Abstract
Background
The prevalence of COVID‐19 has a social and economic impact on people, leaving them distressed and fearful of getting infected.
Objective
To determine the variables attributable to the fear of contracting COVID‐19.
Design
This is a quantitative study based on an online cross‐sectional self‐administered survey in Chile between 10 July 2020 and 10 August 2020.
Setting and participants
A sample of 531, comprising over 18‐year‐old participants from middle‐ and high‐income levels, was selected.
Outcome measures
Estimations were obtained using a probit regression model with marginal effects.
Results
Fear prevailed mainly in women. It has a positive relationship with variables such as chronic illnesses, infectious family or relatives, reduction in economic activity and perception of bad government response to a pandemic. Fear has a negative relationship with knowledge about COVID‐19, education level and ageing. Moreover, those who consider socioeconomic impact less important than health care do not fear a COVID‐19 infection.
Discussionand conclusion
The socioeconomic and health aspects help predict fears. Thus, the government should prioritize these variables in implementing policies. The government's credibility and communication systems can also reduce fears of contracting COVID‐19.
Patient or public contribution
A pilot focus group of COVID‐19–recuperated individuals and some members of our interest groups were consulted in the design stage of the study; this helped in constructing the survey questions. Additionally, three independent individuals volunteered to read and comment on the draft manuscript.
Keywords: Chile, health care, pandemic, perception, probit model
1. INTRODUCTION
The duration of the pandemic has undoubtedly affected the behaviour of people and has triggered fear in many people, because people perceive an increase in the risk of getting sick, especially after the number of infected people increased or a family member became ill or died from being infected by COVID‐19. 1 , 2 Fear is defined as an unpleasant emotional state caused by the perception of menacing stimulus. 3 Therefore, the longer the pandemic lasts, the greater the fears that people may face, affecting their socioeconomic well‐being 4 and mental health. 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 However, in some cases, the longer it takes to find a solution to the pandemic, the more people will not be inclined to take adequate measures to avoid contagious behaviour (social distancing, washing hands and quarantine compliance), because they are motivated to act not only for health‐care issues but also for social and economic reasons (income generation, human relationships or simply contempt with authority). This situation is evident when we observe the second and third pandemic wave around many countries in the globe where people have not received enough health care.
Comparing the pandemic situation a month and a half after the first case of COVID‐19 was diagnosed in Chile (17 April 2020) with the existing situation when we realized this study (19 July 2020), the cases and deaths have increased by 3 176% (3 030 083) and 5 566% (6573), respectively, 10 while the cases and deaths have increased by 4.35% and 2.60%, respectively, for the same period. 11 According to the Central Bank of Chile, the monthly Economic Activity Index between June 2020 and 2019 fell by 12.4%. 12 The number of unemployed people increased by 42.9%, which is the highest unemployment level in Chile between 2010 and 2020. 13 Moreover, from 7 July 2020 to 4 February 2021, the number of deaths and infected people has increased significantly. Specifically, the number of infected people has increased by 144%, reaching a total of 740 237 people, and the number of deaths increased by 185%, reaching a total of 18 731.
Aspects such as restrictive movement policies, confinement to residential properties, saturation of the health‐care system, quality of government response, loss of employment, financial loss, number of infected people, trust in people and/or institutions are some of the factors that may strongly influence people's well‐being and their fears of contracting the disease. 14 All these factors became relevant when there was no certainty about a vaccine and its characteristics. The media coverage may continue to provide information about contagious and perished people. 15 , 16 , 17
The health economics literature is primarily concerned with determining the willingness to pay for a vaccine and determining individuals' preferences for a vaccine under different modalities of effectiveness and side‐effects. The preferences and the willingness to pay for a vaccine could express an individual's fears of getting infected. 1 , 8 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 21
Torales, O'Higgins, Castaldelli‐Maia and Ventriglio 5 mentioned that the pandemic generates stress, anxiety, depression, insomnia, anger and fear globally. For example, home confinement causes a negative psychological impact, affecting the emotional state because of depression and anxiety. 22 The authors added that collective concerns could affect individual daily behaviours, reducing the effectiveness of COVID‐19 strategies and control. Debiec and Olsson 23 mentioned that fear responses could be learned, directly or indirectly, by social transmission. Thus, fear contagion may often happen automatically and unconsciously, making it complicated to control. 24 This can be even more difficult to manage as the number of cases of COVID‐19 increases, and the media provides information about infected and dead people.
However, governments sometimes instil fear in people to raise health awareness of COVID‐19, to change intentions and to motivate them to be vigilant during the pandemic. 15 , 16 , 22 In addition, government's response and credibility can affect the acceptability of vaccination. 25 , 26 As mentioned by Jakovljevic, Sugahara, Timofeyev and Rancic, 27 the quality of governance determines health‐care system performance. However, vaccine rejection may decrease depending on the severity of COVID‐19 infections. It can also decrease if people are aware of vaccination benefits or have family members infected with the virus. 1 , 2
Clark, Davila, Regis and Kraus 28 show that trust in the government has a low influence on individual decisions to take other preventive actions (mask‐wearing, social distancing, hand washing and staying at home) to reduce the probability of getting infected with COVID‐19. Mertens, Gerritsen, Duijndam, Salemink and Engelhard 29 found that fear is caused by health anxiety, regular media use, social media use and risk for loved ones. People's fear is triggered not only by COVID‐19 infections but also by the consequences of the pandemic, such as employment loss, income loss and affected social relationships. 1 , 30 For example, according to Pakpour and Griffiths, 15 people from both low‐income and middle‐income countries are worried about their jobs.
Few studies have explored the main factors that explain the fears of people during the current pandemic. This study aimed to analyse the main factors influencing people's fear of being infected with COVID‐19 to implement appropriate policies aimed at reducing people's fears during the COVID‐19. These factors will be helpful even in future occurrences of pandemics. As mentioned by Li, Yue and Crabbe, 31 the appearance of SARS in 2003 and COVID‐19 in 2019 indicates that it is possible that other coronavirus diseases may occur again in the future.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Study design
The data were collected through an online self‐administered questionnaire distributed to various social media platforms between 10 July 2020 and 10 August 2020. The sample comprised 531 Chilean participants aged between 18 and 90 years, mainly in the middle‐ and higher income levels. The questionnaire was validated by experts and other select individuals through a focus group and a pilot survey.
Furthermore, we asked for the opinions of a small focus group of COVID‐19 survivors and some members of our interest group in the design stage of the study. This helped to refine the survey questions regarding fear issues. Additionally, three independent individuals volunteered to read and comment on the drafted manuscript.
2.2. Sample
We considered a sample of 531 respondents, resulting in a margin error of 4.25% and a confidence level of 95%, assuming maximum variance and infinite population. Among these participants, 59% were men, and 41% were women; 52.0% of all of them had a private health‐care system. Most respondents belong to middle‐income and high‐income levels (83%), and only a few belong to the low‐income (17%) group. Almost 77% are graduates.
The questionnaire included three main parts: the first part had 20 questions related to knowledge about the pandemic, individuals’ perception and fear of their risk of infection, economic and social impacts, and information on medical conditions (chronic diseases, if any). We used the Likert scale with 1 denoting ‘completely disagree’ and 5 denoting ‘completely agree’ or dichotomous questions (yes or not).
The second part of the questionnaire had questions about the willingness to pay for vaccines and reasons for rejecting them. Finally, the third part had ten questions about sociodemographic data and children's education and performance under the pandemic. However, the data obtained from the second and third part questions about willingness to pay and child education were not considered in this study. However, it has been used for other publications, where the questionnaire is available as supplementary material on Cerda et al. 1
We notified the respondents that there is no monetary compensation, the survey is voluntary, and they could stop participating at any time. They were also informed that the data would be generalized: no. individual data and no record of their identities will be revealed. All participants consented to partake in the study.
2.3. Estimation output method
The estimation method consisted of a probit model, expressed as:
where the standardized normal cumulative distribution probability function is represented as:
where xi represents the matrix of explanatory variables for the individual i, β represents the vector of coefficients, and t denotes the normal standardized distribution. The estimation of the parameters was carried out through the maximum‐likelihood method. The estimated marginal effect of an independent variable x is the partial derivative, with respect to x, of the prediction function. The model was estimated using the statistical software STATA 16.0.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Data analysis
The data show that 290 respondents (54.6%) of the 531 individuals showed more fear of contracting the virus after the first and a half months since the pandemic hit Chile. Women's fear surpassed that of men. Specifically, women's fear increased by 89%, while fear of men increased by 30%.
Comparing middle‐aged adults (30 to 59 years) with younger adults (19 to 29 years) and senior people (older than 60 years) yielded the same results to that of women and men proportions. Additionally, people from a lower‐income (64.6%) group had more fears than those from other income‐level groups. Respondents with a higher level of education (45.8%) had less fear than those with a lower level of education (54.2%) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Characteristics |
Sample description n (%) 531 (100) (1) |
Number of people who responded that they had increased their fears of getting infected (%) 290 (100) (2) | Percentage of respondents with more fears over the total value of each characteristic (%) (3) |
---|---|---|---|
Age | |||
18‐29 | 61 (11.5) | 33 (11.4) | 54.1 |
30‐39 | 120 (22.6) | 70 (24.1) | 58.3 |
40‐49 | 136 (25.6) | 73 (25.2) | 53.7 |
50‐59 | 130 (24.7) | 75 (27.9) | 53.9 |
60 and more | 83 (15.6) | 39 (13.4) | 47.0 |
Gender | |||
Female | 212 (39.9) | 177 (61.1) | 83.5 |
Male | 314 (59.3) | 110 (37.9) | 35.0 |
No response | 4 (0.8) | 3 (1.0) | 75.0 |
Education | |||
Preliminary school | 6 (1.1) | 3 (1.0) | 50.0 |
High school | 45 (8.5) | 30 (10.3) | 66.7 |
Technical | 73 (13.8) | 45 (15.5) | 61.6 |
Undergraduate degree | 231 (43.7) | 132 (45.5) | 57.1 |
Graduate degree | 175 (33.0) | 80 (27.6) | 45.7 |
Monthly income (US dollars) | |||
Less than $569 | 90 (17.0) | 58 (20.0) | 64.4 |
$570 to $953 | 62 (11.7) | 34 (11.7) | 54.8 |
$954 to $1 476 | 64 (12.1) | 33 (11.4) | 51.6 |
$1 477 to $2 186 | 83 (15.8) | 51 (17.6) | 61.4 |
Greater to $2 186 | 231 (43.5) | 114 (39.3) | 49.4 |
Type of health system | |||
Public | 225 (42) | 125 (43.1) | 55.6 |
Private | 274 (52) | 146 (50.3) | 53.3 |
Other | 32 (6) | 19 (6.6) | 59.4 |
3.2. Probit model analysis of fear
Several estimations were considered, including various explanatory variables such as the perception of government communication response, the perception of general government response, adaptability to remote work, age, gender, the perception that the socioeconomic impact is more important than health care, the perception of bad government response, the perception of own risk aversion, the existence of chronic diseases, family or relatives with COVID‐19 and education level. Table 2 presents the description of the statistically significant variables.
Table 2.
I strongly disagree | I disagree | Not agree or disagree | I agree | I strongly agree | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
I consider myself a risk lover | 210 (39.5) | 204 (38.4) | 68 (12.8) | 38 (7.2) | 11 (2.1) |
Socioeconomic impact is more important than health care | 236 (44.4) | 197 (37.1) | 64 (12.1) | 22 (4.1) | 12 (2.3) |
Bad government response | 110 (20.7) | 165 (31.1) | 61 (11.5) | 85 (16.0) | 110 (20.7) |
Reduced economic activity | 88 (20.7) | 47 (8.9) | 5 (0.6) | 200 (37.7) | 193 (36.4) |
COVID‐19 knowledge level is good | 90 (17.0) | 53 (10.0) | 41 (7.7) | 200 (37.7) | 147 (17.7) |
I have a chronic sickness | |||||
No | 343 (64.6) | ||||
Yes | 188 (35.4) | ||||
Family or relative with COVID‐19 | |||||
No | 486 (91.5) | ||||
Yes | 45 (8.5) |
The result of the marginal probit model explaining the increase in fear is presented in Table 3. The estimation shows that fear has a positive relationship with variables such as existing chronic sickness, infected family, reduction in economic activity and perception of bad government response to the pandemic. In contrast, fear has a negative relationship with the knowledge about COVID‐19, education level and age. Moreover, those who regard the socioeconomic impact as less important than health care have less fear of contracting COVID‐19.
Table 3.
Dependent variable Fears to get infected increased (Yes = 1, No = 0) |
Marginal effect = dF/dx (robust standard error) |
[95% confidence interval] |
---|---|---|
I have chronic sickness |
0.1130542** (0.048302) |
[0.0183, 0.2077] |
Family or relative with COVID‐19 |
0.168712** (0.0764502) |
[0.0187, 0.3186] |
Reduce economic activity |
0.0764951* (0.0228623) |
[0.0317, 0.1213] |
I consider myself a risk lover |
‐0.0562568** (0.0240797) |
[−0.1035, −0.0091] |
COVID‐19 knowledge level is good |
−0.0907256* (0.0238524) |
[−0.1375, −0.0440] |
Socioeconomic impact less important than health care |
−0.059085** (0.0243702) |
[−0.1069, −0.0113] |
Age |
−0.0310552*** (0.0179509) |
[−0.0662, −0.0041] |
Education |
−0.056076** (0.02547) |
[−0.1060, −0.0062] |
Perception of bad government response |
0.0565721* (0.0174914) |
[0.0223, 0.0909] |
Obs. P 0.551394 pred. P 0.5525956 (at x‐bar); Wald chi2(9) = 53.99 Prob >chi2 = 0.0000 Pseudo‐R2 = 0.0776 Log pseudolikelihood = −337.41454 N = 531 |
P‐value<0.10.
P‐value <0.05.
P‐value <0.01.
4. DISCUSSION
Within the first one and a half months that the pandemic hit Chile, 54% of respondents proved to more fearful of contracting the disease. People who fear a COVID‐19 infection can exceed this percentage.
The increase in fear was higher in women (83.5%) than in men, in which it was 35%. Women in Chile may be more psychologically affected by the COVID‐19 pandemic 32 ; that is, women may be more afraid of contracting the virus.
Our study corroborates other studies that mention that the worries and fear of getting COVID‐19 are influenced not only by the potential health impact but also socioeconomic effects and social vulnerability. 15 , 30 , 33 Our study shows that the reduction in economic activity and perception of bad governmental response to the pandemic increases the fear. This result is consistent with Jakovljevic, Sugahara, Timofeyev and Rancic, 27 with respect to government performance. Additionally, fear is less in people who think that the socioeconomic impact is less important than health care.
The existence of chronic sickness and family infectivity is also a predictor of fear in our study. This finding is consistent with other studies, 2 , 29 , 32 which found that increased fear was related to perceived risk for loved ones and health anxiety, fear of becoming infected and fear of getting in contact with contaminated objects. The impact of having family members ill with symptomatic COVID‐19 affects the perception of the severity of the disease in individuals, generating greater fear of being infected due to its impacts not only on health but also on their socioeconomic environment.
We also found that fear is inversely proportional to age: it decreases when age increases. Despite adult people being reported to be at a higher risk, we found that they are less fearful of contracting the disease than young people. 34 Some authors propose health literacy interventions in older people to promote healthy behaviours. 35 However, we suggest that this attention to literacy should be given to young people because they tend to be more fearful than adult people. 2
Another relevant result of our study is that more educated and well‐informed people about COVID‐19 experience less fear. The result emphasizes the importance of education and information for public health policy. The government should make efforts to equip people with knowledge about COVID‐19. It should provide information on how to combat the virus to the less‐educated and young individuals. 16
Given that the socioeconomic and health aspects help predict fears, governments must respond according to socioeconomic and health needs. They should also implement a good and credible communication system of the health situation and performance to combat the pandemic. 15 , 23 , 24 , 29 , 34 Additionally, as mentioned by Ornell, Schuch, Sordi and Kessler, 36 it is also necessary to implement public mental health policies. Anyway, the results about the variables explaining fear are determined by the temporal context of the pandemic; therefore, they could change over time.
5. CONCLUSIONS
The results of this cross‐sectional study suggest that people who are more fearful of contracting COVID‐19 are women, those with chronic illnesses, infectious families, those with loss of economic activity and those who perceive a bad governmental response to the pandemic. In comparison, fear decreased with the knowledge about the pandemic, education level and age. Therefore, governments should prioritize these variables: health, social and economic policy implementation.
Some limitations of our study are that our sample includes a high proportion of people with relatively high educational levels, and the convenience sampling system. The online data collection method limits the generalizability of the results. Another aspect to consider is that the results about the variables explaining fear are determined by the temporal context of the pandemic; therefore, they could change over time. Therefore, studying the determinants of temporal changes in the fears faced by individuals due to the pandemic is an interesting line of research to develop.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
LG and AC declare they have no conflicts of interest that are relevant to this manuscript.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
AC conceptualized the design, performed formal analysis and investigation, contributed to resources, and wrote the original draft. LG modelled estimation, and wrote, reviewed and edited the manuscript.
Cerda AA, García LY. Factors explaining the fear of being infected with COVID‐19. Health Expect. 2022;25:506–512. 10.1111/hex.13274
Funding information
The authors declare that the study was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data sets that support this study's findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
REFERENCES
- 1. Cerda AA, García LY. Willingness to Pay for a COVID‐19 Vaccine. Appl Heal Th Econ Hea. 2021;19(3):343‐351. 10.1007/s40258-021-00644-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Cerda AA, Garcia LY. Hesitation and refusal factors in individuals’ decision‐making processes regarding a COVID‐19 vaccination. Front Public Health. 2021. 9:1‐14. 10.3389/fpubh.2021.626852 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. de Hoog N, Stroebe W, de Wit JBF. The processing of fear‐arousing communications: How biased processing leads to persuasion. Soc Influence. 2008;3(2):84‐113. 10.1080/15534510802185836 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Wu S. Effects of pandemics‐related uncertainty on household consumption: evidence from the cross‐country data. Front Public Health. 2020;8(798):1‐5. 10.3389/fpubh.2020.615344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Torales J, O'Higgins M, Castaldelli‐Maia JM, Ventriglio A. The outbreak of COVID‐19: coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2020;66(4):317‐320. 10.1177/0020764020915212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Acter T, Uddin N, Das J, Akhter A, Choudhury TR, Kim S. Evolution of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS‐CoV‐2) as coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19) pandemic: A global health emergency. Sci Total Environ. 2020;730:138996. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.138996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Koon OE. The Impact of Sociocultural Influences on the COVID‐19 Measures: Reflections from Singapore. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2020;60(2):e90‐e92. 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2020.04.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. García LY, Cerda AA. Contingent assessment of the COVID‐19 vaccine. Vaccine. 2020;38(34):5424‐5429. 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.06.068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. García LY, Cerda AA. Acceptance of a COVID‐19 vaccine: a multifactorial consideration. Vaccine. 2020;38(48):7587. 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.10.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gobierno de Chile . Cifras Oficiales COVID‐19. Accessed January 20, 2020. www.gob.cl/coronavirus/cifrasoficiales/ttp://epi.minsal.cl/informes‐covid‐19/
- 11. Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología . Bases de datos COVID‐19, Gobierno de Chile. Accessed 7 August, 2020. https://www.minciencia.gob.cl/covid19
- 12. Central Bank of Chile . Monthly economic activity index (Imacec) June. Central Bank of Chile. 2020. Accessed Sept. 20, 2020.
- 13. National Institute of Statistics . Boletín Estadístico: Empleo trimestral June. National Institute of Statistics. Accessed Sept. 20, 2020.
- 14. Brooks SK, Webster RK, Smith LE, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet. 2020;395(10227):912‐920. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Pakpour AH, Griffiths MD. The fear of COVID‐19 and its role in preventive behaviors. J Concurrent Dis. 2020;2(1):58‐63. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Shi J, Smith SW. The effects of fear appeal message repetition on perceived threat, perceived efficacy, and behavioral intention in the extended parallel process model. Health Communication. 2016;31(3):275‐286. 10.1080/10410236.2014.948145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Januraga PP, Gesesew HA, Ward PR. Trust as a determinant factor for condom use among female sex workers in Bali, Indonesia. Trop Med Infect Dis. 2020;5(3):131. 10.3390/tropicalmed5030131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Harapan H, Wagner AL, Yufika A, et al. Acceptance of a COVID‐19 Vaccine in Southeast Asia: A Cross‐Sectional Study in Indonesia. Original Research. Front Public Health. 2020;8:381. 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Wong LP, Alias H, Wong P‐F, Lee HY, AbuBakar S. The use of the health belief model to assess predictors of intent to receive the COVID‐19 vaccine and willingness to pay. Hum Vaccines Immunother. 2020;16(9):2204‐2214. 10.1080/21645515.2020.1790279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Sarasty O, Carpio CE, Hudson D, Guerrero‐Ochoa PA, Borja I. The demand for a COVID‐19 vaccine in Ecuador. Vaccine. 2020;38(51):8090‐8098. 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.11.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Wong LP, Alias H, Wong P‐F, Lee HY, AbuBakar S. The use of the health belief model to assess predictors of intent to receive the COVID‐19 vaccine and willingness to pay. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2020;16(9):2204‐2214. 10.1080/21645515.2020.1790279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Rashid Menhas ZAS, Zheng F, Dai J, Mahmood S. Yu Weng and Xuehui Sang. The psychological impacts of COVID‐19 home confinement and physical activity: a structural equation model analysis. Front Psychol. 2020.11:1‐12 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.614770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Debiec J, Olsson A. Social fear learning: from animal models to human function. Trends in Cogn Sci. 2017;21(7):546‐555. 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Fear can spread from person to person faster than the coronavirus – but there are ways to slow it down. The Conversation; March, 2020. https://theconversation.com/fear‐can‐spread‐from‐person‐to‐person‐faster‐than‐the‐coronavirus‐but‐there‐are‐ways‐to‐slow‐it‐down‐133129
- 25. Ward PR, Attwell K, Meyer SB, Rokkas P, Leask J. Understanding the perceived logic of care by vaccine‐hesitant and vaccine‐refusing parents: a qualitative study in Australia. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(10):e0185955. 10.1371/journal.pone.0185955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Ward PR, Attwell K, Meyer SB, Rokkas P, Leask J. Risk, responsibility and negative responses: a qualitative study of parental trust in childhood vaccinations. J Risk Res. 2018;21(9):1117‐1130. 10.1080/13669877.2017.1391318 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Jakovljevic M, Sugahara T, Timofeyev Y, Rancic N. Predictors of (in)efficiencies of healthcare expenditure among the leading asian economies ‐ comparison of OECD and Non‐OECD Nations. Risk Manag Healthc Policy. 2020;13:2261‐2280. 10.2147/RMHP.S266386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Clark C, Davila A, Regis M, Kraus S. Predictors of COVID‐19 voluntary compliance behaviors: an international investigation. Global Transitions. 2020;2:76‐82. 10.1016/j.glt.2020.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Mertens G, Gerritsen L, Duijndam S, Salemink E, Engelhard IM. Fear of the coronavirus (COVID‐19): predictors in an online study conducted in March 2020. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;74:102258. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Joffe AR. COVID‐19: rethinking the lockdown groupthink review. Front Public Health. 2021;9(98):1‐25 10.3389/fpubh.2021.625778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Li RYM, Yue X‐G, Crabbe MJC. COVID‐19 in Wuhan, China: pressing realities and city management. Opinion. Front Public Health. 2021;8(1079):596913. 10.3389/fpubh.2020.596913 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Özdin S, Bayrak ÖŞ. Levels and predictors of anxiety, depression and health anxiety during COVID‐19 pandemic in Turkish society: The importance of gender. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2020;66(5):504‐511. 10.1177/0020764020927051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Taylor S, Landry CA, Paluszek MM, Fergus TA, McKay D, Asmundson GJG. Development and initial validation of the COVID Stress Scales. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;72:102232. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Mazza C, Ricci E, Biondi S, et al. A Nationwide Survey of Psychological Distress among Italian People during the COVID‐19 Pandemic: Immediate Psychological Responses and Associated Factors. INT J ENV RES PUB HE. 2020;17(9):3165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Do BN, Nguyen P‐A, Pham KM, et al. Determinants of health literacy and its associations with health‐related behaviors, depression among the older people with and without suspected covid‐19 symptoms: a multi‐institutional study. Original Research. Front Public Health. 2020;8(694):1‐9 10.3389/fpubh.2020.581746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Ornell F, Schuch JB, Sordi AO, Kessler FHP. "Pandemic fear" and COVID‐19: mental health burden and strategies. Braz J Psychiatry. 2020;42(3):232‐235. 10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data sets that support this study's findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.