Skip to main content
Wiley - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Wiley - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 Apr 10;15(5):e12596. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12596

Social norms, social identities and the COVID‐19 pandemic: Theory and recommendations

Fergus G Neville 1,, Anne Templeton 2, Joanne R Smith 3, Winnifred R Louis 4
PMCID: PMC8250129  PMID: 34230834

Abstract

Sustained mass behaviour change is needed to tackle the COVID‐19 pandemic, but many of the required changes run contrary to existing social norms (e.g., physical closeness with in‐group members). This paper explains how social norms and social identities are critical to explaining and changing public behaviour. Recommendations are presented for how to harness these social processes to maximise adherence to COVID‐19 public health guidance. Specifically, we recommend that public health messages clearly define who the target group is, are framed as identity‐affirming rather than identity‐contradictory, include complementary injunctive and descriptive social norm information, are delivered by in‐group members and that support is provided to enable the public to perform the requested behaviours.

1. INTRODUCTION

There is currently no effective medical cure for COVID‐19, and sufficient vaccination to achieve collective immunity is a long‐term project. This means that rapid mass behaviour change is necessary to tackle the pandemic by ‘flattening the curve’ of infections and preventing medical services from being overwhelmed. Widespread behaviour change may also form the basis of eradication or ‘COVID‐zero’ strategies. This ‘new normal’ includes the adoption of behaviours such as maintaining physical distance from people outside your household, regular handwashing or sanitising, the wearing of face masks, uptake of a vaccine when it is offered, and complying with test and trace guidelines to quell localised flare‐ups. Physical distancing and quarantine rules are particularly challenging due to the impact they have on employment, travel and social interaction.

There are two main approaches that authorities can use to try to change public behaviour. The first is instrumental compliance which involves commanding behaviour change and expecting obedience through the fear of punishment. However, this strategy may not produce sustainable behaviour change if people only follow guidance when they are visible to authority, and if the public resist commands which are seen as unreasonable or damaging to the good of their social groups (Bonell et al., 2020; Haslam et al., 2014; Jackson, Posch, et al., 2020; Mooijman et al., 2017; Van Bavel et al., 2020). Such change does not result from the internalisation of new ways of behaving into the self‐concept and depends upon external forces or ‘nudges’ (see Mols et al., 2015).

Instead, this paper will focus on the second approach of normative compliance, where the public are persuaded that appeals for protective behaviours benefit their social group and are supported by fellow group members. Interventions may have their greatest effect through the indirect influence of group members rather than the original message (Bond et al., 2012; van Bavel et al., 2020). We therefore argue that the key to mass behaviour change and influence is an understanding of social norms and how these relate to social identities. This paper will discuss what social norms are, how they are linked to social identities, how they shape behaviour and how those in leadership roles can encourage social norm change with reference to the COVID‐19 pandemic.

2. SOCIAL NORMS AND SOCIAL IDENTITIES

Social norms are rules or standards for behaviour that serve as guides for people's actions, help create expectations about how others will act and promote greater coordination in social life (Smith, 2020). Norms can operate at many levels, ranging from the personal to the collective. At the personal level, people have beliefs about the kinds of behaviour that they should (or should not) engage in; these are often referred to as personal or moral norms (e.g., Bicchieri, 2006). Similarly, societies have beliefs about behaviours that are prescribed or proscribed (e.g., ‘golden rules’, taboos). At the intermediate level, groups, such as organisations or families, will also be associated with specific norms: beliefs about how group members should behave and do behave.

Norms can emerge during group interaction as group members are exposed to the opinions or observe the actions of fellow group members. In one of the classic studies in social psychology, Sherif (1936) demonstrated that groups spontaneously generate their own norms and frames of reference when making judgements about ambiguous stimuli (i.e., how much a point of light in a darkened room moves). Once established, this norm becomes adopted by individuals as their own personal frame of reference, maintained even when the person is no longer in the physical presence of other group members. Groups can then use these prior frames to make sense of new events (Moscovici, 1988). For example, when information emerged about a novel coronavirus in January 2020, there was a lot of uncertainty about the virus and its impact, and people looked to others within their social groups for guidance on how to react. In the United States, Democrats and Republicans came to view the threat of the virus very differently as they converged on attitudes and beliefs that were seen to be consistent with the norms of each group (Crimston & Silvanathan, 2020; Gollwitzer et al., 2020; Pew Research Center, 2020), with downstream consequences for engagement in protective actions (Allcott et al., 2020). A similar picture was seen in the United Kingdom with groups' divergent sense of trust in science and public health behaviour (Maher et al., 2020).

Conformity researchers argue that influence arises through two processes: informational influence or normative influence (see e.g., Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). Informational influence (i.e., the need to be ‘right’) occurs when people use the attitudes or actions of those in their social groups as cues to the correct behaviour. Normative influence in this narrower sense (i.e., the need to be ‘liked’) occurs when people conform in order to be accepted by others within their social group. Thus, in the absence of clear laws, a person might wear a face mask either because the fact that others are wearing masks convinces them that it is the right thing to do to reduce the risk of transmission (i.e., informational influence) or because they do not want to stand out negatively by not wearing a face mask (i.e., normative influence).

While early psychology studies demonstrated the power of social groups for shaping behaviour, many people (researchers, politicians, lay people) interpreted the findings in a negative way. That is, groups were to be avoided and their influence resisted. It is easy to see why this was the case: many of the classic studies in social psychology, conducted in the aftermath of World War 2 and the Holocaust, were understandably focused on explaining the dynamics of tyranny and how group processes could produce horrific outcomes (e.g., Haney et al., 1973; Milgram, 1963). However, groups themselves are now not regarded as bad or good psychologically per se. Rather, it is the content of the groups (e.g., their norms, shared beliefs, values) that shape whether social influence will lead to pro‐social outcomes (e.g., the emergence of mutual aid groups) or anti‐social outcomes (e.g., ignoring test and trace guidelines) (Neville & Reicher, 2020).

A dominant framework for understanding how social norms form, change and operate is the social identity approach (Reicher et al., 2010; Spears, 2021), developed in social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and self‐categorisation theory (Turner et al., 1987; see Hornsey, 2008, for a review). The social identity approach proposes that belonging to social groups provides individuals with a definition of the group (i.e., a social identity), and a description and prescription of what is involved in being a group member (i.e., the group's norms). When an identity is activated or salient, people will define themselves and behave accordingly. For example, we may think of ourselves and act in one way at work, as workers, and another way at home, as family members. However, people do not have one single social identity. Individuals belong to multiple groups, such as family groups, neighbourhoods, workplaces or nations, each with its own set of norms. Because norms are tied to social identities, as social relations and definitions of who ‘we’ are shift, so does what ‘we’ approve of and what ‘we’ do. For example, a Scottish man may be likely to wear a mask when his Scottish identity is salient and the category definition of being Scottish includes protecting community members during the crisis (Scottish Government, 2020). In comparison, if his gender identity is salient then it could reduce mask‐wearing due to norms of masculinity (Capraro & Barcelo, 2020; Levita, 2020). Thus, one aspect of effective normative influence is to attempt to ensure that the relevant identities whose norms support the behaviour are salient at the time of the decision.

In thinking about social norms, it is important to note that norms have both injunctive elements (i.e., defining what should be done) and descriptive elements (i.e., defining what is done; see Cialdini et al., 1990, 1991). Injunctive (or prescriptive) norms refer to beliefs about what action is (dis)approved of and motivate conformity primarily because of the social rewards or punishments associated with performance of the behaviour. Descriptive norms describe what is typical or normal behaviour and motivate conformity primarily by providing information about what action is likely to be effective and adaptive in a specific context. Although injunctive and descriptive norms are often aligned, such that what is approved of is typically done (and vice versa; see Eriksson et al., 2015), this is not always the case. Indeed, attempts to change people's behaviour are often prompted by the fact that injunctive norms (e.g., ‘we should not stockpile food’) and perceived descriptive norms (e.g., ‘I've seen empty supermarket shelves on social media’) are not aligned.

In response, behaviour change agents sometimes attempt to communicate that injunctive and descriptive norms are in conflict, in the hope that raising awareness of this inconsistency will motivate people to engage in the desired behaviour (see Cialdini, 2003). However, research has shown that communicating that people are not ‘practising what they preach’ can undermine engagement in the desired behaviour: a so‐called ‘boomerang’ or ‘backlash’ effect (see Smith et al., 2018; Smith & Louis, 2008). For example, if a government message or newspaper article argues that group members should physically distance but is accompanied by images of crowds apparently not complying with this advice, then this is likely to backfire since the descriptive norm (people are not physically distancing) contradicts the injunctive norm (people should physically distance; Steffens, 2020). Moreover, when social norms are aligned such that group members believe others are performing the necessary costly behaviours, free‐rider theories suggest this may lead to slacking (Olson, 1971). However, this may be overcome by making sure norms are defined in collective terms such that people define their identities as members of a social group rather than as individuals (as is often assumed by free‐rider theories).

It is important to note that social norms are not static intra‐psychic entities. Instead, they are the product of intra‐ and inter‐group interaction such that they are continuously debated, regulated and enforced as groups evolve and social context changes (Smith et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2019). When a group member's beliefs or behaviour deviate from the descriptive or injunctive norms of the group, these can be challenged by fellow group members. This ‘self‐regulation’ or ‘self‐policing’ is more likely to be accepted and effective when it comes from within the group rather than from outgroup members such as those in authority (Hornsey et al., 2002; Reicher & Stott, 2020; Stott et al., 2012).

Overall, then, a large body of research has confirmed the power of social norms to determine the form and direction of group members' attitudes and actions, particularly those individuals strongly attached to the group, across many behavioural domains (see J. R. Smith & Louis, 2009, for a review). Norms provide powerful meaning‐makers for how we should act and feel towards others. However, group members' perceptions of what the norms are can be inaccurate. For example, research has shown that university students consistently over‐estimate both the level of approval for excessive drinking and the level of excessive drinking among their peers, and that students' own drinking levels are influenced by these misperceptions (e.g., Prentice & Miller, 1993). Students then drink more to bring their behaviour into line with the (misperceived) social norm. A cross‐sectional study has shown that students believe they wash their hands more frequently than their classmates in illness prevention contexts, and this peer perception correlates with their own behaviour (Dickie et al., 2018). Of most relevance, a recent survey reported that 92% of people graded their own compliance with COVID‐19 restrictions as greater than the population average (Fancourt et al., 2020).

Research on social norms and behaviour change has shown that it is possible (albeit sometimes difficult) to change behaviour with clear messages that reinforce positive norms and correct misperceptions (see e.g., Tankard & Paluck, 2016; cf., Foxcroft et al., 2015), particularly if those messages invoke a shared social identity (see Reynolds et al., 2015). It is also possible to leverage small changes for the better within the group, by highlighting dynamic or change norms (‘A growing minority are doing x’) which have been shown to create normative influence (Mortensen et al., 2019; Sparkman & Walton, 2017). However, to secure long‐term change, it is important to appeal to people's membership in valued groups and to change individual behaviour through changing social norms. This can be achieved through effective social identity‐based leadership.

3. LEADERSHIP AND FOLLOWERSHIP

According to the social identity approach to leadership (see Haslam et al., 2020), leaders gain and maintain influence by shaping social norms: clarifying group members' understanding of what the group does (or does not) stand for, and defining core values, ideals and behaviours. Leaders can do this because they are seen to be representative (or prototypical) of the group (‘they are one of us’): they are seen to embody the attributes that characterise a group and make it distinct from other groups. Leaders are often given leeway to push the group in new directions (e.g., Abrams et al., 2013; Hollander, 1958), particularly if followers perceive that any change is in the best interests of the group (i.e., that leaders are ‘doing it for us’; Steffens et al., 2014). Thus, leaders are in a unique position to change followers' attitudes and actions. Aside from the importance of providing clear and specific guidance about actions to take (see Bonell et al., 2020; Carter et al., 2016), one way that leaders can encourage particular actions is by crafting a sense of who ‘we’ are (e.g., ‘we stay at home to protect our neighbours and the health service’) and embedding these definitions in structures, rituals and practices (e.g., weekly ‘clap for carers’ to thank frontline health workers). For example, Prime Minister of New Zealand, Jacinda Ardern, framed the national category definition to encourage adherence to lockdown measures, collective resilience and mutual aid: ‘For now, I ask that New Zealand does what we do so well. We are a country that is creative, practical, and community minded. We may not have experienced anything like this in our lifetimes, but we know how to rally and we know how to look after one another, and right now what could be more important than that’ (Vignoles et al., in press).

Moreover, a critical task for leaders is to create and facilitate a sense of shared identity. Leaders can do this in the language they use to engage with followers. For example, Steffens and Haslam (2013) found that politicians who won elections used collective pronouns (i.e., ‘we’) two times as often as politicians who lost elections. If invoking a sense of shared identity influences whether one becomes a leader in the first place, it follows that leaders can strengthen their messages and encourage compliance by crafting these messages in ways that invoke group membership and social norms. By doing so, leaders can foster changes in social norms and group behaviour. Indeed, as Haslam (2020) points out, lauded speeches which encouraged behaviour change during the pandemic made by the Queen and Jurgen Klopp (Liverpool FC manager) used collective pronouns every 19 and 22 words, respectively.

However, ‘we‐ness’ is hard to achieve without a sense of trust in leadership. Trust is a key determinant of whether leaders can secure compliance with their policies (Jimenez & Iyer, 2016; Schmelz, 2021) and engender ‘engaged followership’ (Haslam et al., 2020). If leaders are seen to place themselves above the group, it undermines the sense of shared identity that leaders rely on to lead effectively. Group members trust their leader(s) to the extent that they are seen to be ‘one of us’ who are ‘doing it for us’. For example, Angela Merkel, Chancellor of Germany, set the example she wanted her country to follow by self‐isolating after being in contact with someone who tested positive for COVID‐19 (Bennhold & Eddy, 2020).

On the other hand, if leaders are regarded as outside the social group then shared identity is undermined, and trust and therefore influence in shaping public norms are compromised (Haslam, 2020). This point was demonstrated by public reaction to the news that the UK Prime Minister Boris Johnson's chief adviser, Dominic Cummings, broke lockdown restrictions during the height of the crisis. Johnson's refusal to sack his adviser was seen by some to communicate different rules for the public and those in power. This was associated with a drop in public trust in the government to provide accurate information on the pandemic (19% decrease over 6 weeks; Fletcher et al., 2020), and so provided a warrant for people to resist lockdown restrictions (e.g., British Future, 2020; Mahese, 2020). It is important to note, however, that normative influence occurs through shared social identification. Thus, if the public perceived Cummings—and by association the UK government—to be part of their in‐group then this could discourage lockdown adherence as non‐compliance became seen as normative. However, those who saw Cummings and the government as outgroup were more likely to comply with restrictions, as his behaviour was regarded as anti‐normative. In this sense Cummings became an ‘anti‐role model’ (Jackson, Bradford, et al., 2020). When respondents were presented with vignettes about deviation from norms related to COVID‐19 guidance by either in‐group members (fellow nationals) or outgroup members (other national groups), it resulted in negative emotions towards the norm deviators in both groups, and support for retributive measures (stronger towards outgroup members) (Van Assche et al., 2020). However, in the United Kingdom at least, the majority of the public were remarkably quick to adhere to protective health guidance that required new normative behaviours (Drury et al. 2020). The need for state enforcement of compliance with guidance appears to have been minimal. Between the 27 March and 14 May 2020, the police arrested only 36 people in London for breaches of the COVID‐19 legislation (Strategy and Governance (Data Office) and MO6 Covid‐19 Briefing Cell, 2020). In a UK survey of compliance with guidance, 92% of people self‐reported that they had stayed 2 m away from other people when outside their home, and 90% were handwashing more often for 20 s (Duffy, 2020). Even accounting for possible social desirability effects in this type of survey (Daoust et al., 2020), overall public willingness to comply with guidance seems high and examples of non‐compliance are generally related to structural inequalities that prohibit people from complying, such as needing to leave home to earn money due to financial instability (Atchison et al., 2020).

4. SOCIAL NORMS AND SOCIAL SUPPORT

In addition to promoting health behaviours, being part of groups also has numerous health benefits that can help in times of crises such as the COVID‐19 pandemic. Social identities can protect members from stressful situations and improve well‐being because they provide the basis for social support (Haslam et al., 2005, 2009; Jetten et al., 2012). Facing a common fate in a time of crisis can generate or strengthen a sense of shared identity amongst people, which in turn can lead to increased collective resilience and acts of solidarity (Drury et al, 2016, 2019; Neville et al., 2020; Ntontis et al., 2019). Indeed, we are more likely to provide help to those who are seen as in‐group members (Levine et al., 2005), and so a sense of being ‘all in this together’ can lead to social norms of mutual aid and support. This has been evident during the pandemic. One of the most striking elements of the public's response to COVID‐19 is how communities have collectively coordinated support to facilitate compliance with norms of protective health measures, such as distributing medical resources and food to enable vulnerable people to stay at home (for examples, see Covid‐19 Mutual Aid UK, 2020).

5. SOCIAL NORMS AS A RISK FACTOR FOR INFECTION

The high adherence to these new social norms is particularly striking because the COVID‐19 pandemic has required people to go against behaviours that were previously normative, such as being physically co‐present with in‐group members. Maintaining physical distance from our in‐groups—including friends and family—represents a unique challenge because people feel safe and take joy from being close to in‐group members (Alnabulsi & Drury, 2014; Hopkins et al., 2016; Neville et al., 2020; Neville & Reicher, 2011; Novelli et al., 2013; Templeton et al., 2018, 2020). Cruwys et al. (2020) argue that during the COVID‐19 pandemic public health messages should explicitly state the risk associated with being in physical contact with those we care for most and frame the lack of physical contact as an expression of care. Thus, the norms change to keeping away from fellow group members as a way of providing support to the group.

In addition to physical distance, shared group membership can attenuate health risks in other ways such as through reduced disgust and risk perception toward in‐group members (Khazaie & Khan, 2019; Neville et al., 2020; Reicher et al., 2016). Social norm content can also be a catalyst for increased risk taking which may lead to infection. Examples of this from religious festivals include shaving one's head with the same razor as other pilgrims (Hopkins & Reicher, 2017) or being encouraged to bathe in cold polluted waters as part of a ritual (Hopkins et al. 2018). Leadership and public health messages during pandemics must therefore re‐define the normative content of relevant social identities, such that complying with guidance is seen as for the protection and good of the group rather than a violation of pre‐existing social norms.

6. POLARISATION AND PROTEST

A key challenge facing norm compliance in the COVID‐19 pandemic is acknowledging and working with existing identity definitions and norms which otherwise might conflict with engagement in new protective health behaviours (Livingstone et al., 2011). Identity definitions which are incongruent with safe behaviours—for example, around burial practices—can provide obstacles to public uptake of safe norms and lead to conflict with the authorities who try to impose the new measures (Stott & Radburn, 2020). Similarly, leaders risk alienating subgroups when they create norms that cannot be followed by all of society.

The ease and maintenance of norm adoption is partially determined by patterns of societal inequality. For example, despite high public willingness to stay home and self‐isolate, guidance such as staying home is difficult for those from low socio‐economic backgrounds who need to leave home for financial reasons (Atchison et al., 2020). Guidance to distance from vulnerable elderly people is particularly difficult for Black, Asian and Minority Ethnic populations to follow because they are more likely to live in multigenerational households (Loftquist, 2012) and in densely populated areas (Jackson et al., 2000). Failure by leaders to address inequalities and provide populations with the support needed to follow the new norms can result in unrest and protest (Drury & Tekin Guven, 2020; Mazepus & van Leeuwan, 2019). Examples of this can be seen in South America (BBC, 2020) where street protests have argued against the lack of financial support from governments which would allow the public to stay home. Moreover, the differential impacts of the pandemic on different social groups could lead to scapegoating and exclusion of already marginalised groups. To mitigate backlash, facilitate compliance with new norms and encourage a shared sense of unity, leaders must provide support systems that allow all groups to adopt and maintain protective norms (see also, Louis et al., 2020).

7. RECOMMENDATIONS

We have argued that social norms and social identities are critical to the mass behaviour change necessary to tackle the COVID‐19 pandemic. To summarise, we will finish with five recommendations for how to harness the power of social norms to achieve this goal.

  • (1)

    Public health messages should clearly define who the target group is (i.e., what is their social identity, Livingstone, 2020). This should be an identity which is inclusive enough to be relevant to large numbers of people (e.g., the nation) and a social group with which people identify

  • (2)

    Behavioural change should be framed as identity‐affirming rather than identity‐contradictory (Livingstone, 2020). The best way of achieving this is often through invoking higher‐order group values (e.g., as a community we look out for and support each other) which then relate to specifically requested behaviours (e.g., physical distancing) (Drury et al., 2020)

  • (3)

    Public health messages should include both injunctive and descriptive norm information, and these should not contradict one another (Bonell et al., 2020; Drury et al., 2020). Examples of unwanted group behaviour (e.g., stockpiling of food, failing to physically distance) should not be included as these may unintentionally present a descriptive norm (my group is doing these behaviours) which contradicts the injunctive norm (my group does not approve of these behaviours). Alternatively, this descriptive information could be presented in the reverse direction, e.g., the vast majority of group members are acting in this positive manner

  • (4)

    The source of communication is crucial. Messages should come from people who are seen as ‘one of us’ rather than someone from outside (Bonell et al., 2020; Haslam, 2020; Steffens, 2020). This in‐group membership gives credibility to define what is normative for the group and to engage the followership and compliance of fellow group members. Related to this point, behavioural interventions should be co‐designed with group members—rather than imposed by outside forces—such that the community is involved in the development of new norms (Drury et al., 2020; Stott & Radburn, 2020)

  • (5)

    Group members must be capable of performing the requested changes in social norms. Authorities must therefore provide scaffolded support systems to enable the public to perform the required behaviours, and these must be seen to be equitable to avoid polarisation and loss of influence (Crimston & Silvanathan, 2020; Stott & Radburn, 2020)

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Mark Atkinson and Evangelos Ntontis for comments on an earlier draft.

Biography

Dr Fergus Neville is a lecturer in Organisational Studies in the School of Management at the University of St Andrews. His research examines group processes, including the roles of social identities and social norms in group influence and leadership. With colleagues he was awarded an ESRC grant in 2020 to explore how to harness group processes to facilitate the public response to COVID‐19.

REFERENCES

  1. Abrams, D. , Randsley de Moura, G. , & Travaglino, G. A. (2013). A double standard when group members behave badly: Transgression credit to ingroup leaders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 799–815. 10.1037/a0033600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allcott, H. , Boxell, L. , Conway, J. C. , Gentzkow, M. , Thaler, M. , & Yang, D. Y. (2020). Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. Journal of Public Economics, 191, 104254. 10.2139/ssrn.3570274 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alnabulsi, H. , & Drury, J. (2014). Social identification moderates the effect of crowd density on safety at the Hajj. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(25), 9091–9096. 10.1073/pnas.1404953111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Atchison, C. , Bowman, L. , Vrinten, C. , Redd, R. , Pristera, P. , Eaton, J. W. , & Ward, H. (2020). Perceptions and behavioural responses of the general public during the COVID‐19 pandemic: A cross‐sectional survey of UK adults. Health Sciences. 10.1101/2020.04.01.20050039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. BBC . (2020, 9 May). Coronavirus: Chile protests clash with police over lockdown. BBC News. https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world‐latin‐america‐52717402 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bennhold & Eddy . (2020). Germany bans groups of more than 2 to stop coronavirus as Merkel self‐isolates. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2020/03/22/world/europe/germany‐coronavirus‐budget.html [Google Scholar]
  7. Bicchieri, C. (2006). The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bond, R. M. , Fariss, C. J. , Jones, J. J. , Kramer, A. D. I. , Marlow, C. , Settle, J. E. , & Fowler, J. H. (2012). A 61‐million‐person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature, 489(7415), 295–298. 10.1038/nature11421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonell, C. , Michie, S. , Reicher, S. , West, R. , Bear, L. , Yardley, L. , Curtis, V. , Amlot, R. , & Rubin, G. J. (2020). Harnessing behavioural science in publish health campaigns to maintain ‘social distancing’ in response to the COVID‐19 pandemic: Key principles. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 74(8), 617–619. 10.1136/jech-2020-214290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. British Future . (2020). Remembering the kindness of strangers: Division, unity and social connection during and beyond COVID‐19. http://www.britishfuture.org/wp‐content/uploads/2020/07/RememberingTheKindnessOfStrangersReport.pdf [Google Scholar]
  11. Carter, H. , Drury, J. , Rubin, G. J. , Williams, R. , & Amlôt, R. (2016). Mass casualty decontamination in a chemical or radiological/nuclear incident: Further guiding principles. PLoS Currents, 8. 10.1371/currents.dis.569a83b893759346e511a070cb900d52 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Capraro, V. , & Barcelo, H. (2020). The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID‐19 transmission. arXiv preprint arXiv:2005.05467. 10.31234/osf.io/tg7vz [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(4), 105–109. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cialdini, R. B. , Kallgren, C. A. , & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behaviour. In Zanna M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 201–233). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cialdini, R. B. , Reno, R. R. , & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015–1026. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022‐3514.58.6.1015 [Google Scholar]
  16. Covid‐19 Mutual Aid Group UK . (2020). Community resources. https://covidmutualaid.org/community‐resources/ [Google Scholar]
  17. Crimston, C. , & Silvanathan, H. P. (2020). Polarisation. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 107–112). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cruwys, T. , Stevens, M. , & Greenaway, K. H. (2020). A social identity perspective on COVID‐19: Health risk is affected by shared group membership. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 584–593. 10.1111/bjso.12391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Daoust, J. F. , Nadeau, R. , Dassonneville, R. , Lachapelle, E. , Bélanger, É. , Savoie, J. , & van der Linden, C. (2020). How to survey citizens’ compliance with COVID‐19 public health measures? Evidence from three survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 1–8. 10.1080/13548506.2017.1338736 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Deutsch, M. , & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0046408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dickie, R. , Rasmussen, S. , Cain, R. , Williams, L. , & MacKay, W. (2018). The effects of perceived social norms on handwashing behaviour in students. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 23(2), 154–159. 10.1080/13548506.2017.1338736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Drury, J. , Brown, R. , González, R. , & Miranda, D. (2016). Emergent social identity and observing social support predict social support provided by survivors in a disaster: Solidarity in the 2010 Chile earthquake. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2), 209–223.doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2146 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Drury, J. , Carter, H. , Cocking, C. , Ntontis, E. , Tekin Guven, S. , & Amlôt, R. (2019). Facilitating collective resilience in the public in emergencies: Twelve recommendations based on the social identity approach. Frontiers in public health, 7, 141. 10.3389/fpubh.2019.00141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Drury, H. , Reicher, S. , & Stott, C. (2020). COVID‐19 in context: Why do people die in emergencies? It’s probably not because of collective psychology. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 686–693. 10.1111/bjso.12393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Drury, J. , & Guven, S. T. (2020). Emergencies and disasters. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 80–83). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  26. Duffy, B. (2020). Getting used to life under lockdown? Coronavirus in the UK. The Policy Institute Kings College London. https://www.kcl.ac.uk/policy‐institute/assets/getting‐used‐to‐life‐under‐lockdown.pdf [Google Scholar]
  27. Eriksson, K. , Strimling, P. , & Coultas, J. C. (2015). Bidirectional associations between descriptive and injunctive norms. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 59–69. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.09.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fancourt, D. , Bu, F. , Mak, H. W. , & Steptoe, A. (2020). Covid‐19 social study: Results release 26. University College London. https://b6bdcb03‐332c‐4ff9‐8b9d‐28f9c957493a.filesusr.com/ugd/3d9db5_29a5ae83bcd74eb8a238f75fb2d50735.pdf [Google Scholar]
  29. Fletcher, R. , Kalogeropoulos, A. , & Nielsen, R. K. (2020). Trust in UK government and news media COVID‐19 information down, concerns over misinformation from government and politicians up. UK COVID‐19 news and information project: Factsheet 4. Reuters Institute and University of Oxford. https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2020‐06/UK_COVID‐19_News_and_Information_Factsheet_4_FINAL.pdf [Google Scholar]
  30. Foxcroft, D. R. , Moreira, M. T. , Santimano, N. M. L. A. , & Smith, L. A. (2015). Social norms information for alcohol misuse in university and college students. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 12, CD006748. 10.1002/14651858.CD006748.pub4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gollwitzer, A. , Martel, C. , Brady, W. J. , Parnaments, P. , Freedman, I. G. , Knowles, E. D. , & van Bavel, J. J. (2020). Partisan difference in physical distancing predict infections and mortality during the coronavirus pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 1186–1197. https://psyarxiv.com/t3yxa/ [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Haney, C. , Banks, C. , & Zimbardo, P. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1, 69–97. [Google Scholar]
  33. Haslam, S. A. (2020). Leadership. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 25–30). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  34. Haslam, S. A. , Jetten, J. , Postmes, T. , & Haslam, C. (2009). Social identity, health and well‐being: An emerging agenda for applied psychology. Applied Psychology, 58(1), 1–23. 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00379.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Haslam, S. A. , O'Brien, A. , Jetten, J. , Vormedal, K. , & Penna, S. (2005). Taking the strain: Social identity, social support, and the experience of stress. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 355–370. 10.1348/014466605X37468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Haslam, S. A. , Reicher, S. D. , & Platow, M. J. (2010). The new psychology of leadership: Identity, influence and power. Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  37. Haslam, S. A. , Reicher, S. D. , & Birney, M. E. (2014). Nothing by mere authority: Evidence that in an experimental analogue of the Milgram paradigm participants are motivated not by orders but by appeals to science. Journal of Social Issues, 70(3), 473–488. 10.1111/josi.12072 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Haslam, S. A. , Reicher, S. D. , & Platow, M. J. (2020). The new psychology of leadership: Identity, influence and power (2nd ed.). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hollander, E. P. (1958). Conformity, status, and idiosyncrasy credit. Psychological Review, 65, 117–127. 10.1037/h0042501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hopkins, N. , & Reicher, S. D. (2017). Social identity and health at mass gatherings. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47(7), 867–877. 10.1002/ejsp.2288 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hopkins, N. , Reicher, S. D. , Khan, S. S. , Tewari, S. , Srinivasan, N. , & Stevenson, C. (2016). Explaining effervescence: Investigating the relationship between shared social identity and positive experience in crowds. Cognition and Emotion, 30(1), 20–32. 10.1080/02699931.2015.1015969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hopkins, N. , Reicher, S. , Stevenson, C. , Pandey, K. , Shankar, S. , & Tewari, S. (2018). Social relations in crowds: Recognition, validation and solidarity. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(6), 1283–1297. 10.1002/ejsp.2586 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hornsey, M. J. (2008). Social identity theory and self‐categorization theory: A historical review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 204–222. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00066.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hornsey, M. J. , Oppes, T. , & Svensson, A. (2002). “It's OK if we say it, but you can't”: Responses to intergroup and intragroup criticism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(3), 293–307. 10.1002/ejsp.90 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jackson, J. , Bradford, B. , Yesberg, J. , Hobson, Z. , Kyprianides, A. , Pósch, K. , & Solymosi, R. (2020). Public compliance and COVID‐19: Did Cummings damage the fight against the virus, or become a useful anti‐role model? London School of Economics. https://blogs.lse.ac.uk/politicsandpolicy/public‐compliance‐covid19‐june/ [Google Scholar]
  46. Jackson, J. , Posch, C. , Bradford, B. , Hobson, Z. , Kyprianides, A. , & Yesberg, J. (2020). The lockdown and social norms: Why the UK is complying by consent rather than compulsion. London School of Economics. https://blogs.lse.ac.uk/politicsandpolicy/lockdown‐social‐norms/ [Google Scholar]
  47. Jackson, S. , Anderson, R. , Johnson, N. , & Sorlie, P. (2000). The relation of residential segregation to all‐cause mortality: A study in Black and White. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 615–617. 10.2105/AJPH.90.4.615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jetten, J. , Haslam, C. , & Haslam, S. A. (2012). The social cure: Identity, health and well‐being. Psychology Press Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  49. Jimenez, P. , & Iyer, G. S. (2016). Tax compliance in a social setting: The influence of social norms, trust in government, and perceived fairness on taxpayer compliance. Advances in Accounting, 34, 17–26. [Google Scholar]
  50. Khazaie, D. H. , & Khan, S. S. (2019). Shared social identification in mass gatherings lowers health risk perceptions via lowered disgust. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 839–856. 10.1111/bjso.12362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Levine, M. , Prosser, A. , Evans, D. , & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency intervention: How social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 443–453. 10.1177/0146167204271651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Levita, L. (2020). Initial research findings on the impact of COVID‐19 on the well‐being of young people aged 13 to 24 in the UK. COVID‐19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC). https://drive.google.com/file/d/1AOc0wCPqv2gfFSQ_DVmw12vrqQK01z0V/view [Google Scholar]
  53. Livingstone, A. (2020). Covid‐19 through a social psychology lens. Social Psychological Review, 22, 10–12. [Google Scholar]
  54. Livingstone, A. G. , Young, H. , & Manstead, A. S. R. (2011). "We drink, therefore we are". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14(5), 637–649. 10.1177/1368430210392399 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Loftquist, D. (2012). Multigenerational households: 2009‐2011. American community survey briefs. United States Census Bureau. https://www2.census.gov/library/publications/2012/acs/acsbr11‐03.pdf [Google Scholar]
  56. Louis, W. R. , Thomas, E. F. , McGarty, C. , Lizzio‐Wilson, M. , Amiot, C. , & Moghaddam, F. M. (2020). The volatility of collective action: Theoretical analysis and empirical data. Political Psychology, 41, 35–74. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/pops.12671?af=R [Google Scholar]
  57. Mahese, E. (2020). Covid‐19: UK government’s defence of senior aide has damaged public and NHS confidence, says experts. British Medical Journal, 369. 10.1136/bmj.m2109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Maher, P. J. , MacCarron, P. , & Quayle, M. (2020). Mapping public health responses with attitude networks: The emergence of opinion‐based groups in the UK's early COVID‐19 response phase. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(3), 641–652. 10.1111/bjso.12396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mazepus, H. , & van Leeuwan, F. (2019). Fairness matters when responding to disasters: An experimental study of government legitimacy. Governance: An International Journal of Policy, Administration, and Institutions, 33(3), 621–637. 10.1111/gove.12440 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0040525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mooijman, M. , van Dijk, W. W. , Van Dijk, E. , & Ellemers, N. (2017). On sanction‐goal justifications: How and why deterrence justifications undermine rule compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(4), 577. 10.1037/pspi0000084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mols, F. , Haslam, S. A. , Jetten, J. , & Steffens, N. K. (2015). Why a nudge is not enough: A social identity critique of governance by stealth. European Journal of Political Research, 54, 81–98. 10.1111/1475-6765.12073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mortensen, C. R. , Neel, R. , Cialdini, R. B. , Jaeger, C. M. , Jacobson, R. P. , & Ringel, M. M. (2019). Trending norms: A lever for encouraging behaviors performed by the minority. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10, 201–210. 10.1177/1948550617734615 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Moscovici, S. (1988). Notes towards a description of social representations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(3), 211–250. 10.1002/ejsp.2420180303 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Neville, F. , & Reicher, S. (2011). The experience of collective participation: Shared identity, relatedness and emotionality. Contemporary Social Science, 6(3), 377–396. 10.1080/21582041.2012.627277 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Neville, F. G. , & Reicher, S. (2020). Crowds. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 75–79). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  67. Neville, F. G. , Novelli, D. , Drury, J. , & Reicher, S. (2020). Shared social identity transforms social relations in imaginary crowds. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations. 10.1177/1368430220936759 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Novelli, D. , Drury, J. , Reicher, S. , & Stott, C. (2013). Crowdedness mediates the effect of social identification on positive emotion in a crowd: A survey of two crowd events. PloS One, 8(11), e78983. 10.1371/journal.pone.0078983 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ntontis, E. , Drury, J. , Amlôt, R. , Rubin, G. J. , & Williams, R. (2019). Community resilience and flooding in UK guidance: A critical review of concepts, definitions, and their implications. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 27(1), 2–13. 10.1111/1468-5973.12223 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Olson, M. (1971). The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups (2nd ed.). Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  71. Pew Research Center . (2020). R epublicans, Democrats even further apart in coronavirus concerns. https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/wp‐content/uploads/sites/4/2020/06/6‐25‐20‐Econ‐and‐COVID‐19‐FOR‐RELEASE.pdf [Google Scholar]
  72. Prentice, D. A. , & Miller, D. T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 243–256. 10.1037//0022-3514.64.2.243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Reicher, S. , Spears, R. , & Haslam, S. A. (2010). The social identity approach in social psychology. In Wetherell M. S. & Mohanty C. T. (Eds.), Sage identities handbook (pp. 45–62). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  74. Reicher, S. , & Stott, C. (2020). On order and disorder during the COVID‐19 pandemic. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(3), 694–702. 10.1111/bjso.12398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Reicher, S. D. , Templeton, A. , Neville, F. , Ferrari, L. , & Drury, J. (2016). Core disgust is attenuated by ingroup relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(10), 2631–2635. 10.1073/pnas.1517027113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Reynolds, K. J. , Subašić, E. , & Tindall, K. (2015). The problem of behaviour change: From social norms to an ingroup focus. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 45–56. 10.1111/spc3.12155 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Scottish Government . (2020, May 11). Staying at home now is an expression of love, kindness and solidarity. We are doing it for each other, not just ourselves" [Tweet]. https://twitter.com/scotgov/status/1259935387730038784
  78. Schmelz, K. (2021). Enforcement may crowd out voluntary support for COVID‐19 policies, especially where trust in government is weak and in a liberal society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(1), e2016385118. 10.1073/pnas.2016385118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. Harper and Row. [Google Scholar]
  80. Smith, J. R. (2020). Group norms. In Oxford research encyclopaedia of psychology. 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.453 [DOI]
  81. Smith, J. R. , & Louis, W. R. (2008). Do as we say and as we do: The interplay of descriptive and injunctive group norms in the attitude‐behaviour relationship. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 647–666. 10.1348/014466607x269748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Smith, J. R. , & Louis, W. R. (2009). Group norms and the attitude‐behaviour relationship. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 19–35. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00161.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Smith, J. R. , Louis, W. R. , & Abraham, C. (2018). When and how does normative feedback reduce intentions to drink irresponsibly? An experimental investigation. Addiction Research & Theory, 26, 256–266. 10.1080/16066359.2017.1359572 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Smith, L. G. E. , Thomas, E. F. , & McGarty, C. (2015). “We must be the change we want to see in the world”: Integrating norms and identities through social interaction. Political Psychology, 36(5), 543–557. 10.1111/pops.12180 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sparkman, G. , & Walton, G. M. (2017). Dynamic norms promote sustainable behavior, even if it is counternormative. Psychological Science, 28, 1663–1674. 10.1177/0956797617719950 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Spears, R. (2021). Social influence and group identity. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 367–390. 10.1146/annurev-psych-070620-111818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Steffens, N. K. (2020). Compliance and followership during COVID. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 31–35). SAGE. [Google Scholar]
  88. Steffens, N. K. , & Haslam, S. A. (2013). Power through ‘us’: Leaders’ use of we‐referencing language predicts election victory. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e77952. 10.1371/journal.pone.0077952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Steffens, N. K. , Haslam, S. A. , Reicher, S. D. , Platow, M. J. , Fransen, K. , Yang, J. , Ryan, M. K. , Jetten, J. , Peters, K. , & Boen, F. (2014). Leadership as social identity management: Introducing the Identity Leadership Inventory (ILI) to assess and validate a four‐dimensional model. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 1001–1024. 10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.05.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  90. Stott, C. , Hoggett, J. , & Pearson, G. (2012). 'Keeping the peace': Social identity, procedural justice and the policing of football crowds. British Journal of Criminology, 52(2), 381–399. 10.1093/bjc/azr076 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Stott, C. , & Radburn, M. (2020). Social order and disorder. In Jetten J., Reicher S. D., Haslam A. A., & Cruwys T. (Eds.), Together apart: The psychology of COVID‐19 (pp. 93–97). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  92. Strategy and Governance (Data Office) and MO6 Covid‐19 Briefing Cell . (2020, June 3). Fixed penalty notices and Covid‐19 enforcement report. Metropolitan Police. http://www.mynewsdesk.com/material/document/96756/download?resource_type=resource_document [Google Scholar]
  93. Tajfel, H. , & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Worchel S., & Austin W. G. (Eds.), The psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Brooks‐Cole. [Google Scholar]
  94. Tankard, M. E. , & Paluck, E. L. (2016). Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Social Issues and Policy Review, 10, 181–211. 10.1111/sipr.12022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Templeton, A. , Drury, J. , & Philippides, A. (2018). Walking together: Behavioural signatures of psychological crowds (Vol. 5. Royal Society Open Science. 10.1098/rsos.180172.7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Templeton, A. , Drury, J. , & Philippides, A. (2020). Placing large group relations into pedestrian dynamics: Psychological crowds in counterflow. Collective Dynamics, 4, 1–22. 10.17815/CD.2019.23 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Thomas, E. F. , McGarty, C. , Stuart, A. , Smith, L. G. E. , & Bourgeois, L. (2019). Reaching consensus promotes the internalization of commitment to social change. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(5), 615–630. 10.1177/1368430218780320 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Turner, J. C. , Hogg, M. A. , Oakes, P. J. , Reicher, S. , & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self‐categorisation theory. Basil Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  99. Van Assche, J. , Politi, E. , van Dessel, P. , & Phalet, K. (2020). To punish or to assist? Divergent reactions to ingroup and outgroup members disobeying social distancing. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 594–606. 10.1111/bjso.12395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Van Bavel, J. , Baicker, K. , Boggio, P. S. , Capraro, V. , Cichocka, A. , Cikara, M. , Crockett, M. J. , Crum, A. J. , Douglas, K. M. , Druckman, J. N. , Drury, J. , Dube, O. , Ellemers, N. , Finkel, E. J. , Fowler, J. H. , Gelfand, M. , Han, S. , Haslam, S. A. , Jetten, J. , Kitayama, J. , … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID‐19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 460–471. 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Vignoles, V. , Jaser, Z. , Taylor, F. , & Ntontis, E. (in press). Harnessing shared identities to mobilise resilient response to the COVID‐19 pandemic. Political Psychology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social and Personality Psychology Compass are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES