Abstract
Animals use varied acoustic signals that play critical roles in their lives. Understanding the function of these signals may inform about key life-history processes relevant for conservation. In the case of fin whales (Balaenoptera physalus), that produce different call types associated with different behaviours, several hypotheses have emerged regarding call function, but the topic still remains in its infancy. Here, we investigate the potential function of two fin whale vocalizations, the song-forming 20-Hz call and the 40-Hz call, by examining their production in relation to season, year and prey biomass. Our results showed that the production of 20-Hz calls was strongly influenced by season, with a clear peak during the breeding months, and secondarily by year, likely due to changes in whale abundance. These results support the reproductive function of the 20-Hz song used as an acoustic display. Conversely, season and year had no effect on variation in 40-Hz calling rates, but prey biomass did. This is the first study linking 40-Hz call activity to prey biomass, supporting the previously suggested food-associated function of this call. Understanding the functions of animal signals can help identifying functional habitats and predict the negative effects of human activities with important implications for conservation.
Keywords: fin whale, song, food-associated call, prey biomass, ecosystem model
1. Introduction
Animals produce an array of different acoustic signals. These signals can encode various types of information about the signaller's attributes or external environment, and serve various purposes. During the mating season, males of many species produce high intensity and repetitive songs to attract or court females, repel conspecific males, or both [1–3]. It has been suggested that male songs can convey information about the individual's reproductive status, body size or health [4,5] and may be used by females and other males to assess the signaller's quality and competitiveness [6–8]. Numerous bird and mammal species produce food-associated calls. These calls can hold information on type, quality or quantity of food available and be used to announce resource ownership or attract others to the food source [9]. Many species give alarm calls in response to particular predators or predator abundance, thereby informing conspecifics about a threat [10,11], while social contact calls are often used to maintain group cohesion, coordinate group activities and mediate social interactions [12,13]. As animal's acoustic signals play a critical role in their reproduction and survival, understanding the context of production and information content of these signals can give valuable insights into key life-history processes relevant for conservation [14].
Fin whales (Balaenoptera physalus) produce distinct vocalizations but knowledge about the functions of their calls is still limited. The most reported fin whale call worldwide is the 20-Hz note [15–19], a short-frequency downsweep mostly centred around 20 Hz [15]. The 20-Hz call can be produced (i) in regular sequences, forming a stereotypical song [15,16]; (ii) at irregular intervals [20] and (iii) as counter-calls [21]. Songs have only been documented from males [22] and are produced mainly during the known breeding season of the species [23,24]. Thus, it has been hypothesized that male fin whale song is used to attract females, either as an acoustic display [15] or by advertising patchy food resources [22]. Non-song counter-calling and irregular 20-Hz calls are normally produced by animals in groups [20] and probably serve a social function, such as maintaining contact with moving conspecifics [21,25]. Fin whales also produce a 40-Hz call sweeping in frequency from 75 Hz to 40 Hz [20,26,27]. The 40-Hz call is mostly detected in late spring and summer in known feeding areas [26], in association with complex topographical features [28,29] and feeding behaviours [20], suggesting a potential food-associated function.
To investigate the hypothesized fin whale call functions, we examine variation in production rates of song-forming 20-Hz calls and 40-Hz calls with respect to season, year and prey biomass. If males use 20-Hz calls to attract females through acoustic display [15], we expect call production to be mainly driven by season, peaking in winter, the known mating period of the species [23,24]. If, on the other hand, the 20-Hz call is used to attract females via food advertising [22], singing activity will be influenced by both season and prey biomass. Finally, if the 40-Hz call is associated with foraging activity, we predict that calling rates will be positively related to prey biomass, comparable with foraging calls of other species (e.g. bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) bray calls, humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) ‘megapclicks’) [30,31]. To test these predictions, we used a five-year acoustic dataset from bottom-moored hydrophones to extract call rates of each call type. In the absence of concurrent measurements of prey biomass, an ecosystem model was used to provide hindcast simulations of low trophic level (mesozooplankton) biomass for the area and period of acoustic recordings [32,33]. This approach allowed investigating the direct relationship between fin whale vocal behaviour and predicted prey biomass, avoiding interpretation of relationships with time-lagged prey proxies (i.e. chlorophyll).
2. Methods
(a) . Acoustic data collection and analyses
Passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) data were collected at two locations off the Azores Archipelago (figure 1a) using ecological acoustic recorders (EARs) [34] deployed at depths of approximately 200 m. The EAR consists of a Sensor Technology SQ26-01 hydrophone with a response sensitivity ranging from 193 to 194 dB re 1 V/μPa (depending on deployments) and a flat frequency response (±1.5 dB) from 18 Hz to 28 kHz. Hydrophones recorded from March 2008 to October 2012 with several data gaps and duty cycles (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). Despite gaps in the acoustic dataset, all seasons were well represented across the five sampled years.
Acoustic recordings were analysed for two fin whale vocalizations: the 20-Hz call, a 1 s downsweep centred at 20 Hz [15] (figure 1b) and the 40-Hz call, a 0.3 s downsweep from 75 Hz to 40 Hz occurring in irregular sequences [20] (figure 1c). All acoustic data were downsampled to 1 kHz to facilitate analysis. The 20-Hz call was previously analysed from these recordings and for another study [35] by using the low-frequency detection and classification system (LFDCS) [36]. Based on a reference call library of manually identified 20-Hz fin whale calls, the LFDCS detected candidate calls and estimated their pitch-track, which characterizes the frequency and amplitude variation of the signal over time. Each candidate call was compared to the reference library using a quadratic discriminant function analysis (QDFA). LFDCS performance was assessed by comparing detector outputs with manually analysed notes, yielding 0.9% of false positives, 80% of true positives and 20% of missed calls (for more details on the methodology [35]).
Three months (representative of each season) with longer duty cycle recordings (1 h of continuous recordings) were manually inspected to identify song and non-song 20-Hz calls. Results showed that only 2.5% of the files contained non-song 20-Hz calls (Oct: 0%; Nov: 3.5% and Mar: 0%). Thus, we assumed that most 20-Hz calls analysed in this study were part of songs. Identification of 40-Hz calls using automatic detectors is challenging because of the frequency overlap with sei (Balaenoptera borealis) and blue whale (Balaenoptera musculus) calls [37]. So, 40-Hz calls were detected by visually inspecting spectrograms of the entire dataset (2048-point FFT, Hanning window with 50% overlap) using Adobe Audition 3.0 (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA) and annotating each call. The 40-Hz call was identified from its acoustic characteristics [20,26], which clearly differentiates it from the 20-Hz call because of the higher frequencies that downsweep from 75 Hz to 40 Hz over 0.3–1 s. The 40-Hz call was also easily distinguished manually from blue whale D calls, previously identified in this dataset [35], as having a distinctly broader bandwidth and longer duration.
A call rate index was calculated as the total number of 20-Hz or 40-Hz calls detected in a week divided by the recording time, in hours, during that week, to reduce potential bias from the different duty cycles. Hereafter, we will in general refer to 20-Hz or 40-Hz call rates, but these strictly mean the corresponding call rate index.
(b) . Zooplankton model
Stable isotope analysis of skin and faeces indicates that fin whales from the study area feed primarily on zooplankton (mainly euphausiids and copepods) [38,39]. In addition, mesozooplankton biomass derived from a spatial ecosystem and population dynamics model (SEAPODYM) was the most important predictor of the distribution of fin whales in the Azores and across the mid-North Atlantic, while micronekton biomass estimates from the same model had no effect on the movements of the species [40]. Thus, we assumed that zooplankton is the main prey of fin whales in the study area and obtained estimates of zooplankton biomass from the lower trophic level SEAPODYM model (SEAPODYM-LTL) [32,33]. The SEAPODYM-LTL is a spatially explicit ecosystem and population dynamics model that simulates the biomass of mesozooplankton organisms within the epipelagic layer defined by the euphotic depth. The model is driven by physical and biological variables and applies a series of advection–diffusion–reaction equations [32]. Physical variables (temperature and currents) were extracted from the ocean reanalysis GLORYS (https://www.mercator-ocean.fr/en/ocean-science/glorys/), produced with the ocean general circulation model NEMO (http://www.nemo-ocean.eu/), in an eddy-permitting configuration [41–43]. Net primary production and euphotic depth were derived from ocean colour satellite data (http://www.science.oregonstate.edu/ocean). Outputs from these models were interpolated onto a weekly timescale and a spatial resolution of 0.25° × 0.25° to be used by the SEAPODYM-LTL model. The model predicts weekly mesozooplankton biomass on a global spatial grid (0.25° × 0.25°). Predictions for the period 1998–2019 are publicly available (https://marine.copernicus.eu/). The model validation is based on the climatological database COPEPOD that provides standardized mean zooplankton biomass values on a global spatial grid [42,43].
(c) . Spatial scale of data integration
SEAPODYM-LTL estimates of mesozooplankton biomass (hereafter zooplankton biomass) were extracted for the weeks with acoustic recordings (electronic supplementary material, figure S1) and averaged across 0.25° × 0.25° grid cells centred around the hydrophone position. To determine the most appropriate spatial scale (i.e. the number of grid cells) for analysing SEAPODYM-LTL data in relation to acoustic data, the maximum detection range of 20-Hz and 40-Hz fin whale calls was estimated using the sonar equation [44]:
where SL is the transmitted source level (dB rms re 1 µPa at 1 m), TL is one-way transmission loss (dB), NL is the ambient noise level at the receiver (dB rms re 1 µPa) and BW is the processing bandwidth (Hz). Source levels of 20-Hz and 40-Hz calls were calculated using calls localized by three EARs deployed in a nearby area. The propagation range-dependent acoustic model (RAM) [45,46] was used for the calculation of TL. Ambient NL were calculated for the frequency band of each call type and for the quietest and noisiest months within the recording period (see electronic supplementary material, text S1B for more details).
Finally, a sensitivity test of the scale of data integration was performed by analysing annual and monthly patterns of estimates of zooplankton biomass at a range of scales.
(d) . Statistical analyses
Data from summer months (June to August) were excluded because the summer matches the end of fin whale migration through the Azores and whales are rare in the area [40,47,48]; hence the lack of acoustic detections in the summer simply reflects the absence of whales and not changes in calling patterns.
The quasi-Poisson model (a particular case of a generalized linear model, GLM) was used to describe the relationship between 20-Hz and 40-Hz call rates, and a set of independent variables: year, season (according to meteorological definition) and zooplankton biomass. This modelling tool is especially suited to handle overdispersed count variables because it incorporates an overdispersion parameter that allows for more spread than the standard Poisson mean–variance relationship [49,50]. Separate models were built for the 20-Hz and 40-Hz call rates to understand how the same explanatory variables affected each vocalization type. The variance inflation factor (VIF) was calculated for the complete models to measure the strength of correlation between all predictor variables (season, year and zooplankton biomass). VIF values higher than 5 or 10 are considered too high and could cause misinterpretation of model outputs [51]. In our models, VIF values for the three variables were approximately 1. Season and year were used to assess intra and inter-annual variations in the response variables. Given that zooplankton biomass varied with season, an interaction between these two variables was also included in the models. No interaction between season and year was included because seasonality in calling did not vary with year. Detailed descriptions of the statistical models are given in the electronic supplementary material, text S1C.
The best model was selected based on the lowest quasi-Akaike's information criterion (QAIC). Drop-in deviance tests (based on the F-test to account for overdispersion) were computed for the best model to validate each explanatory variable's statistical significance. The process started with the null model, and each explanatory variable was added sequentially until reaching the best model defined by the QAIC criterion. Moreover, the Wald test was applied to each parameter of the model to test the null hypothesis that the respective parameter is equal to zero.
Model assumptions were verified by plotting residuals versus fitted values to check for heterogeneity of variance and residual QQ plots to check for normality. Half-normal probability plots of the residuals with simulated envelope were computed [52] to check whether the choice of the random component of the model was appropriate and identify possible outliers in the data [53]. Additionally, the temporal dependency of the residuals was assessed [54] to detect autocorrelation in the data. An autocorrelation at lag 1 was detected for the 20-Hz call rates, implying there was a correlation between call rates in successive weeks. To account for the temporal autocorrelation, one-week lagged values of 20-Hz call rates were included in the model as a predictor variable. All statistical analyses were performed using the software R (v. 4.0.2) [55].
3. Results
(a) . Detection range and zooplankton biomass spatial scale
Median detection ranges at the deployment locations were 64 km for the 20-Hz call and 18 km for the 40-Hz call (electronic supplementary material, table S1). Therefore, SEAPODYM-LTL estimates of zooplankton biomass extracted for the weeks with acoustic recordings (electronic supplementary material, figure S1) were averaged across four grid cells of 0.25° × 0.25° centred around the hydrophone position (55 × 55 km). Changing the number of grid cells to nine (83 × 83 km) or 16 (194 × 194 km) had little or no effect on the annual and monthly patterns of estimated zooplankton biomass (electronic supplementary material, figure S2).
(b) . Temporal occurrence of calls and zooplankton biomass
Rates of the 20-Hz call increased in autumn, peaked in winter, decreased in spring and were null in summer (figure 2a). Conversely, 40-Hz call rates were low in autumn, increased in late winter, reached highest values in spring and decreased again in summer (figure 2b). Zooplankton biomass showed a clear peak in spring (April–May), decreased throughout the summer and early autumn and increased again in winter (figure 2c).
(c) . Model of the 20-Hz call
Season was the most important predictor of the 20-Hz call, followed by year and one-week lagged call rates (57% deviance explained; electronic supplementary material, table S2). Zooplankton biomass had no significant effect on the 20-Hz call (table 1; electronic supplementary material, table S3). The call rate was significantly higher in winter than in autumn and spring but did not differ between these later seasons (figure 3a). 2012 had significantly lower call rates than all other years except 2009 (figure 3b; electronic supplementary material, table S3). Overall, the model residuals did not show any pattern, indicating a good fit to the data. Most of the residuals were within the simulated envelope (electronic supplementary material, figure S3).
Table 1.
d.f. | Dev. | Res. d.f. | Res. dev | F | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NULL | 142 | 3066.7 | ||||
season | 2 | 955.38 | 140 | 2111.3 | 47.25 | <0.001 |
year | 4 | 601.36 | 136 | 1510.0 | 14.87 | <0.001 |
lag-1-call rate | 1 | 209.92 | 135 | 1300.0 | 20.76 | <0.001 |
(d) . Model of the 40-Hz call
The best model for the 40-Hz call rate included only zooplankton biomass (20% deviance explained; electronic supplementary material, table S2). Call rate increased with increasing zooplankton biomass (table 2 and figure 4; electronic supplementary material, table S3). Although zooplankton biomass varied seasonally (figure 2c), the interaction between these two variables had no significant effect on 40-Hz call rates. Model residuals did not show outliers and indicated the model was adequate to describe the data (electronic supplementary material, figure S4).
Table 2.
d.f. | Dev. | Res. d.f. | Res. dev | F | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NULL | 143 | 197.81 | ||||
zooplankton | 1 | 39.62 | 142 | 158.18 | 25.94 | <0.001 |
4. Discussion
Our study shows that the production of 40-Hz calls in fin whales is positively associated with prey biomass, providing supporting evidence of a food-associated signal, as previously suggested [20,26–29]. The 40-Hz call rates increased with increasing biomass of zooplankton, the main component of the fin whale diet [38,39]. Conversely, the production of 20-Hz calls was mainly influenced by season and to a lesser extent by year, but temporal patterns were independent of zooplankton biomass. This finding corroborates the widely accepted view that 20-Hz songs are used in a reproductive context [15,22] but suggests their function is independent of food biomass.
(a) . 20-Hz song function
The winter peak in 20-Hz calls found in this study is consistent with the known seasonality of the fin whale song in the Northern Hemisphere [15,16,35,56]. The fact that the 20-Hz song peaks during the breeding season of the species [23,24], is produced only by males [22] and is well-suited for long-range communication [57], support the widely accepted hypothesis that fin whale song is a male advertisement signal [15,22]. Similarly, it has been suggested that male fin whales sing to attract females, mediate interactions with other males, or a combination of both [15,22,58]. Croll et al. [22] proposed that male fin whale song could attract females by conveying information on aggregations of patchily distributed prey. In the resident fin whale population of the Gulf of California [59], male singing co-occurs with winter foraging on dense aggregations of krill [22,60]. In this study, zooplankton biomass had no effect on fin whale singing activity, as would be expected if male songs signal food aggregations. Similarly, other studies did not find a relationship between prey proxies (i.e. acoustic backscatter strength) and the production of song-forming 20-Hz calls [29,61]. Thus, findings from this and previous studies do not support the hypothesis that fin whale song serves to attract females via food advertising. Instead, these results are in line with the hypothesis that singing may be an acoustic display [15] directed towards females or other males. Evidence from a range of taxa indicates that songs can convey honest information on singer's motivation and quality which may be used both by females in mating decisions, and by other males in competitive interactions [4,5]. However, there are also species in which song traits do not correlate with male quality [62] and further research is needed to directly test this hypothesis in fin whales.
The effect of year on singing activity was greatly influenced by 2012, which showed significantly lower call rates when compared to 2008, 2010 and 2011. The most plausible explanation is that decreased call rates in 2012 reflect lower numbers of fin whales in the area. This is partly supported by visual observations collected by the Fisheries Observer Program Data showing that fin whale encounter rates (sightings/100 km) were null in 2012, compared to 0.9 in 2010 and 9.2 in 2011. Year-to-year variability in fin whale numbers is usually attributed to environmental changes affecting prey distribution and abundance [37,63]. In this study, models did not show an effect of prey biomass on singing activity. In addition, lower values of modelled zooplankton biomass in 2012 were only found in April and May (figure 2c), the end of the singing season. It could also be argued that inter-annual variability in call rates results from differences in call detectability due to variations in background noise from shipping. Although noise levels in the study area did not vary significantly between 2010 and 2012 [64], effects of shipping noise on call detectability should be investigated in the future.
(b) . 40-Hz call function
This study confirms the temporal separation between fin whale 40-Hz calls and 20-Hz calls [26]. More importantly, we demonstrate that production of fin whale 40-Hz calls was best predicted by zooplankton biomass alone across all years and seasons, with call rates increasing with increasing prey biomass. These results lend support to previous suggestions of a food-related function of the 40-Hz call [20,26,28,29]. In the eastern North Pacific, 40-Hz calls peaked in early summer at known important feeding habitats [26]. In the Canadian Pacific, distance from the shelf break and backscatter intensity (as the proxy of potential prey) were important determinants of fin whale 40-Hz calls [29]. In addition, 40-Hz calls were generally produced by whales in groups, engaged in long, possibly foraging, dives [60] or surface feeding activities [20]. Together, findings from this and previous studies provide strong evidence for the use of 40-Hz calls in a feeding context.
Some food-associated calls are produced only in feeding contexts, with animals adjusting call types or rates as a function of the type, quality or quantity of food available [65]. More commonly, food-associated calls are given in multiple contexts and are not food-specific [9]. Irrespective of their degree of context-specificity, there is increasing evidence that food-associated calls provide receivers with information about a food source or feeding event and often are used to attract them to a foraging site. In many cases, food-associated calling functions to recruit potential mates or kin, increasing the inclusive fitness of callers [66], or to recruit non-related partners and allies, potentially enhancing social status and bonds [67]. Attracting conspecifics to a feeding site may also increase the foraging efficiency of callers, by facilitating prey capture or defence, or helping with predator vigilance [68]. There are also examples where food-associated calls are not used to attract others but to reduce or mediate competitive interactions over food by establishing resource ownership [69]. Clearly, the ultimate function of food-associated calling varies greatly with the social and ecological environment of animals [9].
Fin whales do not live in stable social groups [70] and the distribution of their prey is ephemeral [71]. Thus, it is unlikely that fin whale 40-Hz calls serve to attract kin or social partners, either to provide them with increased foraging benefits or to assist defending food patches. Also, the lower detections of fin whale 40-Hz calls during the breeding season reported here and in other studies [20,26], suggests that the primary function of this call is not to attract potential mates, trading-off food for reproductive benefits. In other cetaceans, food-associated vocalizations have been recorded during cooperative foraging behaviours (e.g. humpback whales [72], killer whales (Orcinus orca) [73]) and may assist with prey herding and capture [74]. With the exception of a single report of fin whales feeding at the surface in perfect synchrony [75], there is no evidence of cooperative feeding in fin whales. Nevertheless, attracting other whales to the foraging site may increase the chances of tracking prey movements, thus prolonging feeding opportunities for callers, as suggested for cliff swallows (Petrochelidon pyrrhonota) feeding on insect swarms [76]. Fin whales often occur in temporary foraging aggregations in our study area and elsewhere [26,47]. Fin whale 40-Hz calls could be used to convey information about the individual location to regulate spacing between foragers, or establish ownership of food patches, as described for other species [69]. At present though, we do not know the functional significance(s) of the 40-Hz fin whale call when produced in feeding contexts.
The recent description of two acoustically tracked fin whales producing 40-Hz calls while moving past each other [27], gives some indication that this call might also serve as a contact or social call. In birds and mammals, functionally specific vocalizations, like food or alarm calls, are often used in different behavioural contexts [9]. Blue whale ‘D calls’ were firstly described as food-associated and social calls because, as the 40-Hz fin whale calls, were recorded during foraging behaviours in feeding areas [77,78] and from whales in groups [79,80]. Later though, one study reported D calls also produced in a reproductive context where two males were aggressively interacting with each other while escorting a female [81]. Thus, it is likely that more functions for the 40-Hz call may be revealed with the increasing research effort on fin whale vocal behaviour.
5. Conclusion
Our study is the first to show a positive association between the production of the 40-Hz call and modelled biomass of prey, providing additional evidence of the use of this call in feeding contexts. Our findings are also consistent with earlier work indicating that the song-forming 20-Hz call is used in reproductive contexts, but the absence of a relationship with prey biomass does not support the assumption that this call is used by males to advertise a food source and attract potential mates. Instead, the 20-Hz song may be a male acoustic display used in intersexual and intrasexual interactions. Our study also illustrates how spatio-temporally resolved simulations of zooplankton biomass, which is challenging to measure in the field, can provide valuable insights into the environmental context and potential functions of baleen whale vocalizations.
Understanding call function and monitoring vocal behaviours associated with the state of individuals or groups (e.g. reproductive status and success, and social complexity), habitat quality (e.g. food resources) or animal density (e.g. call rates) can help identify functional habitats, predict negative human impacts and support conservation planning [14]. Information on the temporal and spatial occurrence of fin whale 40-Hz calls may inform when and where animals engage in foraging and provide important clues to the environmental factors promoting foraging behaviour on this species. Similarly, the 20-Hz song may give unique insights into the location and characteristics of the areas used for mating. Studies combining visual and acoustic observations of callers and receivers simultaneously, offering information on the behavioural context of call production along with responses of conspecifics, could significantly advance our understanding of fin whale vocal behaviour.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Marc Lammers, for providing the EARs and technical support, and to Rui Prieto, Sérgio Gomes, Norberto Serpa, skilled skippers and crew that participated in the preparation and deployment of the EARs at DOP/IMAR. We also thank the reviewers that significantly improved the original manuscript. Data on fin whale sightings from 2010–2012 were kindly provided by the Azores Fisheries Observer Program (POPA) (www.popaobserver.org).
Data accessibility
Datasets used in the analyses of this paper are available in the electronic supplementary material and from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.00000003s [82]. Mesozooplankton model outputs (netcdf format) are available on the Copernicus Marine Service (https://marine.copernicus.eu/; GLOBAL_REANALYSIS_BIO_001_033) and are updated twice a year (next 2021 release will have resolution 1/12° x day).
Authors' contributions
M.R.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, methodology, writing-original draft, writing-review and editing; S.P.J.: data curation, formal analysis, methodology, writing-review and editing; I.C.: investigation, methodology, writing-review and editing; H.M.: formal analysis, methodology, writing-review and editing; P.L.: methodology, resources, writing-review and editing; A.P.: formal analysis, methodology, writing-review and editing; T.A.M.: conceptualization, methodology, supervision, writing-review and editing; L.M.: formal analysis and methodology; M.A.S.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, investigation, project administration, supervision, writing-review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (FCT) Azores 2020 Operational Programme and Fundo Regional da Ciência e Tecnologia (FRCT) through research projects TRACE (PTDC/ MAR/74071/2006), MAPCET (M2.1.2/F/012/2011) and AWARENESS (PTDC/BIA-BMA/30514/2017), co- funded by FEDER, COMPETE, QREN, POPH, ERDF, ESF, the Lisbon Regional Operational Programme and the Portuguese Ministry for Science and Education. Okeanos R&D Centre is supported by FCT through the strategic fund (UIDB/05634/2020). M.R. was supported by a DRCT doctoral grant (M3.1.a/F/028/2015). S.P.J. was funded by EC funds (SUMMER H2020-EU.3.2.3.1), I.C. by FCT through AWARENESS – (PTDC/BIA-BMA/30514/2017). H.M. acknowledges support by CMAF-CIO (funded by FCT, Portugal, through the projects UID/MAT/00006/2013 and UIDB/04561/2020, respectively). A.P. was supported by AWARENESS project (PTDC/BIA-BMA/30514/2017) and UIDB/50019/2020 – I.D.L. and T.A.M. by CEAUL and the LMR ACCURATE project (contract no. N3943019C2176). M.A.S. was funded by FCT and EC funds (IF/00943/2013, SUMMER H2020-EU.3.2.3.1, GA 817806).
References
- 1.Bennet-Clark HC. 1971. Acoustics of insect song. Nature 234, 255-259. ( 10.1038/234255a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Amorim MCP, Vasconcelos RO, Fonseca PJ. 2015. Fish sounds and mate choice. In Sound communication in fishes (ed. Ladich F), pp. 1-33. Wien, Austria: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Payne RS, McVay S. 1971. Songs of humpback whales. Science 173, 585-597. ( 10.1126/science.173.3997.585) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nowicki S, Searcy WA. 2004. Song function and the evolution of female preferences: why birds sing, why brains matter. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1016, 704-723. ( 10.1196/annals.1298.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tregenza T, Simmons LW, Wedell N, Zuk M. 2006. Female preference for male courtship song and its role as a signal of immune function and condition. Anim. Behav. 72, 809-818. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.01.019) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.De Kort SR, Eldermire ERB, Cramer ERA, Vehrencamp SL. 2009. The deterrent effect of bird song in territory defense. Behav. Ecol. 20, 200-206. ( 10.1093/beheco/arn135) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Moseley DL, Lahti DC, Podos J. 2013. Responses to song playback vary with the vocal performance of both signal senders and receivers. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20131401. ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.1401) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ballentine B, Hyman J, Nowicki S. 2004. Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav. Ecol. 15, 163-168. ( 10.1093/beheco/arg090) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clay Z, Smith CL, Blumstein DT. 2012. Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: what do these calls really mean? Anim. Behav. 83, 323-330. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dezecache G, Berthet M. 2018. Working hypotheses on the meaning of general alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 142, 113-118. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.06.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Thorley JB, Clutton-Brock T. 2017. Kalahari vulture declines, through the eyes of meerkats. Ostrich 88, 177-181. ( 10.2989/00306525.2016.1257516) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Marler P. 2004. Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1016, 31-44. ( 10.1196/annals.1298.034) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kondo N, Watanabe S. 2009. Contact calls: information and social function. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 51, 197-208. ( 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2009.00399.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Teixeira D, Maron M, Rensburg BJ. 2019. Bioacoustic monitoring of animal vocal behavior for conservation. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 1, 1-15. ( 10.1111/csp2.72) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Watkins WA, Tyack P, Moore KE, Bird JE. 1987. The 20-Hz signals of finback whales (Balaenoptera physalus). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1901-1912. ( 10.1121/1.395685) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Thompson PO, Findley LT, Vidal O. 1992. 20-Hz pulses and other vocalizations of fin whales, Balaenoptera physalus, in the Gulf of California, Mexico. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 3051-3057. ( 10.1121/1.404201) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Watkins WA, Daher MA, Reppucci GM, George JE, Martin DL, DiMarzio NA, Gannon DP. 2000. Seasonality and distribution of whale calls in the North Pacific. Oceanography 13, 62-67. ( 10.5670/oceanog.2000.54) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Širović A, Hildebrand JA, Wiggins SM, McDonald MA, Moore SE, Thiele D. 2004. Seasonality of blue and fin whale calls and the influence of sea ice in the Western Antarctic Peninsula. Deep. Res. Part II Top. Stud. Oceanogr. 51, 2327-2344. ( 10.1016/j.dsr2.2004.08.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Aulich MG, McCauley RD, Saunders BJ, Parsons MJG. 2019. Fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) migration in Australian waters using passive acoustic monitoring. Sci. Rep. 9, 1-12. ( 10.1038/s41598-019-45321-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Watkins WA. 1981. Activities and underwater sounds of fin whales. Sci. Rep. Whale Res. Inst. 33, 83-117. [Google Scholar]
- 21.McDonald MA, Hildebrand JA, Webb SC. 1995. Blue and fin whales observed on a seafloor array in the Northeast Pacific. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98, 712-721. ( 10.1121/1.413565) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Croll AD, Clark CW, Acevedo A, Tershy B, Flores S, Gedamke J, Urban J. 2002. Only male fin whales sing loud songs. Nature 417, 5-7. ( 10.1038/417809a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ohsumi S, Nishiwaki M, Hibiya T. 1958. Growth of fin whale in the Northern Pacific. Sci. Rep. Whales Res. Inst. 13, 97-133. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kjeld MJ. 1992. Sex hormone concentrations in blood serum from the north Atlantic fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus). J. Endocrinol. 134, 405-413. ( 10.1677/joe.0.1340405) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Edds-Walton PL. 1997. Acoustic communication signals of mysticete whales. Bioacoustics 8, 47-60. ( 10.1080/09524622.1997.9753353) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Širović A, Williams LN, Kerosky SM, Wiggins SM, Hildebrand JA. 2013. Temporal separation of two fin whale call types across the eastern North Pacific. Mar. Biol. 160, 47-57. ( 10.1007/s00227-012-2061-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wiggins SM, Hildebrand JA. 2020. Fin whale 40-Hz calling behavior studied with an acoustic tracking array. 36, 964-971. ( 10.1111/mms.12680) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Burnham RE. 2019. Fin whale call presence and type used to describe temporal distribution and possible area use of clayoquot sound. Northwest Sci. 93, 66-74. ( 10.3955/046.093.0106) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Burnham RE, Duffus DA, Ross T. 2021. Remote sensing and mapping habitat features pertinent to fin whale life histories in coastal and offshore waters of Vancouver Island, British Columbia. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 537, 151511. ( 10.1016/j.jembe.2021.151511) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stimpert AK, Wiley DN, Au WWL, Johnson MP, Arsenault R. 2007. ‘Megapclicks’: acoustic click trains and buzzes produced during night-time foraging of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Biol. Lett. 3, 467-470. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0281) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Janik VM. 2000. Food-related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 923-927. ( 10.1098/rspb.2000.1091) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lehodey P, Murtugudde R, Senina I. 2010. Bridging the gap from ocean models to population dynamics of large marine predators: a model of mid-trophic functional groups. Prog. Oceanogr. 84, 69-84. ( 10.1016/j.pocean.2009.09.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lehodey P, Conchon A, Senina I, Domokos R, Calmettes B, Jouanno J, Hernandez O, Kloser R. 2015. Optimization of a micronekton model with acoustic data. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 72, 1399-1412. ( 10.1093/icesjms/fsu233) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lammers MO, Brainard RE, Au WWL, Mooney TA, Wong KB. 2008. An ecological acoustic recorder (EAR) for long-term monitoring of biological and anthropogenic sounds on coral reefs and other marine habitats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 1720-1728. ( 10.1121/1.2836780) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Romagosa M, Baumgartner M, Cascão I, Lammers MO, Marques TA, Santos RS, Silva MA. 2020. Baleen whale acoustic presence and behaviour at a mid-Atlantic migratory habitat, the Azores Archipelago. Sci. Rep. 10, 1-11. ( 10.1038/s41598-020-61849-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Baumgartner MF, Mussoline SE. 2011. A generalized baleen whale call detection and classification system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 129, 2889-2902. ( 10.1121/1.3562166) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Širovic A, Rice A, Chou E, Hildebrand JA, Wiggins SM, Roch MA. 2015. Seven years of blue and fin whale call abundance in the Southern California Bight. Endanger. Species Res. 28, 61-76. ( 10.3354/esr00676) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Arregui M, Borrell A, Víkingsson G, Ólafsdóttir D, Aguilar A. 2018. Stable isotope analysis of fecal material provides insight into the diet of fin whales. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 34, 1059-1069. ( 10.1111/mms.12504) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Silva MA, Borrell A, Prieto R, Gauffier P, Bérubé M, Palsbøl PJ, Colaço A. 2019. Stable isotopes reveal winter feeding in different habitats in blue, fin and sei whales migrating through the Azores. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6, 181800. ( 10.1098/rsos.181800) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Pérez-Jorge S, Tobeña M, Prieto R, Vandeperre F, Calmettes B, Lehodey P, Silva MA. 2020. Environmental drivers of large-scale movements of baleen whales in the mid-North Atlantic Ocean. Divers. Distrib. 26, 1-16. ( 10.1111/ddi.13038) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bernard B, et al. 2006. Impact of partial steps and momentum advection schemes in a global ocean circulation model at eddy-permitting resolution. Ocean Dyn. 56, 543-567. ( 10.1007/s10236-006-0082-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Masina S, Storto A, Ferry N, Valdivieso M, Haines K, Balmaseda M, Zuo H, Drevillon M, Parent L. 2017. An ensemble of eddy-permitting global ocean reanalyses from the MyOcean project. Clim. Dyn. 49, 813-841. ( 10.1007/s00382-015-2728-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.von Schuckmann K, et al. 2020. Copernicus marine service ocean state report, Issue 4. J. Oper. Oceanogr. 13, S1-S172. ( 10.1080/1755876X.2020.1785097) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lurton X. 2002. An introduction to underwater acoustics: principles and applications. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Collins MD. 1993. A split-step Padé solution for the parabolic equation method. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 93, 1736-1742. ( 10.1121/1.406739) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Collins MD. 1995. User's guide for RAM versions 1.0 and 1.0p. See http://staff.washington.edu/dushaw/AcousticsCode/ram.pdf.
- 47.Visser F, Hartman KL, Pierce GJ, Valavanis VD, Huisman J. 2011. Timing of migratory baleen whales at the azores in relation to the north atlantic spring bloom. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 440, 267-279. ( 10.3354/meps09349) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Prieto R, Tobeña M, Silva MA. 2017. Habitat preferences of baleen whales in a mid-latitude habitat. Deep. Res. Part II Top. Stud. Oceanogr. 141, 155-167. ( 10.1016/j.dsr2.2016.07.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ver Hoef JM, Boveng PL. 2007. Quasi-Poisson vs. negative binomial regression: how should we model overdispersed count data? Ecology 88, 2766-2772. ( 10.1890/07-0043.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. 2009. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Montgomery DC, Peck EA. 1992. Introduction to linear regression analysis. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Atkinson AC. 1985. Plots, transformations, and regression: an introduction to graphical methods of diagnostic regression analysis. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Demétrio CGB, Hinde J, Moral RA. 2014. Models for overdispersed data in entomology. In Ecological modelling applied to entomology (eds Ferreira CP, Godoy WAC), pp. 219-259. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zuur AF, Ieno EN. 2016. A protocol for conducting and presenting results of regression-type analyses. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 636-645. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12577) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Team R. 2017. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Nieukirk SL, Stafford KM, Mellinger DK, Dziak RP, Fox CG. 2004. Low-frequency whale and seismic airgun sounds recorded in the mid-Atlantic Ocean. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 1832. ( 10.1121/1.1675816) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Payne R, Webb D. 1971. Orientation by means of long range acoustic signaling in baleen whales. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 188, 110-141. ( 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1971.tb13093.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Oleson EM, Širović A, Bayless AR, Hildebr JA. 2014. Synchronous seasonal change in fin whale song in the North Pacific. PLoS ONE 9, 1-18. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0115678) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.López MEJ, Palacios DM, Legorreta AJ, Urbán JR, Mate BR. 2019. Fin whale movements in the Gulf of California, Mexico, from satellite telemetry. PLoS ONE 14, e0209324. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0209324) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Croll DA, Acevedo-Gutiérrez A, Tershy BR, Urbán-Ramírez J. 2001. The diving behavior of blue and fin whales: is dive duration shorter than expected based on oxygen stores? Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 129, 797-809. ( 10.1016/S1095-6433(01)00348-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Burkhardt E, et al. 2021. Seasonal and diel cycles of fin whale acoustic occurrence near Elephant Island, Antarctica. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8, 201142. ( 10.1098/rsos.201142) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cardoso GC, Atwell JW, Hu Y, Ketterson ED, Price TD. 2012. No correlation between three selected trade-offs in birdsong performance and male quality for a species with song repertoires. Ethology 118, 584-593. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02047.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Escajeda E, Stafford KM, Woodgate RA, Laidre KL. 2020. Variability in fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) occurrence in the Bering Strait and southern Chukchi Sea in relation to environmental factors. Deep. Res. Part II Top. Stud. Oceanogr. 177, 104782. ( 10.1016/j.dsr2.2020.104782) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Romagosa M, Cascão I, Merchant ND, Lammers MO, Giacomello E, Marques TA, Silva MA. 2017. Underwater ambient noise in a baleen whale migratory habitat off the azores. Front. Mar. Sci. 4, 1-14. ( 10.3389/fmars.2017.00109) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Di Bitetti MS. 2003. Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour 140, 565-592. ( 10.1163/156853903322149441) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Judd TM, Sherman PW. 1996. Naked mole-rats recruit colony mates to food sources. Anim. Behav. 52, 957-969. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0244) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Slocombe KE, Kaller T, Turman L, Townsend SW, Papworth S, Squibbs P, Zuberbühler K. 2010. Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 1959-1966. ( 10.1007/s00265-010-1006-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Sridhar H, Beauchamp G, Shanker K. 2009. Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis. Anim. Behav. 78, 337-347. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Gros-Louis J. 2004. The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Anim. Behav. 67, 431-440. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Whitehead H, Carlson C. 1988. Social behaviour of feeding finback whales off Newfoundland: comparisons with the sympatric humpback whale. Can. J. Zool. 66, 217-221. ( 10.1139/z88-031) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Strand E, Klevjer T, Knutsen T, Melle W. 2020. Ecology of mesozooplankton across four North Atlantic basins. Deep Sea Res. Part II Top. Stud. Oceanogr. 180, 104844. ( 10.1016/j.dsr2.2020.104844) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.D'Vincent CG, Nilson RM, Hanna RE. 1985. Vocalization and coordinated feeding behavior of the humpback whale Megaptera novaeangliae in Southeastern Alaska, USA. Sci. Rep. Whale Res. Inst. Tokyo 36, 41-47. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Guinet C. 1992. Predatory behaviour of killer whales (Orcinus orca) around the Crozet Islands. Can. J. Zool. 70, 1656-1667. ( 10.1139/z92-231) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cerchio S, Dahlheim M. 2001. Variation in feeding vocalizations of humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae from southeast Alaska. Bioacoustics 11, 277-295. ( 10.1080/09524622.2001.9753468) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Canese S, Cardinali A, Fortuna CM, Giusti M, Lauriano G, Salvati E, Greco S. 2006. The first identified winter feeding ground of fin whales (Balaenoptera physalus) in the Mediterranean Sea. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK 86, 903-907. ( 10.1017/S0025315406013853) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Brown CR, Brown MB, Shaffer ML. 1991. Food-sharing signals among socially foraging cliff swallows. Anim. Behav. 42, 551-564. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80239-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Oleson E, Calambokidis J, Burgess W, McDonald M, LeDuc C, Hildebrand J. 2007. Behavioral context of call production by eastern North Pacific blue whales. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 330, 269-284. ( 10.3354/meps330269) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Oleson EM, Wiggins SM, Hildebrand JA. 2007. Temporal separation of blue whale call types on a southern California feeding ground. Anim. Behav. 74, 881-894. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Lewis LA, et al. 2018. Context-dependent variability in blue whale acoustic behaviour. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5, 180241. ( 10.1098/rsos.180241) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.McDonald MA, Calambokidis J, Teranishi AM, Hildebrand JA. 2001. The acoustic calls of blue whales off California with gender data. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 1728-1735. ( 10.1121/1.1353593) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Schall E, Di Iorio L, Berchok C, Filún D, Bedriñana-Romano L, Buchan SJ, Van Opzeeland I, Sears R, Hucke-Gaete R. 2020. Visual and passive acoustic observations of blue whale trios from two distinct populations. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 36, 365-374. ( 10.1111/mms.12643) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Romagosa M, Pérez-Jorge S, Cascão I, Mouriño H, Lehodey P, Pereira A, Marques TA, Matias L, Silva MA. 2021. Data from: Food talk: 40-Hz fin whale calls are associated with prey biomass. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.00000003s) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Romagosa M, Pérez-Jorge S, Cascão I, Mouriño H, Lehodey P, Pereira A, Marques TA, Matias L, Silva MA. 2021. Data from: Food talk: 40-Hz fin whale calls are associated with prey biomass. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.00000003s) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Datasets used in the analyses of this paper are available in the electronic supplementary material and from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.00000003s [82]. Mesozooplankton model outputs (netcdf format) are available on the Copernicus Marine Service (https://marine.copernicus.eu/; GLOBAL_REANALYSIS_BIO_001_033) and are updated twice a year (next 2021 release will have resolution 1/12° x day).