Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Eur J Neurosci. 2020 Dec 17;55(9-10):2836–2850. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14979

Heart-brain interactions during social and cognitive stress in hypertensive disease: a multidimensional approach

Agustina Legaz 1,2,3, Adrián Yoris 1,4, Lucas Sedeño 1, Sofía Abrevaya 1,4, Miguel Martorell 1,4, Florencia Alifano 1,4, Adolfo M García 1,2,5,6,*, Agustín Ibañez 1,2,6,7,8,*
PMCID: PMC8231407  NIHMSID: NIHMS1708615  PMID: 32965070

Abstract

Hypertensive disease (HTD), a prominent risk factor for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases, is characterized by elevated stress-proneness. Since stress levels are underpinned by both cardiac and neural factors, multidimensional insights are required to robustly understand their disruption in HTD. Yet, despite their crucial relevance, heart rate variability (HRV) and multimodal neurocognitive markers of stress in HTD remain controversial and unexplored, respectively. To bridge this gap, we studied cardiodynamic as well as electrophysiological and neuroanatomical measures of stress in HTD patients and healthy controls. Both groups performed the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), a validated stress-inducing task comprising a baseline and a mental stress period. During both stages, we assessed a sensitive HRV parameter (the low frequency/high frequency [LF/HF ratio]) and an online neurophysiological measure (the heartbeat-evoked potential [HEP]). Also, we obtained neuroanatomical data via voxel-based morphometry (VBM) for correlation with online markers. Relative to controls, HTD patients exhibited increased LF/HF ratio and greater HEP modulations during baseline, reduced changes between baseline and stress periods, and lack of significant stress-related HRV modulations associated with the grey matter volume of putative frontrostriatal regions. Briefly, HTD patients presented signs of stress-related autonomic imbalance, reflected in a potential basal stress overload and a lack of responsiveness to acute psychosocial stress, accompanied by neurophysiological and neuroanatomical alterations. These multimodal insights underscore the relevance of neurocognitive data for developing innovations in the characterization, prognosis, and treatment of HTD and other conditions with autonomic imbalance. More generally, these findings may offer new insights into heart-brain interactions.

Keywords: Heart rate variability, hypertension, psychosocial stress

Graphical abstract

graphic file with name nihms-1708615-f0001.jpg

Hypertensive disease patients gave signs of stress-related autonomic imbalance, reflected in a potential basal stress overload effect and a lack of responsiveness to acute psychosocial stress, accompanied by neurophysiological and neuroanatomical alterations. These multimodal findings encourage the development of applied innovations in the characterization, prognosis, and treatment of hypertension, and offer new insights into the heart-brain interactions.

Introduction

Hypertensive disease (HTD), defined by high blood pressure (Unger et al., 2020), ranks among the highest risk factors for disabling cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases (Ruediger et al., 2004; Thayer et al., 2010). Moreover, this disorder is typified by elevated stress-proneness (Wirtz et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2017a), a factor that also undermines cardiovascular function (Cohen et al., 2007; McEwen, 2007) and further compromises the patients’ physical and mental health (Wiener et al., 2020). Since stress levels are underpinned by both cardiac and neural mechanisms (Gianaros & Sheu, 2009; Allen et al., 2014), multidimensional insights are required to robustly understand their disruption in HTD. Measures of heart rate variability (HRV) are robust markers of mental stress (Taelman et al., 2009; Allen et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2018), especially if complemented with neurocognitive assessments (MacKinnon et al., 2013; Wei et al., 2018). However, HRV studies on HTD are controversial due to methodological inconsistencies and limitations (including sub-optimal methods to induce stress, unreliable markers of autonomic activity, and inconsistent HRV measures). Here, we administered the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), a validated stress-inducing task (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Allen et al., 2017), to HTD patients and healthy controls, targeting a highly sensitive HRV parameter –i.e., the low frequency/high frequency (LF/HF) ratio– as well as electrophysiological and neuroimaging markers of heart-brain interactions.

Autonomic responses to induced mental stress are reliably indexed by HRV modulations (Taelman et al., 2009; Castaldo et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2018). Compared to other stress measures, HRV offers a relatively simple, non-invasive approach which has been increasingly used to assess autonomic function in various populations and paradigms (Thayer & Sternberg, 2006; Vlcek et al., 2008; Chandola et al., 2010). Specifically, the LF/HF ratio outperforms single frequency-domain components in capturing the sympathovagal balance (Pagani et al., 1984; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996; Vlcek et al., 2008; Goldstein et al., 2011) during acute psychological stress (Xhyheri et al., 2012; Allen et al., 2014). In HTD patients, autonomic imbalance involves increased sympathetic and decreased parasympathetic activity at rest (Langewitz et al., 1994; Madsen et al., 2008; Thayer et al., 2010), alongside increased sympathetic reactivity (Kaushik et al., 2004; Wirtz et al., 2006) and lack of significant modulations of the LF/HF ratio (Garafova et al., 2014) during stress; all aligned with an impaired responsiveness to autonomic demands (Madsen et al., 2008). However, few studies have targeted the latter variable and most have favored sub-optimal mental stress inductors, such as the Stroop test (Garafova et al., 2014) or mental arithmetic paradigms (Ruediger et al., 2004), i.e., purely cognitive (non-socially-laden) tasks considered to induce small or inconsistent effects in isolation (Allen et al., 2014).

Furthermore, although studies that concurrently examined brain markers and peripheral measures in HTD have been encouraged (Jennings & Zanstra, 2009), no previous report seems to have explored multidimensional brain correlates of stress in HTD. In particular, the use of a task indexed with structural and temporal brain measures may reveal different spatiotemporal mechanisms (Melloni et al., 2015; García-Cordero et al., 2016; Melloni et al., 2016; Ibáñez, 2018; Fittipaldi et al., 2020) underlying stress alterations, paving the way for new insights to inform the characterization and future treatments of this disease (Devor et al., 2013). To this end, we assessed ongoing modulations of the heartbeat evoked potential (HEP), an automatic cortical measure of interoceptive cardiac signals (Schandry & Montoya, 1996; Pollatos et al., 2005) modulated by stress levels in healthy subjects (Schulz et al., 2013) and cardiac patients (Gray et al., 2007). Moreover, HEP modulations have been linked to HRV in normotensives (MacKinnon et al., 2013). Crucially, this neurophysiological marker is disrupted in HTD patients (Yoris et al., 2018), suggesting that sensing of autonomic visceral and vascular body signals (Craig, 2002; Critchley & Harrison, 2013; Salamone et al., 2020) is associated with peripheral cardiovascular disruptions. This might reflect altered allostasis, that is, neural predictions of interoceptive signals to regulate the body’s physiological system and support psychological phenomena including recognition of and reaction to psychosocial stress (Kleckner et al., 2017; Mocayar Marón et al., 2019). Also, from a neuroanatomical perspective, stress-induced HRV has been related to higher grey matter volume in fronto-temporo-insular regions in healthy subjects (Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2012), whereas HTD patients show a disrupted association between brain volume and interoceptive performance (Yoris et al., 2018). In sum, HRV measures can be combined with HEP and neuroimaging to track the neurovisceral bases of stress in HTD.

We pursued this goal using the TSST, a test that reliably induces psychosocial stress in healthy and clinical populations (Heim et al., 2000; Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004; Kudielka et al., 2007), including HTD patients (Wirtz et al., 2006). Specifically, the TSST achieves this by combining social and cognitive components that entail evaluative threat and outcome uncontrollability –two factors that surpass other stressors in eliciting stress responses such as cortisol levels (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). Our multimodal approach encompasses measurements of a critical HRV feature (LF/HF ratio), neurophysiological (HEP), and neuroanatomical correlates (voxel-based morphometry [VBM]). Considering previous findings (Wirtz et al., 2006; Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2012; MacKinnon et al., 2013; Schulz et al., 2013; Garafova et al., 2014; Kleckner et al., 2017; Yoris et al., 2018; Mocayar Marón et al., 2019), we predicted that HTD patients would exhibit significantly increased and undifferentiated HRV (LF/HF ratio) during baseline and acute mental stress induced through the TSST. In this sense, higher HRV was expected to be accompanied by increased HEP modulations, associated to allostatic/interoceptive dysregulations. Finally, we expected that HTD patients would exhibit reduced associations between HRV and the volume of key fronto-temporo-insular regions. In brief, this multimodal study aims to shed novel light on multidimensional markers of health and mental stress.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study comprised 25 HTD patients and 27 healthy controls with no history of hypertension. Power estimation analysis confirmed the robustness of our sample size (Information S1). Subjects in the HTD group were chronic outpatients of the Metabolic and Arterial Hypertension Unit of the Favaloro Foundation Hospital, diagnosed following current revised criteria (Sánchez et al., 2003). Their blood pressure fell in Grade 1 (systolic blood pressure between 140–159 mmHg and/or diastolic blood pressure between 90–99 mmHg), within the hypertension classification of the 2007 European Society of Hypertension (ESH) and the European Society of Cardiology (ESC) guidelines for the management of arterial hypertension (Mancia et al., 2007). Office blood pressure readings and ambulatory blood pressure monitoring confirmed the patients’ condition (Table 1). All subjects in the HTD group were under antihypertensive medication (Table S1), but intake was suspended 48 hours before the study to prevent drug-related confounds (Yoris et al., 2018). None of the participants presented psychiatric symptoms, as assessed through extensive clinical interviews based on the WHO’s ICD-10 diagnostic guidelines (WHO, 1992). Additionally, neither controls nor patients presented cognitive or emotional impairments (for details, see Cognitive and emotional assessment section, and Table 1), lacunar infarcts, or white (WM) or grey matter (GM) lesions (Figure S1). Both groups were matched for gender, age, education, and body mass index (Table 1). All participants signed an informed consent in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology.

Table 1.

Demographic, cardiovascular, cognitive and emotional measures

Variables Groups Statistics
Controls HTD
patients
Demographic results
Gender (F:M) 18:09 13:12 χ2 = 0.63, P = 0.43
Age 66.04 (7.29) 67.56 (9.34) F1 ,50 = 0.43, P = 0.51, ηp2 = 0.00
Education 16.59 (2.39) 15.00 (4.54) F1 ,50 = 2.55, P = 0.12, ηp2 = 0.04
Body mass index 25.62 (3.37) 26.89 (3.45) F1 ,50 = 1.78, P = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.03
Handedness (R:L) 26:0 23:01 ---
Clinical cardiovascular assessment
ABPM (systolic 24hrs.) 114.47 (18.21) 127.60 (13.25) F1,37 = 6.67, P = 0.01*, ηp2 = 0.15
OBP systolic 132.85 (13.10) 143.51 (17.74) F1 ,33 = 4.19, P = 0.04*, ηp2 = 0.11
OBP diastolic 77.68 (6.99) 82.31 (4.92) F1 ,31 = 4.46, P = 0.04*, ηp2 = 0.12
Cognitive symptom assessment
IFS 25.26 (2.35) 23.95 (3.68) F1 ,48 = 2.27, P = 0.13, ηp2 = 0.04
Emotional symptom assessment
BDI – II 11.47 (9.10) 9.38 (4.66) F1 ,32 = 0.58, P = 0.450, ηp2 = 0.18

Results are presented as mean (SD). The asterisk indicates significant differences. Demographic and clinical data were assessed through ANOVAs. Gender was analyzed with Pearson’s chi-squared (χ2) test. Effects sizes were calculated through partial eta (ηp2). ABPM: ambulatory blood pressure monitor; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory – II; IFS: INECO Frontal Screening; OBP: office blood pressure.

Cognitive and emotional assessment

All participants were evaluated with the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) battery (Torralva et al., 2009), which is sensitive to executive dysfunction (Torralva et al., 2009; Gleichgerrcht et al., 2011). This test includes eight subtests: (1) motor programming (Luria series, “fist, edge, palm”); (2) conflicting instructions (subjects must hit the table once when the administrator hits it twice, or twice when the administrator hits it only once); (3) motor inhibitory control; (4) numerical working memory (backward digit span); (5) verbal working memory (months backwards); (6) spatial working memory (modified Corsi tapping test); (7) abstraction capacity (inferring the meaning of proverbs); and (8) verbal inhibitory control (modified Hayling test). The maximum possible score on the IFS is 30 points. Furthermore, we used the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) (Beck et al., 1996), a 21-item scale for the assessing of emotional, behavioral, and somatic symptoms of depression. Higher BDI-II scores indicate more severe depression symptoms. Between-group comparisons assessed through one-way ANOVA indicated non-significant differences between controls and HTD patients (Table 1).

Trier Social Stress Test

Responses to stress were assessed with the TSST (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Allen et al., 2017), a gold-standard test to induce acute psychosocial stress, as indexed through key biomarkers (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004; Allen et al., 2014). In light of our hypothesis, we focused on two periods of the test: the baseline period (used to determine a basal state) and the stress period (which involves three stressful tasks: speech preparation, speech, and mental arithmetic). First, in the baseline period (lasting 5 minutes), participants were asked to remain still, keep their eyes open, and avoid thinking about anything in particular. Then, subjects were given 5 minutes to prepare a speech and convince an evaluator that they were perfect candidates for a job (Speech preparation). Immediately after, participants delivered their monologue facing an unfamiliar evaluator trained to withhold any type of social engagement or positive feedback (Speech). Finally, participants completed a 5-minute serial-subtraction task (Mental arithmetic), assessed by the same evaluator. ECG and EEG signals were recorded during the whole test to assess HRV and heart rate (see ECG data preprocessing and analysis section for details), as well as HEP modulations (see EEG data preprocessing and analysis section for details), respectively.

State-anxiety assessment during TSST

Exposure to stress generally leads to subjectively negative experiences (Allen et al., 2014), and the TSST, in particular, increases anxiety (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Rohrmann et al., 1999; Rimmele et al., 2009). To confirm that participants, especially the HTD sample, found the TSST stressful, we compared state anxiety using the state scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Speilberger & Vagg, 1984) before baseline and immediately after stress period. Two-tailed paired t-tests showed increased anxiety across groups immediately after the stress period, evidencing that the task was actually stressful (for details, see Table 2).

Table 2.

State-anxiety during TSST

Groups Variables Statistics
State-
anxiety
baseline
State-
anxiety
stress
All groups 27.05 (6.31) 31.16 (8.26) t47 = −3.27, P = 0.002*, d = 0.47
Controls 28.03 (4.53) 30.03 (6.74) t25 = −1.44, P = 0.162, d = 0.28
HTD patients 26.86 (7.98) 32.50 (9.75) t21 = −3.21, P = 0.004*, d = 0.68

State-anxiety was assessed through the state scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Speilberger & Vagg, 1984). Results are presented as mean (SD). The asterisk indicates significant difference. Effect sizes were calculated through Cohen’s d. HTD: Hypertensive disease.

ECG data preprocessing and analysis

Preprocessing of heart rate data

We measured HRV to estimate the influence of cardiodynamic differences between groups during the TSST (Castaldo et al., 2015). We used the Kubios HRV program (Tarvainen et al., 2014) and imported the beat-to-beat RR interval data from the ECG (on the MATLAB platform [MATLAB R2017b, The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA]). This software analyzes HRV in both time and frequency domains. Given the short timespans, we applied an autoregressive algorithm to analyze the power spectrum (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). This algorithm generates a power spectral analysis with different frequency bands: high frequency (HF: 0.15 to 0.40 Hz), low frequency (LF: 0.04 to 0.15 Hz), and very low frequency (VLF: 0.00 to 0.04 Hz). The LF/HF ratio has been suggested as a measure of sympathovagal balance (Eckberg, 1997; Taelman et al., 2009). We calculated these frequency components in normalized units, which represent the relative value of each power component in proportion to the total power minus the VLF component (Pagani et al., 1986; Malliani, 1991). Moreover, The Kubios HRV also yields a mean heart rate measure for each period.

HRV analysis

HRV was analyzed based on normalized units from the LF/HF ratio. To remove data unrelated to psychophysiological process and enhance the power, values above 2 SDs LF/HF ratio were excluded (Zimmerman, 1994; Osborne & Overbay, 2004; de la Fuente et al., 2019). The mean HRV (LF/HF) modulation during the TSST periods (baseline and stress) was compared between groups via a one-way ANOVA. Effect sizes were reported with partial eta squared (ηp2). Between-group comparisons were performed on SPSS software (version 22.0, IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA).

Heart rate analysis

Acute stress raises heart rate, as confirmed in previous TSST studies (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002; Rimmele et al., 2007; Yamakawa et al., 2009). We assessed heart rate differences between periods for each group via two-tailed paired t-tests. Both groups presented a significant increase in heart rate between baseline and stress periods (see Table 3), confirming that the stress period of the TSST was actually stressful.

Table 3.

Heart rate during TSST

Groups Variables Statistics
HR
baseline
HR
stress
Controls 65.46 (5.15) 72.73 (5.24) t24 = −7.67, P < 0.001*, d = 1.53
HTD patients 68.73 (9.13) 84.19 (21.43) t20 = −3.28, P = 0.004*, d = 0.71

Results are presented as mean (SD). The asterisk indicates significant differences. Effect sizes were calculated through Cohen’s d. HTD: Hypertensive disease; HR: Heart rate.

EEG data preprocessing and analysis

EEG signal preprocessing

Data was obtained with a Biosemi Active-two 128-channel system at 1024 Hz. Preprocessing and analysis were performed using EEGLAB functions (EEGLAB 14.1.1b, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA), running under MATLAB. EEG data was resampled offline at 256 Hz and band-pass filtered during recording (0.1 to 100 Hz) and off-line (0.3 to 50 Hz) to remove undesired frequency components (Yoris et al., 2018). The reference was set by default to link mastoids. EEG data were segmented between −200 to 700 ms of the ECG R-wave peak. Following a widely reported EEG approach (Couto et al., 2014; Couto et al., 2015a; García-Cordero et al., 2016; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2017; Yoris et al., 2017; Salamone et al., 2018; Yoris et al., 2018; Fittipaldi et al., 2020), segments with cardiac-field artifacts and eye movement contamination (including slow and fast blinks, and vertical and horizontal eye movements) were rejected via independent component analysis (ICA). In this analysis, spatial filters are derived by producing the set of maximally temporally independent signals in the EEG data (Delorme et al., 2007). The components of the data that resemble electromyogenic artifacts (broad bandwidth, peripheral distribution) were selected through visual inspection by a well-trained expert and projected out of the channel, leaving clean data only. To avoid possible biases related to the rejection criteria, the same expert was in charge of the whole visual inspection procedure, a standard for artifact rejection in EEG research (Muthukumaraswamy, 2013). The epochs were baseline-corrected (baseline: −200 ms to 0 ms) (Szczepanski et al., 2014). Finally, noisy epochs were also rejected following a visual inspection procedure (Yoris et al., 2018). No significant differences were found in the number of remaining epochs between groups in each period (Information S3).

HEP analysis

The HEP was obtained by sampling EEG epochs time-locked to the R-wave. HEP modulations between TSST baseline and TSST stress were analyzed through a point-by-point Monte Carlo permutation test (5000 permutations, P < 0.05) (Manly, 2006), as done previously with this ERP (Couto et al., 2015b; García-Cordero et al., 2016; Yoris et al., 2018; Salamone et al., 2020) and other components (Ibanez et al., 2012; Ibáñez et al., 2013; Amoruso et al., 2014; Melloni et al., 2016). This method offers a solution to the multiple comparisons problem and does not depend on multiple comparison corrections or Gaussian distribution assumptions (Nichols & Holmes, 2002). We analyzed the HEP signal only after the 200-ms mark to avoid the influence of cardiac field artifacts (Kern et al., 2013). HEP analyses were based on an extended frontal region of interest (ROI) (40 frontal electrodes: Biosemi C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 C6 C7 C8 C9 C10 C11 C12 C13 C14 C15 C16 C17 C18 C19 C20 C21 C22 C23 C24 C25 C26 C27 C28 C29 C30 C31 C32 D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7 D8) (Figure 1b) (Pollatos & Schandry, 2004; Gray et al., 2007; Yoris et al., 2018). To examine the topographic distribution within the extended frontal ROI, we also assessed three separate sub-ROIs within the above set of electrodes, namely: a right frontal (Biosemi C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 C6 C7 C8 C9 C10 C11 C12 C13), a left frontal (Biosemi C24 C25 C26 C30 C31 C32 D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7), and a central frontal (Biosemi C14 C15 C16 C17 C18 C19 C20 C21 C22 C23 C27 C28 C29) ROI (Figure S2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

(a) Differences in HRV during the TSST: Baseline and stress periods (superior row), subtraction between stress and baseline (inferior row). Significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (P < 0.05). Individual modulations are represented inside and outside the box as points. The middle box line indicates the group’s mean values. (b) HEP modulation in frontal ROI during the baseline and stress periods during the TSST: Differences between groups were calculated via Monte Carlo permutations analyses (5000 permutations, P < 0.05), point by point (Manly, 2006). Following previous reports, a minimum extension of five consecutive points was establish to ensure temporally robust results (García-Cordero et al., 2016; Salamone et al., 2018). Shadowed bars around ERP modulations indicate SEM. Grey boxes indicate significant differences. (c) Association between HEP modulation and HRV during the baseline and stress periods of the TSST: Significant correlations are indicated with an asterisk (P < 0.05). (d) Regressions between GM volume and HRV during the baseline and stress periods of the TSST: these analyses were conducted to identify regions in each group that were associated with the HRV during the TSST (P < 0.001 uncorrected, extent threshold = 30 voxels). Baseline (left), stress (right). HEP: Heart evoked potential; HRV: Heart rate variability; HTD: Hypertensive disease; TSST: Trier Stress Social Test.

Associations between HEP and HRV

Pearson’s correlation analyses (P < 0.05) were used to examine a possible association between HEP modulation and HRV during the TSST, in both groups. This was done by calculating the mean average of the significant window of the HEP modulation during each TSST period (baseline and stress).

MRI Measures

MRI acquisition and preprocessing

MRI acquisition and preprocessing steps are reported as recommended by the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Nichols et al., 2017; Poldrack et al., 2017). Eight subjects from the original sample were excluded due to absence of imaging data, artifacts or acquisition problems. The final MRI sample consisted of 23 controls and 21 HTD patients demographically matched (see Table S2). Using a 1.5-T Phillips Intera scanner with a standard head coil (8 channels), we acquired whole-brain T1-weighted anatomical 3D spin echo volumes, parallel to the plane connecting the anterior and posterior commissures, with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 7489 ms; echo time (TE) = 3420 ms; flip angle = 8º; 196 slices, matrix dimension = 256 x 240; voxel size = 1 x 1 x 1 mm3; sequence duration = 7 min.

The resulting images were preprocessed on the DARTEL Toolbox, following validated procedures (Ashburner & Friston, 2000; Yoris et al., 2018), and analyzed on Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University of London, London, UK, available in http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) running on MATLAB. T1-weighted images in native space were first segmented using the default parameters of SPM12 (bias regularization was set to 0.001, and bias FWHM was set to 60-mm cut-off) into WM, GM, and cerebrospinal fluid (used to estimate the total intracranial volume). Then we ran the “DARTEL (create template)” module using the GM and WM segmented images—with the default parameters— to create a template from the complete data set (to increase the accuracy of inter-subject alignment) (Ashburner, 2007). Then, we used the “Normalise to MNI Space” module from DARTEL Tools to affine register the last template from the previous step into the MNI Space. This transformation was then applied to all the individual GM segmented scans to also be brought into standard space. Subsequently, all images were modulated to correct volume changes by Jacobian determinants, and avoid a bias in the intensity of an area due to its expansion during warping. Finally, data were smoothed using an 8 mm full-width-at-half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel to accommodate for inter-subject differences in anatomy. The size of the kernel was selected based on previous recommendations (Good et al., 2001).

Association between HRV during TSST and neuroimaging measures

Regression analyses were performed to account for the relation between HRV in TSST periods (baseline and stress) and GM, using the outputs from VBM preprocessing. Given that HRV has been related to a widely distributed set of fronto-temporo-insular regions (Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2012), we applied a specific mask comprising these areas (anterior cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, gyrus rectus, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior frontal middle gyrus, amygdala, basal ganglia [caudate nucleus, putamen, pallidum], insula, hippocampus, and parahippocampus) and performed a voxel-wise analysis inside them (Sedeno et al., 2016; de la Fuente et al., 2019; Bachli et al., 2020). This mask was developed based on the regions of the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) Atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Total intracranial volume was used as a covariate to discard the influence of brain-size differences. However, differences in overall brain volume between groups were assessed through one-way ANOVA. No significant differences were found (F1,42 = 0.00, P = 0.992, ηp2 < 0.01) between patients (M = 1386.73, SD = 128.83) and controls (M = 1386.35, SD = 122.66). Statistical significance was set at P < 0.001 uncorrected, extent threshold = 30 voxels (Steeb et al., 2018; de la Fuente et al., 2019). A power estimation analysis confirmed the robustness of our results (Information S2).

Results

HRV during the TSST

Between-group comparisons of HRV during the TSST via one-way ANOVA revealed significant differences for the baseline period (F1,46 = 10.54, P = 0.00, ηp2 = 0.18), such that patients (M = 4.30, SD = 3.97) showed higher LF/HF ratio than controls (M = 1.58, SD = 1.27). No significant differences were found in the stress period (F1,40 = 0.37 P = 0.54, ηp2 = 0.009) between patients (M = 3.34, SD = 1.87) and controls (M = 3.66, SD = 1.36) (Figure 1a).

Also, to test for potential group differences in the transition between periods (specifically, the groups’ adjustment to the stress period), we subtracted baseline HRV from stress HRV for each sample, thus controlling for baseline fluctuations and adjusting for normal intrasubject variability. A one-way ANOVA between both subtractions showed significant differences (F1,38 = 6.37 P = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.14), with lower values for patients (M = −0.60, SD = 2.99) than controls (M = 1.63, SD = 1.78) (Figure 1a).

The same results were obtained when the sample was analyzed without excluding outliers. Between-group comparisons of HRV during the TSST revealed significant differences for the baseline period (F1,50 = 5.39, P = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.09), such that patients (M = 4.82, SD = 5.20) showed higher LF/HF ratio than controls (M = 2.20, SD = 2.60). In the stress period, no significant differences were found (F1,50 = 0.04, P = 0.82, ηp2 = 0.001) between patients (M = 3.99, SD = 2.39) and controls (M = 4.18, SD = 3.66). Finally, when comparing the groups’ transition between periods, a one-way ANOVA between subtractions of HRV conditions showed significant differences (F1,50 = 4.29, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.07), with lower values for patients (M = −0.83, SD = 5.55) than controls (M = 1.97, SD = 4.18).

HEP modulation during the TSST and association with HRV

During the baseline period, significantly greater HEP modulations were observed in the patients compared to controls over the frontal ROI between 566 and 589 ms (P < 0.05) (Figure 1b). Conversely, the stress period yielded no significant differences between groups. These results were replicated (P < 0.05) over the right frontal [460 to 480 ms; 562 to 597 ms] and left frontal [570 to 593 ms] sites when each sub-ROI was separately assessed (Figure S2).

In the baseline period, a significant negative association between the HEP and HRV was observed in controls (r = −0.49, P = 0.02) and patients (r = −0.51, P = 0.05). Conversely, the stress period yielded no such correlation in either group (Controls: r = −0.16, P = 0.51; patients: r = −0.28, P = 0.46) (Figure 1c).

Association between HRV and GM volume

HRV during baseline was associated with the GM volume of the right middle orbital frontal cortex and left insula in controls, and with the left middle frontal cortex in patients. In contrast, during the stress period, controls presented a significant positive association between HRV and GM volume of right inferior and superior frontal cortices as well as the right putamen. No significant correlations were found in patients in the stress period (Figure 1d and Table 4).

Table 4.

Regression between GM in fronto-temporo-insular regions and HRV during the TSST

Brain region Peak t Peak P
(uncl)
x y z
Controls - TSST baseline
Middle orbitofrontal R 5.53 < 0.001 37.5 57 −1.5
5.46 < 0.001 42 51 −9
4.63 < 0.001 42 46.5 −18
5.27 < 0.001 −45 42 −18
Insula L 4.78 < 0.001 −42 4.5 −1.5
Controls - TSST stress
Putamen R 5.29 < 0.001 28.5 18 6
4.56 < 0.001 34.5 25.5 10.5
4.20 < 0.001 42 10.5 12
Inferior orbitofrontal R 4.35 < 0.001 49.5 33 −6
Superior orbitofrontal R 4.28 < 0.001 27 58.5 0
HTD patients - TSST baseline
Middle frontal L 5.17 < 0.001 −39 24 49.5

Regressions between GM volume and HRV during TSST: No associations between GM volume and HRV during TSST stress were found in HTD patients. HTD: Hypertensive disease; R: Right; L: Left.

Discussion

Our results evidence stress-related HRV dysregulation in HTD associated with electrophysiological and neuroimaging markers. Relative to controls, patients exhibited increased LF/HF ratio and greater HEP modulations during baseline, as well as reduced changes between baseline and stress periods, and lack of significant stress-related HRV associations with GM volume of putative frontrostriatal regions. This novel multidimensional evidence suggests that HTD patients present core markers of increased stress in non-stressful conditions and impaired neurovisceral responsiveness to acute stress. Altogether, our findings add to a promising theoretical and clinical agenda.

First, HTD patients exhibited increased LF/HF ratio during baseline, which has been associated to basal sympathetic hyperactivity and parasympathetic withdrawal (Wirtz et al., 2006; Madsen et al., 2008; Xhyheri et al., 2012; Garafova et al., 2014). This pattern is indicative of basal autonomic imbalance. More particularly, since these same modulations are core signatures of mental stress in healthy subjects (Kim et al., 2018), our results may indirectly represent markers of basal stress in HTD, possibly emerged from a constant overreaction to daily life stress (Kaushik et al., 2004). In this line, greater frontal HEP modulations found in patients during baseline, could be interpreted as an interoceptive hypervigilance driven by a stress overload. Indeed, cortisol levels have been related to HEP amplitudes (Schulz et al., 2013). Also, the absence of significant associations between HRV and GM volume of key brain regions subserving cardiac activity strengthens the view that neurovisceral regulations are disrupted in HTD compared to controls. Indeed, controls’ HRV was related with regions’ GM volume underlying adaptive behavior, emotion regulation, and autonomic regulation (Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2012), namely: the right middle orbital frontal cortex (Thayer et al., 2009) and left insula (Oppenheimer et al., 1996; Ibanez et al., 2010; Wei et al., 2018; Ibáñez, 2019). The association between HRV and HEP in controls (MacKinnon et al., 2013; Shaffer et al., 2014) was replicated during baseline. However, the patients’ reduced association (only marginally significant) might suggest suboptimal neurovisceral interactions. Briefly, these results indicate that, even in the absence of explicit stressors, HTD patients present multimodal signatures of altered stress-related mechanisms, suggesting a basal stress overload effect.

The above interpretation is consistent with results from the stress period. Although no HRV differences were found between groups during stress, the significantly smaller between-periods changes in LF/HF ratio observed in HTD reflects the absence of HRV adjustment to acute mental stress. The lack of endogenous variability in neurally mediated peripheral systems reinforces the view of impaired autonomic responsiveness (Madsen et al., 2008; Thayer et al., 2010; Shaffer et al., 2014). Indeed, this pattern could be explained by the exacerbated modulations of these markers during baseline. However, other studies did report increased sympathetic activity during stress in HTD (Kaushik et al., 2004; Nyklicek et al., 2005; Wirtz et al., 2006). While that may seem at odds with our findings, most of these studies used autonomic measures that differ from the present HRV index. Also, studies that use the LF/HF ratio tracked markedly younger samples (characterized by increased sympathetic activity) and suboptimal stress-inducing methods (Ruediger et al., 2004; Garafova et al., 2014). Moreover, during the stress period, the lack of significant GM associations with HRV modulations in the HTD group compared to controls and altered HEP modulations (Gray et al., 2007) supports the hypothesis of homeostatic disruptions and neurovisceral interaction disturbances in acute stress. Indeed, these dynamics are markedly different in healthy subjects. As corroborated in our study, normotensives show greater between-periods changes in LF/HF ratio, as well as right-sided frontrostriatal GM volume correlates during task-evoked ANS regulation (Lane et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2009; Wei et al., 2018). The absence of these markers of normal autonomic reactivity to stress (Delaney & Brodie, 2000; Castaldo et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2018) and healthy ANS balance (Thayer et al., 2010) in HTD could explain the loss of significant differences between groups in HRV modulation and HEP amplitude during stress. Altogether, these findings further point to defective multidimensional responsiveness to acute mental stress in HTD patients.

Our findings carry theoretical and clinical implications. Basal autonomic imbalance and the lack of adjustment to acute stress in HTD could be explained through the Neurovisceral Integration Across a Continuum of Time (NIACT) model (Kemp et al., 2017), a novel temporal framework which characterizes HRV as a link between psychological moments and mortality risk. Specifically, it conceptualizes how everyday moments both affect and are affected by the HRV in ways that have long-term effects on mortality risk, since it indexes the vagus nerve –which plays a critical role over allostatic systems. Consequently, basal HRV may index everyday psychophysiological resources, providing the best indication of wellbeing and future health. Conversely, task-driven activity, including psychosocial stress, would reflect autonomic responsiveness (Kemp et al., 2017). In this line, increased basal LF/HF ratio in HTD suggests peripheral cardiovascular disruptions triggering allostatic load (McEwen, 1998). Our results also extend the NIACT model onto neurocognitive dimensions, by showing the HEP and GM density in fronto-temporo-insular areas constitute key markers of neurovisceral interaction. For instance, the differential HEP modulation observed between groups at baseline may reflect the abovementioned allostatic overload in HTD. This aligns with an embodied perspective suggesting that peripheral cardiovascular impairments, previously associated to interoceptive deficits (Yoris et al., 2018), may disrupt allostatic-interoceptive dynamics, preventing successful allostasis. More specifically, the greater frontal HEP modulations found in patients during baseline could be interpreted as interoceptive hypervigilance driven by stress-related allostatic overload. Moreover, the pathophysiological implications of this state (Karatsoreos & McEwen, 2011) might explain the development and maintenance of hypertension (Liu et al., 2017b; Mocayar Marón et al., 2019) and its detrimental impact on patients’ health (Ruediger et al., 2004; Thayer et al., 2010). Further developments along these lines can lead to more comprehensive modelling of stress in healthy and pathological populations.

From a clinical perspective, our results highlight basal HRV monitoring as an objectively cardio-cognitive marker in risk populations. Similarly, multilevel neurovisceral indicators could afford objective assessments of the patients' ability to adaptively react to acute psychosocial stressors, complementing standard self-report evaluations (Kim et al., 2018). In line with previous works (García-Cordero et al., 2016; Salamone et al., 2018; Yoris et al., 2018; Fittipaldi et al., 2020), these multimodal insights underscore the relevance of neurocognitive data for the development of applied innovations in the diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment of conditions with autonomic imbalance and increased morbidity (Thayer et al., 2010; Castaldo et al., 2015; Allen et al., 2017; Kemp et al., 2017).

Limitations and avenues for further research

The present work features some limitations and opens a new agenda for further research. First, it is based on a modest sample size. However, it is adequate according to power estimation analysis (Information S1 and S2) and similar to other works with convergent results (Wirtz et al., 2006; Garafova et al., 2014). Moreover, the consistency of our results across cardiodynamic, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging dimensions, with moderate to large effect sizes, further attests to their robustness. Still, non-significant results may vary with larger samples, calling for further replication. Second, our findings may have been partially influenced by the patients’ medication. However, following previous reports (Yoris et al., 2018), we suspended intake 48 hours before testing to minimize its potential impact. Still, valuable information could be obtained in future works by comparing basal and acute stress levels in treated and untreated patients. Another possible limitation is the restricted range of stress parameters. Although HRV and HEP are valid proxies to assess acute mental stress, future studies should include other parameters, like salivary cortisol (Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994; Kudielka et al., 2007) hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis activity, sympathetic-adrenal-medullary activation or immune system activity measurements (Nyklicek et al., 2005; Wirtz et al., 2006). Moreover, the sensitivity of the LF/HF ratio to sympatho-vagal balance is debated, especially in short-term resting paced breathing (at 0.1 Hz) recordings, where LF power may be almost entirely vagally mediated (Billman, 2013; Shaffer et al., 2014). Nonetheless, in our work, we assumed that the LF component mainly reflected sympathetic activity, since the TSST involved a baseline period assessed at normal breathing and a psychosocial stress period that has been widely validated to produce sympathetic activation (Allen et al., 2014; Allen et al., 2017). We encourage future studies to take this issue into consideration. Finally, between-group HEP differences in the present study emerged in a short time span. However, this ERP may manifest in windows of variable extension (Gray et al., 2007; Schulz et al., 2015; Salamone et al., 2018; Salamone et al., 2020). Future studies could illuminate which subject- or task-related factors determine the duration of (stress-sensitive) HEP effects.

Conclusions

Our study revealed an autonomic imbalance in HTD patients, reflected in a potential basal stress overload effect and a lack of regulation to acute psychosocial stress, accompanied by neurophysiological and neuroanatomical alterations. Further work along these lines could better elucidate the multidimensional signatures of stress in HTD patients and normotensives, yielding new insights into heart-brain interactions.

Supplementary Material

supinfo

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the Metabolic and Arterial Hypertension Unit of the Favaloro Foundation Hospital for their collaboration.

Financial support

This work was supported by CONICET; FONCYT-PICT [2017-1818, 2017-1820]; CONICYT/FONDECYT Regular [grant number 1170010]; FONDAP [grant number 15150012]; GBHI ALZ UK-20-639295; and the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat), funded by the National Institutes of Aging of the National Institutes of Health under award number R01AG057234, an Alzheimer’s Association grant (SG-20-725707), the Rainwater Foundation, and the Global Brain Health Institute. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of these institutions.

Abbreviations

ANS

autonomic nervous system

GM

grey matter

HEP

heart-evoked potential

HRV

heart rate variability

HTD

hypertensive disease

LF/HF

low frequency/high frequency

NIACT

Neurovisceral Integration Across a Continuum of Time model

ROI

region of interest

SPM12

Statistical Parametric Mapping software v.12

TSST

Trier Social Stress Test

VBM

voxel-based morphometry

WM

white matter

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Data Accessibility Statement

Experimental data is available online on the Open Science Framework at: https://osf.io/ybgn5.

References

  1. Allen AP, Kennedy PJ, Cryan JF, Dinan TG & Clarke G (2014) Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 38, 94–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen AP, Kennedy PJ, Dockray S, Cryan JF, Dinan TG & Clarke G (2017) The Trier Social Stress Test: Principles and practice. Neurobiol. Stress., 6, 113–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amoruso L, Sedeño L, Huepe D, Tomio A, Kamienkowski J, Hurtado E, Cardona JF, González MÁÁ, Rieznik A & Sigman M (2014) Time to tango: expertise and contextual anticipation during action observation. NeuroImage, 98, 366–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage, 38, 95–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ashburner J & Friston KJ (2000) Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. NeuroImage, 11, 805–821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bachli MB, Sedeño L, Ochab JK, Piguet O, Kumfor F, Reyes P, Torralva T, Roca M, Cardona JF & Campo CG (2020) Evaluating the reliability of neurocognitive biomarkers of neurodegenerative diseases across countries: A machine learning approach. NeuroImage, 208, 116456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck AT, Steer RA & Brown GK (1996) Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, 78, 490–498. [Google Scholar]
  8. Billman GE (2013) The LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front. Physiol, 4, 26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buske-Kirschbaum A, Geiben A, Höllig H, Morschhäuser E & Hellhammer D (2002) Altered responsiveness of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic adrenomedullary system to stress in patients with atopic dermatitis. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab, 87, 4245–4251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Castaldo R, Melillo P, Bracale U, Caserta M, Traissi M & Pecchia L (2015) Acute mental stress assessment via short term HRV analysis in healthy adults: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Biomed. Signal Process. Control, 18, 370–377. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D & Miller GE (2007) Psychological stress and disease. Jama, 298, 1685–1687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Couto B, Adolfi F, Velasquez M, Mesow M, Feinstein J, Canales-Johnson A, Mikulan E, Martinez-Pernia D, Bekinschtein T, Sigman M, Manes F & Ibanez A (2015a) Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: A preliminary study. Autonomic neuroscience : basic & clinical, 193, 132–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Couto B, Adolfi F, Velasquez M, Mesow M, Feinstein J, Canales-Johnson A, Mikulan E, Martinez-Pernia D, Bekinschtein T, Sigman M, Manes F & Ibanez A (2015b) Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: A preliminary study. Auton. Neurosci, 193, 132–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Couto B, Salles A, Sedeno L, Peradejordi M, Barttfeld P, Canales-Johnson A, Dos Santos YV, Huepe D, Bekinschtein T, Sigman M, Favaloro R, Manes F & Ibanez A (2014) The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 9, 1253–1260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Craig AD (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev Neurosci, 3, 655–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Critchley HD & Harrison NA (2013) Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron, 77, 624–638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chandola T, Heraclides A & Kumari M (2010) Psychophysiological biomarkers of workplace stressors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 35, 51–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. de la Fuente A, Sedeño L, Schurmann S, Ellmann C, Sonzogni S, Belluscio L, García-Cordero I, Castagnaro E, Boano M & Cetkovich M (2019) Multimodal neurocognitive markers of interoceptive tuning in smoked cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 1425–1434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Delaney J & Brodie D (2000) Effects of short-term psychological stress on the time and frequency domains of heart-rate variability. Percept. Mot. Ski, 91, 515–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Delorme A, Sejnowski T & Makeig S (2007) Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. NeuroImage, 34, 1443–1449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Devor A, Bandettini PA, Boas DA, Bower JM, Buxton RB, Cohen LB, Dale AM, Einevoll GT, Fox PT & Franceschini MA (2013) The challenge of connecting the dots in the BRAIN. Neuron, 80, 270–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dickerson SS & Kemeny ME (2004) Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull, 130, 355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eckberg DL (1997) Sympathovagal balance: a critical appraisal. Circulation, 96, 3224–3232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fittipaldi S, Abrevaya S, de la Fuente A, Pascariello GO, Hesse E, Birba A, Salamone P, Hildebrandt M, Martí SA & Pautassi R (2020) A multidimensional and multi-feature framework for cardiac interoception. NeuroImage, 116677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Garafova A, Penesova A, Cizmarova E, Marko A, Vlcek M & Jezova D (2014) Cardiovascular and sympathetic responses to a mental stress task in young patients with hypertension and/or obesity. Physiol. Res, 63, S459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Garcia-Cordero I, Esteves S, Mikulan EP, Hesse E, Baglivo FH, Silva W, Garcia MDC, Vaucheret E, Ciraolo C, Garcia HS, Adolfi F, Pietto M, Herrera E, Legaz A, Manes F, Garcia AM, Sigman M, Bekinschtein TA, Ibanez A & Sedeno L (2017) Attention, in and Out: Scalp-Level and Intracranial EEG Correlates of Interoception and Exteroception. Frontiers in neuroscience, 11, 411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. García-Cordero I, Sedeño L, De la Fuente L, Slachevsky A, Forno G, Klein F, Lillo P, Ferrari J, Rodriguez C, Bustin J, Torralva T, Baez S, Yoris A, Esteves S, Melloni M, Salamone P, Huepe D, Manes F, A., G.M. & Ibañez, A. (2016) Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, 371, 20160006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gianaros PJ & Sheu LK (2009) A review of neuroimaging studies of stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity: emerging evidence for a brain-body pathway to coronary heart disease risk. NeuroImage, 47, 922–936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gleichgerrcht E, Roca M, Manes F & Torralva T (2011) Comparing the clinical usefulness of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Frontal Screening (IFS) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) in frontotemporal dementia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 33, 997–1004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldstein DS, Bentho O, Park MY & Sharabi Y (2011) Low-frequency power of heart rate variability is not a measure of cardiac sympathetic tone but may be a measure of modulation of cardiac autonomic outflows by baroreflexes. Exp. physiol, 96, 1255–1261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ & Frackowiak RS (2001) A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage, 14, 21–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gray MA, Taggart P, Sutton PM, Groves D, Holdright DR, Bradbury D, Brull D & Critchley HD (2007) A cortical potential reflecting cardiac function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A , 104, 6818–6823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Heim C, Ehlert U & Hellhammer DH (2000) The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 1–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ibanez A, Gleichgerrcht E & Manes F (2010) Clinical effects of insular damage in humans. Brain structure & function, 214, 397–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ibanez A, Melloni M, Huepe D, Helgiu E, Rivera-Rei A, Canales-Johnson A, Baker P & Moya A (2012) What event-related potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience? Soc Neurosci, 7, 632–649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ibáñez A (2018) Brain oscillations, inhibition and social inappropriateness in frontotemporal degeneration. Brain : a journal of neurology, 141, e73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ibáñez A (2019) Insular networks and intercognition in the wild. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 115, 341–344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ibáñez A, Cardona JF, Dos Santos YV, Blenkmann A, Aravena P, Roca M, Hurtado E, Nerguizian M, Amoruso L & Gómez-Arévalo G (2013) Motor-language coupling: direct evidence from early Parkinson’s disease and intracranial cortical recordings. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 49, 968–984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jennings JR & Zanstra Y (2009) Is the brain the essential in hypertension? NeuroImage, 47, 914–921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Karatsoreos IN & McEwen BS (2011) Psychobiological allostasis: resistance, resilience and vulnerability. Trends Cogn. Sci, 15, 576–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kaushik RM, Mahajan SK, Rajesh V & Kaushik R (2004) Stress profile in essential hypertension. Hypertens. Res, 27, 619–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kemp AH, Koenig J & Thayer JF (2017) From psychological moments to mortality: a multidisciplinary synthesis on heart rate variability spanning the continuum of time. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 83, 547–567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kern M, Aertsen A, Schulze-Bonhage A & Ball T (2013) Heart cycle-related effects on event-related potentials, spectral power changes, and connectivity patterns in the human ECoG. NeuroImage, 81, 178–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH & Koo BH (2018) Stress and heart rate variability: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investig., 15, 235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kirschbaum C & Hellhammer DH (1994) Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 313–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM & Hellhammer DH (1993) The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, Simmons WK, Quigley KS, Dickerson BC & Barrett LF (2017) Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nat. Hum. Behav, 1, 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH & Kirschbaum C (2007) Ten Years of Research with the Trier Social Stress Test-Revisited. Soc. Neurosci, 56, 83. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lane R, McRae K, Reiman E, Chen K, Ahern G & Thayer J (2009) Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. NeuroImage, 44, 213–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Langewitz W, Rüddel H & Schächinger H (1994) Reduced parasympathetic cardiac control in patients with hypertension at rest and under mental stress. Am. Heart J. , 127, 122–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Liu MY, Li N, Li WA & Khan H (2017a) Association between psychosocial stress and hypertension: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurol. Res, 39, 573–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Liu YZ, Wang YX & Jiang CL (2017b) Inflammation: the common pathway of stress-related diseases. Front. Hum. Neurosci, 11, 316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. MacKinnon S, Gevirtz R, McCraty R & Brown M (2013) Utilizing heartbeat evoked potentials to identify cardiac regulation of vagal afferents during emotion and resonant breathing. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback, 38, 241–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Madsen LB, Rasmussen JK, Møller DS, Nyvad O & Pedersen EB (2008) Heart rate variability in white-coat hypertension. Blood Press. Monit, 13, 65–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Malliani APM; Lombardi F; Cerutti S (1991) Cardiovascular neural regulation explored in the frequency domain. Circulation, 84, 482–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mancia G, De Backer G, Dominiczak A, Cifkova R, Fagard R, Germano G, Grassi G, Heagerty AM, Kjeldsen SE & Laurent S (2007) 2007 Guidelines for the management of arterial hypertension: The Task Force for the Management of Arterial Hypertension of the European Society of Hypertension (ESH) and of the European Society of Cardiology (ESC). Eur. Heart J, 28, 1462–1536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Manly BFJ (2006) Randomization, bootstrap and Monte Carlo methods in biology. Chapman and Hall/CRC. [Google Scholar]
  58. McEwen BS (1998) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N. Engl. J. Med, 338, 171–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. McEwen BS (2007) Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev, 87, 873–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Melloni M, Billeke P, Baez S, Hesse E, De la Fuente L, Forno G, Birba A, García-Cordero I, Serrano C & Plastino A (2016) Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain, 139, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Melloni M, Sedeno L, Hesse E, Garcia-Cordero I, Mikulan E, Plastino A, Marcotti A, Lopez JD, Bustamante C, Lopera F, Pineda D, Garcia AM, Manes F, Trujillo N & Ibanez A (2015) Cortical dynamics and subcortical signatures of motor-language coupling in Parkinson's disease. Sci Rep, 5, 11899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mocayar Marón FJ, Ferder L, Saraví FD & Manucha W (2019) Hypertension linked to allostatic load: from psychosocial stress to inflammation and mitochondrial dysfunction. Stress, 22, 169–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Muthukumaraswamy S (2013) High-frequency brain activity and muscle artifacts in MEG/EEG: a review and recommendations. Front. Hum. Neurosci, 7, 138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nichols TE, Das S, Eickhoff SB, Evans AC, Glatard T, Hanke M, Kriegeskorte N, Milham MP, Poldrack RA & Poline J-B (2017) Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI. Nat. Neurosci, 20, 299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nichols TE & Holmes AP (2002) Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp, 15, 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nyklicek I, Bosch JA & Amerongen AV (2005) A generalized physiological hyperreactivity to acute stressors in hypertensives. Biol. Psych, 70, 44–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Oppenheimer S, Kedem G & Martin W (1996) Left-insular cortex lesions perturb cardiac autonomic tone in humans. Clinical Autonomic Research, 6, 131–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Osborne JW & Overbay A (2004) The power of outliers (and why researchers should always check for them). Pract. Assess. Res. Eval, 9, 6. [Google Scholar]
  69. Pagani M, Lombardi F, Guzzetti S, Rimoldi O, Furlan R, Pizzinelli P, Sandrone G, Malfatto G, Dell'Orto S & Piccaluga E (1986) Power spectral analysis of heart rate and arterial pressure variabilities as a marker of sympatho-vagal interaction in man and conscious dog. Circ. Res, 59, 178–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pagani M, Lombardi F, Guzzetti S, Sandrone G, Rimoldi O, Malfatto G, Cerutti S & Malliani A (1984) Power spectral density of heart rate variability as an index of sympatho-vagal interaction in normal and hypertensive subjects. J. Hypertens. Suppl. , 2, S383. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, Gorgolewski KJ, Matthews PM, Munafò MR, Nichols TE, Poline J-B, Vul E & Yarkoni T (2017) Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci, 18, 115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pollatos O, Kirsch W & Schandry R (2005) Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp, 26, 54–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Pollatos O & Schandry R (2004) Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology, 41, 476–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rimmele U, Seiler R, Marti B, Wirtz PH, Ehlert U & Heinrichs M (2009) The level of physical activity affects adrenal and cardiovascular reactivity to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 190–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rimmele U, Zellweger BC, Marti B, Seiler R, Mohiyeddini C, Ehlert U & Heinrichs M (2007) Trained men show lower cortisol, heart rate and psychological responses to psychosocial stress compared with untrained men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 627–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rohrmann S, Hennig J & Netter P (1999) Changing psychobiological stress reactions by manipulating cognitive processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol, 33, 149–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ruediger H, Seibt R, Scheuch K, Krause M & Alam S (2004) Sympathetic and parasympathetic activation in heart rate variability in male hypertensive patients under mental stress. J. Hum. Hypertens, 18, 307–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Salamone PC, Esteves S, Sinay VJ, Garcia-Cordero I, Abrevaya S, Couto B, Adolfi F, Martorell M, Petroni A, Yoris A, Torquati K, Alifano F, Legaz A, Cassara FP, Bruno D, Kemp AH, Herrera E, Garcia AM, Ibanez A & Sedeno L (2018) Altered neural signatures of interoception in multiple sclerosis. Hum. Brain Mapp, 39, 4743–4754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Salamone PC, Sedeño L, Legaz A, Bekinschtein T, Martorell M, Adolfi F, Fraile-Vazquez M, Arriagada NR, Favaloro L & Peradejordi M (2020) Dynamic neurocognitive changes in interoception after heart transplant. Brain Commun. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sánchez RA, Ramos F, Giannone C, Fischer P, Masnatta L, Baglivo HP, Ramírez AJ & Hollenberg NK (2003) Parallel renal and extremity blood supply abnormalities in nonmodulation: responses to ACE inhibition. Hypertension, 41, 919–924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schandry R & Montoya P (1996) Event-related brain potentials and the processing of cardiac activity. Biol. Psychol, 42, 75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Schulz A, Köster S, Beutel ME, Schächinger H, Vögele C, Rost S, Rauh M & Michal M (2015) Altered patterns of heartbeat-evoked potentials in depersonalization/derealization disorder: neurophysiological evidence for impaired cortical representation of bodily signals. Psychosom. Med, 77, 506–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schulz A, Strelzyk F, Ferreira de Sa DS, Naumann E, Vogele C & Schachinger H (2013) Cortisol rapidly affects amplitudes of heartbeat-evoked brain potentials--implications for the contribution of stress to an altered perception of physical sensations? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 2686–2693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sedeno L, Couto B, Garcia-Cordero I, Melloni M, Baez S, Morales Sepulveda JP, Fraiman D, Huepe D, Hurtado E, Matallana D, Kuljis R, Torralva T, Chialvo D, Sigman M, Piguet O, Manes F & Ibanez A (2016) Brain Network Organization and Social Executive Performance in Frontotemporal Dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc, 22, 250–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Shaffer F, McCraty R & Zerr CL (2014) A healthy heart is not a metronome: an integrative review of the heart's anatomy and heart rate variability. Front. Psychol, 5, 1040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Speilberger CD & Vagg PR (1984) Psychometric properties of the STAI: a reply to Ramanaiah, Franzen, and Schill. Journal of personality assessment, 48, 95–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Steeb B, García-Cordero I, Huizing MC, Collazo L, Borovinsky G, Ferrari J, Cuitiño MM, Ibáñez A, Sedeño L & García AM (2018) Progressive compromise of nouns and action verbs in posterior cortical atrophy. Front. Psychol, 9, 1345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Szczepanski SM, Crone NE, Kuperman RA, Auguste KI, Parvizi J & Knight RT (2014) Dynamic changes in phase-amplitude coupling facilitate spatial attention control in fronto-parietal cortex. PLoS Biol., 12, e1001936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Taelman J, Vandeput S, Spaepen A & Van Huffel S (2009) Influence of Mental Stress on Heart Rate and Heart Rate Variability. IFMBE Proc., 22, 1366–1369. [Google Scholar]
  90. Tarvainen MP, Niskanen J-P, Lipponen JA, Ranta-Aho PO & Karjalainen PA (2014) Kubios HRV–heart rate variability analysis software. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed, 113, 210–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (1996) Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation, 93, 1043–1065. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ 3rd & Wager TD (2012) A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 36, 747–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Saus-Rose E & Johnsen BH (2009) Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann. Behav. Med, 37, 141–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Thayer JF & Sternberg E (2006) Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci, 1088, 361–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS & Brosschot JF (2010) The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Int. J. Cardiol, 141, 122–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, López P & Manes F (2009) INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): A brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia–CORRECTED VERSION. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc , 15, 777–786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B & Joliot M (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15, 273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Unger T, Borghi C, Charchar F, Khan NA, Poulter NR, Prabhakaran D, Ramirez A, Schlaich M, Stergiou GS & Tomaszewski M (2020) 2020 International Society of Hypertension global hypertension practice guidelines. Hypertension, 75, 1334–1357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Vlcek M, Radikova Z, Penesova A, Kvetnansky R & Imrich R (2008) Heart rate variability and catecholamines during hypoglycemia and orthostasis. Auton. Neurosci, 143, 53–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wei L, Chen H & Wu G-R (2018) Heart rate variability associated with grey matter volumes in striatal and limbic structures of the central autonomic network. Brain Res., 1681, 14–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. WHO (1992) The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  102. Wiener A, Rohr CS, Naor N, Villringer A & Okon-Singer H (2020) Emotion Regulation in Essential Hypertension: Roles of Anxiety, Stress, and the Pulvinar. Front. Behav. Neurosci. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Wirtz PH, von Kanel R, Mohiyeddini C, Emini L, Ruedisueli K, Groessbauer S & Ehlert U (2006) Low social support and poor emotional regulation are associated with increased stress hormone reactivity to mental stress in systemic hypertension. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab, 91, 3857–3865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Xhyheri B, Manfrini O, Mazzolini M, Pizzi C & Bugiardini R (2012) Heart rate variability today. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis, 55, 321–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Yamakawa K, Matsunaga M, Isowa T, Kimura K, Kasugai K, Yoneda M, Kaneko H & Ohira H (2009) Transient responses of inflammatory cytokines in acute stress. Biological psychology, 82, 25–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Yoris A, Abrevaya S, Esteves S, Salamone P, Lori N, Martorell M, Legaz A, Alifano F, Petroni A, Sanchez R, Sedeno L, Garcia AM & Ibanez A (2018) Multilevel convergence of interoceptive impairments in hypertension: New evidence of disrupted body-brain interactions. Hum. Brain Mapp, 39, 1563–1581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Yoris A, Garcia AM, Traiber L, Santamaria-Garcia H, Martorell M, Alifano F, Kichic R, Moser JS, Cetkovich M, Manes F, Ibanez A & Sedeno L (2017) The inner world of overactive monitoring: neural markers of interoception in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological medicine, 47, 1957–1970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Zimmerman DW (1994) A note on the influence of outliers on parametric and nonparametric tests. J. Gen. Psychol, 121, 391–401. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supinfo

RESOURCES