived in more unequal communities. But because the Tsimane people generally have low blood pressure, the differences were too small to have much effect on their health. People who lived in more unequal communities were also three times more likely to have respiratory infections, but the reason for this was unclear.

This shows that social determinants such as a person’s wealth or inequality can affect health, even in communities with less rigid social hierarchies. In industrial societies the effect may be worse in part because they are compounded by lifestyle factors, such as diets rich in fat and sugar, and physical inactivity which can also increase blood pressure. This information may help policy makers reduce health disparities by addressing some of the social determinants of health and the lifestyle factors that cause them.

Introduction

It is relatively uncontroversial that people with greater access to resources – usually operationalized as income, wealth, or broader indicators of socio-economic position, rank, or status (used interchangeably here) – are likely to be in better health as resources can be converted into better nutritional status, access to health care, or insulation against health risks. Such benefits of absolute rank are also commonly found in nonhuman primates (Cowlishaw and Dunbar, 1991; Pusey et al., 1997; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020; van Noordwijk and van Schaik, 1999). However, there is increasing evidence that relative access to resources, that is, one’s relative position in a socio-economic hierarchy, may also affect health. Across developed societies, there is causal evidence for a health gradient along socio-economic hierarchies, independent of absolute wealth or use of health-care services (Ecob and Davey Smith, 1999; Marmot et al., 1991; Oakes et al., 1973; Sorlie et al., 1995; Wolfson et al., 1993). In other words, these studies find that relative rank – how one compares to others – is a critical variable in determining health outcomes (Anderson et al., 2012; Luttmer, 2005; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020; Wood et al., 2012).

The steepness of socio-economic hierarchies (i.e., income or wealth inequality) is also associated with both physical and mental health outcomes – including self-rated health, all-cause mortality, heart disease, respiratory disease, obesity, or homicide rates – independent of absolute wealth (Nowatzki, 2012; Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015; Wilkinson and Pickett, 2006). While these findings are hotly debated and tests of this inequality hypothesis have been critiqued on methodological grounds (Kondo et al., 2009; Lynch et al., 2004; Macinko et al., 2003; Subramanian and Kawachi, 2004; Wagstaff and van Doorslaer, 2000), a formal meta-analysis on studies controlling for individual wealth found significant associations between inequality and mortality or self-rated health in high-income countries (Kondo et al., 2009). Thus, relative position in a socio-economic hierarchy and the steepness of such hierarchies seem to matter for health.

The most cited mechanism for such hierarchy-health associations is that hierarchies cause psychosocial stress, which in turn leads to poorer health outcomes (Chen and Miller, 2013; Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015). Chronic stress leads to altered hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis function, including chronically elevated cortisol levels. Increased cortisol can cause neural atrophy, cardiovascular damage, obesity, or immunosuppression, all resulting in increased susceptibility to chronic and infectious disease (Aiello et al., 2018; García et al., 2017; Kunz-Ebrecht et al., 2004; Quon and McGrath, 2014; Sapolsky, 2004). In addition, submission in status competition and learned helplessness are associated with depression in humans and other primates (Hagen, 2011; Nesse, 2000; Stieglitz et al., 2014). Experimental studies in nonhuman primates show that dominance rank also affects gene expression and immune function (Snyder-Mackler et al., 2016; Tung et al., 2012). Related results in humans show that early life experiences and other forms of social stress are also associated with increases in inflammation and blunted immunological responses to cortisol (Aiello et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2011; Miller et al., 2009).

But why are hierarchies stressful or otherwise detrimental to health? An evolutionary-medicine perspective suggests that many detrimental health outcomes may result from adaptive tradeoffs, as fitness gains are prioritized over detrimental health outcomes, developmental constraints, as long-term negative health effects result from short-term accommodations to conditions during development, or from evolutionary mismatch, as our bodies struggle to deal with conditions atypical for our species (Eaton et al., 2002; Gluckman et al., 2016; Lea et al., 2017; Nesse and Williams, 1994; Wells et al., 2017). Given the consistent fitness benefits of high status (Stulp et al., 2016; Von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016), and given that fitness is always relative, humans arguably have evolved motivations for status-striving that are independent of one’s absolute access to resources (Johnson et al., 2012; Shenk et al., 2016). Status-striving activates the stress response, and not just for low-rankers: depending on how rank is achieved and maintained, high- or low-ranking individuals may be more stressed (Abbott et al., 2003; Sapolsky, 2005). Crucial to who is stressed is the availability of social support, which can be as or even more important for health and fitness as rank per se (Sapolsky, 2005; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020). Other factors primarily impact low-ranking individuals: in many primate (and some human) societies, subordinates are regularly subjected to aggression and intimidation by higher-ranking individuals (Silk, 2003), resulting in the lack of control and learned helplessness that often cause depression (Sapolsky, 2005; Sapolsky, 2004).

Greater inequality, that is, steeper hierarchies, entails more skewed payoff distributions and thus also favors more intense competition and risk-taking as behavioral strategies, especially among low-ranking individuals; this is argued to explain the persistent association between income inequality and homicide rates as most homicides result from escalated contests over status (Daly, 2016; Daly and Wilson, 1997). If skewed payoff distributions and oppression of low-rankers favor life-history strategies focused on short-term payoffs (i.e., ‘faster’ life-history strategies sensu Wells et al., 2017), this could also explain hierarchy-health associations via present-oriented decision-making at the expense of long-term health (Daly and Wilson, 1997; Griskevicius et al., 2011; Pepper and Nettle, 2014). These relationships are expected even when hierarchies are based on prestige, rather than dominance, since prestige-based hierarchies still correlate with social support, insulation against shocks, influence, sense of control, and access to mates (Gurven et al., 2000; Sugiyama and Sugiyama, 2003; von Rueden et al., 2014).

Thus, stress and negative health consequences due to socio-economic hierarchies can result from perpetual status-striving, unequal distribution of social support, lack of control and learned helplessness, intensified competition especially among low-ranking individuals, and from physiological accommodations to generally ‘faster’ life histories. In short, hierarchies may cause stress and affect health largely because individuals engage in competitive strategies that function to maximize fitness at the expense of health, while failure to succeed in such competition negatively affects mental health. In addition, if hierarchies constrain access to resources individuals may face developmental constraints, causing long-term tradeoffs that negatively impact health.

While such adjustments of physiology and behavior to the local competitive environment may in principle generalize to all human societies, the effects of hierarchy on health may be exacerbated in industrialized, high-income countries due to mismatch. Specifically, such societies could represent a mismatch with the ancestral environments in which our competitive strategies have evolved because (i) socio-economic hierarchies may be steeper and more rigid than was typical of our hunter-gatherer ancestors (Borgerhoff Mulder et al., 2009; Kaplan et al., 2009), and include features such as lack of kin support, limited upward mobility, structural violence, and systemic racism, all of which are well-known to negatively affect health (Gravlee, 2009; Sapolsky, 2004); and (ii) novel lifestyle factors such as obesogenic diets, lack of physical activity, and chronic inflammation turn previously relatively harmless responses, such as temporarily elevated blood pressure or depressed mood, into ‘mismatch’ diseases, such as hypertension, atherosclerosis, and major depression (Gurven et al., 2012; Kaplan et al., 2017; Miller and Raison, 2016; Stieglitz et al., 2015). In sum, mismatch diseases often arise when risk factors that used to elicit an adequate acute response become chronic problems, which could well be the case with modern socio-economic hierarchies, and interact with novel lifestyle factors that push our physiology into novel and unhealthy ranges.

In summary, humans, much like other primates, are sensitive to their relative rank and the distribution of fitness outcomes, and adjust behavior and physiological responses accordingly, resulting in negative influences of hierarchy on health. Several open questions remain, however. First, the inequality hypothesis remains hotly debated since parsing inequality from other correlated variables is difficult and requires careful statistical methods. Second, it remains unclear to what extent the observed health consequences of relative status and inequality in high-income countries (i) represent tradeoffs of potentially adaptive responses to lower relative rank and/or to inequality, or (ii) are caused by evolutionary mismatch, that is, novel conditions that cause maladaptive outcomes. If health consequences stem from tradeoffs from adaptive responses, then hierarchy should be associated with health in any population, independent of absolute access to resources. However, if the impacts of status and inequality are caused by evolutionary mismatch, then we would not expect detrimental effects on health in all societies, though we might observe related physiological responses in a subclinical range.

Small-scale societies practicing traditional subsistence lifestyles (henceforth ‘subsistence societies’) are an important test case for the universality of hierarchy-health associations as they generally have more informal, egalitarian hierarchies with relatively high individual autonomy and mobility (Borgerhoff Mulder et al., 2009; Kaplan et al., 2009; Mattison et al., 2016), and suffer from infectious rather than chronic disease as major sources of morbidity and mortality (Eaton et al., 1988; Gurven et al., 2007; Gurven et al., 2016; Gurven and Kaplan, 2007; Kaplan et al., 2017; Pontzer et al., 2018). Further, individuals in many subsistence societies have immune systems that are well calibrated by frequent exposure to pathogens and microbiota, and predominantly experience acute responses to infections (Blackwell et al., 2016a; McDade, 2005), unlike the chronic low-grade inflammation that links stress to hypertension, cardiovascular disease, and depression in high-income countries (Gurven et al., 2008a). Lastly, competition for mates and resources in such societies is usually fairly local, meaning that the scale at which relative rank and inequality should be measured is more recognizable than in large-scale modern societies with mass media, where people are simultaneously part of many hierarchies. Thus, subsistence societies may help us discern whether associations between hierarchy and health are caused by tradeoffs expected in any society, by evolutionary mismatch in modern, industrialized populations, or a combination of both.

Few studies have examined associations between rank or inequality and health in subsistence societies. Among Dominican farmers, socio-economic indicators were unrelated to cortisol levels whereas local influence was associated with lower cortisol (Decker, 2000). Among egalitarian Garisakang horticulturalists in Papua New Guinea, higher income coming from greater market exposure was associated with higher cortisol, whereas other locally relevant measures of wealth and status were not (Konečná and Urlacher, 2017). While results are mixed, there is some converging evidence that suggests market integration generates psychosocial stress in subsistence societies, arguably due to the threat of cultural loss and discrimination often experienced through contact with majority groups.

Among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists in Bolivia, it has been reported that traditional forms of status generally support a status-health gradient, but studies on income or wealth show mixed results. In a sample of four communities, politically influential men had lower cortisol and a lower incidence of respiratory infection, though there were also many null results, and higher income was associated with higher cortisol (von Rueden et al., 2014). In one village, women’s political influence was associated with improved growth and health outcomes for their children (Alami et al., 2020). Across 13 Tsimane villages, relative wealth was associated with better self-reported health (Undurraga et al., 2010); however, average self-reported health was lower in wealthier villages. In a larger sample of villages, relative income associated with lower body mass index (BMI) among individuals with smaller support networks (Brabec et al., 2007).

In terms of the relationship between inequality and health within communities, studies among Tsimane have also shown mixed results. One study found no association between income inequality and body fat (Godoy et al., 2005), but income inequality was associated with more negative emotions (Godoy et al., 2006). Greater wealth inequality did not associate with self-reported health in one study (Undurraga et al., 2010) but did associate with better self-reported health and lower self-reported stress in another, controlling for individual and village wealth level (Undurraga et al., 2016). Overall, these results provide mixed evidence for associations between inequality and health.

Here, we test for links between hierarchy and health among the Tsimane, expanding upon previous studies in several ways. First, we simultaneously assess the effects of within-community-relative wealth, mean community wealth, and community-level wealth inequality. Second, while previous studies have mostly relied on just one or two indirect health outcomes such as BMI, we include 13 different dependent variables (Table 1) capturing various health outcomes, including infectious disease morbidity, psychological well-being, social conflicts and connections, and other stressors. Third, we explicitly test whether these psychological and social variables and other stressors (henceforth ‘psychosocial variables’) mediate links between wealth and health as predicted if the adverse health effects of hierarchy occur through psychosocial stress. Note though that some of these ‘psychosocial’ variables may also be associated with health through more direct mechanisms, for example, non-social problems (food insecurity, debt, etc.) may cause stress but also represent poorer access to resources, which could affect health through energetic constraints. Fourth, we greatly increase the sample size relative to previous studies with inequality measured in 40 communities and wealth in 871 households, representing approximately one quarter of the Tsimane population (see Table 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Thus, our study represents the most comprehensive test of hierarchy-health associations in a subsistence society.

Table 1. Overview of study variables and descriptive statistics.

For an overview of the sample relative to all people known to the Tsimane Health and Life History Project and at risk of having wealth data, see Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Variable N Obs Median SD Min Max
Adult outcomes: psychosocial
Depression (possible range 16–64) 528 670 40.0 7.1 23.0 62.0
Conflicts (possible range 0–4) 342 401 2.0 0.7 0.0 4.0
Labor partners (count)* 304 399 2.0 2.0 1.0 13.0
Non-social problems (possible range 0–7) 339 398 3.0 1.2 0.0 7.0
Urinary cortisol (pg/ml) 588 811 155,191 149,602 93 851,308
Adult outcomes: health
Body mass index (kg/m2) † 1901 5179 23.3 2.8 16.0 36.6
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) 1622 3195 110.0 12.8 60.0 190.0
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) 1622 3195 70.0 10.0 24.0 136.0
Self-rated health (1 excellent to 5 very bad) 1307 2523 4.0 0.5 1.0 5.0
Total morbidity (possible range 0–18)‡ 1306 1542 2.0 1.1 0.0 5.0
Infections/parasites (yes/no) ‡ 1306 1542 25.2%
Respiratory disease (yes/no) ‡ 1306 1542 21.9%
Gastrointestinal (yes/no) ‡ 1306 1542 36.3%
Adult predictors
Age (years) 1931 5383 35.0 15.1 16.0 91.0
Sex (0 = female, 1 = male) 1931 5383 46.2
Juvenile outcomes: health
Body mass index (kg/m2) 1765 4747 16.6 2.1 10.2 27.6
Total morbidity (count) 1423 1569 1.0 0.8 0.0 4.0
Infections/parasites (yes/no) ‡ 1423 1569 13.6%
Respiratory disease (yes/no) ‡ 1423 1569 42.4%
Gastrointestinal (yes/no) ‡ 1423 1569 41.2%
Juvenile predictors
Age (years) 1772 4783 7.0 4.1 0.0 15.0
Sex (0 = female, 1 = male) 1772 4783 49.6
Household predictors
Household size 871 1045 4.0 2.7 1.0 14.0
Household wealth (Bs) 871 1045 7675 5675 386 56,664
Community predictors
Community size (adults > 15) 40 55 72.0 81.2 27.0 346.0
Distance to market town (km) 40 55 43.0 44.2 5.0 140.0
Mean community wealth (Bs) 40 55 8373 2331 3930 16,250
Community wealth inequality (Gini) 40 55 0.27 0.07 0.15 0.53
*

Reverse coded in analyses to make higher values worse outcomes.

Whether higher or lower body mass index is better is a bit ambiguous: in high-income countries higher body mass index is associated with worse health, lower status, and greater inequality, whereas in low-income countries the reverse may be true.

See Table 2 for an overview of the most common morbidities by category.

We specifically test the following predictions stemming from the hypotheses that relative socio-economic position as well as the steepness of socio-economic hierarchies affect health and well-being, and that these effects are mediated by psychosocial stress.

  • P1: Higher relative wealth is associated with better psychosocial and health outcomes.

  • P2a: Greater wealth inequality is associated with worse psychosocial and health outcomes, and P2b: this should hold especially for low-rankers.

  • P3: Psychosocial variables mediate wealth and inequality-health links found under P1 and P2.

Table 1 gives an overview of all variables used to test these predictions.

Study population

The Tsimane are a population of >16,000 Indigenous Amerindians living in >90 communities at the edge of the Amazon basin in lowland Bolivia. Tsimane communities consist of dispersed household clusters tied together by networks of kinship, cooperative production and consumption (Hooper et al., 2015; Jaeggi et al., 2016), as well as usually a school and soccer field. Community meetings convene to discuss and resolve important matters, including conflicts within the community. As such, we treat the community as the salient scale of status competition (Alami et al., 2020; von Rueden et al., 2018; von Rueden et al., 2008; von Rueden et al., 2019; von Rueden et al., 2014) and calculated relative wealth and inequality at this level.

The Tsimane remained relatively isolated from the larger Bolivian economy until the 1970s and still widely practice traditional subsistence (swidden horticulture, hunting, and fishing), which contributes >90% of their calories (Gurven et al., 2017; Kraft et al., 2018). Cattle, introduced by missionaries and ranchers, are owned by a small minority of Tsimane. Over the past few decades, wage labor opportunities with loggers or ranchers and produce sales in the local market towns of San Borja and Yucumo have been increasing, as have formal schooling, Spanish fluency, and access to modern amenities such as electricity and health care. The population thus exhibits quantifiable gradients of modernization (see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Overview of wealth and inequality distributions.

(A) Mean wealth by age of household head. (B) Mean wealth by population-level wealth Z-score. (C) Map of study communities (n = 40) and mean wealth at the community level. (D) Map of community-level wealth inequality. Note: (A) and (B) use raw wealth, while (C) and (D) are based on age-corrected values. Heat maps in (C) and (D) give a rough sense of the distribution; circle size indicates the number of sampled households (range = 9–81). Data for individual villages are not directly shown to protect confidentiality. Yucumo and San Borja are local market towns inhabited by non-Tsimane, Mission is the site of a Catholic mission and the largest Tsimane settlement.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Overview of the sample.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Ever sampled by THLHP refers to the period potentially included in this study, that is, up to December 2015; note that this sample includes 92 communities. Target communities (N = 40) were those with any wealth data collected during the periods included here (2006/2007, 2013). The main reason why 2075 people who lived in a target community did not have wealth data is likely that no one in their household was available to be interviewed about their assets, most likely because they were temporarily absent from the community (e.g., people sometimes stay near their far-away fields, go on extended hunting trips, or visit town or other communities). The majority of the 681 individuals who lived in a household with wealth data but lacked age, sex, and data on at least one of the outcome variables were most likely small children and infants who had not yet been sampled in detail. For a further missingness breakdown of the sample by specific outcome variable, see Table 1. THLHP: Tsimane Health and Life History Project.

In terms of morbidity and mortality, the Tsimane are characterized by high infectious disease burden, with respiratory infections as the leading cause of death at all ages (Gurven et al., 2007). Additionally, parasites such as helminths and giardia are highly prevalent (Blackwell et al., 2013; see also Table 2). These conditions result in frequent, acute immune responses (Blackwell et al., 2016a) but still a low incidence of chronic conditions such as hypertension or atherosclerosis, due to high levels of physical activity and other protective factors (Gurven et al., 2012; Gurven et al., 2016; Gurven et al., 2009; Kaplan et al., 2017).

Table 2. Overview of the most common morbidities.

Three of the most common Clinical Classifications System (CCS) categories (number in parentheses) and the six most prevalent diagnoses within each category (in decreasing order down rows, ICD-10 codes in parentheses). Musculoskeletal conditions (CCS 13) were also common but not analyzed independently.

Infectious and parasitic diseases(CCS 1) Diseases of the respiratory system (CCS 8) Diseases of the digestive system (CCS 9)
Pediculosis due to Pediculus humanus capitis (B85.0) Acute nasopharyngitis (common cold) (J00) Intestinal helminthiasis (B82.0)
Tinea unguium (B35.1) Acute streptococcal tonsillitis; unspecified (J03.00) Infectious gastroenteritis and colitis (A09)
Candidiasis of vulva and vagina (B37.3) Streptococcal pharyngitis (J02.0) Dyspepsia (K30)
Pediculosis; unspecified (B85.2) Acute upper respiratory infection; unspecified (J06.9) Gastro-esophageal reflux disease with esophagitis (K21.0)
Superficial mycosis; unspecified (B36.9) Acute bronchitis due to Mycoplasma pneumonia (J20.0) Giardiasis (lambliasis) (A07.1)
Necatoriasis (B76.1) Bronchopneumonia; unspecified organism (J18.0) Gastritis; unspecified, without bleeding (K29.70)

Results

Tsimane wealth and inequality

Because wealth varied considerably by age (Figure 1A), we used an age-corrected measure of relative wealth that reflects one’s wealth relative to this age trajectory (see Materials and methods). This corrects for random variation in the age structure of sampled communities and arguably better captures the essence of relative socio-economic rank: what matters is how one compares to others, relative to general trends such as wealth (status, influence, etc.) accumulating with age. At the high end of the wealth distribution (Figure 1B), much of the variation was driven by livestock, especially cattle. Figure 1C–D illustrates variation in mean wealth and wealth inequality among the study communities. Mean wealth was generally lowest in communities located in the interior forest (Figure 1C, bottom right), which are remote and inaccessible by road for much of the year (due to washed-out bridges); and in those communities downriver from San Borja (Figure 1C, top), which experience frequent flooding and are within or adjacent to a protected bioreserve that limits resource extraction. Somewhat unexpectedly, mean wealth was higher further from the market town of San Borja (correlation between mean wealth and distance to market r = 0.36, df = 38, p=0.02). We operationalized inequality by calculating community-level Gini coefficients for age-corrected wealth (see Materials and methods). Wealth inequality was generally higher in communities closer to the market towns of San Borja and Yucumo, where Tsimane can sell produce and purchase market goods, though some villages near towns also show low inequality (Figure 1D) (correlation between Gini and distance to market r = –0.38, df = 38, p=0.01). Inequality was marginally lower in richer communities (r = –0.22, df = 38, p=0.17). Community size was not significantly related to distance (r = –0.18, p=0.26), mean wealth (r = 0.11, p=0.50), or inequality (r = 0.00, p=0.99). In sum, villages near towns had both higher inequality and lower mean wealth due to both more wealthy individuals and more very poor individuals in these communities.

Modeling strategy

To examine the effects of household wealth and community wealth inequality on psychosocial or health outcomes, we used Bayesian multilevel models with appropriate controls and random effects at the individual, household, and community level (see Materials and methods). Wealth was divided into relative wealth, centered on the community mean, and mean community wealth. Operationalizing wealth this way means we are in principle able to tease apart within-community wealth differentials, that is, one’s position in the local socio-economic hierarchy, from community-level differences in access to resources, that is, mean community wealth (Kreft et al., 1995). However, in practice, models with wealth centered on the village produced virtually identical estimates to models with wealth centered on the sample as a whole (see Supplementary file 1a-1m), largely because villages did not differ strongly in mean wealth (median –0.03, range: –1.0–0.66 Z-scores, 80% between –0.43 and 0.37). Thus, community-relative and population-relative wealth were highly correlated (r = 0.92).

Bayesian models produce a posterior distribution of parameter estimates that can be summarized in various ways (McElreath, 2020). Here, we provide coefficient plots (Figures 25) showing posterior medians, as well as 75% and 95% highest posterior density intervals; we also provide prediction plots as supplements to these figures. In the text, we report results as standardized coefficients (β) for Gaussian models or as log odds (β) and odds ratios (ORs) for logistic models, both represented by the posterior mean, as well as the proportion of the posterior above zero (P>0), that is, the likelihood of a positive association. Higher or lower values of this number represent stronger certainty for a non-zero effect, while values near 0.5 indicate complete uncertainty about the direction of an association, if any. In addition, we report Cohen’s d as a standardized measure of effect size to allow comparison between continuous and binary variables; d is reported as the posterior median and the median absolute deviation (MAD) (a more robust measure of dispersion than the standard deviation). For simplicity, we refer to effect sizes of d > 0.2 as ‘strong,’ those >0.1 as ‘moderate,’ and consider the rest to be ‘weak’ though potentially still suggestive of a general pattern. Similarly, we refer to posterior support of >0.975 (or <0.025, if negative) as ‘high certainty’ and those with support >0.875/<0.125 as ‘moderate certainty,’ corresponding to the entire 95% or 75% highest posterior density intervals respectively not overlapping with 0, and we consider the rest to be ‘uncertain.’ However, we encourage readers to use the full information on the posteriors to inform their own inference. Means and 95% credible intervals for all parameters are reported in Supplementary file 1a-1o. These tables also provide Bayesian R2 (Gelman et al., 2019) as a goodness-of-fit measure, indicating that in most models the predictors and random effects jointly explained about 20–40% of the variance in the data (R2 range: 0.16–0.91).

Figure 2. Wealth and inequality posterior parameter values for models with adults (>15 years).

Points are posterior medians and lines are 75% (thick) and 95% (thin) highest posterior density intervals. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that is greater than zero (P>0). All models control for age, sex, distance to market town, and community size. Rough categories of dependent variables (psychosocial, continuous health outcomes, and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first two rows, the outcomes are expressed as Z-scores, the bottom row as log odds. See Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—figure supplement 2, and Figure 2—figure supplement 3 for predicted associations of household wealth, community wealth, and wealth inequality, respectively.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Predicted conditional effects of relative household wealth on all psychosocial and health outcomes for adults.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Lines are posterior means and shaded areas are 95% credible intervals on mean values. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that supports the predicted negative association between wealth and the outcome (P<0). All predictions control for age, sex, inequality, distance to market town, community size, and mean community wealth, holding all other variables at the mean, with sex = female. Rough categories of dependent variables (psychosocial, continuous health outcomes, and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first two rows, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as probabilities.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Predicted conditional effects of mean community wealth on all psychosocial and health outcomes for adults.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Lines are posterior means and shaded areas are 95% credible intervals on mean values. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that supports the predicted negative association between wealth and the outcome (P<0). All predictions control for age, sex, inequality, distance to market town, community size, and mean community wealth, holding all other variables at the mean, with sex = female. Rough categories of dependent variables (psychosocial, continuous health outcomes, and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first two rows, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as probabilities.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Predicted conditional effects of wealth inequality (Gini coefficients) on all psychosocial and health outcomes for adults.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Lines are posterior means and shaded areas are 95% credible intervals on mean values. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that supports the predicted negative association between wealth and the outcome (P<0). All predictions control for age, sex, inequality, distance to market town, community size, and mean community wealth, holding all other variables at the mean, with sex = female. Rough categories of dependent variables (psychosocial, continuous health outcomes, and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first two rows, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as probabilities.

Figure 5. Covariate posterior parameter values for models with juveniles (≤15 years).

Figure 5.

Points are posterior medians and lines are 75% (thick) and 95% (thin) highest posterior density intervals. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that is greater than zero (P>0). Full models are given in Supplementary file 1n and o. Rough categories of dependent variables (continuous health outcomes and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first row, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as log odds.

Is wealth related to health outcomes?

Overall, for adults, household wealth was associated, with various effect sizes and degrees of confidence, with beneficial health outcomes except gastrointestinal illness, which showed no association (Figure 2; Supplementary file 1f-1m). Community mean wealth had more mixed associations with health outcomes. Specifically, household wealth was associated with lower systolic blood pressure (β = −0.01, P>0=0.37, Cohen’s d = −0.01 [0.02]) and lower diastolic blood pressure (β = −0.04, P>0=0.02, d = −0.05 [0.02]), though both effect sizes were small and only the latter association had high certainty. Community mean wealth was strongly and with high certainty associated with lower systolic (β = −0.29, P>0=0.00, d = −0.34 [0.09]) and diastolic (β = −0.21, P>0=0.01, d = −0.24 [0.11]) blood pressure. Household wealth also associated with better self-rated health (reverse coded β = −0.02, P>0=0.14, d = −0.03 [0.02]), lower odds of infectious (β = −0.06, P>0=0.26, d = −0.02 [0.05], OR = 0.94) and respiratory (β = −0.04, P>0=0.69, d = −0.03 [0.05], OR = 0.96) illness, and lower total morbidity (β = −0.02, P>0=0.23, d = −0.02 [0.04]), though again most effect sizes were small and there was high uncertainty. There was no evidence for an association with gastrointestinal infection. However, there was a moderate though uncertain association between community mean wealth and lower gastrointestinal illnesses (β = −0.32, P>0=0.21, d = −0.16 [0.21], OR = 0.72). Household wealth was weakly and uncertainly associated with lower BMI (β = −0.01, P>0=0.24, d = −0.02 [0.04]), but community mean wealth was weakly associated with higher BMI (β = 0.06, P>0=0.74, d = 0.12 [0.20]). Using population-relative wealth, rather than community-relative wealth had little effect on these associations (Supplementary file 1a-1m). In sum, despite mostly small effect sizes and high uncertainty, the general pattern was for wealthier adults to have better outcomes.

For juveniles ≤ 15 years of age (Figure 3; Supplementary file 1n,o), household wealth was weakly associated with lower total morbidity (β = −0.04, P>0=0.06, d = −0.06 [0.04]), and in particular, moderately lower odds of respiratory illness (β = −0.24, P>0<0.01, d = −0.13 [0.05], OR = 0.79). However, both household and community mean wealth were associated with higher odds of gastrointestinal illness (β = 0.13, P>0=0.95, d = 0.07 [0.04], OR = 1.14; β = 0.49, P>0=0.81, d = 0.27 [0.30], OR = 1.63) and community mean wealth was associated with other infections (β = 0.81, P>0=0.87, d = 0.44 [0.37], OR = 2.25) and higher total morbidity (β = 0.05, P>0=0.82, d = 0.31 [0.33]) with mostly strong effect sizes but high uncertainty. In sum, for juveniles, wealth was moderately associated with reduced risk of respiratory illness, while community wealth was strongly associated with several negative health outcomes.

Figure 3. Wealth and inequality posterior parameter values for models with juveniles (≤15 years).

Points are posterior medians and lines are 75% (thick) and 95% (thin) highest posterior density intervals. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that is greater than zero (P>0). All models control for age, sex, distance to market town, and community size. Rough categories of dependent variables (continuous health outcomes and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first row, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as log odds. See Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for predicted associations of household wealth, community wealth, and wealth inequality.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Predicted conditional effects of household wealth, community wealth, and inequality (Gini coefficients) on all health outcomes for juveniles (<15 years).

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Lines are posterior means and shaded areas are 95% credible intervals on mean values. Numbers in each panel represent the posterior probability, that is, the proportion of the posterior that supports an association between inequality and the outcome. All predictions control for age, sex, distance to market town, and community size, holding all other variables at the mean, with sex = female. For the first two columns, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the remainder as probabilities.

Is inequality related to health outcomes?

For adults, inequality was associated with higher levels of three morbidity-related outcomes and lower levels of four outcomes (Figure 2; Supplementary file 1f,m). Consistent with predictions of worse health with inequality (P2a), greater inequality was weakly associated with higher blood pressure (systolic: β = 0.05, P>0=0.98, d = 0.06 [0.03]; diastolic: β = 0.02, P>0=0.75, d = 0.03 [0.04]), and strongly with a greater likelihood of respiratory illness (β = 0.35, P>0=0.93, d = 0.20 [0.13], OR = 1.36). Despite these harmful associations with inequality, people in more unequal communities had a strongly lower likelihood of other infections (β = −0.62, P>0=0.02, d = −0.33 [0.16], OR = 0.54) and to a more uncertain degree, total morbidity (β = −0.07, P>0=0.25, d = −0.07 [0.13]), and gastrointestinal infections (β = −0.12, P>0=0.22, d = −0.06 [0.09], OR = 0.89). Associations with BMI were negligible (β = 0.01, P>0=0.62, d = 0.03 [0.08]).

In contrast, for juveniles (Figure 3; Supplementary file 1m,o), BMI was lower in more unequal communities (β = −0.06, P>0=0.05, d = −0.08 [0.05]). Inequality had little effect on total morbidity and was moderately associated with less infectious illness (β = −0.23, P>0=0.24, d = −0.13 [0.19], OR = 0.79), but greater respiratory illness (β = 0.21, P>0=0.81, d = 0.11 [0.13], OR = 1.23) and gastrointestinal illness (β = 0.17, P>0=0.74, d = 0.10 [0.13], OR = 1.19), both of which are highly prevalent among juveniles.

Is wealth related to psychosocial outcomes?

For adults, greater household wealth was associated with better outcomes in four of five psychological and social measures, with no association for the fifth (Figure 2; Supplementary file 1a-1e). Household wealth was strongly and with high certainty associated with having more labor partners (reverse coded β = –0.13, P>0=0.01, d = −0.49 [0.20]), and weakly and uncertainly, with fewer depressive symptoms (β = −0.04, P>0=0.14, d = −0.05 [0.05]), fewer non-social problems (i.e., self-reported concerns over food insecurity, debt, and illness; β = −0.06, P>0=0.12, d = −0.08 [0.07]), and lower urinary cortisol (β = −0.02, P>0=0.27, d = −0.02 [0.04]). There was no support for an association with social conflicts. Unlike household wealth, community mean wealth was not clearly associated with any psychosocial outcome, though there were strong but uncertain associations with more labor partners (reverse coded β = −0.16, P>0=0.29, d = −0.77 [0.94]) but also more non-social problems (β = 0.28, P>0=0.77, d = 0.33 [0.45]).

Is inequality related to psychosocial outcomes?

Contrary to predictions, inequality was largely associated with fewer stressors and psychological or social problems (Figure 2; Supplementary file 1a-1e). The strongest evidence was for fewer non-social problems in more unequal communities (β = −0.15, P>0=0.07, d = −0.17 [0.13]), with weak evidence for fewer conflicts (β = −0.04, P>0=0.33, d = −0.01 [0.09]), and more labor partners (β = −0.05, P>0=0.32, d = −0.29 [0.38]) with more inequality.

Do psychosocial variables mediate relationships between wealth or wealth inequality and health?

We tested the prediction (P3) that the effects of wealth or inequality on health were mediated via psychosocial pathways using formal mediation analysis (Baron and Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2007). Specifically, this involves estimating the association between wealth/inequality and psychosocial variables (‘path a’), as well as between psychosocial variables and health outcomes (‘path b’); if both are statistically significant and the association between wealth/inequality and health outcomes (‘path c’, or direct effect) is weaker, then there is evidence that there is an indirect effect of wealth/inequality on health via psychosocial variables (i.e., the psychosocial variable is a mediator). As reported above, paths a were mostly supported for household wealth, that is, household wealth was associated with four of the five psychosocial variables, but not for community wealth or inequality. Supplementary file 1q-1s presents mediation analyses with each health outcome variable and each psychosocial variable as a potential mediator, including estimates of the direct (path c, as reported above) and indirect effects, the mediator effects (path b), and the proportion mediated (indirect effect/total effect). See Appendix 1 for a discussion and graphical depiction of the causal relationships assumed by this mediation approach.

The only convincing evidence for mediation was found for depression and non-social problems mediating the effect of household wealth on diastolic blood pressure; specifically, household wealth was negatively associated with diastolic blood pressure (path c) as well as with depression and non-social problems (paths a; see above, Figure 2), and both depression (β = −0.03, P>0=0.20) and non-social problems (β = −0.08, P>0=0.05) were themselves negatively associated with diastolic blood pressure (paths b). However, there were no other cases where both paths a and b were well supported, the indirect effects of household wealth, community wealth, or inequality were virtually always zero for any mediator (including depression and non-social problems), and the proportion mediated was generally small or highly uncertain (Supplementary file 1q-1s). Overall, there was little evidence of mediation.

Effect of covariates on outcomes

Of the included covariates, many were associated with outcomes. For adults (Figure 4; Supplementary file 1a-1m), age was positively associated with all negative health outcomes except respiratory illness as well as depression and social conflict. Male sex was associated with increased blood pressure but lower depression, conflicts, non-social problems, urinary cortisol, infection illness, and total morbidity, and with better self-rated health. Increasing distance from the market town was associated with increased blood pressure, more conflicts, respiratory illness, and gastrointestinal illness, as well as lower BMI. However, it was also associated with lower depression and urinary cortisol. Community size was generally associated with more positive psychological and social variables, but also higher blood pressure and infection. Household size was associated with worse psychological and social condition, with the exception of labor partners, which were higher for large households. Results for juveniles largely reflect similar associations (Figure 5; Supplementary file 1n and o).

Figure 4. Covariate posterior parameter values for models with adults (>15 years).

Figure 4.

Points are posterior medians and lines are 75% (thick) and 95% (thin) highest posterior density intervals. Numbers in each panel represent the proportion of the posterior distribution that is greater than zero (P>0). Full models are given in Supplementary file 1a-1m. Rough categories of dependent variables (psychosocial, continuous health outcomes, and binary health outcomes) are distinguished by rows and colors. For the first two rows, the outcomes are measured as Z-scores, the bottom row as log odds.

In some cases, the inclusion of covariates improved model R2 statistics, though in many models changes in fit were negligible (Supplementary file 1a-1o). In general, the inclusion of covariates reduced the variance attributable to random effects for individual, household, and community. Posterior distributions for wealth and inequality associations were all similar whether covariates were included or excluded (i.e., the posteriors overlap substantially), though there were some minor differences between the posterior means that were largely inconsequential for inference.

Is there evidence for more complex wealth-health associations?

Finally, we conducted several post-hoc tests to examine whether wealth-health associations were contingent on sex or whether relative wealth effects were contingent on levels of inequality and vice versa. For example, inequality could trigger increased stress and competitiveness only in men given a history of higher reproductive skew in males (Daly, 2016) and inequality might affect the wealthier and poorer differently (P2b), that is, poorer individuals may fare even worse in more unequal contexts. For this reason, we included wealth-by-inequality, wealth-by-sex, or inequality-by-sex interactions in models. A number of models favored interactions though there was little consistency across outcomes (Figure 6; Supplementary file 1p). For depression, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and self-rated health, poorer men showed worse outcomes than wealthier men, though there was little effect of wealth for women. In contrast, poorer women reported more non-social problems. Poor individuals showed both increased conflicts and reduced labor partners in unequal places, while wealthier individuals reported more conflicts and fewer labor partners in equal communities. In unequal communities, wealth had little effect on respiratory illness, while in more equal places, wealthier individuals were less likely to be diagnosed with respiratory illness. Contra P2b, there was no consistent indication that inequality was worse for poorer individuals, while males were somewhat more affected by being poor.

Figure 6. Interactions between sex, wealth, and inequality.

Figure 6.

Plots show the predicted values for each outcome and Gini Z-score. Red shading indicates poorer individuals (wealth Z = –2), blue indicates wealthier individuals (Z = 2). For each model, the proportion of the posterior >0 is shown in the numbers above: GxW: Gini × Wealth; SxG = Sex × Gini; SxW = Sex × Wealth.

Discussion

We tested whether within-community relative wealth, community wealth, and community-level wealth inequality were associated with a broad range of psychological, social, and health outcomes in a large sample of households and communities in a relatively egalitarian small-scale subsistence society. Overall, our results showed substantial heterogeneity in terms of the direction and magnitude of associations between wealth, wealth inequality, and health, which contrasts with the more consistent socio-economic health gradients in high-income countries. Nevertheless, some findings supported an association between wealth or inequality and health outcomes, though these associations were not mediated by psychosocial factors.

Consistent with the prediction that higher relative position in a socio-economic hierarchy improves outcomes (P1), we found that household wealth relative to others in the community, capturing one’s rank within the local socio-economic hierarchy, was associated with lower blood pressure, and for juveniles, lower total morbidity and fewer respiratory infections. Relative household wealth was also generally associated with better health and psychosocial outcomes, but with more uncertainty in the posterior estimates, and for juveniles, relative household wealth was associated with increased gastrointestinal illness. Community mean wealth, capturing the absolute access to resources of households within that community, was also strongly associated with lower blood pressure for adults, but there was high uncertainty in estimates for other outcomes. Conversely, in support of P2a inequality was associated with higher blood pressure in adults and more respiratory disease in both adults and juveniles. It was also associated with lower BMI in juveniles, which in this energetically limited population likely represents a negative outcome. However, contra P2a inequality was also associated with lower levels of other infections (mostly fungal and yeast infections and lice) as well as fewer non-social problems, and there were several null results (Figures 2 and 3). Although most effect sizes were weak to moderate (most Cohen’s d < 0.2), these statistically weak results could still have significant biological and clinical impacts, as elaborated below.

The finding that higher inequality associated with greater likelihood of respiratory disease is perhaps the most significant in terms of well-being and biological fitness. Respiratory illness is the leading cause of mortality at all ages in this population (Gurven et al., 2007) and continues to be a major source of morbidity. The likelihood of being diagnosed with respiratory illness was predicted to differ greater than threefold, 8–28%, between the least and most unequal communities indicating substantial fitness costs to inequality. However, this effect of inequality appears to primarily affect wealthy individuals, bringing their prevalence up to the level of poorer individuals (Figure 6). In one Tsimane community (with relatively high average income compared to other communities), von Rueden et al., 2014 found lower risk of respiratory infection among influential men but no effect on respiratory infection (though trending in direction of higher risk) for men with higher income. With current data, we cannot determine the mechanism responsible for this association between inequality and respiratory disease. The association could reflect differences in immune function as suggested by other research on psychosocial influences on infectious disease (Aiello et al., 2018; Chen and Miller, 2013; McDade et al., 2016), despite a lack of evidence for psychosocial mediation here. The association between inequality and respiratory disease could also be spurious, despite our best efforts to control for relevant covariates, or it could reflect differences in exposure not captured by distance to town or community size (such as population density or frequency of contact with outsiders); in this context, it should be noted that effects of inequality on health are arguably only expected for outcomes for which there is a socio-economic gradient in the first place (Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015), which was not the case for respiratory disease here.

One of the strongest, most certain and most consistent associations of wealth (both household and community level) and inequality was with blood pressure, a major contributor to chronic disease in high-income countries. There was a clear socio-economic gradient in blood pressure within and between communities, and blood pressure was higher in more unequal communities. These effects were observed primarily in men. While most Tsimane are not hypertensive and do not have heart disease (Gurven et al., 2012; Kaplan et al., 2017), the predicted effects of wealth and inequality on blood pressure were substantial: systolic blood pressure was predicted to increase by 0.32 SD (i.e., 4.0 mmHg) and diastolic blood pressure by 0.40 SD (3.7 mmHg) in the most unequal compared to the most equal communities; conversely, wealth was protective such that the lowest blood pressures were predicted for people in the richest communities (7.4 mmHg systolic and 4.2 mmHg diastolic lower) and the richest households within communities (1.0 mmHg systolic and 3.1 mmHg diastolic lower). In high-income countries, such changes in blood pressure correspond to as much as a 10% change in the risk of major cardiovascular disease events (see Figure 2 in Ettehad et al., 2016). Among the Tsimane, it corresponds to as much as 40 years of age-related increases in blood pressure (Gurven et al., 2012). As novel, obesogenic foods enter the Tsimane diet (Kraft et al., 2018), market integration increases stress (Konečná and Urlacher, 2017; von Rueden et al., 2014), sanitation improves (Dinkel et al., 2020), and protective lifestyle factors like physical activity and helminth infections are changing (Gurven et al., 2013; Gurven et al., 2016), people in unequal communities, especially the poor (see Figure 6), may be at increasingly greater risk of chronic disease. Increases in blood pressure with modernization have also been reported in many other subsistence populations, and may partly stem from stress caused by integrating into a dominant culture (Dressler, 1999; Konečná and Urlacher, 2017). In this context, it is also worth noting that while the range of our village-level Gini values (0.15–0.43) was similar to that of income inequality among high-income countries (e.g., Denmark: 0.24; USA: 0.45), it was considerably lower than the range of wealth inequality in these countries (e.g., Japan: 0.55; USA: 0.81 [Nowatzki, 2012]). Thus, the reported associations between wealth/inequality and blood pressure may still be relatively harmless for the Tsimane, but lay the foundation for chronic disease under more mismatched conditions.

An alternative interpretation for some of these associations may be that causality is reversed, with poor health leading to less wealth or exacerbated inequality. On the face of it, this seems plausible for respiratory illness, which reduces work productivity. However, the fact that we see no direct association with wealth for adults, and only an association with inequality, seems to argue against such a mechanism. We did find an association between wealth and respiratory disease for juveniles – perhaps having sicker children puts some strain on wealth accumulation. For blood pressure, it is harder to imagine how reverse causality might occur since the blood pressure changes we observed are unlikely to affect wealth. Regardless, a limitation of our data is that we cannot determine the direction of causation given our cross-sectional design. Other confounds might also be possible, for example, if people preferentially assort by health or wealth by moving between villages.

Beyond respiratory disease and blood pressure, many associations were inconclusive. This heterogeneous picture may seem surprising given robust directional findings from studies in high-income countries, especially for SES health gradients. One possibility for this difference is that hierarchy-stress associations produce more consistent health effects in an epidemiological context characterized by chronic, rather than infectious, disease. As argued above, our finding that one of the most consistent wealth-health associations was with blood pressure would support this argument since hypertension is a risk factor for most chronic diseases and consistently associated with socio-economic position and inequality in high-income countries (Kim et al., 2008; Shahu et al., 2019), but unlikely to be harmful for most Tsimane (Gurven et al., 2012; Kaplan et al., 2017). However, there are also consistent associations between socio-economic position and infectious disease in high-income countries (Aiello et al., 2018; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020), suggesting that epidemiological context alone does not account for inconsistent results.

Another possible source of heterogeneity is the scale at which relative wealth and inequality are measured. Literature reviews suggest that at an international scale as many as 83% of studies find associations, while in studies of areas the size of neighborhoods, only 45% find associations (Kondo et al., 2012; Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015; Wilkinson and Pickett, 2006). Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015 suggest that this heterogeneity reflects the scale at which inequality is perceived as most salient. Here, we assessed relative wealth and inequality at the scale of the residential community, a salient arena of daily cooperation and competition (Alami et al., 2020; Gurven et al., 2015; Gurven et al., 2008b; Jaeggi et al., 2016; von Rueden et al., 2008; von Rueden et al., 2014). This local level is also similar in scale to group-level hierarchies in other social species that show hierarchy-associated stress responses (Sapolsky, 2005; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2016; Tung et al., 2012). Furthermore, substituting community-relative wealth with wealth relative to the whole Tsimane population made little difference for results (Supplementary file 1a-1m), suggesting that the choice of scale within this relatively small-scale society did not matter. Modern technologies, such as television, may upset these comparisons and the functioning of hierarchy-related adaptations by making the global seem local; however, few Tsimane have regular access to television and other media. Nevertheless, it is possible that at least some Tsimane perceive inequality in reference to the local non-Tsimane population, or other regions of Bolivia, which was not captured by our study. Interacting with members of the dominant culture can be a source of stress (Dressler, 1999; Konečná and Urlacher, 2017; von Rueden et al., 2014), even if the Tsimane are arguably doing fairly well financially compared to other rural Bolivians (Godoy et al., 2007). Thus, we might not have been able to capture a relevant scale of comparison for some people, which could explain why associations at other scales were less consistent. However, this argument also applies to studies in high-income countries – where the relevant scales could be anything from neighborhoods to countries – and does not necessarily explain why results were inconsistent (as opposed to simply weaker) when measured at a less salient scale.

Finally, another explanation for heterogeneous associations is that our measure of household wealth may capture several distinct dimensions of socio-economic status, with partly orthogonal effects on health. On the one hand, greater wealth affords more respect and influence within communities, which is associated with lower cortisol and better health among the Tsimane (von Rueden et al., 2014) and elsewhere (Decker, 2000); this is likely the dimension captured by our subjective status data. On the other hand, household wealth is accumulated through participation in the market economy, which is associated with greater stress – higher cortisol, blood pressure – among the Tsimane (von Rueden et al., 2014) and elsewhere (Dressler, 1999; Konečná and Urlacher, 2017). The risks of different infectious diseases may also vary along these dimensions, with people who more frequently visit town and interact with outsiders possibly being more exposed to respiratory pathogens (Kaplan et al., 2020). Thus, household wealth may in part be inconsistently associated with health because of these opposing processes.

Several psychosocial variables were directly associated with health. Conflicts and depression were associated with lower BMI and blood pressure, perhaps indicating the effects of stress or lack of access to resources (depression is associated with low productivity among the Tsimane; Stieglitz et al., 2014). Depression and non-social problems were associated with worse self-rated health, again possibly via stress or direct effects of resource availability. However, associations between wealth or inequality and health outcomes were not mediated when including psychosocial variables in models (P3), and there was almost no evidence for indirect effects proceeding through these pathways. An obvious limitation is that our sample sizes for the mediation analysis were smaller than for other analyses (Supplementary file 1q-1s), though most were still large enough to capture any meaningful effect. It is also possible that our measures of psychosocial stress were inadequate, for example, a single urinary cortisol measure likely captures overall differences in cortisol excretion (Yehuda et al., 2003), but does not capture changes in diurnal cortisol patterns that are typically associated with chronic stress (García et al., 2017; Miller et al., 2007). But the lack of mediation found here may also point to more nuanced mechanisms such as changes in physical activity related to different subsistence strategies or other lifestyle factors not accounted for here. For subsistence societies experiencing socio-economic change, whether relative status increases, decreases, or has no effect on stress and health may depend on the status measure and its association with social support. A study of four Tsimane communities found that influential men with greater social support had lower cortisol (von Rueden et al., 2014), but higher cash income associated with higher cortisol (von Rueden et al., 2014; see also Konečná and Urlacher, 2017). In another study of the Tsimane, higher incomes predicted lower BMIs, unless individuals had relatively more social support (Brabec et al., 2007). It therefore remains unclear what mechanisms were responsible for the wealth-health associations found here, though hierarchy is known to affect immune function, and thereby infectious disease morbidity independently of stress and associated HPA activity (Aiello et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2011; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2016).

In sum, we present the most comprehensive test of hierarchy-health associations in a subsistence society to date. In support of an evolutionary argument that conceptualizes hierarchy-health effects as stemming from evolved reaction norms adjusting people’s behavior and physiology to the rank and local competitive regime they find themselves in (Daly and Wilson, 1997; Griskevicius et al., 2011; Pepper and Nettle, 2014), we found that wealth and inequality were associated with several health outcomes, though other associations were negligible or in the opposite direction to that predicted. In support of the argument that most hierarchy-health effects in high-income countries are caused by evolutionary mismatch (Sapolsky, 2004), we found that inequality was associated with blood pressure but in a range unlikely to affect health; however, this association could lead to hypertension, cardiovascular and metabolic disease as inequality further increases due to increased market integration and/or as novel foods and lifestyle factors enter the population (Gurven et al., 2012; Gurven et al., 2016; Kaplan et al., 2017; Kraft et al., 2018). Our study thus contributes to an evolutionary approach to public health that considers tradeoffs and mismatch as important links between socio-ecology, lifestyle, and health (Eaton et al., 2002; Wells et al., 2017).

Materials and methods

Data collection and preparation

All data were collected under the auspices of the Tsimane Health and Life History Project (THLHP) (Gurven et al., 2017) by a team of Bolivian medical professionals and Tsimane researchers.

Wealth and wealth inequality

Wealth data were collected in 2006–2007 and 2013. Here, we only included wealth data collected prior to a rare catastrophic flood in February 2014 that destroyed crops and household goods in the vast majority of Tsimane communities (Trumble et al., 2018). Figure 1—figure supplement 1 summarizes how many individuals were included in the sample, out of all individuals ever sampled by the THLHP. Household wealth was assessed through an inventory of commonly owned items including traditional goods, that is, items manufactured from local organic materials (e.g., canoes, bows and arrows), market goods, that is, industrially produced items obtained through trade or purchase (e.g., bicycles, motorbikes), and livestock (e.g., pigs, cows), which were subsequently converted into their local market value in Bolivianos and summed (Figure 1).

Objective household wealth arguably provides only an indirect measure of people’s subjective wealth and status (Norton, 2013), but these data were most widely available for this study. Furthermore, household wealth correlated significantly, albeit weakly, with subjective status (Amir et al., 2019; Woolard et al., 2019; r = 0.17, df = 147, p<0.05) and subjective wealth rank (r = 0.29, df = 150, p<0.001). Previous work among the Tsimane (Undurraga et al., 2016) has also shown that more visible forms of wealth, such as the household items counted here, influenced subjective health more than less visible forms of wealth, such as the size of cultivated fields. To prevent differences in age sampling between villages from affecting wealth and inequality estimates, we followed Borgerhoff Mulder et al., 2009 and adjusted wealth values for the age of the head of household by fitting generalized additive models for location scale and shape (GAMLSS) to the distribution of wealth-by-age to obtain wealth-by-age Z-scores. Wealth Z-scores derived from GAMLSS, representing centile values, were used in all analyses in part because wealth was skewed in distribution, and also expected to have diminishing returns at higher values (i.e., 100 Bolivianos are worth more to a poor individual than a wealthy one). However, to determine whether Z-scoring with GAMLSS altered results by normalizing the shape of the wealth distribution, we also repeated analyses with standardized wealth (i.e., [household wealth – population-average wealth]/standard deviation of population-average wealth), which preserves the skew. There were no qualitative differences in inference between the two methods, largely because Z-scoring with GAMLSS primarily affects outliers on the far high end of the distribution. Note that ‘Z-score’ can have two slightly different meanings; for wealth and BMI (see below), we generally mean centile values from GAMLSS unless otherwise noted, for all other variables Z-scores refer simply to standardized values (i.e., [x – mean(x)]/sd(x)).

Mean wealth and wealth inequality at the community level (for communities with ≥ 9 households) were calculated after converting wealth Z-scores back into equivalent values in Bolivianos at age 50 (see Figure 1). We used the Gini coefficient to measure inequality; other inequality measures (e.g., median share, 90/10 ratio) generally correlate highly (r > 0.94) with Gini (Kondo et al., 2009) and were therefore not considered. In other studies, local scales of measuring inequality, such as at the community level used here, tend to produce smaller effects on health than those at larger scales, such as states or countries (Kondo et al., 2009; Wilkinson and Pickett, 2006). In the Tsimane context, it is unclear whether that will be the case given low residential mobility and concentration of work and socializing within communities. However, Tsimane visit other communities and sporadically engage in market-based interaction with non-Tsimane, and comparisons with wealthier neighbors can contribute to Tsimane status aspirations (Schultz, 2019). Nevertheless, as mentioned above (Study population), we consider the community to be the most relevant arena for status competition among Tsimane (though substituting community-relative wealth with population-relative wealth made little difference; see Supplementary file 1a-1m). Note that most studies on health effects of inequality use income inequality (but see Nowatzki, 2012), which is less unequally distributed than wealth. Cash income among the Tsimane during this study period was sporadic and many households may have no income in a given sampling period, which leads to overestimated Ginis. We therefore preferred wealth and wealth inequality as a more reliable measure of households’ long-term access to resources and its distribution.

Psychological, social, and health variables

The THLHP has been recording biomedical and anthropological data during roughly annual medical examinations and interviews by THLHP physicians and research assistants on an increasing number of communities since 2002. Here, we included any data collected within 2 years of an individual’s wealth data, that is, the potential range of data was 2004–2009 and 2011–2015. Table 1 summarizes how many individuals out of all the ones with wealth data (see also Figure 1—figure supplement 1) were included for each outcome variable.

Depressive symptoms were measured using an adapted 18-item questionnaire (Stieglitz et al., 2014), the responses to which were summed to yield an overall depression score. The same interview also asked whether participants experienced conflicts with several kinds of social partners as well as non-social problems, such as food insecurity, illness, or debt; affirmative answers were summed to yield a composite measure of social conflicts and non-social problems, respectively. A household’s cooperation network was measured as the number of people from different households who helped in that household’s fields in a given year. Cortisol was measured in first-morning urine using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays and corrected for specific gravity (see von Rueden et al., 2014 for details). BMI Z-scores were calculated by GAMLSS using Tsimane-specific growth curves (Blackwell et al., 2016b) (R package at: https://github.com/adblackwell/localgrowth (Blackwell, 2021) copy archived at swh:1:rev:81ce799bdc6da90d48e5ad8afd6ad0f3b19494d2) as well as the total distribution of Tsimane adult BMIs, representing deviations from the local population average for a given age and sex. Diastolic and systolic blood pressures were measured by THLHP physicians using an aneroid sphygmomanometer. Self-rated general health was measured using a five-point scale from (‘very bad’ [1] to ‘excellent’ [5]). Morbidity at the time of the medical check-up was assessed by physicians using the International Classification of Disease, 10th edition (ICD-10 classification) and then grouped into 18 clinically meaningful categories following the Clinical Classifications System (CCS) (https://www.hcup-us.ahrq.gov/toolssoftware/ccs/ccsfactsheet.jsp); morbidities in any of these categories were summed to give a total morbidity score potentially ranging from 0 (no morbidities) to 18 (at least one morbidity in each category). In addition, we also examined the presence/absence of infectious and parasitic diseases (CCS 1, hereafter ‘infections’), diseases of the respiratory system (CCS 8, ‘respiratory illness’), and diseases of the digestive system (CCS 9, ‘gastrointestinal illness’), which represent the most common causes of morbidity and mortality in this population (Gurven et al., 2020; Gurven et al., 2007). See Table 2 for examples of the six most common diagnoses in these three categories. Distance to the town of San Borja was measured as nearest route (whether by river or road) from the center of the community and provides a proxy for access to modern amenities. Community size and household size were summarized from complete population censuses conducted regularly by the THLHP. Thus, they include all individuals, not just those sampled for wealth or other covariates.

Data analysis

Prior to analysis, all variables except binary variables were standardized into Z-scores. Urinary cortisol was log transformed prior to standardization to reduce skew, as is common practice. All outcomes were modeled as Gaussian, except the presence/absence of specific morbidities (Bernoulli). Each analysis modeled an individual-level outcome as a function of individual-, household-, and community-level characteristics (Table 1). Thus, we fit the following base model for each outcome:

Outcomeijkl ~ β0 + (β1 * Sexj) + (β2 * Agej)+ (β3 * relative household wealthk) + (β4 * Community-level Ginil) + (β5 * Community-level mean wealthl) + (β6 * Community Sizel) + (β7 * Distance of community to market townl) + (β8 * Household Sizel) uj+ uk + ul+ eijkl

wherein the subscripts denote measurement i, individual j, household k, and community l, respectively. β0 is the intercept, all other βs are slopes, us are random intercepts, and e is the residual error (not available for Bernoulli responses). Variance inflation factors (VIFs) indicated virtually no collinearity among predictors (all VIFs < 3).

In order to test whether potential wealth-health associations were mediated by psychosocial stress, we reran all health models (blood pressure, self-rated health, total morbidity, infections, respiratory and gastrointestinal illness) with pertinent psychosocial variables as covariates and used the mediation function in the sjstats package (Lüdecke, 2021) to estimate direct and indirect effect. In addition, we also ran a series of exploratory analyses in which we added interaction terms.

We used Bayesian multilevel models fit with the brms package v. 2.13.5 (Bürkner, 2017) in R 4.0.2 for all analyses. All models used regularizing priors (fixed effects: normal, mean = 0, SD = 1; random effects: half-Cauchy, location = 0, scale = 2), which imposes conservatism on parameter estimates and reduces the risk of inferential errors (Gelman et al., 2013; McElreath, 2020). All models converged well as assessed by inspecting trace plots and standard diagnostics (all Rhat < 1.01). All data and R code are available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4567498 with any updates at https://github.com/adblackwell/wealthinequality (Jaeggi et al., 2021, copy archived at swh:1:rev:da16ac6b20732fe1939478450d81ac32fdcce202).

Acknowledgements

We thank the Tsimane for their generous participation and years of collaboration, and THLHP personnel for their herculean efforts and dedication in data collection. We thank Erik Ringen, Jordan Martin, participants in the Club EvMed series, as well as Milagros Ruiz, Prabhat Jha, and one anonymous reviewer for comments and suggestions that greatly improved this article.

The Tsimane Health and Life History Project has been funded by NSF (BCS0136274, BCS0422690) and NIH/NIA (RF1AG054442, R01AG024119, R56AG024119). AJ was supported by the Swiss NSF (PBZHP3-133443) and by the SAGE Center for the Study of the Mind. JS acknowledges Institute for Advanced Study Toulouse funding from the French National Research Agency (ANR) under grant ANR-17-EURE-0010 (Investissements d’Avenir program). BAB acknowledges research funding from the Max Planck Society, through the Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture. The authors report no conflicts of interests.

Appendix 1

Causal relationships assumed by mediation analysis

The causal relationships between independent variable, mediator, and dependent variable assumed by standard mediation analysis are depicted in Appendix 1—figure 1.

Appendix 1—figure 1. Causal relationships assumed by the mediation analyses.

Appendix 1—figure 1.

In our case, the independent variables are absolute wealth, relative wealth, and inequality, the mediators are the psychosocial variables, and the dependent variables are the health outcomes (see Supplementary file 1q-1s).

The next causal diagram, Appendix 1—figure 2 highlights a potential problem of this approach, which treats each mediator separately even though several mediators are present.

Appendix 1—figure 2. Causal diagram highlighting multiple mediation.

Appendix 1—figure 2.

For example, it could be that wealth/inequality cause both higher levels of depression and higher levels of cortisol. In this scenario, leaving one mediator out prevents us from accurately estimating the ‘direct effect’ of our independent variable on health outcomes, limiting us only to the total effect absent the mediating pathway.

Furthermore, our results could also be influenced by collider bias in the case that different mediating variables are themselves causally linked, as in Appendix 1—figure 3.

Appendix 1—figure 3. Causal diagram highlighting multiple mediation and collider bias.

Appendix 1—figure 3.

Following the example above, it could be that cortisol also independently causes higher levels of depression (or vice versa). In this situation, the mediating psychosocial variable 1 functions as a collider between wealth/inequality and psychosocial variable 2, and we cannot properly assess the impact of our independent variables on the outcome without conditioning on all mediation variables simultaneously.

Despite these potential problems, we preferred the present approach of treating each mediator independently because including all mediators in the same analysis would have required imputing most of the values, because the sample overlap was small. Furthermore, we have good reasons to believe that this approach would not change inference. Namely, in a previous version of the mediation analysis (https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.06.11.20121889v1) we performed a principle components analysis on all psychosocial variables and found that they were relatively uncorrelated, as the main principle component only contained depression loadings. Furthermore, we had included depression, non-social problems, and cortisol (with imputation) in the same models with the same inference as in the present version of the analysis: there was no convincing evidence for mediation.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Adrian V Jaeggi, Email: adrian.jaeggi@iem.uzh.ch.

Aaron D Blackwell, Email: aaron.blackwell@wsu.edu.

Milagros Ruiz, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom.

George H Perry, Pennsylvania State University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung PBZHP3-133443 to Adrian V Jaeggi.

  • National Science Foundation BCS0136274 to Hillard Kaplan.

  • National Science Foundation BCS0422690 to Michael Gurven.

  • National Institutes of Health R01AG024119 to Hillard Kaplan, Michael Gurven.

  • National Institutes of Health RF1AG054442 to Hillard Kaplan, Michael Gurven.

  • National Institutes of Health R56AG024119 to Hillard Kaplan, Michael Gurven.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – original draft.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – original draft.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – original draft.

Data curation, Writing – original draft.

Data curation, Writing – original draft.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – original draft.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: Institutional Review Board approval was granted by UNM (HRRC # 07-157) and UCSB (# 3-16- 0766), as was informed consent at three levels: (1) Tsimane government that oversees research projects, (2) village leaders and community meetings, and (3) study participants.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. This file contains the following tables with additional information on the statistical models.

a: Model summary – depression. b: Model summary – social conflicts. c: Model summary – fewer labor partners. d: Model summary – non-social problems. e: Model summary – cortisol. f: Model summary – BMI. g: Model summary – systolic blood pressure. h: Model summary – diastolic blood pressure. i: Model summary – worse self-rated health. j: Model summary – total morbidity. k: Model summary – infections. l: Model summary – respiratory illness. m: Model summary – gastrointestinal illness. n: Gaussian model summaries for juveniles. o: Logistic model summaries for juveniles. p: Overview of exploratory interaction effects. q: Mediation of wealth effects. r: Mediation of inequality effects. s: Mediation of mean community wealth effects.

elife-59437-supp1.docx (117KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data and R code are available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4567498 with any updates at https://github.com/adblackwell/wealthinequality (copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:da16ac6b20732fe1939478450d81ac32fdcce202).

The following dataset was generated:

Jaeggi AV, Blackwell AD, von Rueden C, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Garcia AR, Kraft TS, Beheim BA, Hooper PL, Kaplan H, Gurven M. 2021. Data and code for "Do wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society?". Zenodo.

References

  1. Abbott DH, Keverne EB, Bercovitch FB, a SC, Mendoza SP, Saltzman W, Snowdon CT, Ziegler TE, Banjevic M, Garland T, Sapolsky RM. Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Hormones and Behavior. 2003;43:67–82. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00037-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiello AE, Simanek AM, Stebbins RC, Dowd JB. The Routledge International Handbook of Psychosocial Epidemiology. Routledge; 2018. Psychosocial influences on infectious disease. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alami S, Von RC, Seabright E, Kraft TS, Blackwell AD, Stieglitz J, Kaplan HS, Gurven MD, von Rueden CR, Blackwell AD, Seabright E, Kraft TS, Kaplan HS, Gurven MD. Mother’s social status is associated with child health in a horticulturalist population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2020;287:20192783. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.2783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amir D, Valeggia C, Srinivasan M, Sugiyama LS, Dunham Y. Measuring subjective social status in children of diverse societies. PLOS ONE. 2019;14:e0226550. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson C, Kraus MW, Galinsky AD, Keltner D. The Local-Ladder Effect: Social Status and Subjective Well-Being. Psychological Science. 2012;23:764–771. doi: 10.1177/0956797611434537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blackwell AD, Martin M, Kaplan H, Gurven M. Antagonism between two intestinal parasites in humans: The importance of co-infection for infection risk and recovery dynamics. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280:20131671. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blackwell AD, Trumble BC, Suarez M, Stieglitz J, Beheim B, Snodgrass JJ, Kaplan H, Gurven M. Immune function in Amazonian horticulturalists. Annals of Human Biology. 2016a;43:382–396. doi: 10.1080/03014460.2016.1189963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blackwell AD, Urlacher SS, Beheim BA, von Rueden C, Jaeggi A, Stieglitz J, Trumble BC, Gurven MD, Kaplan HS. Growth references for Tsimane forager-horticulturalists of the Bolivian Amazon. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2016b;162:441–461. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.23128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blackwell AD. localgrowth. 81ce799Software Heritage. 2021 https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:20cec38a324e1e17b05a7622b3df22c20a09d230;origin=https://github.com/adblackwell/localgrowth;visit=swh:1:snp:0bd7ad9673aa22c3936a9fbe0bdc7c7f3329500d;anchor=swh:1:rev:81ce799bdc6da90d48e5ad8afd6ad0f3b19494d2
  11. Borgerhoff Mulder M, Bowles S, Hertz T, Bell A, Beise J, Clark G, Fazzio I, Gurven M, Hill K, Hooper PL, Irons W, Kaplan H, Leonetti D, Low B, Marlowe F, Mcelreath R, Naidu S, Nolin D, Piraino P, Quinlan R, Schniter E, Sear R, Shenk M, Smith EA, von Rueden C, Wiessner P. Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science. 2009;326:682–688. doi: 10.1126/science.1178336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brabec M, Godoy RA, Reyes-García V, Leonard WR. BMI, income, and social capital in a native Amazonian society: Interaction between relative and community variables. American Journal of Human Biology. 2007;19:459–474. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bürkner PC. brms : An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;80:v080.i01. doi: 10.18637/jss.v080.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen E, Miller GE. Socioeconomic Status and Health: Mediating and Moderating Factors. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9:723–749. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cowlishaw G, Dunbar R. Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Animal Behaviour. 1991;41:1045–1056. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80642-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Daly M, Wilson M. Life expectancy, economic inequality, homicide, and reproductive timing in Chicago neighborhoods. British Medical Journal. 1997;314:1271–2174. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7089.1271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Daly M. Killing the Competition: Economic Inequality and Homicide. New York: Routledge; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Decker SA. Salivary cortisol and social status among Dominican men. Hormones and Behavior. 2000;38:29–38. doi: 10.1006/HBEH.2000.1597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dinkel KA, Costa ME, Kraft TS, Stieglitz J, Cummings DK, Gurven M, Kaplan H, Trumble BC. Relationship of sanitation, water boiling, and mosquito nets to health biomarkers in a rural subsistence population. American Journal of Human Biology. 2020;32:1–13. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dressler WW. Modernization, stress, and blood pressure: New directions in research. Human Biology. 1999;71:583–605. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Eaton SB, Konner M, Shostak M. Stone agers in the fast lane: chronic degenerative diseases in evolutionary perspective. The American Journal of Medicine. 1988;84:739–749. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(88)90113-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Eaton ES, Strassman BI, Nesse RM, Neel J, Ewald PW, Williams GC, Weder AB, Eaton Stanley B, Lindeberg S, Konner MJ, Mysterud I, Cordain L. Evolutionary health promotion. Prev Med (Baltim) 2002;34:109–118. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ecob R, Davey Smith G. Income and health: what is the nature of the relationship? Social Science & Medicine. 1999;48:693–705. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00385-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ettehad D, Emdin CA, Kiran A, Anderson SG, Callender T, Emberson J, Chalmers J, Rodgers A, Rahimi K. Blood pressure lowering for prevention of cardiovascular disease and death: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet. 2016;387:957–967. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01225-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. García AR, Gurven M, Blackwell AD. A matter of perception: Perceived socio-economic status and cortisol on the island of Utila, Honduras. American Journal of Human Biology. 2017;29:23031. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Dunson DB, Vehtari A, Rubin DB. Bayesian Data Analysis. 3rd editio. ed. New York: Chapman and Hall; 2013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gelman A, Goodrich B, Gabry J, Vehtari A. R-squared for Bayesian regression models. The American Statistician. 2019;73:307–309. doi: 10.1080/00031305.2018.1549100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gluckman PD, Beedle AS, Buklijas T, Low F, Hanson M. Principles of Evolutionary Medicine. Oxford: Oxford Unniversity Press; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Godoy R, Byron E, Reyes-García V, Vadez V, Leonard WR, Apaza L, Huanca T, Pérez E, Wilkie D. Income inequality and adult nutritional status: Anthropometric evidence from a pre-industrial society in the Bolivian Amazon. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61:907–919. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Godoy RA, Reyes-García V, McDade T, Huanca T, Leonard WR, Tanner S, Vadez V. Does village inequality in modern income harm the psyche? Anger, fear, sadness, and alcohol consumption in a pre-industrial society. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;63:359–372. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Godoy R, Reyes-Garcia V, Huanca T, Leonard WR, McDade T, Tanner S, Seyfried C. On the measure of income and the economic unimportance of social capital: Evidence from a native Amazonian society of farmers and foragers. Journal of Anthropological Research. 2007;63:239–260. doi: 10.3998/jar.0521004.0063.207. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gravlee CC. How race becomes biology: Embodiment of social inequality. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2009;139:47–57. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Delton AW, Robertson TE. The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;100:1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/a0022403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gurven M, Allen-Arave W, Hill K, Hurtado M. It’s a Wonderful Life”: signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2000;21:263–282. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gurven M, Kaplan HS. Longevity among hunter-gatherers: a cross-cultural examination. Population and Development Review. 2007;33:321–365. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2007.00171.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gurven M, Kaplan H, Zelada Supa A. Mortality experience of Tsimane Amerindians of Bolivia: Regional variation and temporal trends. American Journal of Human Biology. 2007;19:376–398. doi: 10.1002/ajhb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gurven M, Kaplan H, Winking J, Finch C, Crimmins EM. Aging and inflammation in two epidemiological worlds. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2008a;63:196–199. doi: 10.1093/gerona/63.2.196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gurven M, Zanolini A, Schniter E. Culture sometimes matters: Intra-cultural variation in pro-social behavior among Tsimane Amerindians. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 2008b;67:587–607. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gurven MD, Kaplan H, Winking J, Eid Rodriguez D, Vasunilashorn S, Kim JK, Finch C, Crimmins E. Inflammation and infection do not promote arterial aging and cardiovascular disease risk factors among lean horticulturalists. PLOS ONE. 2009;4:e6590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gurven M, Blackwell AD, Rodríguez DE, Stieglitz J, Kaplan H. Does blood pressure inevitably rise with age? Longitudinal evidence among forager-horticulturalists. Hypertension. 2012;60:25–33. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.189100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gurven M, Jaeggi AV, Kaplan H, Cummings D. Physical activity and modernization among bolivian amerindians. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e55679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gurven M, Jaeggi A, Von Rueden CR, Hooper PL, Kaplan H. Does market integration buffer risk, erode traditional sharing practices, and increase inequality? A test among Bolivian forager-farmers. Human Ecology. 2015;43:515–530. doi: 10.1007/s10745-015-9764-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gurven MD, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Blackwell AD, Michalik DE, Finch CE, Kaplan HS. Cardiovascular disease and type 2 diabetes in evolutionary perspective: A critical role for helminths? Evol Med Public Heal. 2016;2016:338–357. doi: 10.1093/emph/eow028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gurven M, Kaplan H, Stieglitz J, Trumble B, Blackwell AD, Beheim B, Hooper P. The Tsimane Health and Life History Project: Integrating anthropology and biomedicine. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2017;26:54–73. doi: 10.1002/evan.21515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gurven M, Kraft TS, Alami S, Adrian JC, Linares EC, Cummings D, Rodriguez DE, Hooper PL, Jaeggi AV, Gutierrez RQ, Suarez IM, Seabright E, Kaplan H, Stieglitz J, Trumble B. Rapidly declining body temperature in a tropical human population. Science Advances. 2020;6:eabc6599. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abc6599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hagen EH. Evolutionary theories of depression: A critical review. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie. 2011;56:716–726. doi: 10.1177/070674371105601203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hooper PL, Gurven M, Winking J, Kaplan HS. Inclusive fitness and differential productivity across the life course determine intergenerational transfers in a small-scale human society. Proc R Soc B - Biol Sci. 2015;282:20142808. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jaeggi A, Hooper PL, Beheim BA, Kaplan HS, Gurven MD. Reciprocal exchange patterned by market forces helps explain cooperation in a small-scale society. Current Biology. 2016;26:2180–2187. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Jaeggi AV, Blackwell AD, von Rueden C, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Garcia AR, Kraft T, Beheim BA, Hooper PL, Kaplan H, Gurven MD. wealthinequality. da16ac6Software Heritage. 2021 doi: 10.7554/eLife.59437. https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:363447d08e95b3aa00b9a2a624e17729c0202e5e;origin=https://github.com/adblackwell/wealthinequality;visit=swh:1:snp:d690509ede234bc49c3501922627641de5630f85;anchor=swh:1:rev:da16ac6b20732fe1939478450d81ac32fdcce202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  50. Johnson SL, Leedom LJ, Muhtadie L. The dominance behavioral system and psychopathology: Evidence from self-report, observational, and biological studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138:692–743. doi: 10.1037/a0027503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kaplan HS, Hooper P, Gurven M. The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philos Trans R Soc B - Biol Sci. 2009;364:3289–3299. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kaplan H, Thompson RC, Trumble BC, Wann LS, Allam AH, Beheim B, Frohlich B, Sutherland ML, Sutherland JD, Stieglitz J, Rodriguez DE, Michalik DE, Rowan CJ, Lombardi GP, Bedi R, Garcia AR, Min JK, Narula J, Finch CE, Gurven M, Thomas GS. Coronary atherosclerosis in indigenous South American Tsimane: a cross-sectional cohort study. Lancet. 2017;15:1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30752-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kaplan HS, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Mendez Mamany R, Gutierrez Cayuba M, Maito Moye L, Alami S, Kraft T, Quispe Gutierrez R, Copajira Adrian J, Thompson RC, Thomas GS, Michalik DE, Eid Rodriguez D, Gurven MD. Voluntary collective isolation as a best response to COVID-19 for indigenous populations? A case study and protocol from the Bolivian Amazon. Lancet. 2020;6736:1–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)31104-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kim D, Kawachi I, Vander HS, Ezzati M. Is inequality at the heart of it? Cross-country associations of income inequality with cardiovascular diseases and risk factors. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;66:1719–1732. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kondo N, Sembajwe G, Kawachi I, van Dam RM, Subramanian S, Yamagata Z. Income inequality, mortality, and self rated health: meta-analysis of multilevel studies. BMJ. 2009;339:b4471. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b4471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kondo N, van Dam RM, Sembajwe G, Subramanian S, Kawachi I, Yamagata Z. Income inequality and health: The role of population size, inequality threshold, period effects and lag effects. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2012;66:e11. doi: 10.1136/jech-2011-200321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Konečná M, Urlacher SS. Male social status and its predictors among Garisakang forager-horticulturalists of lowland Papua New Guinea. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2017;38:789–797. doi: 10.1016/J.EVOLHUMBEHAV.2017.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kraft TS, Stieglitz J, Trumble BC, Martin M, Kaplan H, Gurven M. Nutrition transition in 2 lowland Bolivian subsistence populations. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2018;108:1–13. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kreft IGG, de Leeuw J, Aiken LS. The Effect of Different Forms of Centering in Hierarchical Linear Models. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1995;30:1–21. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3001_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, Steptoe A. Differences in cortisol awakening response on work days and weekends in women and men from the Whitehall II cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:516–528. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00072-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lea AJ, Tung J, Archie EA, Alberts SC. Developmental plasticity. Evol Med Public Heal. 2017;2017:162–175. doi: 10.1093/emph/eox019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lüdecke D. Sjstats: Statistical functions for regression models. Zenodo. 2021 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.1284473. [DOI]
  63. Luttmer EFP. Neighbors as negatives: Relative earnings and well-being. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2005;120:963–1002. doi: 10.1162/003355305774268255. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lynch J, Smith GD, Harper S, Hillemeier M, Ross N, a KG, Wolfson M. Is income inequality a determinant of population health? Part 1. A systematic review. The Milbank Quarterly. 2004;82:5–99. doi: 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00302.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Macinko JA, Shi L, Starfield B, Wulu JT. Income inequality and health: A critical review of the literature. Medical Care Research and Review. 2003;60:407–452. doi: 10.1177/1077558703257169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MS. Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Marmot MG, Stansfeld S, Patel C, North F, Head J, White I, Brunner E, Feeney A, Smith GD. Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet. 1991;337:1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mattison SM, Smith EA, Shenk MK, Cochrane EE. The evolution of inequality. Evol Anthropol Issues, News, Rev. 2016;25:184–199. doi: 10.1002/evan.21491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McDade TW. THE ECOLOGIES OF HUMAN IMMUNE FUNCTION. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2005;34:495–521. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.34.081804.120348. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. McDade TW, Georgiev A, Kuzawa CW. Trade-offs between acquired and innate immune defenses in humans. Evol Med Public Heal. 2016;2016:1–16. doi: 10.1093/emph/eov033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McElreath R. Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and Stan. 2nd editio. ed. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  72. Miller GE, Chen E, Zhou ES. If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:25–45. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, Cole S, Kobor MS. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. PNAS. 2009;106:14716–14721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902971106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:959–997. doi: 10.1037/a0024768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Miller GE, Murphy MLM, Cashman R, Ma R, Ma J, Arevalo JMG, Kobor MS, Cole SW. Greater inflammatory activity and blunted glucocorticoid signaling in monocytes of chronically stressed caregivers. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2014;41:191–199. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Miller AH, Raison CL. The role of inflammation in depression: From evolutionary imperative to modern treatment target. Nature Reviews. Immunology. 2016;16:22–34. doi: 10.1038/nri.2015.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Nesse RM, Williams GC. Why We Get Sick: The New Science of Darwinian Medicine. New York: Vintage; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  78. Nesse RM. Is depression an adaptation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:14–20. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Norton MI. All ranks are local: Why humans are both (painfully) aware and (surprisingly) unaware of their lot in life. Psychological Inquiry. 2013;24:124–125. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2013.794689. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Nowatzki NR. Wealth inequality and health: A political economy perspective. Int J Heal Serv. 2012;42:403–424. doi: 10.2190/HS.42.3.c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Oakes TW, Syme SL, Feldman R, Friedman GD, Siegelaub AB, Collen MF. Social factors in newly discovered elevated blood pressure. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1973;14:198–204. doi: 10.2307/2137111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Pepper G, Nettle D. In: Applied Evolutionary Anthropology. Gibson MA, Lawson DW, editors. New York: Springer; 2014. Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviour: An evolutionary perspective; pp. 225–243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Pickett KE, Wilkinson RG. Income inequality and health: A causal review. Social Science & Medicine. 2015;128:316–326. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.12.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Pontzer H, Wood BM, Raichlen DA. Hunter-gatherers as models in public health. Obesity Reviews. 2018;19:24–35. doi: 10.1111/obr.12785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Pusey AE, Williams J, Goodall J. The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science. 1997;277:828–831. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5327.828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Quon EC, McGrath JJ. Subjective socioeconomic status and adolescent health: A meta-analysis. Heal Psychol. 2014;33:433–447. doi: 10.1037/a0033716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sapolsky RM. Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers. 3rd ed. New York: Holt; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  88. Sapolsky RM. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science. 2005;308:648–652. doi: 10.1126/science.1106477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schultz AF. Status determinants, social incongruity and economic transition: Gender, relative material wealth and heterogeneity in the cultural lifestyle of forager-horticulturalists. PLOS ONE. 2019;14:e0220432. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Shahu A, Herrin J, Dhruva SS, Desai NR, Davis BR, Krumholz HM, Spatz ES. Disparities in Socioeconomic Context and Association With Blood Pressure Control and Cardiovascular Outcomes in ALLHAT. Journal of the American Heart Association. 2019;8:e012277. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.012277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shenk MK, Kaplan HS, Hooper PL. Status competition, inequality, and fertility: Implications for the demographic transition. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2016;371:20150150. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Silk JB. Practice random acts of aggression and senseless acts of intimidation: The logic of status contests in social groups. Evol Anthropol Issues, News, Rev. 2003;11:221–225. doi: 10.1002/evan.10038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Snyder-Mackler N, Sanz J, Kohn JN, Brinkworth JF, Morrow S, Shaver AO, Grenier JC, Pique-Regi R, Johnson ZP, Wilson ME, Barreiro LB, Tung J. Social status alters immune regulation and response to infection in macaques. Science. 2016;354:1041–1045. doi: 10.1126/science.aah3580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Snyder-Mackler N, Burger JR, Gaydosh L, Belsky DW, Noppert GA, Campos FA, Bartolomucci A, Yang YC, Aiello AE, Rand AO, Harris KM, Shively CA, Alberts SC, Tung J. Social determinants of health and survival in humans and other animals. Science. 2020;368:eaax9553. doi: 10.1126/science.aax9553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Sorlie PD, Backlund E, Keller JB. US mortality by economic, demographic, and social characteristics: the National Longitudinal Mortality Study. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85:949–956. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.85.7.949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Stieglitz J, Jaeggi A, Blackwell AD, Trumble BC, Gurven M, Kaplan HS. In: Sociality, Hierarchy, Health: Comparative Biodemography. Weinstein M, Lane MA, editors. National Academies Press; 2014. Work to live and live to work: Productivity, transfers, and psychological well-being in adulthood and old age; pp. 195–220. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Stieglitz J, Trumble BC, Thompson ME, Blackwell AD, Kaplan H, Gurven M. Depression as sickness behavior? A test of the host defense hypothesis in a high pathogen population. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2015;49:130–139. doi: 10.1016/J.BBI.2015.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Stulp G, Sear R, Schaffnit SB, Mills MC, Barrett L. The Reproductive Ecology of Industrial Societies, Part II: The Association between Wealth and Fertility. Human Nature. 2016;27:445–470. doi: 10.1007/s12110-016-9272-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Subramanian S, Kawachi I. Income inequality and health: What have we learned so far? Epidemiologic Reviews. 2004;26:78–91. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sugiyama LS, Sugiyama MS. Social roles, prestige, and health risk: Social niche specialization as a risk-buffering strategy. Human Nature. 2003;14:165–190. doi: 10.1007/s12110-003-1002-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Jaeggi A, Beheim BA, Schwartz M, Seabright E, Cummings D, Kaplan HS, Gurven M. Parental hormones are associated with crop loss and family sickness following catastrophic flooding in lowland Bolivia. Physiology & Behavior. 2018;193:101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Tung J, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP, Hansen KD, Michopoulos V, Toufexis D, Michelini K, Wilson ME, Gilad Y. Social environment is associated with gene regulatory variation in the rhesus macaque immune system. PNAS. 2012;109:6490–6495. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202734109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Undurraga EA, Nyberg C, Eisenberg DTA, Magvanjav O, Reyes-García V, Huanca T, Leonard WR, McDade TW, Tanner S, Vadez V, Godoy R. Individual wealth rank, community wealth inequality, and self-reported adult poor health: A test of hypotheses with panel data (2002-2006) from native amazonians, bolivia. Medical Anthropology Quarterly. 2010;24:522–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1548-1387.2010.01121.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Undurraga EA, Nica V, Zhang R, Mensah IC, Godoy RA. Individual health and the visibility of village economic inequality: Longitudinal evidence from native Amazonians in Bolivia. Economics and Human Biology. 2016;23:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2016.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP. The effects of dominance rank and group size on female lifetime reproductive success in wild long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Primates; Journal of Primatology. 1999;40:105–130. doi: 10.1007/BF02557705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. von Rueden C, Gurven M, Kaplan H. The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2008;29:402–415. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. von Rueden CR, Trumble BC, Emery Thompson M, Stieglitz J, Hooper PL, Blackwell AD, Kaplan HS, Gurven M. Political influence associates with cortisol and health among egalitarian forager-farmers. Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health. 2014;2014:122–133. doi: 10.1093/emph/eou021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Von Rueden CR, Jaeggi A. Men’s status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. PNAS. 2016;113:10824–10829. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606800113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. von Rueden C, Alami S, Kaplan H, Gurven M. Sex differences in political leadership in an egalitarian society. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2018;39:402–411. doi: 10.1016/J.EVOLHUMBEHAV.2018.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. von Rueden CR, Redhead D, O’Gorman R, Kaplan H, Gurven M. The dynamics of men’s cooperation and social status in a small-scale society. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2019;286:20191367. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Wagstaff A, van Doorslaer E. Income inequality and health: What does the literature tell us? Annual Review of Public Health. 2000;21:543–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8276.2007.01012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Wells J, Sear R, Stearns S, Nesse R, Johnstone R. Evolutionary public health: introducing the concept. Lancet. 2017;390:500–509. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30572-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE. Income inequality and population health: A review and explanation of the evidence. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62:1768–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Wolfson M, Rowe G, Gentleman JF, Tomiak M. Career earnings and death: A longitudinal analysis of older Canadian men. Journal of Gerontology. 1993;48:S167–S179. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.4.S167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wood AM, Boyce CJ, Moore SC, Brown GDA. An evolutionary based social rank explanation of why low income predicts mental distress: A 17 year cohort study of 30,000 people. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;136:882–888. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Woolard K, Garcia AR, Stieglitz J, Kaplan HS, Gurven M, Trumble BC. Do perceptions of social status and material wealth associate with biomarkers of health in a relatively egalitarian population? American Journal of Human Biology. 2019;31:2019 [Google Scholar]
  117. Yehuda R, Halligan SL, Yang RK, Guo LS, Makotkine I, Singh B, Pickholtz D. Relationship between 24-hour urinary-free cortisol excretion and salivary cortisol levels sampled from awakening to bedtime in healthy subjects. Life Sciences. 2003;73:349–358. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(03)00286-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Milagros Ruiz1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This novel study tests the associations between wealth and wealth inequality with a range of health outcomes in the Tsimane. While these associations have been widely evaluated in urbanised populations around the world, the present study provides unique evidence by studying a small-scale subsistence society in Bolivia.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Relative wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by George Perry as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but as described below that additional analyses are required before it is published, we would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is "in revision at eLife". Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)

Essential revisions:

1. The presentation of results are confusing to the reader. Since count scores, binary and continuous outcomes are examined, it would be easier if standardised β coefficients were reported and compared throughout the paper instead of switching between raw and exponentiated coefficients. The results currently use z-scores for continuous predictor variables to facilitate interpretation of some model coefficients. However, it's clear (Figure 1B) that z-scoring wealth distorts absolute wealth differentials: 1 unit differences in the z score correspond to very large differences in wealth for the wealthy, and very small differences in wealth for the poor. This could influence the modeling results (in a direction of diminishing true wealth effects) and affect comparisons about the influence of relative and absolute wealth (e.g., lines 275-276). Comparing model coefficients of standardizing betas instead of z-scores of the predictor variables may therefore be more accurate, especially for very skewed distributions. In addition, can some outcomes be reverse coded so that higher coefficients consistently reflect worse health? This change would forgo the need for the last column in Table 1.

2. There is insufficient discussion on the role of covariates in the hypothesised associations since only fully adjusted models are reported. It would be helpful to add some metrics, such as the reduction in chi square or likelihood ratio statistics with the main variable and then with all covariates. This would also inform whether there is residual confounding.

3. Since the study reports 26 associations (wealth and wealth inequality with 13 outcomes separately), it is difficult to glean overall conclusions from the large number of mixed findings. It may be helpful to present associations between wealth and wealth inequality with health, in turn, followed by how these hypothesised associations were attenuated by potential psychosocial factors. Since psychosocial variables are theorised as mediators, but are reported concurrently with the health outcomes, the causal inferences being made in the paper are unclear. Given the large number of results, it would be helpful to add subheadings to parse out different parts of the Results section.

4. This may vary by discipline, but it is inappropriate to test mediators by adjusting for them as covariates in epidemiology. Mediation is normally assessed using causal modelling techniques. Furthermore, the study appears to use a cross-sectional data set making it difficult to distinguish between cause and effect irrespective of methods used. Therefore, the study cannot provide any definitive mediational results. The Introduction nicely summarises the theoretical basis of a psychosocial stress pathway, so perhaps the authors could explore the potential role of psychosocial factors in wealth-health associations without making strong claims. By reporting that there was weak evidence of associations between wealth/wealth inequality with psychosocial factors (paths A) and between psychosocial factors and health outcomes (paths B), in turn, according to the Baron and Kenny approach, one could say that there was weak, albeit indirect, evidence of these factors as potential mediators. Given the hypothesised relations, it is clearer not to refer to psychosocial factors when referring to health outcomes and vice versa.

5. The results for community-level inequality measures were difficult to understand. Based on Figure 1, it looks like higher inequality communities have small samples, with the exception of Mission. Do these sample size differences affect the reliability of the inequality effect estimates? Related: are community-level measures based on the households sampled, or on the entire community including individuals who may not be in some of the analyses? Or are household samples exhaustive for these communities? In the inequality analysis, do measures based only on the households sampled (if non-exhaustive) affect whether the Gini coefficients are representative of the community (especially in small samples, e.g. n=9)?

6. We found it challenging to interpret some of the results without more detail on the modelling strategy. Even after reading the methods, it was unclear who was included in the analysis. For example, in Table 1 the age range is 0-89.7 years. Does this mean that infants, children, and adolescents are grouped in the analysis with adults? If so, what's the justification for this approach, given that these age groups may experience wealth, morbidity, and mortality risk in qualitatively different manners (not captured by modeling age as a covariate)? Page 8 claims that the study includes 1/3 of the Tsimane population so does the analytic sample include approx. 5,300 persons of the 16K?

7. For the morbidity data, were morbidities counted based on occurrence at the time of the medical check-up, or also retrospectively across some span of time? If the former, to what degree do you think this would introduce measurement error?

8. Since Bayesian imputation might lead to some errors in estimating effects, the authors should comment on this. Furthermore, statistical methods to deal with missing data are useful to a degree, but Methods should report sample sizes and the proportion and reason for such high levels of missing data. If we read Table 1 correctly, the number of adults with health data vary from 850 adults for self-rated health to 9000 for BMI? Data on psychosocial factors are also quite low – if missing data are so high, how reliable are these data? Is this why there was weak evidence on the role of psychosocial factors? The study should provide information on the data quality in Methods and reflect on these limitations in Discussion.

9. There was some discussion between reviewers as they had some differences in interpretation of the results. One reviewer found that wealth inequality-health effects were more substantial than wealth-health, which is contrary to the authors' interpretation. For example, if comparing Figures 2 and 3, wealth associations with health were mixed across outcomes (3 strong associations out of 8), but wealth inequality associations were more consistent (5 strong associations out of 8). If this interpretation is correct, it would be important to consider the relative inconsistency/consistency of associations between outcomes since the paper sometimes refer to key findings for "health" as a global metric. Among the paper's strengths is its inclusion of a wide range of unique health outcomes, making it important to highlight any differences in results between health outcomes and to discuss potential reasons why household wealth is a weaker predictor of systolic and diastolic blood pressure than wealth inequality, for example.

10. The limitation of the difficulty of disentangling association vs. causation and the potential of reverse causation (due to the cross-sectional design) needs to be addressed more directly and clearly. If inferential analyses cannot be made to support the causation case, the quality of the evidence being presented in the paper should be discussed more critically.

11. The paper is written in an evolutionary medicine framework, with contrasts drawn between a "trade-offs" model and a mismatch model. This is an interesting presentation, but I'm concerned that the mismatch model is essentially unfalsifiable here. If social gradients did not exist in the Tsimane, mismatch would probably be invoked. But based on the results presented here, they do, including for markers of cardiovascular health-and this is also presented as support for evolutionary mismatch. An equally likely interpretation is that responses to relative wealth and social inequality across different types of human societies are shared, counter to the mismatch hypothesis. Because mismatch can be supported whether or not health-wealth associations are found, P4a and P4b become somewhat poorly defined predictions. For P4b, it's also worth noting that social gradients in health are observed in high-income countries for both chronic and infectious disease (the current pandemic is a good example of the latter). The results are valuable to understanding the evolutionary history of social gradients in humans, but a more critical take on which hypotheses it can robustly test would improve the manuscript.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Do wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society?" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Milagros Ruiz as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by George Perry as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Prabhat Jha (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

The authors have addressed all points raised during the review. We commend the authors for their substantial efforts on the manuscript, including the

additional analyses. We have only one further essential revision, which we hope will be a straightforward addition to the manuscript:

1. In order to help readers understand the range of results (e.g., many observed associations were weak or uncertain), we recommend that the authors expand the discussion with a few statements about the high degree of heterogeneity in their findings explicitly, especially given the wealth of evidence from high-income developed nations which generally find strong and directionally consistent effects of SES on health.

Reviewer #1:

The authors have addressed all major theoretical and methodological points raised during the review. We appreciate the work of the authors to address all of the reviewer comments, including the additional analyses.

Reviewer #2:

I commend the authors for addressing all the review comments seriously. I have no further review comments.

Reviewer #3:

Overall, this is a strong revision that is highly responsive to previous reviewer comments and concerns. As before, I think the manuscript presents a timely, important analysis of the social determinants of health in small-scale societies, using a uniquely rich data set. Several substantive changes help improve clarity and readability from the original submission. In particular, I appreciate the decision to uniformly code the direction of health outcomes, straightforward statements about the authors' own interpretation of the data, and the inclusion of sufficient methodological detail for readers to also draw their own conclusions. The revision also does a much-improved job discussing the relationship of the analysis to evolutionary mismatch. I'm strongly supportive of publication.

At the same time, the results are still quite complex to work through, and many of the observed associations are weak or uncertain enough that it's unclear whether they're "real" (this is exacerbated somewhat by the many statistical tests, which raises questions about multiple hypothesis testing). To be clear, I do not think further or altered analyses are needed: the complexity in the results seems likely to reflect, at least in large part, real-world complexity in wealth-health relationships in this society. However, the authors might want to consider discussing the high degree of heterogeneity in their findings explicitly, especially considering the sense one gets from the literature on wealthy, developed nations, that SES effects on morbidity/mortality are often both strong and directionally consistent.

eLife. 2021 May 14;10:e59437. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59437.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. The presentation of results are confusing to the reader. Since count scores, binary and continuous outcomes are examined, it would be easier if standardised β coefficients were reported and compared throughout the paper instead of switching between raw and exponentiated coefficients.

We agree that a better way to compare associations with binary and Gaussian outcome variables was desirable. We have now added Cohen’s d as a standardized measure of effect size for all coefficients, which makes effect sizes easily comparable. For binary outcomes we now also report the β values (on the log-odds scale) as well as the exponentiated values (odds ratios). Furthermore, in order to keep the text of the Results section consistent we have now defined criteria for referring to the strength of effect sizes and the certainty of associations (lines 350-356):

“For simplicity, we refer to effect sizes of d>0.2 as “strong”, those >0.1 as “moderate”, and consider the rest to be “weak” though potentially still suggestive of a general pattern. […] However, we encourage readers to use the full information on the posteriors to inform their own inference.”

The results currently use z-scores for continuous predictor variables to facilitate interpretation of some model coefficients. However, it's clear (Figure 1B) that z-scoring wealth distorts absolute wealth differentials: 1 unit differences in the z score correspond to very large differences in wealth for the wealthy, and very small differences in wealth for the poor. This could influence the modeling results (in a direction of diminishing true wealth effects) and affect comparisons about the influence of relative and absolute wealth (e.g., lines 275-276). Comparing model coefficients of standardizing betas instead of z-scores of the predictor variables may therefore be more accurate, especially for very skewed distributions.

The reviewers are correct that the distribution of absolute wealth is skewed, and that the use of wealth z-scores as calculated with GAMLSS, representing centiles, normalizes this skew (see left and right panels in Author response image 1). This is precisely the reason to use z-scores instead of absolute wealth. The logic is that we expect there to be diminishing returns per dollar in terms of absolute wealth, as wealth increases. For a poor person, a small increase in wealth would be expected to have a large effect, whereas for a wealthy person a similar increase in money would have virtually no effect. By using z-scores for relative wealth we normalize this effect of diminishing returns. Note that the Gini coefficients however were calculated based on the actual wealth values, not the z-scores, to reflect the actual inequality.

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

Nevertheless, we agree that the reviewers made a great point: it is possible that normalizing the wealth distribution misses some key information about wealth differentials that people might be sensitive to and that could therefore affect their behavior and health. We therefore ran an additional set of models with standardized wealth (i.e. household wealth – average wealth / standard deviation of wealth), which preserves the skewed distribution (see Figure above, middle panel). There were no meaningful qualitative changes in the results. We’ve added the following to the text to explain this approach (lines 753-760):“Wealth z-scores derived from GAMLSS, representing centile values, were used in all analyses in part because wealth was skewed in distribution, and also expected to have diminishing returns at higher values (i.e. 100 Bolivianos are worth more to a poor individual than a wealthy one). […] There were no qualitative differences in inference between the two methods, largely because z-scoring with GAMLSS primarily affects outliers on the far high end of the distribution.”

We also disambiguate the slightly different meanings of z scores (lines 760-763):

“Note that “zscore” can have two slightly different meanings; for wealth and BMI (see below), we generally mean centile values from GAMLSS unless otherwise noted, for all other variables Z scores refer simply to standardized values (i.e. [x – mean(x)] / sd(x))”.

In addition, can some outcomes be reverse coded so that higher coefficients consistently reflect worse health? This change would forgo the need for the last column in Table 1.

Thank you for this good suggestion. We’ve reverse coded the labor partners and self-rated health variables. This makes the tables and figures clearer as all positive associations now refer to beneficial outcomes and all negative ones to detrimental outcomes (though it does make the language in the text slightly more complicated).

2. There is insufficient discussion on the role of covariates in the hypothesised associations since only fully adjusted models are reported. It would be helpful to add some metrics, such as the reduction in chi square or likelihood ratio statistics with the main variable and then with all covariates. This would also inform whether there is residual confounding.

Thanks again for a good suggestion. We’ve added models with no covariates to the supplementary tables, i.e. only including household wealth and inequality, as well as a section and figures to the results discussing the effects of covariates (Section “Effect of covariates on outcomes”, Figures 4 and 5). Many covariates were associated with outcomes, but wealth and inequality (plus random effects) alone explained most of the variance in all models. We’ve also highlighted the effect of mean community wealth more in Figures 2 and 3 as well as in the results text (sections “Is wealth related to …?”).

3. Since the study reports 26 associations (wealth and wealth inequality with 13 outcomes separately), it is difficult to glean overall conclusions from the large number of mixed findings. It may be helpful to present associations between wealth and wealth inequality with health, in turn, followed by how these hypothesised associations were attenuated by potential psychosocial factors. Since psychosocial variables are theorised as mediators, but are reported concurrently with the health outcomes, the causal inferences being made in the paper are unclear. Given the large number of results, it would be helpful to add subheadings to parse out different parts of the Results section.

Thanks for this comment. We are keenly aware of the difficulty of presenting such a large number of analyses, and appreciate the suggestion of using subheadings and a more systematic reporting of results. We’ve now reorganized the Results section with several subheadings to break out the effects of wealth and inequality on physical and psychological and social variables, focusing on the health outcomes first. Specifically, the subheadings are:

  • – Is wealth related to health outcomes? (this includes household wealth and community wealth)

  • – Is inequality related to health outcomes?

  • – Is wealth related to psychosocial outcomes? (this includes household wealth and community wealth)

  • – Is inequality related to psychosocial outcomes?

  • – Do psychosocial variables mediate relationships between wealth or wealth inequality and health?

  • – Effects of covariates on outcomes

We also present all associations within each subsection more systematically, e.g. “household wealth was associated … with reductions in all negative health outcomes except gastrointestinal illness”, prior to giving more detail on how strong and well supported the associations were. We also changed the main results figures to coefficient plots, which should help provide a better overview – the previously presented counterfactual plots have now been added as supplements to these coefficient plots. As a result of all these changes, and the addition of standardized effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and reverse coding (see above), the reader can now more easily and systematically evaluate the findings.

We briefly also considered combining all outcome variables of the same probability distribution (Gaussian, binomial) into a single model, with a random effect for outcome type (Depression, BMI, etc.); this would allow us to estimate a single overall effect of each predictor, with random slopes for each outcome type. However, we decided against this approach because it hinges on some strong assumptions, such as the residuals of all Gaussian outcome variables having the same dispersion captured by a single σ parameter. Instead, readers can use the coefficient plots and Cohen’s d values to evaluate effect sizes on a case by case basis. In combination with the probabilistic inference facilitated by Bayesian modeling we believe this provides readers with all the necessary information to draw their own conclusions.

We have also added a more detailed treatment of causal inference (see also response to next comment) by adding DAGs and additional text in Appendix 1.

Finally, the first paragraph of the discussion provides a summary of the findings in relation to the predictions.

4. This may vary by discipline, but it is inappropriate to test mediators by adjusting for them as covariates in epidemiology. Mediation is normally assessed using causal modelling techniques. Furthermore, the study appears to use a cross-sectional data set making it difficult to distinguish between cause and effect irrespective of methods used. Therefore, the study cannot provide any definitive mediational results. The Introduction nicely summarises the theoretical basis of a psychosocial stress pathway, so perhaps the authors could explore the potential role of psychosocial factors in wealth-health associations without making strong claims. By reporting that there was weak evidence of associations between wealth/wealth inequality with psychosocial factors (paths A) and between psychosocial factors and health outcomes (paths B), in turn, according to the Baron and Kenny approach, one could say that there was weak, albeit indirect, evidence of these factors as potential mediators. Given the hypothesised relations, it is clearer not to refer to psychosocial factors when referring to health outcomes and vice versa.

We agree that our approach to the mediation analysis was a bit too informal. We have addressed this in several ways. First, as mentioned above, we created a separate section of the results for reporting the mediation analyses, with the subheading “Do psychosocial variables mediate relationships between wealth or wealth inequality and health?”. Second, we begin this section by outlining the formal criteria for mediation according to Baron and Kenny (Lines 467-479):

“We tested the prediction (P3) that the effects of wealth or inequality on health were mediated via psychosocial pathways using formal mediation analysis (Baron and Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2007). […] See Appendix 1 for a discussion and graphical depiction of the causal relationships assumed by this mediation approach.”

Third, we more clearly highlight cases where path A and path B are well supported, before mentioning that indirect effects were largely negligible, and that there was little evidence overall for mediation:

“The only convincing evidence for mediation was found for depression and non-social problems mediating the effect of household wealth on diastolic blood pressure; specifically, household wealth was negatively associated with diastolic blood pressure (path c) as well as with depression and non-social problems (paths a; see above, Figure 2), and both depression (β=-0.03, P>0=0.20) and non-social problems (β=0.08, P>0=0.05) were themselves negatively associated with diastolic blood pressure (paths b). However, there were no other cases where both path a and path b were well supported, the indirect effects of household wealth, community wealth, or inequality were virtually always 0 for any mediator (including depression and non-social problems), and the proportion mediated was generally small or highly uncertain (Tables S18-S20). Overall, there was little evidence of mediation.”

Finally, we added Appendix 1 which discusses the mediation analysis and its causal assumptions in more detail. Specifically, we present formal DAGs of the causal relationships assumed by the mediation analyses we performed and highlight potential problems due to treating each mediator independently, when in fact two or more mediators might be causally affected by wealth/inequality and/or each other. As discussed in detail in Appendix 1, we ultimately don’t think this is a problem because our previous analysis included several mediators simultaneously and arrived at the same conclusion as the present approach: that there was little evidence for wealth/inequality associations with health to be mediated by the “psychosocial” variables we used.

All that being said, the reviewers are right that the data are largely cross-sectional and hence we cannot provide causality in the Granger sense (e.g. increases in wealth inequality precede increases in psychosocial stress which then lead to detrimental health outcomes). We now also mention this explicitly in the Discussion (Lines 663-664):

“a limitation of our data is that we cannot determine the direction of causation given our cross-sectional design”.

5. The results for community-level inequality measures were difficult to understand. Based on Figure 1, it looks like higher inequality communities have small samples, with the exception of Mission. Do these sample size differences affect the reliability of the inequality effect estimates? Related: are community-level measures based on the households sampled, or on the entire community including individuals who may not be in some of the analyses? Or are household samples exhaustive for these communities? In the inequality analysis, do measures based only on the households sampled (if non-exhaustive) affect whether the Gini coefficients are representative of the community (especially in small samples, e.g. n=9)?

Thanks for these good questions and the opportunity to clarify. Originally, we had used number of households sampled as our measure of community size, rather than full community size. We replaced this variable with the actual number of adults in the community (based on a complete census). In practice this made little difference, since sample size per community and complete community size are highly correlated, meaning we sampled relatively evenly across communities (r = 0.95).

Gini coefficients are necessarily based just on the households with wealth measured, and not the complete community. However, they are not limited to individuals with other covariates, so remain the same across analyses. It is plausible that Gini measures might be less reliable for smaller samples, however, we find no evidence for systematic bias in Gini estimates by community sample size. We’ve added the following to the paper to further characterize the relationships between community level variables (lines 311-317):

“Wealth inequality was generally higher in communities closer to the market towns of San Borja and Yucumo, where Tsimane can sell produce and purchase market goods, though some villages near towns show low inequality (Figure 1D) (correlation between Gini and distance to market r=-0.38, df=38, p=0.01). Inequality was marginally lower in richer communities (r=0.22, df=38, p=0.17). Community size was not significantly related to distance (r=-0.18, p=0.26), mean wealth (r=0.11, p=0.50), or inequality (r=0.00, p=0.99). ”

6. We found it challenging to interpret some of the results without more detail on the modelling strategy. Even after reading the methods, it was unclear who was included in the analysis. For example, in Table 1 the age range is 0-89.7 years. Does this mean that infants, children, and adolescents are grouped in the analysis with adults? If so, what's the justification for this approach, given that these age groups may experience wealth, morbidity, and mortality risk in qualitatively different manners (not captured by modeling age as a covariate)? Page 8 claims that the study includes 1/3 of the Tsimane population so does the analytic sample include approx. 5,300 persons of the 16K?

Thanks for pointing this out. We’ve updated table 1 to make the sample clearer, reporting the number of unique individuals and number of observations for each variable, and further clarifying sample size at the individual, household, and community levels. Additionally, each model in the supplementary tables now lists the sample size for that analysis in terms of observations and clustering at all three levels.

In light of the comment about the wide age range, we have now divided the sample and analyses into adults > 15 years of age (since the youngest head of household in our sample is 16) and juveniles =< 15. This makes clear that some variables were collected only on adults, especially the psychosocial outcomes, and also allows us to show where effects are similar or different for juveniles vs. adults. There are now separate tables, figures, and results for adults and juveniles.

To further clarify who was included in the analysis we added a figure supplement to Figure 1, with the caption:

“Ever sampled by THLHP refers to the period potentially included in this study, i.e. up to December 2015; note that this sample includes 92 communities. […] For a further missingness breakdown of the sample by specific outcome variable see Table 1”.

We hope that this helps clarify the sample composition relative to the larger Tsimane population. We also corrected the statement mentioned in the comment to say that the sample included “approximately one quarter of the Tsimane population” (Line 213).

7. For the morbidity data, were morbidities counted based on occurrence at the time of the medical check-up, or also retrospectively across some span of time? If the former, to what degree do you think this would introduce measurement error?

These are based on occurrence at the time of medical check-up, with repeat check-ups for some individuals. We now make this more explicit in lines 812-813:

“Morbidity at the time of the medical check-up was assessed by physicians using the ICD-10 classification”.

Likely this is less sensitive than a continuous measure over time would be. However, since prevalence’s are high (22-36%) and many infections are chronic (especially parasitic ones), we don’t think this is a major limitation.

8. Since Bayesian imputation might lead to some errors in estimating effects, the authors should comment on this. Furthermore, statistical methods to deal with missing data are useful to a degree, but Methods should report sample sizes and the proportion and reason for such high levels of missing data. If we read Table 1 correctly, the number of adults with health data vary from 850 adults for self-rated health to 9000 for BMI? Data on psychosocial factors are also quite low – if missing data are so high, how reliable are these data? Is this why there was weak evidence on the role of psychosocial factors? The study should provide information on the data quality in Methods and reflect on these limitations in Discussion.

In the original version imputation was only used in the mediation models since in those models we needed to combine samples on multiple different outcomes that only partially overlapped. We have since redone those models (see response to comment 4) and have decided not to use imputation, given the large amount of missing data from some overlapping samples. The actual sample size for these models is now noted, and we have added a note about sample limitations for mediation analyses in the discussion (line 684-686):

“An obvious limitation is that our sample sizes for the mediation analysis were smaller than for other analyses (Table S18-S20), though most were still large enough to capture any meaningful effect.”

More generally, however, the reason some variables have much higher missing values is because the larger epidemiological surveillance of our project always collects basic information like BMI and medical diagnoses, but hormone sampling, depression, etc. were separate intersecting studies on sub-samples of our larger population – most done either with stratified age sampling, or on ages 40+ (given the primary THLHP research focus on aging).

9. There was some discussion between reviewers as they had some differences in interpretation of the results. One reviewer found that wealth inequality-health effects were more substantial than wealth-health, which is contrary to the authors' interpretation. For example, if comparing Figures 2 and 3, wealth associations with health were mixed across outcomes (3 strong associations out of 8), but wealth inequality associations were more consistent (5 strong associations out of 8). If this interpretation is correct, it would be important to consider the relative inconsistency/consistency of associations between outcomes since the paper sometimes refer to key findings for "health" as a global metric. Among the paper's strengths is its inclusion of a wide range of unique health outcomes, making it important to highlight any differences in results between health outcomes and to discuss potential reasons why household wealth is a weaker predictor of systolic and diastolic blood pressure than wealth inequality, for example.

Firstly, as mentioned above, the new figures 2-5 (coefficient plots), the more consistent coding of outcome variables, and the standardized effect sizes should make it easier to evaluate the relative importance of predictor variables. Secondly, we’ve strengthened the discussion quite a bit in terms of the implications of findings, particularly on blood pressure and respiratory illness, which are probably the two most meaningful associations. In editing the discussion we’ve mostly avoided general discussions about “health” as a global metric, because overall the results for both wealth and inequality are quite mixed. We also don’t discuss the number of associations as a metric, since to some extent this is artificial (i.e. there are two measures for blood pressure, total morbidity incorporates the binary diagnoses that are presented separately, etc.) – this is another reason why we avoided analyzing all outcomes in a single multivariate model to generate an “overall effect” of wealth / inequality (see response to comment 3). Beyond highlighting that greater wealth in general was more consistently associated with beneficial outcomes than lower inequality (1st paragraph of discussion) and discussing the strongest associations, i.e. respiratory disease and blood pressure, and their potential mechanisms (2nd and 3rd paragraph of the discussion, respectively), as well as links between psychosocial factors and health (6th paragraph) we found that comparing other associations (or lack thereof) in more detail would be too speculative and take up too much space. However, the results are now presented in a way that should more easily facilitate readers’ evaluation of specific findings.

10. The limitation of the difficulty of disentangling association vs. causation and the potential of reverse causation (due to the cross-sectional design) needs to be addressed more directly and clearly. If inferential analyses cannot be made to support the causation case, the quality of the evidence being presented in the paper should be discussed more critically.

The reviewers are correct that we cannot disentangle association from causation given our study design. We’ve now added the following to the discussion to make these limitations more explicit (lines 656-665):

“Another possible interpretation for some of these associations may be that causality is reversed, with poor health leading to less wealth or exacerbated inequality. […] Other confounds might also be possible, for example if people preferentially assort by health or wealth by moving between villages.”

11. The paper is written in an evolutionary medicine framework, with contrasts drawn between a "trade-offs" model and a mismatch model. This is an interesting presentation, but I'm concerned that the mismatch model is essentially unfalsifiable here. If social gradients did not exist in the Tsimane, mismatch would probably be invoked. But based on the results presented here, they do, including for markers of cardiovascular health-and this is also presented as support for evolutionary mismatch. An equally likely interpretation is that responses to relative wealth and social inequality across different types of human societies are shared, counter to the mismatch hypothesis. Because mismatch can be supported whether or not health-wealth associations are found, P4a and P4b become somewhat poorly defined predictions. For P4b, it's also worth noting that social gradients in health are observed in high-income countries for both chronic and infectious disease (the current pandemic is a good example of the latter). The results are valuable to understanding the evolutionary history of social gradients in humans, but a more critical take on which hypotheses it can robustly test would improve the manuscript.

These are very good points and we agree that the predictions related to mismatch were underspecified. We have now refrained from attempting to make contrasting predictions regarding mismatch vs tradeoffs. Instead, we focus solely on the more fundamental predictions that relatively higher socio-economic position should have beneficial effects (P1), that higher inequality should have detrimental effects (P2), and that associations between wealth/inequality and health should be mediated by psychosocial stress (P3); these are the predictions that follow from the “relative wealth hypothesis”, the “inequality hypothesis” and their proposed biological mechanisms (see e.g. Lynch et al. 2004).

We have kept the evolutionary medicine framing though and expanded the introduction of the mismatch concept in lines 121-135. Rather than presenting tradeoffs and mismatch as contrasting hypotheses, we now frame them as complementary: people might have evolved to shift their behavior and physiology in particular ways depending on local context, e.g. they might be more aggressive, have higher cortisol levels and higher blood pressure when living in a highly competitive environment, and these shifts might become especially detrimental to health when coupled with evolutionarily novel lifestyles. For instance, environments may be more competitive than before, leading to even higher levels of cortisol or blood pressure, and/or this altered physiology is especially damaging in combination with novel risk factors like obesogenic diets, lack of physical activity, etc., leading to chronic disease. In other words, what’s novel in a mismatched environment is not that physiology responds to the competitive environment, but that this response is more likely to cause disease. We hope that this expanded introduction makes it easier to understand our interpretation and discussion of the blood pressure results (lines 622-653): that we see here the beginnings of what seems to be such a common problem in modern societies – that blood pressure is affected by the socioeconomic context – and while this may not cause much harm among the Tsimane, given their overall excellent cardiovascular health, it lays the foundation for chronic problems when coupled with novel risk factors.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The authors have addressed all points raised during the review. We commend the authors for their substantial efforts on the manuscript, including the additional analyses. We have only one further essential revision, which we hope will be a straightforward addition to the manuscript:

1. In order to help readers understand the range of results (e.g., many observed associations were weak or uncertain), we recommend that the authors expand the discussion with a few statements about the high degree of heterogeneity in their findings explicitly, especially given the wealth of evidence from high-income developed nations which generally find strong and directionally consistent effects of SES on health.

We thank the editors and reviewers for their time and the positive evaluations, and for the opportunity to revise our paper. We have restructured the Discussion and added new writing, which we believe strengthens the Discussion substantially. Specifically, the first paragraph states all the main findings, including the heterogeneity of results (Lines 427 – 431):

“Overall, our results showed substantial heterogeneity in terms of the direction and magnitude of associations between wealth, wealth-inequality, and health, which contrasts with the more consistent SES-health gradients in high-income countries. Nevertheless, some findings supported an association between wealth or inequality and health outcomes, though these associations were not mediated by psychosocial factors.”

This is followed by the existing paragraphs discussing the clinical relevance of the respiratory disease and blood pressure results, as well as alternative explanations for these. Then follow several paragraphs putting the heterogeneity of results into context, all but the last one of which are new (Lines 507-553):

“Beyond respiratory disease and blood pressure, many associations were inconclusive. […] Thus, household wealth may in part be inconsistently associated with health because of these opposing processes.”

Reviewer #3:

[…] The results are still quite complex to work through, and many of the observed associations are weak or uncertain enough that it's unclear whether they're "real" (this is exacerbated somewhat by the many statistical tests, which raises questions about multiple hypothesis testing). To be clear, I do not think further or altered analyses are needed: the complexity in the results seems likely to reflect, at least in large part, real-world complexity in wealth-health relationships in this society. However, the authors might want to consider discussing the high degree of heterogeneity in their findings explicitly, especially considering the sense one gets from the literature on wealthy, developed nations, that SES effects on morbidity/mortality are often both strong and directionally consistent.

Thank you for the positive evaluation and for this suggestion. We’ve modified the discussion to better reflect this point, adding several paragraphs (see excerpts above). Specifically, we discuss the possibilities that wealth-health associations may be less consistent among the Tsimane because (i) the epidemiological context is dominated by infectious, rather than chronic disease, (ii) we did not assess wealth and inequality at larger scales, such as in comparison to the local rural population, or all of Bolivia, and (iii) household wealth reflects both local status as well as market integration, which have partly opposing effects on stress and possibly infectious disease exposure and thereby health.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Jaeggi AV, Blackwell AD, von Rueden C, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Garcia AR, Kraft TS, Beheim BA, Hooper PL, Kaplan H, Gurven M. 2021. Data and code for "Do wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society?". Zenodo. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. This file contains the following tables with additional information on the statistical models.

    a: Model summary – depression. b: Model summary – social conflicts. c: Model summary – fewer labor partners. d: Model summary – non-social problems. e: Model summary – cortisol. f: Model summary – BMI. g: Model summary – systolic blood pressure. h: Model summary – diastolic blood pressure. i: Model summary – worse self-rated health. j: Model summary – total morbidity. k: Model summary – infections. l: Model summary – respiratory illness. m: Model summary – gastrointestinal illness. n: Gaussian model summaries for juveniles. o: Logistic model summaries for juveniles. p: Overview of exploratory interaction effects. q: Mediation of wealth effects. r: Mediation of inequality effects. s: Mediation of mean community wealth effects.

    elife-59437-supp1.docx (117KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data and R code are available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4567498 with any updates at https://github.com/adblackwell/wealthinequality (copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:da16ac6b20732fe1939478450d81ac32fdcce202).

    The following dataset was generated:

    Jaeggi AV, Blackwell AD, von Rueden C, Trumble BC, Stieglitz J, Garcia AR, Kraft TS, Beheim BA, Hooper PL, Kaplan H, Gurven M. 2021. Data and code for "Do wealth and inequality associate with health in a small-scale subsistence society?". Zenodo.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES