Abstract
How individuals interact with their environment and respond to changes is a key area of research in evolutionary biology. A physiological parameter that provides an instant proxy for the activation of the automatic nervous system, and can be measured relatively easily, is modulation of heart rate. Over the past four decades, heart rate has been used to assess emotional arousal in non-human animals in a variety of contexts, including social behaviour, animal cognition, animal welfare and animal personality. In this review, I summarize how measuring heart rate has provided new insights into how social animals cope with challenges in their environment. I assess the advantages and limitations of different technologies used to measure heart rate in this context, including wearable heart rate belts and implantable transmitters, and provide an overview of prospective research avenues using established and new technologies, with a special focus on implications for applied research on animal welfare.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Measuring physiology in free-living animals (Part II)’.
Keywords: animal welfare, cognition, emotional arousal, heart rate, individual differences, social behaviour
1. General introduction
Since the mid 1970s, starting with a seminal book by Hans Seyle [1], investigating individual responses to challenges in the environment, i.e. stressors, has been an important area of research in evolutionary biology. Physiological activation helps organisms to adapt to environmental changes and maintain homeostasis [2]. Activation of the sympathetic branch of the automatic nervous system can be captured by an increase in heart rate, defined as the number of contractions of the heart per minute (beats per minute, bpm) [3], which indicates the initiation of a physiological stress response, defined as a coping response to external and internal stimuli [1] and emotional arousal, a state of heightened psychological activity [4]. These concepts are closely related, where the physiological stress response is often seen as a general response to all stimuli, whereas emotional arousal takes the animal's perception of the situation into consideration [5]. Measures of heart rate variability, i.e. variation in the time interval between heartbeats, have been suggested to reflect the balance between the regulation of the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the automatic nervous system [6,7]. As these responses are plastic and context-dependent [8], heart rate measures can provide important insight into how animals perceive and respond to their environment [9]. Further, heart rate is positively correlated to metabolic rate and thus can be used as a reliable proxy of energy expenditure [10]. Ultimately, stress-related increases in heart rate can increase the overall energy expenditure of individuals [11], which is constrained by physical and/or physiological conditions [10]. Heart rate response to stress can be considerable: for example, in yellow-eyed penguins, Megadyptes antipodes, heart rate was more than twice as high during partner reunion with mutual calling (174 bpm) compared to heart rate during resting (77 bpm) and heart rate returned to baseline levels within seconds after the stimulus ended [12]. In greater white-fronted geese, Anser albifrons, maximum heart rate during agonistic encounters increased four times in comparison to mean heart rate during resting (110 bpm), whereas mean heart rates during walking, feeding and preening was similar to resting heart rate (98–118 bpm). Mean heart rate during flight was similar to maximum heart rates during agonistic encounters (413 bpm) and heart rate returned to baseline values in less than 10 s [13]. In mammals, heart rate during exercise (running) increased linearly with running speed and depended on body size (resting heart rates: 63–581 bpm; maximum heart rate during running: 183–679 bpm) [14]. In domestic pigs, Sus scrofa, mean heart rate increased from 52 bpm during resting to 79 bpm during walking and a maximum heart rate of 157 bpm when losing a physical encounter [15].
Here, I review studies that used heart rate as a measure of emotional arousal in the fields of social behaviour, animal cognition, animal welfare and personality research (table 1). A first study, published in the late 1970s, used heart rate as a measure of anthropogenic disturbance, measured by custom-designed wearable transmitter harnesses in bighorn sheep, Ovis canadensis [30]. After this early study in free-ranging animals, only 10 years later, the first laboratory studies in primates measured heart rate responses to social interactions and different social stimuli via electrodes attached to the skin surface or implanted transmitters [23,36,37]. It took another 10 years before heart rate became a more broadly used measure of emotional arousal in a different context in the late 1990s, with researchers also starting to use different technologies (implanted radio-transmitter, wearable heart rate belts, artificial eggs; table 1). I further provide an outlook into future research avenues that could potentially be explored with new technologies allowing for heart rate measurements to be more broadly applied in different contexts, freely moving animals and a wider variety of species.
Table 1.
Summary of studies investigating individual heart rate in different species and contexts, and the type of technology used to measure heart rate.
context | species | technology | reference |
---|---|---|---|
social behaviour | blind cave crayfish, Orconectes australis packardi | implanted wires connected to external impedance detectors | Li et al. [16] |
cattle, Bos taurus | wearable heart rate belt | Laister et al. [17] | |
goats, Capra hircus | wearable heart rate belt | Briefer et al. [18] | |
greater white-fronted geese, Anser albifrons | implanted radio-transmitter | Ely et al. [13] | |
greylag geese, Anser anser | implanted radio-transmitter | Wascher et al. [19,20] | |
king penguins, Aptenodytes patagonicus | implanted electrodes connected to external recorder | Viblanc et al. [21,22] | |
pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina | implanted radio-transmitter | Boccia et al. [23] | |
rats, Rattus norvegicus | implanted radio-transmitter | Sgoifo et al. [24] | |
red deer, Cervus elaphus | rumen-located transmitters | Turbill et al. [25] | |
rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta | implanted radio-transmitter | Aureli et al. [26] | |
anthropogenic disturbance | Adélie penguins, Pygoscelis adeliae | artificial eggs | Nimon et al. [27] |
American black bears, Ursus americanus | implanted radio-transmitter | Ditmer et al. [28] | |
American oystercatchers, Haematopus palliates | artificial eggs | Borneman et al. [29] | |
bighorn sheep, Ovis canadensis | wearable heart rate belt | MacArthur et al. [30] | |
brown bears, Ursus arctos | implanted radio-transmitter | Le Grand et al. [31] | |
koalas, Phascolarctos cinereus | wearable heart rate belt | Ropert-Coudert et al. [32] | |
largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides | implanted radio-transmitter | Graham & Cooke [33] | |
wandering albatrosses, Diomedea exulans | implanted electrodes connected to external recorder | Weimerskirch et al. [34] | |
yellow-eyed penguin, Megadyptes antipodes | artificial eggs | Ellenberg et al. [12] | |
cognitive task | cattle, Bos taurus | wearable heart rate belt | Hagen et al. [35] |
chimpanzee, Pan troglodytes | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometer | Boysen & Berntson [36,37] | |
chickens, Gallus gallus domesticus | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometers | Edgar et al. [38] | |
dogs, Canis familiaris | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometers | Maros et al. [39] | |
dogs, Canis familiaris | wearable heart rate belt | Siniscalchi et al. [40] | |
greylag geese, Anser anser | implanted radio-transmitter | Wascher et al. [19,20] | |
European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris | implanted radio-transmitter | Glassman et al. [41] | |
horses, Equus caballus | wearable heart rate belt | Mengoli et al. [42]; Trösch et al. [43] | |
little blue penguin chicks, Eudyptula minor | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometers | Nakagawa et al. [44] | |
Nigerian dwarf goats, Capra hircus | wearable heart rate belt | Langbein et al. [45] | |
rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss | aquarium with the electrode cage | Höjesjö et al. [46] | |
rats, Rattus norvegicus | implanted radio-transmitter | Van den Buuse et al. [47] | |
sheep, Ovis aries | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometer | Désiré et al. [48] | |
individual differences | chicken, Gallus gallus domesticus | implanted radio-transmitter | Korte et al. [49] |
greylag geese, Anser anser | implanted radio-transmitter | Kralj-Fišer et al. [50] | |
rats, Rattus norvegicus | implanted radio-transmitter | Sgoifo et al. [24] | |
environmental enrichment | African green monkeys, Chlorocebus aethiops | implanted radio-transmitter | Hinds et al. [51] |
dogs, Canis familiaris | wearable heart rate belt | Amaya et al. [52]; Bowman et al. [53] | |
housing condition | cattle, Bos taurus | electrodes attached to skin surface, connected to cardiotachometer | Boissy & Le Neindre [54] |
mice, Mus musculus | implanted radio-transmitter | Meijer et al. [55] | |
rhesus monkeys, Mucaca mulatta | implanted radio-transmitter | Line et al. [56] | |
precision livestock farming | Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar | implanted radio-transmitter | Brijs et al. [57] |
cattle, Bos taurus | wearable heart rate belt | Jorquera-Chavez et al. [58] | |
chickens, Gallus gallus domesticus | digital egg monitor | Lierz et al. [59] | |
domestic pigs, Sus scrofa domesticus | wearable heart rate belt | Lewis et al. [60] |
2. Social modulation of heart rate
Heart rate has been shown to be strongly affected by social interactions in a wide range of species. Individuals' responses during social interactions can be quantified using mean or maximum heart rate during the encounter [61], heart rate increase (calculated as the difference between the momentary baseline heart rate value a couple of seconds (2–3) before the interaction and either the mean heart rate value during the interaction or the instantaneous maximum heart rate [21,61]), recovery period until the heart rate returns to baseline [62], or excess heart rate (calculated as the mean heart rate increase over baseline heart rate times the duration of that increase [21]). Heart rate during and after agonistic encounters usually increases (blind cave crayfish, Orconectes australis packardi: [16]; bighorn sheep: [30]; domestic pigs: [15]; greylag geese, Anser anser: [19] (see figure 1a in comparison to heart rate during take-off figure 1b); greater white-fronted geese: [13]; king penguins, Aptenodytes patagonicus: [21]; pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina: [23]; table 2). In greylag geese, the mean heart rate during aggressive encounters (157 bpm) is significantly higher compared to mean heart rate during other behaviours, for example resting (84 bpm), head up (131 bpm), preening (117 bpm) or feeding (118 bpm). Heart rate in greylag geese increased from 134 bpm during standing and walking, to 159 bpm during swimming and 368 bpm when flying [63]. Heart rate increases after agonistic encounters in greylag geese on average lasted for 8 s (range: 1–261 s; C.A.F. Wascher 2006, unpublished data). Affiliative behaviours generally reduce heart rate (cattle, Bos taurus: [17]; pigtail macaque [23]; rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta: [26]) and being paired with a long-term social partner was shown to buffer an increase in heart rate during aggressive encounters in male greylag geese [64]. Responses often differ depending on an individual's role as initiator or receiver in the interaction, or whether the individual is dominant or subordinate to its interaction partner. For example, in rats, Rattus norvegicus, and greylag geese, individuals winning agonistic encounters had significantly higher levels of emotional arousal compared to the defeated individuals [24,61]. Already the approach by a dominant group member and the associated risk of aggression were found to significantly increase heart rate in rhesus macaques [26], and even indirect social interactions can affect heart rate, for example observing aggressive interactions as a bystander [20,21], or visual and olfactory presentation of dominant or aggressive individuals [36,46]. The relationship between social status and heart rate can manifest itself in rank-dependent differences in resting heart rates, as described in red deer, Cervus elaphus, where subordinate individuals have lower resting heart rates compared to dominant individuals, which might result in a better capacity to minimize energy requirements [25].
Figure 1.
Examples of heart rate modulation in greylag geese. (a) Aggressive interaction, focal individual walking towards another individual in a threat posture, other individual walks away. (b) Departure event of the focal individual. Different behavioural states are shown in different colours. (c) Bystander event, approximately 100 m from the focal individuals a group of geese fly-by. (d) Goose flock is approached by a dog. (Online version in colour.)
Table 2.
Example heart rates during resting and agonistic interactions in different species.
species | resting heart rate | heart rate during agonistic interactions | references |
---|---|---|---|
bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis) | 52 | 66 | MacArthur et al. [30] |
blind cave crayfish (Orconectes australis packardi) | 78 | 163 | Li et al. [16] |
domestic pig | 52 | 129 | Marchant et al. [15] |
greater white-fronted geese (Anser albifrons) | 100 | 400 | Ely et al. [13] |
greylag geese (Anser anser) | 84 | 157 | Wascher et al. [19,20] |
king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) | 67 | 82 | Viblanc et al. [21,22] |
pigtail macaque (Macaca nemestrina) | 133 | 176 | Boccia et al. [23] |
Current research shows that heart rate provides a suitable proxy for estimating individual investment into the social domain, and that measuring heart rate in a variety of social contexts facilitates investigating the adaptive benefits of social relationships across different time frames. However, presently, only a few studies report detailed individual physiological responses to social interactions and often these studies describe short-term effects only instead of investigating any long-term impacts of emotional arousal on energy expenditure, health, reproductive output or longevity.
3. Measuring heart rate in the context of animal cognition research
Generally, the study of animal cognition investigates mechanisms by which animals acquire, process, store and act upon information in the environment [65]. A major constraint in understanding these processes is that cognition can only be indirectly assessed, for example by observing behaviours in response to experimental manipulation, and that behavioural responses to stimuli can be difficult to interpret if individuals differ in the expression of emotional arousal—or do not show any notable behavioural change in response to stimuli [20]. However, given that physiological changes to an external stimulus strongly depend on an individual's perception and evaluation of the event [5], measuring physiological parameters such as heart rate can be a promising quantitative tool to assess cognitive processes and arousal in response to stimuli in the environment. Lambs, Ovis aries, and rats responded with a heart rate increase to novel, sudden and unpredictable stimuli [47,48]. When engaging in learning tasks, heart rate increased in goats, Capra hircus [45], horses, Equus caballus [42] and cattle [35] but decreased in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris [41]. In the future, improved biologging technology to measure heart rate in different contexts could allow more objective quantification of cognitive function in a wide range of species, as already shown by the use of neurologgers to measure electroencephalograms in order to quantify sleep patterns in free-living animals [66–69].
Heart rate monitoring allows us to gain insights into the perceived level of difficulty of a cognitive challenge or whether individuals can differentiate between specific stimuli. Studies measuring heart rate during the presentation of conspecific and heterospecific photographs showed that chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, respond differently to familiar humans and strangers [37], and their different heart rate response to aggressive, friendly or unfamiliar conspecifics suggests that chimpanzees can also discriminate between those categories [36]. Similarly, heart rate measured in response to auditory stimuli indicated that dogs, Canis familiaris, can differentiate between conspecific barks given in different contexts [39], and blue penguin chicks, Eudyptula minor, had a higher heart rate during playbacks of calls from familiar individuals (siblings and neighbours) compared to calls of unfamiliar individuals [44]. Interestingly, penguin chicks did not express observable behavioural differences between the treatments, highlighting again that heart rate can reveal individuals' ability to discriminate between stimuli where it could not have been inferred from behavioural observations.
Heart rate can also reveal whether and to what degree animals respond to emotions of other individuals, i.e. emotional contagion. When watching conspecifics experiencing stimuli of negative emotional valence, dogs and horses responded with a higher heart rate compared to seeing others experiencing a positive treatment [43,70], and hens, Gallus gallus domesticus, significantly increased heart rate when their young were exposed to a mild stressor [38]. Similarly, greylag geese responded with a higher increase in heart rate when observing interactions involving closely affiliated individuals (pair-partner and family members) instead of non-affiliated individuals, even in the apparent absence of a behavioural response to these interactions (figure 1c)) [20].
Although heart rate presents a good means for quantifying the level of emotional arousal and does so more accurately compared to behavioural measures, it does not provide information about the valence of emotional arousal, because heart rate increases in response to both positive and negative experiences [18,71]. Potentially, different parameters of heart rate variability may allow us to better disentangle effects of positive versus negative valence [72,73]. Future research investigating heart rate during cognitive experiments could provide not only insights into the cognitive abilities of individuals and advance the field of animal cognition, but also improve our understanding of heart rate modulation in response to a variety of carefully chosen stimuli.
4. Individual differences in heart rate
Consistent inter-individual variation in phenotypic traits, including behavioural and physiological traits, has been described in a wide variety of species. With regard to physiology, individuals differ in both reactivity to challenges in the environment as well as in their baseline levels [74]. In some species, individuals with more aggressive behavioural traits have been shown to increase their heart rate more during aggressive interactions compared to less aggressive individuals [50,75]. The heart rate of chickens from different pedigrees (low and high feather pecking lines) did not significantly differ during baseline conditions or during restraint stress, but heart rates in high feather pecking lines tended to be elevated for longer [49]. In yellow-eyed penguins, individuals differed in their responses and habituation to anthropogenic disturbances [76]. Domestication is also assumed to affect physiological traits. Wild animals had higher average heart rates compared to farmed individuals in Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar [77], and dogs have higher resting heart rate compared to wolves, Canis lupus: [78].
While most studies reported in this review focus on heart rate or heart rate variability, research on individual responses to challenges could also include more detailed investigations into individual recovery periods [22]. Also, repeatability of physiological responses over time and across contexts has hardly been investigated but would provide insights into the development and maintenance of consistent phenotypes, i.e. ‘animal personality’. In summary, a better understanding of individual differences in physiological responses across different contexts could reveal links between behaviour and physiology that affect behavioural decision making and the evolution of different life-history strategies [74,79]. For example, individual stress responses can be linked to the energy demands of various reproductive stages [80,81], season [11,82,83], moulting [84,85], length of light–dark cycle [86], predation risk [87] and parasite load [88].
5. Implications for animal welfare
Good animal welfare is generally assumed when animals are not experiencing unnecessary stress. Since heart rate can be used to quantify the activation of the physiological stress response, monitoring heart rate can ultimately be considered a useful tool when attempting to improve animal welfare in a variety of contexts. The benefit of using heart rate as a measure of the physiological stress response, in contrast to, for example, glucocorticoid levels, is that heart rate can reflect individual levels of arousal accurately and at a high temporal resolution, and even small physiological responses to specific stimuli, in the absence of visible behavioural reactions, can be isolated [89].
Heart rate can be used to monitor arousal levels of individuals when evaluating the effectiveness of measures intended to reduce stress, for example, when enrichment or other changes are introduced in animal shelters or laboratories. In kennelled dogs, the presentation of auditory and olfactory enrichment decreased heart rate and changed heart rate variability, indicating stress reduction [52,53], and mice, Mus musculus, that were housed in enriched conditions had a significantly lower heart rate compared to individuals housed under minimal husbandry conditions without enrichment and periods of individual housing [55]. Generally, social isolation can cause significant increases in heart rate (alpaca, Vicugna pacos: [90]; dogs: [78]; cattle: [54]; cynomolgus monkeys, Macaca fascicularis: [91]; goats: [18]; wolves [78]). However, affiliative behaviours, even if performed by heterospecifics, can have a stress-reducing effect; grooming carried out by a familiar human decreased heart rate in laboratory-kept rhesus monkeys [92], dairy cows [93] and lambs [94]. Measuring heart rate can also reveal ineffective enrichment designs or unsuccessful attempts at stress reduction in animal housing. For example, individually housed rhesus monkeys in a laboratory environment did not show any changes in heart rate in response to increasing cage sizes [56] and auditory enrichment did not result in the expected reduction in heart rate in African green monkeys, Chlorocebus aethiops [51].
Biologging technologies to measure heart rate could be widely used to assess the effects of anthropogenic disturbance on wildlife. This is especially relevant as previous studies have shown that individuals can show pronounced emotional arousal, as measured by heart rate, in the absence of obvious behavioural changes. For example, American black bears, Ursus americanus, and bighorn sheep showed no behavioural responses but significantly increased heart rate in response to anthropogenic disturbances, such as drone overflights and vehicle traffic [28,30]. Anthropogenic sounds alone were identified as a stressor that led to elevated heart rate in farm animals [95] and fish [33]. Also, direct human contact can increase heart rate in wild animals; for example, brown bears, Ursus arctos, significantly increased heart rate in response to dog hunts and human encounters [31] and in captive koalas, Phascolarctos cinereus, heart rate increased in the presence of tourists compared to situations without humans [32]. Human approach towards nesting birds may not always lead to females leaving the nest, but elevated heart rate for extended periods can increase energy expenditure (Adélie penguins, Pygoscelis adeliae: [27]; Humboldt penguin, Spheniscus humboldti: [12]; yellow-eyed penguin [12]; wandering albatrosses, Diomedea exulans: [34]). By contrast, other populations might be found to be more resilient; for example, nesting American oystercatchers, Haematopus palliates, did not significantly increase heart rate in response to a variety of human disturbances, including human approach, off-road vehicles and aircraft overflights [29], and greylag geese did not significantly increase heart rate in response to familiar humans approaching [96] (figure 1d). Responses to human approach can also be affected by previous experience with humans and wild animals can habituate to the presence of humans [22,97]. Knowledge about physiological responses of wild animals to human disturbances can be used to develop guidelines and recommendations to minimize the impact of human activities [98].
A core application for using heart rate measurements to assess animal welfare is in the context of livestock management, given the large numbers of animals kept for food production and increasing demands for improved ethical production of animal products. Decisions on transport and husbandry routines should aim to minimize arousal levels, and heart rate monitoring can provide important evidence for approaches that aim to improve animal welfare. For example, when transporting sheep, heart rate revealed differences in the perceived stress depending on the methods of driving and loading sheep [99,100], and cognitively enriched pigs have a lower heart rate during feeding announcement compared to a control group [101]. Recently, heart rate measures have also been implemented in precision livestock farming, defined as the use of advanced technologies for continuous automated real-time animal monitoring to optimize the contribution of each animal [58–60,102,103]. In future, heart rate could be used on a large scale to identify early signs of decreased welfare or poor health in groups of farmed animals, pets or critically endangered animals in a conservation context (table 1).
6. Summary and future research directions
Heart rate provides an accurate and comprehensive measure of emotional arousal in non-human animals [9]. Here, I propose that research on heart rate modulation in a wide variety of species and different contexts could be of interdisciplinary interest and would provide important insights for basic research, animal welfare and conservation.
One of the biggest advantages of heart rate measures is that it allows relatively accessible measuring of short-term levels of emotional arousal in response to single events (e.g. vocalizations, social interactions and human disturbance), but heart rate can also assess the impact of long-term stressors on individuals' physiology (e.g. seasonal effects, noise pollution) and reveal chronic stress. Assessment of heart rate can be cheaper compared to other measures of activation of the physiological stress response, for example, non-invasive measures of glucocorticoid metabolites in faeces [104].
A major methodological challenge in evolutionary biology research is that, when quantifying activation of the physiological stress response, studies often compare absolute levels of arousal or stress, with little consideration about the adaptive benefit or cost associated with the response [105]. Activation of the physiological stress response has been proposed to have both adaptive and maladaptive aspects [105]. Short-term increases in heart rate are considered adaptative to help individuals to cope with a given stimulus in the environment. However, increases in heart rate result in an increase in energy expenditure and long-term maintenance of an elevated physiological stress response might be costly and have negative effects on an individual's health and fitness (e.g. [53]). For example, stress through group instability caused increased coronary artery atherosclerosis in dominant male cynomolgus monkeys compared to subordinate individuals [106]. A better understanding of potential fitness consequences of activation of the physiological stress response in behavioural biology is important for interpreting findings about heart rate responses in different contexts. Recording multiple physiological parameters simultaneously, e.g. body temperature, heat flux, blood and tissue oxygenation, blood metabolites and gases, gastric pH and motility, or diving air volume and respiratory rate, could help to achieve this by providing a more comprehensive representation of an individual's physiological status [57,107,108].
Heart rate measurements allow us to validate behavioural indicators of emotional arousal and activation of the physiological stress response. Whereas commonly used physiological measures that require laboratory analysis of hormonal changes are expensive, time consuming or practically not feasible, behavioural observations and analyses of certain indicative behaviours, such as vigilance, can be used to analyse responses to different stimuli. Correlating behavioural observations with simultaneous measures of heart rate provides an important validation for using behavioural indicators of individual arousal and stress. In horses, heart rate and heart rate variability are correlated with snorting in stressful conditions [109], ear posture is indicative of emotional arousal in dairy cows [110], and in greylag geese, the maximum heart rate during aggressive encounters is positively related to the frequency of shaking and duration of vigilance after the conflict [62]. However, other studies found no relationship between considered behavioural measures and heart rate in some contexts [111,112]. If a robust relationship between frequency or intensity of behavioural measures and emotional arousal is validated, behavioural observations can be used to assess levels of arousal at low material cost. However, more studies reporting heart rate are needed to carefully validate behavioural indicators of stress under consideration of factors like species, context and individual variation.
More research is needed to investigate in which contexts emotional arousal and activation of the physiological stress response are adaptive and help an animal to cope with stressors, and in which contexts the activation of the physiological stress response is maladaptive and has negative impacts on an individual's health [113]. Here, long-term monitoring over months or years is technologically feasible and could, especially in managed animal species (e.g. farm animals, animals in shelters), be linked to outcomes of daily routines or veterinary inspections. Long-term monitoring of heart rate would also allow us to disentangle short-term reactivity to environmental stimuli from long-term activation of the physiological stress response, which is not well understood yet [11,103]. Heart rate recordings could be incorporated in automated health surveillance processes in a wide variety of different settings [114,115].
A major constraint to assessing heart rate is the availability of suitable devices. In this review, I have presented research applying different biologging technology, ranging from fully implantable transmitters to wearable heart rate belts that can be applied non-invasively (table 1). Different technology makes measuring heart rate possible in a wide range of species [107,116], including fish [51], marine mammals and turtles [28,95], domestic pets and farmed animals [33], but different approaches have specific social, ethical and regulatory challenges [117]. For example, implanting transmitters is an invasive process and might not be ethically acceptable in all contexts and species. Commercially available wearable technology, such as heart rate belts, are not easily applicable to wild animals because they often need readjustment and manual removal. Especially for small species, e.g. birds, there are additional ethical considerations to be made regarding to the weight of the device. To minimize ethical risks of prolonged invasive measuring techniques, robust statistical methods to analyse the collected data are needed, which can help to refine the study design in accordance with the 3Rs (reduce, refine and replace) [118,119]. Next to ethical considerations, different devices also differ in their price range and method of actual heart rate recording. Most heart rate equipment records an electrocardiogram [120] and some require microphones to acoustically record the frequency of heartbeats [121]. All technical solutions measuring heart rate in non-human animals need to be carefully corrected for outlier values, which can arise when the algorithm used to measure the electrocardiogram is not carefully validated for the species in question or when the signal is temporarily lost in un-restrained animals [122]. Therefore, similar to other physiological parameters, heart rate measurements require careful validation and removal of outliers that are not biologically meaningful [120,122,123].
To summarize, using heart rate to gain an improved understanding of how individuals cope with challenges in their environment is of interest for several areas of evolutionary biology, including research on social behaviour, animal cognition and individual differences. For example, understanding the adaptive costs and benefits of social behaviour is a central topic in evolutionary ecology, or quantifying individual levels of stress in response to anthropogenic disturbances or environmental challenges and evaluating the effectiveness of measures intended to reduce stress is of particular importance in applied animal welfare research and conservation. Technological advances in biologging can help to further advance the application of heart rate monitors in diverse areas of animal behaviour research and contribute to studying emotional arousal in different contexts.
Acknowledgements
I thank Friederike Hillemann for her valuable input on the manuscript. I also thank Lucy Hawkes, Vincent A. Viblanc and an anonymous reviewer for their very helpful comments.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Competing interests
I declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This work has received no funding.
References
- 1.Seyle H. 1976. The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- 2.von Holst D. 1998. The concept of stress and its relevance for animal behavior. In Advances in the study of behavior 27, 1-131. New York, NY: Academic Press. ( 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60362-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Spodick DH, Raju RL, Bishop RL, Rifkin RD. 1992. Operational definition of normal sinus heart rate. Am. J. Cardiol. 69, 1245-1246. ( 10.1016/0002-9149(92)90947-w) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Anderson DJ, Adolphs R. 2014. A framework for studying emotions across species. Cell 157, 187-200. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Veissier I, Boissy A. 2007. Stress and welfare: two complementary concepts that are intrinsically related to the animal's point of view. Physiol. Behav. 92, 429-433. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.11.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.von Borell E, et al. 2007. Heart rate variability as a measure of autonomic regulation of cardiac activity for assessing stress and welfare in farm animals — A review. Physiol. Behav. 92, 293-316. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. 2006. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev. General Psychol. 10, 229-240. ( 10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Romero LM, Dickens MJ, Cyr NE. 2009. The reactive scope model — A new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress. Horm. Behav. 55, 375-389. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.12.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gaidica M, Dantzer B. 2020. Quantifying the autonomic response to stressors—one way to expand the definition of ‘stress’ in animals. Integr. Comp. Biol. 60, 113-125. ( 10.1093/icb/icaa009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Halsey LG, et al. 2019. Flexibility, variability and constraint in energy management patterns across vertebrate taxa revealed by long-term heart rate measurements. Funct. Ecol. 33, 260-272. ( 10.1111/1365-2435.13264) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wascher CAF, Kotrschal K, Arnold W. 2018. Free-living greylag geese adjust their heart rates and body core temperatures to season and reproductive context. Sci. Rep. 8, 2142. ( 10.1038/s41598-018-20655-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ellenberg U, Mattern T, Seddon PJ. 2013. Heart rate responses provide an objective evaluation of human disturbance stimuli in breeding birds. Conserv. Physiol. 1, 1-11. ( 10.1093/conphys/cot013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ely CR, Ward DH, Bollinger KS. 1999. Behavioral correlates of heart rates of free-living greater white-fronted geese. Condor 101, 390-395. ( 10.2307/1370002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baudinette RV. 1978. Scaling of heart rate during locomotion in mammals. J. Comp. Physiol. B 127, 337-342. ( 10.1007/BF00738418) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marchant JN, Mendl MT, Rudd AR, Broom DM. 1995. The effect of agonistic interactions on the heart rate of group-housed sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 46, 49-56. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00636-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Li H, Listeman LR, Doshi D, Cooper RL. 2000. Heart rate measures in blind cave crayfish during environmental disturbances and social interactions. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A: Mol. Integr. Physiol. 127, 55-70. ( 10.1016/S1095-6433(00)00241-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Laister S, Stockinger B, Regner A-M, Zenger K, Knierim U, Winckler C. 2011. Social licking in dairy cattle—Effects on heart rate in performers and receivers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 130, 81-90. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.12.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Briefer EF, Tettamanti F, McElligott AG. 2015. Emotions in goats: mapping physiological, behavioural and vocal profiles. Anim. Behav. 99, 131-143. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wascher CAF, Arnold W, Kotrschal K. 2008. Heart rate modulation by social contexts in greylag geese (Anser anser). J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 100-107. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.122.1.100) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wascher CAF, Scheiber IBR, Kotrschal K. 2008. Heart rate modulation in bystanding geese watching social and non-social events. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1653-1659. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.0146) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Viblanc VA, Valette V, Kauffmann M, Malosse N, Groscolas R. 2012. Coping with social stress: heart rate responses to agonistic interactions in king penguins. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1178-1185. ( 10.1093/beheco/ars095) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Viblanc VA, Smith AD, Gineste B, Groscolas R. 2012. Coping with continuous human disturbance in the wild: insights from penguin heart rate response to various stressors. BMC Ecol. 12, 10. ( 10.1186/1472-6785-12-10) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. 1989. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiol. Behav. 45, 667-670. ( 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90089-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sgoifo A, Pozzato C, Costoli T, Manghi M, Stilli D, Ferrari PF, Ceresini G, Musso E. 2001. Cardiac autonomic responses to intermittent social conflict in rats. Physiol. Behav. 73, 343-349. ( 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00455-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Turbill C, Ruf T, Rothmann A, Arnold W. 2013. Social dominance is associated with individual differences in heart rate and energetic response to food restriction in female red deer. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 86, 528-537. ( 10.1086/672372) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Aureli F, Preston SD, De Waal F. 1999. Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. J. Comp. Psychol. 113, 59-65. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.59) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nimon AJ, Schroter RC, Stonehouse B. 1995. Heart rate of disturbed penguins. Nature 374, 415. ( 10.1038/374415a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ditmer MA, Vincent JB, Werden LK, Tanner JC, Laske TG, Iaizzo PA, Garshelis DL, Fieberg JR. 2015. Bears show a physiological but limited behavioral response to unmanned aerial vehicles. Curr. Biol. 25, 2278-2283. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Borneman TE, Rose ET, Simons TR. 2014. Minimal changes in heart rate of incubating American Oystercatchers (Haematopus palliatus) in response to human activity. Condor 116, 493-503. ( 10.1650/CONDOR-14-48.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.MacArthur RA, Johnston RH, Geist V. 1979. Factors influencing heart rate in free-ranging bighorn sheep: a physiological approach to the study of wildlife harassment. Can. J. Zool. 57, 2010-2021. ( 10.1139/z79-265) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Le Grand L, Thorsen NH, Fuchs B, Evans AL, Laske TG, Arnemo JM, Sæbø S, Støen O-G. 2019. Behavioral and physiological responses of Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos) to dog hunts and human encounters. Front. Ecol. Evol. 7, 134. ( 10.3389/fevo.2019.00134) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ropert-Coudert Y, Brooks L, Yamamoto M, Kato A. 2009. ECG response of koalas to tourists proximity: a preliminary study. PLoS ONE 4, e7378. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0007378) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Graham AL, Cooke SJ. 2008. The effects of noise disturbance from various recreational boating activities common to inland waters on the cardiac physiology of a freshwater fish, the largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides). Aq. Conserv. Mar. Freshw. Ecosyst. 18, 1315-1324. ( 10.1002/aqc.941) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Weimerskirch H, Shaffer SA, Mabille G, Martin J, Boutard O, Rouanet JL. 2002. Heart rate and energy expenditure of wandering albatross. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 475-483. ( 10.1242/jeb.205.4.475) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hagen K, Broom DM. 2004. Emotional reactions to learning in cattle. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 85, 203-213. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2003.11.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Boysen ST, Berntson GG. 1989. Conspecific recognition in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): cardiac responses to significant others. J. Comp. Psychol. 103, 215. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.103.3.215) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Boysen ST, Berntson GG. 1986. Cardiac correlates of individual recognition in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 321. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.321) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Edgar JL, Lowe JC, Paul ES, Nicol CJ. 2011. Avian maternal response to chick distress. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3129-3134. ( 10.1098/rspb.2010.2701) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Maros K, Pongracz P, Bardos G, Molnar C, Farago T. 2008. Dogs can discriminate barks from different situations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 114, 159-167. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Siniscalchi M, Lusito R, Vallortigara G, Quaranta A. 2013. Seeing left- or right-asymmetric tail wagging produces different emotional responses in dogs. Curr. Biol. 23, 2279-2282. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Glassman LW, Hagmann CE, Qadri MA, Cook RG, Romero LM. 2019. The effect of learning on heart rate and behavior of European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Exp. Zool. 331, 506-516. ( 10.1002/jez.2319) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mengoli M, Pageat P, Lafont-Lecuelle C, Monneret P, Giacalone A, Sighieri C, Cozzi A. 2014. Influence of emotional balance during a learning and recall test in horses (Equus caballus). Behav. Proc. 106, 141-150. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.05.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Trösch M, Pellon S, Cuzol F, Parias C, Nowak R, Calandreau L, Lansade L. 2020. Horses feel emotions when they watch positive and negative horse–human interactions in a video and transpose what they saw to real life. Anim. Cogn. 23, 643-653. ( 10.1007/s10071-020-01369-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nakagawa S, Waas J, Miyazaki M. 2001. Heart rate changes reveal that little blue penguin chicks (Eudyptula minor) can use vocal signatures to discriminate familiar from unfamiliar chicks. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50, 180-188. ( 10.1007/s002650100355) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Langbein J, Nürnberg G, Manteuffel G. 2004. Visual discrimination learning in dwarf goats and associated changes in heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiol. Behav. 82, 601-609. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.05.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Höjesjö J, Axelsson M, Dahy R, Gustavsson L, Johnsson JI. 2015. Sight or smell? Behavioural and heart rate responses in subordinate rainbow trout exposed to cues from dominant fish. PeerJ 3, e1169. ( 10.7717/peerj.1169) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.van den Buuse M, van Acker SABE, Fluttert M, Kloet ERD. 2001. Blood pressure, heart rate, and behavioral responses to psychological ‘novelty’ stress in freely moving rats. Psychophysiology 38, 490-499. ( 10.1111/1469-8986.3830490) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Désiré L, Veissier I, Després G, Boissy A. 2004. On the way to assess emotions in animals: do lambs (Ovis aries) evaluate an event through Its suddenness, novelty, or unpredictability? J. Comp. Psychol. 118, 363-374. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.363) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Korte SM, Ruesink W, Blokhuis HJ. 1998. Heart rate variability during manual restraint in chicks from high- and low-feather pecking lines of laying hens. Physiol. Behav. 65, 649-652. ( 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00206-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kralj-Fišer S, Weiß BM, Kotrschal K. 2010. Behavioural and physiological correlates of personality in greylag geese (Anser anser). J. Ethol. 28, 363-370. ( 10.1007/s10164-009-0197-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hinds SB, Raimond S, Purcell BK. 2007. The effect of harp music on heart rate, mean blood pressure, respiratory rate, and body temperature in the African green monkey. J. Med. Primatol. 36, 95-100. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0684.2006.00157.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Amaya V, Paterson MBA, Descovich K, Phillips CJC. 2020. Effects of olfactory and auditory enrichment on heart rate variability in shelter dogs. Animals 10, 1385. ( 10.3390/ani10081385) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bowman A, Dowell FJ, Evans NP. 2017. The effect of different genres of music on the stress levels of kennelled dogs. Physiol. Behav. 171, 207-215. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.01.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Boissy A, Le Neindre P. 1997. Behavioral, cardiac and cortisol responses to brief peer separation and reunion in cattle. Physiol. Behav. 61, 693-699. ( 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00521-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Meijer M, Kramer K, Remie R. 2006. The effect of routine experimental procedures on physiological parameters in mice kept under different husbandry conditions. Anim. Welf. 15, 31-38. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Line SW, Morgan KN, Markowitz H, Strong S. 1990. Increased cage size does not alter heart rate or behavior in female rhesus monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 20, 107-113. ( 10.1002/ajp.1350200205) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Brijs J, Føre M, Gräns A, Clark T, Axelsson M, Johansen J. 2021. Bio-sensing technologies in aquaculture: how remote monitoring can bring us closer to our farm animals. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200218. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0218) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jorquera-Chavez M, Fuentes S, Dunshea FR, Warner RD, Poblete T, Jongman EC. 2019. Modelling and validation of computer vision techniques to assess heart rate, eye temperature, ear-base temperature and respiration rate in cattle. Animals 9, 1089. ( 10.3390/ani9121089) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lierz M, Gooss O, Hafez HM. 2006. Noninvasive heart rate measurement using a digital egg monitor in chicken and turkey embryos. J. Avian Med. Surg. 20, 141-146. ( 10.1647/2005-017R.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lewis CRG, Hulbert LE, McGlone JJ. 2008. Novelty causes elevated heart rate and immune changes in pigs exposed to handling, alleys, and ramps. Livestock Sci. 116, 338-341. ( 10.1016/j.livsci.2008.02.014) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wascher CAF, Scheiber IBR, Weiß BM, Kotrschal K. 2009. Heart rate responses to agonistic encounters in greylag geese, Anser anser. Anim. Behav. 77, 955-961. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wascher CAF, Fraser ON, Kotrschal K. 2010. Heart rate during conflicts predicts post-conflict stress-related behavior in greylag geese. PLoS ONE 5, e15751. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0015751) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wascher CAF, Kotrschal K. 2013. The costs of sociality measured through heart rate modulation. In The social life of greylag geese (eds Scheiber IBR, Groningen R, Weiß BM, Hemetsberger J, Kotrschal K), pp. 142-154. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ( 10.1017/CBO9781139049955.012) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Wascher CAF, Weiß BM, Arnold W, Kotrschal K. 2012. Physiological implications of pair-bond status in greylag geese. Biol. Lett. 8, 347-350. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0917) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Shettleworth SJ. 2001. Animal cognition and animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 61, 277-286. ( 10.1006/anbe.2000.1606) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kendall-Bar JM, Vyssotski AL, Mukhametov LM, Siegel JM, Lyamin OI. 2019. Eye state asymmetry during aquatic unihemispheric slow wave sleep in northern fur seals (Callorhinus ursinus). PLoS ONE 14, e0217025. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0217025) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Aulsebrook AE, Connelly F, Johnsson RD, Jones TM, Mulder RA, Hall ML, Vyssotski AL, Lesku JA. 2020. White and amber light at night disrupt sleep physiology in birds. Curr. Biol. 30, 3657-3663. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.085) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Connelly F, Johnsson RD, Aulsebrook AE, Mulder RA, Hall ML, Vyssotski AL, Lesku JA. 2020. Urban noise restricts, fragments, and lightens sleep in Australian magpies. Environ. Pollut. 267, 115484. ( 10.1016/j.envpol.2020.115484) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.van Hasselt SJ, Rusche M, Vyssotski AL, Verhulst S, Rattenborg NC, Meerlo P.. 2020. Sleep time in the European starling is strongly affected by night length and moon phase. Curr. Biol. 30, 1664-1671. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.052) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Quaranta A, Siniscalchi M, Vallortigara G. 2007. Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 17, R199-R201. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Reefmann N, Bütikofer Kaszàs F, Wechsler B, Gygax L. 2009. Physiological expression of emotional reactions in sheep. Physiol. Behav. 98, 235-241. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.05.017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Zupan M, Buskas J, Altimiras J, Keeling LJ. 2016. Assessing positive emotional states in dogs using heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiol. Behav. 155, 102-111. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Reefmann N, Wechsler B, Gygax L. 2009. Behavioural and physiological assessment of positive and negative emotion in sheep. Anim. Behav. 78, 651-659. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Careau V, Thomas D, Humphries MM, Reale D. 2008. Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos 117, 641-653. ( 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2008.16513.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sgoifo A, Costoli T, Meerlo P, Buwalda B, Pico’-Alfonso MA, De Boer S, Musso E, Koolhaas J. 2005. Individual differences in cardiovascular response to social challenge. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 59-66. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.07.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ellenberg U, Mattern T, Seddon PJ. 2009. Habituation potential of yellow-eyed penguins depends on sex, character and previous experience with humans. Anim. Behav. 77, 289-296. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Johnsson JI, Höjesjö J, Fleming IA. 2001. Behavioural and heart rate responses to predation risk in wild and domesticated Atlantic salmon. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 58, 788-794. ( 10.1139/f01-025) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Jean-Joseph H, Kortekaas K, Range F, Kotrschal K. 2020. Context-specific arousal during resting in wolves and dogs: effects of domestication? Front. Psychol. 11, 568199. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568199) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Roche DG, Careau V, Binning SA. 2016. Demystifying animal ‘personality’ (or not): why individual variation matters to experimental biologists. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 3832-3843. ( 10.1242/jeb.146712) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Viblanc VA, Smith AD, Gineste B, Kauffmann M, Groscolas R. 2015. Modulation of heart rate response to acute stressors throughout the breeding season in the king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 1686-1692. ( 10.1242/jeb.112003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Dickens MJ, Nephew BC, Romero LM. 2006. Captive european starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in breeding condition show an increased cardiovascular stress response to intruders. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 79, 937-943. ( 10.1086/506007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Portugal SJ, Green JA, Cassey P, Frappell PB, Butler PJ. 2009. Predicting the rate of oxygen consumption from heart rate in barnacle geese Branta leucopsis: effects of captivity and annual changes in body condition. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 2941-2948. ( 10.1242/jeb.034546) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Linek N, Bjarnason Á, Partecke J, Wikelski M. 2021. A songbird adjusts its heart rate and body temperature in response to season and fluctuating daily conditions. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200213. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0213) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Cyr NE, Wikelski M, Romero LM. 2008. Increased energy expenditure but decreased stress responsiveness during molt. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 81, 452-462. ( 10.1086/589547) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Portugal SJ, White CR, Green JA, Butler PJ. 2018. Flight feather moult drives minimum daily heart rate in wild geese. Biol. Lett. 14, 20180650. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0650) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Welbers AAMH, van Dis NE, Kolvoort AM, Ouyang J, Visser ME, Spoelstra K, Dominoni DM. 2017. Artificial light at night reduces daily energy expenditure in breeding great tits (Parus major). Front. Ecol. Evol. 5, 55. ( 10.3389/fevo.2017.00055) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Vaugeois M, Venturelli PA, Hummel SL, Accolla C, Forbes VE. 2020. Population context matters: predicting the effects of metabolic stress mediated by food availability and predation with an agent- and energy budget-based model. Ecol. Model. 416, 108903. ( 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2019.108903) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Hicks O, Burthe SJ, Daunt F, Newell M, Chastel O, Parenteau C, Green JA. 2018. The role of parasitism in the energy management of a free-ranging bird. J. Exp. Biol. 221, jeb190066. ( 10.1242/jeb.190066) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Beerda B, Schilder MBH, van Hooff JARAM, de Vries HW, Mol JA. 1998. Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 58, 365-381. ( 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00145-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Pollard JC, Littlejohn RP. 1995. Effects of social isolation and restraint on heart rate and behaviour of alpacas. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 45, 165-174. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00588-J) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Watson SL, Shively CA, Kaplan JR, Line SW. 1998. Effects of chronic social separation on cardiovascular disease risk factors in female cynomolgus monkeys. Atherosclerosis 137, 259-266. ( 10.1016/S0021-9150(97)00277-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Grandi LC, Ishida H. 2015. The physiological effect of human grooming on the heart rate and the heart rate variability of laboratory non-human primates: a pilot study in male rhesus monkeys. Front. Vet. Sci. 2, 50. ( 10.3389/fvets.2015.00050) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Schmied C, Waiblinger S, Scharl T, Leisch F, Boivin X. 2008. Stroking of different body regions by a human: effects on behaviour and heart rate of dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 25-38. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Coulon M, Nowak R, Peyrat J, Chandèze H, Boissy A, Boivin X. 2015. Do lambs perceive regular human stroking as pleasant? Behavior and heart rate variability analyses. PLoS ONE 10, e0118617. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0118617) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Talling JC, Waran NK, Wathes CM, Lines JA. 1996. Behavioural and physiological responses of pigs to sound. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 48, 187-201. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(96)01029-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wascher CAF, Scheiber IBR, Braun A, Kotrschal K. 2011. Heart rate responses to induced challenge situations in greylag geese (Anser anser). J. Comp. Psychol. 125, 116-119. ( 10.1037/a0021188) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ellenberg U, Mattern T, Houston DM, Davis LS, Seddon PJ. 2012. Previous experiences with humans affect responses of Snares penguins to experimental disturbance. J. Ornithol. 153, 621-631. ( 10.1007/s10336-011-0780-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Holmes N, Giese M, Kriwoken LK. 2005. Testing the minimum approach distance guidelines for incubating Royal penguins Eudyptes schlegeli. Biol. Conserv. 126, 339-350. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.06.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Baldock NM, Silby RM. 1990. Effects of handling and transportation on the heart rate and behaviour of sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28, 15-39. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(90)90044-E) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Parrott RF, Hall SJG, Lloyd DM. 1998. Heart rate and stress hormone responses of sheep to road transport following two different loading procedures. Anim. Welf. 7, 257-267. [Google Scholar]
- 101.Zebunke M. 2013. Effects of cognitive enrichment on behavioural and physiological reactions of pigs. Physiol. Behav. 118, 70-79. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.05.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Banhazi TM, Lehr H, Black JL, Crabtree H, Schofield P, Tscharke M, Berckmans D. 2012. Precision livestock farming: an international review of scientific and commercial aspects. Int. J. Agricult. Biol. Eng. 5, 1-9. ( 10.3965/j.ijabe.20120503.00?) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Trondrud LM, Pigeon G, Albon S, Arnold W, Evans AL, Irvine RJ, Król E, Ropstad E, Stien A, Veiberg V, Speakman JR, Loe LE. 2021. Determinants of heart rate in Svalbard reindeer reveal mechanisms of seasonal energy management. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200215. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0215) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Palme R. 2019. Non-invasive measurement of glucocorticoids: advances and problems. Physiol. Behav. 199, 229-243. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.11.021) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Seyle H. 1973. The evolution of the stress concept. Am. Sci. 61, 692-699. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Clarkson TB, Lusso FM, Taub DM. 1982. Social status, environment, and atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 2, 359-368. ( 10.1161/01.ATV.2.5.359) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Williams CL, Ponganis PJ. 2021. Diving physiology of marine mammals and birds: the development of biologging techniques. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200211. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0211) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Laske T, Garshelis D, Iles T, Iaizzo P. 2021. An engineering perspective on the development and evolution of implantable cardiac monitors in free living animals. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200217. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0217) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Scopa C, Palagi E, Sighieri C, Baragli P. 2018. Physiological outcomes of calming behaviors support the resilience hypothesis in horses. Sci. Rep. 8, 17501. ( 10.1038/s41598-018-35561-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Lambert H. 2019. Positive and negative emotions in dairy cows: can ear postures be used as a measure? Behav. Proc. 158, 172-180. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Squibb K. 2018. Poker Face: discrepancies in behaviour and affective states in horses during stressful handling procedures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 202, 34-38. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.02.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Safryghin A, Hebesberger DV, Wascher CAF. 2019. Testing for behavioral and physiological responses of domestic horses (Equus caballus) across different contexts – consistency over time and effects of context. Front. Psychol. 10, 849. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00849) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Proudfoot K, Habing G. 2015. Social stress as a cause of diseases in farm animals: current knowledge and future directions. Vet. J. 206, 15-21. ( 10.1016/j.tvjl.2015.05.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Berezowski J, Rüegg SR, Faverjon C. 2019. Complex system approaches for animal health surveillance. Front. Vet. Sci. 6, 153. ( 10.3389/fvets.2019.00153) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Bartlett PC, Van Buren JW, Neterer M, Zhou C. 2010. Disease surveillance and referral bias in the veterinary medical database. Prev. Vet. Med. 94, 264-271. ( 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2010.01.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Williams HJ, Shipley JR, Rutz C, Wikelski M, Wilkes M, Hawkes L. 2021. Future trends in measuring physiology in free-living animals. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200230. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0230) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Palmer A, Greenhough B. 2021. Out of the laboratory, into the field: perspectives on social, ethical and regulatory challenges in UK wildlife research. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200226. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0226) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Harrison XA. 2021. A brief introduction to the analysis of time-series data from biologging studies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200227. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0227) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Holton MD, Wilson RP, Teilmann J, Siebert U. 2021. Animal tag technology keeps coming of age: an engineering perspective. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200229. ( 10.1098/rstb.2020.0229) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Bjarnason Á, Gunnarsson A, Árnason T, Oddgeirsson M, Sigmarsson AB, Gunnarsson Á. 2019. Validation of ECG-derived heart rate recordings in Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua L.) with an implantable data logging system. Anim. Biotelemetry 7, 13. ( 10.1186/s40317-019-0176-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Nimon AJ, Schroter RC, Oxenham RK. 1996. Artificial eggs: measuring heart rate and effects of disturbance in nesting penguins. Physiol. Behav. 60, 1019-1022. ( 10.1016/0031-9384(96)00079-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Schöberl I, Kortekaas K, Schöberl FF, Kotrschal K. 2015. Algorithm-supported visual error correction (AVEC) of heart rate measurements in dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Behav. Res. 47, 1356-1364. ( 10.3758/s13428-014-0546-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Marchant-Forde RM, Marlin DJ, Marchant-Forde JN. 2004. Validation of a cardiac monitor for measuring heart rate variability in adult female pigs: accuracy, artefacts and editing. Physiol. Behav. 80, 449-458. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.