Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2021 Jun 25;17(6):e1008369. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008369

Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking

Maarten Afschrift 1,*, Friedl De Groote 2,#, Ilse Jonkers 2,#
Editor: Adrian M Haith3
PMCID: PMC8266079  PMID: 34170903

Abstract

Standing and walking balance control in humans relies on the transformation of sensory information to motor commands that drive muscles. Here, we evaluated whether sensorimotor transformations underlying walking balance control can be described by task-level center of mass kinematics feedback similar to standing balance control. We found that delayed linear feedback of center of mass position and velocity, but not delayed linear feedback from ankle angles and angular velocities, can explain reactive ankle muscle activity and joint moments in response to perturbations of walking across protocols (discrete and continuous platform translations and discrete pelvis pushes). Feedback gains were modulated during the gait cycle and decreased with walking speed. Our results thus suggest that similar task-level variables, i.e. center of mass position and velocity, are controlled across standing and walking but that feedback gains are modulated during gait to accommodate changes in body configuration during the gait cycle and in stability with walking speed. These findings have important implications for modelling the neuromechanics of human balance control and for biomimetic control of wearable robotic devices. The feedback mechanisms we identified can be used to extend the current neuromechanical models that lack balance control mechanisms for the ankle joint. When using these models in the control of wearable robotic devices, we believe that this will facilitate shared control of balance between the user and the robotic device.

Author summary

The stability of human standing and walking is remarkable, given that from a mechanical point of view standing and walking are highly unstable and therefore require well-coordinated control actions from the central nervous system. The nervous system continuously receives information on the state of the body through sensory inputs, which is processed to generate descending motor commands to the muscles. It remains, however, unclear how the central nervous system uses information from multiple sensors to control walking balance. In standing balance, such sensorimotor transformations have been studied. When standing balance is perturbed, previous studies suggest that the central nervous system estimates the movement of the whole body center of mass to activate muscles and control balance. Here, we investigated whether the same sensorimotor transformations underlie control of walking balance. We found that changes in muscle activity and ankle moments in response to perturbations of walking balance were indeed proportional to center of mass movement. These findings suggest that common processes underlie control of standing and walking balance. Our work is significant because it captures the result of complex underlying neural processes in a simple relation between the body’s center of mass movement and corrective joint moments that can be implemented in the control of prostheses and exoskeletons to support balance control in a human-like manner.

1 Introduction

Most humans are extremely good at standing and walking without falling even in uncertain environments. This is remarkable, given the instability of the human skeletal system. Continuous adaptations of muscle activity are needed to control the relatively high position of the center of mass (COM) above a small base of support. This is achieved through sensorimotor transformations: the nervous system continuously receives sensory inputs, which are processed to generate descending motor commands to muscles [1]. These sensorimotor transformations cannot be explained by local reflexes alone but rely on supra-spinal processes that integrate sensory information from multiple sources to derive information relevant to the motor task, i.e. stabilizing the musculoskeletal system [2, 3]. Indeed, there is evidence that muscles are controlled by task-level feedback from COM kinematics while standing. Changes in muscle activity and joint moments in response to external mechanical perturbations of standing can be explained by delayed feedback from COM kinematics [46]. However, it is yet unclear whether COM kinematics feedback also captures sensorimotor transformations during walking.

Compared to standing, the sensorimotor transformations controlling balance during walking are less commonly studied. Prior studies of perturbed walking mainly described the postural strategies and found that subjects modulate muscle activity [710] to adjust foot placement (stepping strategy) [11] and the location of the center of pressure (COP) in the stance foot [8, 12]. Adjustments of COP location in the stance foot are mainly achieved through modulating the ankle moment and are therefore referred to as ankle strategy [8, 12, 13].

Following frontal plane perturbations, balance is mainly controlled using a stepping strategy. Medio-lateral foot placement is strongly correlated with COM position and velocity [11, 1316]. Furthermore, reactive bi-lateral gluteus medius activity, which has an important contribution to foot placement, is also correlated with COM kinematics [14, 17]. These results suggest that COM kinematics are indeed important task-level variables driving the control of medio-lateral foot placement during perturbed walking [18].

Following sagittal plane perturbations, both stepping and ankle strategies are used to control balance. COM kinematics feedback can explain fore-aft foot placement [15]. However, the correlation between COM kinematics and foot placement is weaker in the sagittal than in the frontal plane [19], which can be attributed to the higher reliance on the ankle strategy. Indeed, when eliminating the ankle strategy through stilt walking, a strong correlation between COM kinematics and foot placement is observed in the sagittal plane [20].

It is currently unclear if, similar as in perturbed standing, delayed feedback of COM kinematics also underlies the ankle strategy in perturbed walking. Consistent with the finding that control of task-level feedback rather than joint-level feedback underlies reactive standing balance [3], we expect that reactive ankle muscle activity and ankle joint moments during walking can be described by delayed linear feedback of COM position and velocity but not by delayed feedback from local joint angles and velocities. In contrast to standing, body configuration and intrinsic mechanical stability vary considerably during the gait cycle and with gait speed. As a result, the effect of a motor command (i.e. muscle excitation) on body movement strongly depends on the phase in the gait cycle ([10, 21]). We therefore expect that sensorimotor gains, describing the strength of the feedback relation between COM kinematics and muscle activity, are modulated during the gait cycle and with gait speed. First, it has been shown that local reflexes, assessed through H-reflexes, are modulated during the gait cycle [22] and with walking speed [23]. Second, reactive muscle activity changes when applying mechanical or sensory perturbations at different instances in the gait cycle [1, 7, 9, 10]. For example, large changes in reactive calf muscle activity are observed during mid-stance [9, 10] but not during early stance or swing [10]. It has however not been investigated whether COM kinematics feedback with phase-dependent gains can explain these changes in reactive muscle activity.

If COM kinematics feedback can explain the ankle strategy and is indeed modulated during the gait cycle, this raises the question which sensory mechanisms underlie this modulation. Cutaneous stimulation studies suggest that sensory information from tactile sensors in the foot modulates reflex gains during walking [24, 25]. We therefore investigated whether tactile sensors also modulate task-level feedback gains and thereby contribute to the observed modulation of reactive muscle activity.

In this study, we investigated the sensorimotor transformations underlying the ankle strategy to control balance during walking. We hypothesized that, comparable to standing, feedback from COM kinematics, and not local joint-level feedback, can explain reactive ankle muscle activity and ankle joint moments in perturbed walking. However, given the changes in the stability of the musculoskeletal system and changes in body kinematics during the gait cycle and with walking speed, we hypothesized that feedback gains will be modulated during the gait cycle and with gait speed based on input from tactile sensors. To test these hypotheses, we evaluated the relation between the inputs of the feedback law (delayed COM and ankle joint kinematics) and the resulting motor command (muscle activity and joint torques) in four available datasets of perturbed standing and walking at different walking speeds in young healthy adults. This linear feedback model is based on previous studies on standing balance [3] and was adapted to allow modulation of feedback gains during the gait cycle. The available datasets contained different perturbation modalities, i.e. support surface translations and pushes, which excited the (neuromechanical) system in multiple ways (Fig 1), thereby guaranteeing the generalizability of our findings.

Fig 1. Overview datasets and hypotheses.

Fig 1

We answered three research questions related to sensorimotor transformations underlying balance control by combining four previously published datasets with motion capture data of unperturbed and perturbed standing and walking. We used data of surface perturbation in standing [26] and walking [9, 10, 27] to evaluate if the ankle strategy is related to deviations in COM kinematics. Balance was perturbed during walking using both discrete and continuous surface perturbations [10, 27] and discrete pelvis push perturbations [9]. The potential modulation of feedback gains during the gait cycle was evaluated using a dataset with continuous surface perturbations during walking [27] and a dataset with perturbations applied at discrete instances of the gait cycle [10]. Finally, we used data with discrete surface translations in standing and walking to evaluate if altered sensorimotor transformations can explain the adjusted kinematic strategies to control balance observed in older adults. In all these experiments combined, joint kinematics and ground reaction forces were measured in 41 young subjects in total.

2 Results

In short, we showed that (1) sensorimotor transformations underlying the ankle response to perturbations of standing and walking can largely be explained by task-level feedback of COM kinematics and not with delayed linear feedback of joint kinematics and (2) COM kinematics feedback gains are modulated within the gait cycle and with walking speed.

2.1 Model for task-level feedback of COM kinematics

We evaluated the relation between COM kinematics and ankle muscle activity and joint moment in perturbed standing and walking. Therefore, we tested whether reactive muscle activity and reactive joint moments could be explained by a linear combination of the delayed deviation of COM position and velocity from the unperturbed reference trajectories.

ΔTa(t)=Kp(s)ΔCOM(t-τT)+Kv(s)ΔCO˙M(t-τT) (1)
ΔEMG(t)=Kp(s)ΔCOM(t-τm)+Kv(s)ΔCO˙M(t-τm) (2)

with ΔTa the reactive ankle joint moment, ΔEMG(t) the reactive ankle muscle activity, Kp(s) the position gain, Kv(s) the velocity gain, and ΔCOM and ΔCO˙M the deviation of the the whole body COM position and velocity. Note that the feedback gains (Kp(s) and Kv(s)) depend on the phase in the gait cycle (s). A neural delay of 60ms was used for muscle activity (τm) and 100ms for joint moments (τT). The delay in muscle activity is caused by the time needed for signal transmission and sensory integration in the nervous system [3], whereas the delay in joint moments is larger due to the additional electromechanical delay between muscle excitation and the development of force in the muscle. The sensitivity of the results to the time delay is discussed in Fig 7.

We compared the ability of the COM feedback model to explain experimental data with an alternative model based on delayed feedback of ankle angle and angular velocity.

ΔTa(t)=Kp(s)Δqa(t-τT)+Kv(s)Δq˙a(t-τT) (3)
ΔEMG(t)=Kp(s)Δqa(t-τm)+Kv(s)Δq˙a(t-τm) (4)

with Δqa and Δq˙a the deviation of the ankle joint angle and velocity from the unperturbed reference trajectory. A shorter neural delay of 40ms was used for muscle activity (τm) and 80ms for joint moments to model the shorter latency of local reflex as compared to centrally mediated feedback pathways.

Feedback gains were estimated from the measured kinematics, joint moments and muscle activity by solving a least squares regression. Inputs, i.e., COM kinematics or ankle kinematics and outputs, i.e., ankle moments were selected respectively at 150ms and 230–250ms after perturbation onset since large deviations in COM kinematics were observed at this time instance for all different types of perturbations (see Methods section for details). Similarly, feedback gains for the ankle muscles were estimated based on COM kinematics and muscle activity respectively at 150ms and 210ms after perturbation onset. Feedback gains were estimated for each subject individually and with the data pooled over all subjects to evaluate if subjects use similar feedback gains. A visual representation of this method to estimate feedback gains can be found in Fig 2.

Fig 2. Least-squares regression to evaluate the relation between delayed feedback of COM kinematics and ankle joint moment in reponse to belt-speed perturbations during walking (Data from [10]).

Fig 2

Deviations in COM position and velocity from unperturbed trajectory (gray), 150 ms after a belt speed perturbation, were used as input in a least-squares regression model. The deviation in ankle moment from the unperturbed trajectory, with an additional 100ms neural delay, was used as output in the regression model(B). The uncentered coefficients of determination (R2) and Root Mean Square Errors (RMSE) of the measured and reconstructed joint moments are reported to quantify the fit of the linear models (C).

Uncentered coefficients of determination (R2) and Root Mean Square Errors (RMSE) of the measured and reconstructed joint moments, as well as muscle activity are reported to quantify the fit of the linear models. Similar as in [28], all quantities and results are non-dimensionalized using COM height during quiet standing (lmax), the gravitational acceleration (g) and body mass (m). COM positions were normalized by lmax, speeds by glmax, torques by mglmax and muscle activity by maximal voluntary contraction values in the standing data and by maximal activity observed during unperturbed walking in the walking data (maximal voluntary contraction was not available in the walking data).

2.2 COM feedback explains ankle muscle activity and moment in perturbed standing

In agreement with previously published observation [5, 29], we found that reactive ankle joint moments and muscle activity during standing can be explained by delayed feedback of COM kinematics. Delayed feedback of COM position and velocity can explain the change in ankle joint moment (R2 = 0.94, RMSE 0.008) and muscle activity (R2 between 0.62 and 0.89,) in response to forward and backward perturbations of different amplitudes (Fig 3 and Table 1). Tibialis anterior activity increased proportional to COM position and velocity in response to a backward directed perturbation and gastrocnemius activity increased proportional to COM position and velocity in response to forward directed perturbations (Fig 3). Feedback gains for the ankle moment, R2 and RMSE values were similar when computed for individual subjects or pooled over all subjects, indicating that different subjects use similar feedback gains (Table 1). Ankle moments reconstructed using COM kinematics were more similar to the measured data than ankle moments reconstructed using ankle joint kinematics (Table 1).

Fig 3. Perturbed standing (data from [26]).

Fig 3

Here we show the relation between COM kinematics and ankle moment (A-C) and the relation between ankle kinematics and ankle moment (D-F) 150ms after perturbation onset. Each dot represents a single perturbation trial with a platform translation in forward (blue) or backward (red) direction with different magnitudes (color tint). A feedback model based on delayed COM position (A) and velocity (B) feedback can explain 94% of the variance in ankle moment (C) in response to support surface translations in forward and backward directions of different magnitudes. The linear feedback model based on angle angle (D) and angular velocity (E) explained only 21% of the variance in ankle moment (F). When analysing the data for each subject individually in the COM feedback model(G-J), we found that gastrocnemius and soleus activity increased with forward COM velocity and tibialis anterior activity increased with backward COM velocity. Each dot in graphs (G-J) represents a single perturbation trial of a selected subject. Note that the electromyography data was analysed for individual subjects instead of pooled over all subjects due to the limitations related to normalizing electromyography data.

Table 1. Perturbed standing (data from [26]).

RMSE and R2 values of the reconstruction of the ankle joint moment and calf muscle activity in perturbed standing with COM feedback or ankle joint kinematics feedback. Joint moments are reported pooled over all subjects (Pooled) and for individual subjects (Subj.) with the standard deviation over subjects (std). P-values are reported of the paired ttest that compares the model with COM feedback and the model with ankle joint kinematics feedback. The results for muscle activity are only reported for individuals and not pooled over all subjects due to the limitations related to normalizing electromyography data.

COM feedback Joint feedback p:R2 p:RMS
R2 ± (std) RMS ± (std) R2 ± (std) RMS ± (std)
Ankle moment (Pooled) 0.94 0.008 0.21 0.028
Ankle moment (Subj.) 0.92 (0.07) 0.008 (0.002) 0.31 (0.15) 0.027 (0.007) < 0.001 < 0.001
Gastrocnemius 0.62 (0.38) 0.004 (0.004) 0.55 (0.33) 0.008 (0.005) 0.12 0.11
Soleus 0.82 (0.18) 0.011 (0.011) 0.58 (0.28) 0.018 (0.015) 0.01 0.02
Tibialis anterior 0.89 (0.14) 0.039 (0.031) 0.58 (0.25) 0.13 (0.11) 0.08 0.12

2.3 COM feedback explains ankle muscle activity and moment in perturbed walking

Similar as in standing balance, we found that reactive ankle joint moments and muscle activity can be explained better by delayed feedback of COM kinematics, than by delayed linear feedback from ankle kinematics, in perturbed walking. COM kinematics explained ankle joint moments and muscle activity across perturbation protocols.

First, we analysed the relation between COM kinematics, joint angles and ankle moments in a dataset with pelvis-push perturbations [9] applied at toe-off of the contra-lateral leg. We found that the reactive ankle moment and muscle activity after the perturbation could largely be reconstructed by delayed COM feedback (Fig 4 and Table 2). Tibialis anterior activity increased proportional to COM position and velocity in response to a backward directed perturbation (Fig 4I) and gastrocnemius activity increased proportional to COM position and velocity in response to forward directed perturbations (Fig 4J).

Fig 4. Pelvis push perturbations walking (data from [9]).

Fig 4

Reactive ankle moment as a function of the deviation in COM position (A) and velocity (B) from the average reference trajectory during unperturbed walking (gray dots), in response to backward perturbations (blue dots) and forward perturbations (red dots). Least squares regression was used to estimate the position (Kp) and velocity (Kv) feedback gains, i.e. slope of green lines (A and B). The uncentered correlation coefficient (R2) and RMSE is represented in pane D by plotting the measured ankle moment as a function of the reconstructed moment based on COM kinematics. In the same dataset, a similar relation between reactive ankle moment and deviation in ankle joint angle (E) and velocity (F) was evaluated. Ankle moments reconstructed using COM kinematics (D) were more similar to the measured data than ankle moments reconstructed using ankle kinematics (G). There strong correlation between delayed COM velocity and the ankle moment (H) is also reflected in changes in tibialis anterior activity (I) and gastrocnemius activity (J). tibialis anterior activity increases with negative COM velocity (i.e. blue regression line), and gastrocnemius activity increased with positive deviation in COM velocity (i.e. red regression line). Note that a representative example was selected for the muscle activity (H-J) rather than visualizing data pooled over all subjects, since this is only possible for the ankle moment and not for muscle activity due to the limitations related to normalization of electromyography data. This is data of walking at 0.6 m/s with the data pooled over all subjects.

Table 2. Pelvis push perturbations walking (data from [9]).

RMSE and R2 of the linear regression between delayed COM kinematics and reactive ankle muscle activity in perturbed walking. Joint moments are reported pooled over all subjects (Pooled) and for individual subjects (Subj.) with the standard deviation over subjects (std). P-values are reported of the paired ttest that compares the model with COM feedback and the model with ankle joint kinematics feedback. The results of muscle activity are only reported for individuals and not pooled over all subjects due to the limitations related to normalizing electromyography data.

COM-feedback Joint-feedback p-R2 p-RMS
R2 ± std RMS ± std R2 ± std RMS ± std
walking 0.62 m/s
Ankle moment (Pooled) 0.87 0.02 0.01 0.05
Ankle moment (Subj.) 0.88 (0.03) 0.02 (0.003) 0.23 (0.21) 0.05 (0.008) < 0.001 < 0.001
Gastrocnemius 0.61 (0.21) 0.07 (0.04) 0.58 (0.18) 0.07 (0.04) 0.61 0.83
Tibialis anterior 0.73 (0.11) 0.02 (0.01) 0.50 (0.09) 0.03 (0.01) 0.004 0.004
walking 1.25 m/s
Ankle moment (Pooled) 0.84 0.02 0.33 0.04
Ankle moment (Subj.) 0.85 (0.08) 0.02 (0.01) 0.50 (0.29) 0.03 (0.012) 0.003 0.006
Gastrocnemius 0.65 (0.21) 0.05 (0.02) 0.56 (0.18) 0.06 (0.02) 0.17 0.19
Tibialis anterior 0.68 (0.14) 0.02 (0.01) 0.45 (0.20) 0.03 (0.02) 0.002 0.05

Second, we performed the same analysis in a dataset with a sudden increase or decrease in speed of the treadmill belts. To compare both perturbations modalities, we only considered belt-speed perturbations that were applied at the same time instant of the gait cycle as the pelvis push perturbations (i.e. around toe-off of the contra lateral leg). Similar as in the pelvis-push perturbations, we found that the reactive ankle moment after the perturbation could largely be reconstructed by delayed COM feedback (R2 = 0.61) (Fig 5).

Fig 5. Perturbed walking (data from [10]).

Fig 5

Gain modulation in response to beltspeed perturbations applied at different phases in the gait cycle. Reconstruction of the reactive ankle moments is better in a model with delayed COM feedback (green) compared to local joint level feedback (R) (C,D). The proportional feedback of COM position (A) and COM velocity (B) is modulated during the gait cycle. The bars and large dots represent feedback gains estimated based on the pooled data over all subjects. The small dots represent gains estimation in individual subjects. The perturbation onset timing in the gait cycle had a significant influence on the position gains, velocity gains, R2 and RMS values.

In both experiments, ankle moments reconstructed using COM kinematics were more similar to the measured data than ankle moments reconstructed using ankle kinematics (Table 2 and Fig 5).

2.4 Modulation COM feedback during the gait cycle and with walking speed

We found that the COM feedback gains are dependent on the phase in the gait cycle and are modulated with walking speed both when perturbations happen at discrete time instants and continuously. The observed change in feedback gains in the gait cycle in combination with the high R2 values during mid-stance(30% of stride) and low R2 values during initial (10% of stride) and terminal stance (50% of stride) (Fig 5) indicates that the control of COM kinematics (1) changes during the gait cycles, (2) is most pronounced during mid-stance and (3) is only active when the foot is in contact with ground.

First, we used a dataset with changes in the belt speed in anterior and posterior direction at discrete instances in the gait cycle [10]. At the level of ankle joint moments, estimated feedback gains (Kp and Kv) depended on the phase in the gait cycle (Fig 6: all subjects, Fig 5: one representative subject). The position and velocity gains are highest during mid-stance and lowest during the swing phase. The variance in ankle moment explained by the COM feedback model (R2 values) is highest during mid-stance (Fig 5). A similar modulation is observed at the level of reactive ankle muscle activity (Fig 6). The proportional increase in Gastrocnemius and Soleus muscle activity with deviations in COM kinematics in response to forward perturbations was highest during mid-stance (Fig 6). The proportional increase in tibialis anterior activity with deviations in COM kinematics in response to backward perturbations was most pronounced during the first half of the stance phase. Remarkably, during mid-stance, calf muscle activity was also inhibited proportional to backward displacement and velocity of the COM in backward directed perturbations (Fig 6). This indicates that both muscle excitation and inhibition are proportional to deviations in COM kinematics in this phase of the gait cycle.

Fig 6. Discrete treadmill perturbations walking (data from [17]).

Fig 6

Representative example of the relation between deviations in delayed center of mass velocity and reactive joint moments (row 1) and activity of the tibialis anterior (row 2), soleus (row 3) and gastrocnemius (row 4) in response to perturbations during walking. Perturbations were applied at four instances during the stance phase of the left leg resulting in eight responses at joint level when combining data of the left and right leg (e.g. right leg is in swing during mid-stance of the left leg). Unperturbed walking is visualised in gray, increases in belt speed (i.e. forward fall) in red and decrease in belt speed in blue (i.e. backward fall). Note that the total joint moment and muscle activity rather than the deviation from the average unperturbed data is shown here to also provide information on the muscle activity and joint moments during unperturbed walking.

Second, we used a dataset with continuous changes in the speed of both belts while walking at 0.8, 1.2, and 1.6 m/s [27]. The main advantage of this data is that approximately 400 strides of perturbed walking could be analysed for each subject at multiple walking speed. To investigate phase-dependency of the reactive ankle moment within the stance phase, we divided the stance phase in 16 bins and computed a linear model for delayed COM feedback to ankle joint moment in each of those bins (Fig 7). Similar as in the discrete perturbations, we found that the changes in ankle joint moments during the gait cycle are closely related to COM position and velocity (R2 up to 0.7 during mid-stance, Fig 8). Similar to the discrete perturbations, the R2 values and feedback gains are low during early and late stance and high during mid-stance (Fig 8). In addition, we found that feedback gains and the variance explained (R2) by the linear regression decreased with increasing walking speed (Fig 8). Note that also for this dataset, the reconstructed ankle moment is more similar to the measured data when using COM feedback than using joint kinematics feedback for walking at slow and medium speeds(p-R2 = 0.005 and p-RMS < 0.001 for walking at 0.8 m/s and 1.2 m/s) but not for walking at faster speeds (p-R2 = 0.75 and p-RMS = 0.59). The average R2 and RMS values during the stance phase are respectively 0.38 and 0.014 with COM feedback and 0.23 and 0.018 with ankle kinematics feedback. The feedback model was not evaluated at muscle level since no electromyography data was collected in this dataset.

Fig 7. Continuous treadmill perturbations walking (data from [27]).

Fig 7

Representative example of the relation between ankle joint moment and COM position and velocity in walking with continuous changes in the speed of both belts. The different columns in the plots are time bins equally spaced during the stance phase of walking. The colored dots represent the different strides of this subjects (with the color tint representing the deviation in COM position), the slopes of the green green lines are the resulting position and velocity gains from the least squares regressions. The change in slope of the green lines show the phase-dependency of the linear regression between COM position and velocity and the ankle moment during perturbed walking. Note that we show the total joint moment and muscle activity and not only the deviation from the average unperturbed data in each gait phase. This to indicate if a specific muscle is active, or the magnitude of a joint moment, in the gait phase during unperturbed walking.

Fig 8. Continuous treadmill perturbations walking (data from [27]).

Fig 8

The COM position (A) and velocity (B) feedback gains describing the reactive ankle joint moment depend on the walking speed. Both the position and velocity gains decrease with increasing walking speed. R2 values decrease and RMSE increase with increasing walking speed, indicating a worse fit of the feedback model as walking speed increases. The errorbars and lines represent the average and standard deviation of the fit for individual subjects, the dots represent the results from the analysis based on pooled data over all subjects.

To evaluate if the modulation of feedback gains is important to describe the ankle strategy during the gait cycle, we compared the model with phase-dependent gains with a model with constant gains during the gait cycle (i.e. Kp and Kv are constant 1). For this analysis, we used the dataset with continuous treadmill perturbations during walking as this dataset contained perturbation data of many steps throughout the entire gait cycle. Due to the lower number of variables, we found that the RMSE increased in the model with constant gains compared to the model with variable gains, especially at the end of the stance phase (Fig 9F). Similarly, the R2 value decreased in the model with constant gains, especially during the first part of the stance phase. However, the average decrease in R2 values (0.08) and in RMSE (0.002) in the constant gain model compared to the phase dependent gain model was small (Fig 9B and 9F).

Fig 9. Continuous treadmill perturbations walking (data from [27]).

Fig 9

R2 and RMSE of the linear regression between COM kinematics and ankle moment in a model with phase-dependent feedback gains (A, dark blue). Relative to the model with phase-dependent gains, R2 and RMSE values increased in models with constant feedback gains (B, red), constant feedback gains multiplied by vertical ground reaction force (C, green) and constant feedback gains multiplied by the COP position (D, blue). Data pooled over all subjects are visualised in color and results for individual subjects are visualised in gray. A similar increase in RMSE and decrease in R2 was found in the three models, indicating that the quality of fit did not increase with information from the vertical force or COP. Results of the statistical analysis can be found in S1 Table in Fig 7.

2.5 Modulation COM feedback during gait cycle cannot be fully explained by tactile information from the foot

We hypothesized that the observed modulation of feedback gains during the gait cycle might be driven by cutaneous information from the foot. To test this hypothesis, we modified the constant gain model to include delayed feedback from cutaneous sensory information represented by either the location of the COP in the foot or the vertical ground reaction force. To evaluate whether this model could explain the phase-dependent modulation of COM feedback, we used both a model in which the constant COM feedback gains were multiplied by delayed feedback from the vertical ground reaction force (Fy) or COP location (COPb, minimal distance between the COP and the bound of the foot).

Ta(t)=Fy(t-τ)(KpΔCOM(t-τ)+KvΔCO˙M(t-τ)) (5)
Ta(t)=COPb(t-τ)(KpΔCOM(t-τ)+KvΔCO˙M(t-τ)) (6)

Modulation of feedback gains by the vertical ground reaction force or COP did not improve the fit (R2 and RMSE) compared to the model with constant gains (Fig 9C, 9D, 9G and 9H). In addition, for walking at 1.2 m/s, the modulation of constant feedback gains with the vertical ground reaction force resulted even in a significant decrease in the R2 values compared to the model with constant gains (p = 0.01, see Table in S1 Table for details).

3 Discussion

3.1 Common sensorimotor transformations during standing and walking

We showed that common sensorimotor transformations can explain the ankle response in perturbed standing and walking. Both reactive ankle muscle activity and joint moments can be reconstructed by a linear combination of delayed COM position and velocity in perturbed standing and walking, rather than with linear feedback from ankle kinematics. Similar as in standing balance [3], we showed that the reactive EMG activity and ankle moments was better described by delayed feedback of COM kinematics across walking speeds and different perturbation modalities, compared to delayed feedback of ankle kinematics. This result supports the hypothesis that the nervous system estimates relevant task-level variables from multi-sensory information to activate muscles to control balance in response to perturbations [6, 18]. The neural mechanisms behind the task-level feedback of COM kinematics remains however unclear. Sensory perturbation experiments, with for example galvanic stimulation, might provide more insight in neural integration of sensory information from multiple sources to estimate COM kinematics.

3.2 Modulation feedback control during walking

We hypothesized that task-level feedback from COM kinematics are modulated during walking to compensate for changes in body configuration during the gait cycle. We indeed found that estimated feedback gains changed over the gait cycle in two datasets with perturbations during different phases of the gait cycle [10, 27]. Both the feedback gains and the variance in ankle moment explained by the linear regression were high during mid-stance but low during initial and terminal stance and during swing. Hence, the ankle strategy is mainly used during mid stance, which is logical from a mechanical point of view. In the ankle strategy, balance is controlled by modulating the interaction with the ground [8]. More specifically, activity of the ankle muscles changes the COP position in the foot to control balance [8, 10]. This mechanism has a high potential to control COM movement during mid-stance since the COP is approximately in the middle of the foot and can move in forward and backward directions. Hence, our results indicate that the proportional feedback of COM kinematics is modulated to exploit this change in potential to adjust the COP position within the foot.

Our results suggest that the modulation of COM feedback gains is similar to the modulation of local reflexes during walking. The similarity in phase-dependency and speed-dependency of COM feedback gains and H-reflex during the gait cycle suggests that modulation of COM gains and local reflexes are related. Both the H-reflex of the soleus and gastrocnemius and the gains of the COM feedback that explains the ankle strategy after perturbations are high during mid-stance [22] and low during early stance, terminal stance and during the swing phase. Furthermore, we also observed that the gains of the COM feedback decrease with increased walking speed and are higher in standing compared to walking, which is similar to the modulation of h-reflex magnitude with walking speed [23].

The sensory mechanism underlying the observed modulation in COM feedback gains during the gait cycle remains unclear. Given changes in foot-ground interaction throughout the gait cycle and the dependency of reflex activity on tactile information from the foot during walking [24, 25], we hypothesized that information from the COP location and vertical ground reaction force could explain the modulation of reflex gains. In contrast with this hypothesis, we found that the fit of the feedback model did not improve when scaling the feedback gains with COP information or the vertical ground reaction force (Fig 9). We believe that there are two potential explanations for this observation. First, this might indicate that tactile information in the foot cannot explain the modulation of COM feedback gains during the gait cycle. Second, this might also indicate that the simplifying assumption of modelling information from cutaneous sensors with linear feedback of vertical ground reaction force or COP location is not valid. In the future, cutaneous stimulations experiments could generate more insight in the modulation of task-level feedback gains during walking [25].

The decrease in R2 values and feedback gains with increased walking speed indicates that task-level feedback of COM kinematics is less important in fast walking. There are multiple explanations for the decreased importance of task-level feedback with increased walking speed. These changes in task-level feedback might reflect an increase in inherent stability of the skeleton system with walking speed. Alternatively, subjects might rely less on an ankle strategy and more on a stepping strategy at higher walking speeds. The ankle strategy can act sooner than the stepping strategy, which has no effect before the next foot contact, might be less important during fast walking [12]. Step frequency increases with increased walking speed, thereby reducing the duration of the swing phase. As a result, the next foot placement occurs earlier and therefore the delay between perturbation onset and foot placement to control balance decreases. In addition, the shorter duration of the stance phase with increased walking speed might limit the potential of the ankle strategy to control balance.

When comparing the different types of perturbations in walking (Fig 10), we found that both the position and velocity feedback gains are higher in the pelvis push perturbations (walking at 1.25 m/s) compared to the support surface perturbations (walking at 1.2 m/s, Fig 10). This might be the result of oversimplification of task-level feedback with linear feedback of COM kinematics. We could only explain up to 80% of the variance in ankle moment with this simple feedback model. Non-linear feedback or feedback from other task-level variables such as COM accelerations or vestibular information [6, 30] might contribute to reactive muscle activity and joint moments in perturbed walking.

Fig 10. Position and velocity gains computed in the dataset with perturbed standing (green [26]), walking with discrete (blue [10]) and continuous(red [27]) surface perturbations and discrete pelvis push perturbations (purple [9]).

Fig 10

The feedback gains for walking are presented as a function of the percentage of the stance phase. Note that the percentage in the stance phase represents the phase at which the feedback model was evaluated (i.e. 250 ms after perturbation onset).

3.3 Task-level versus joint-level feedback

The observation that reactive muscle activity and joint moments were better described by a model with delayed feedback of COM kinematics, compared to delayed feedback of ankle kinematics, does not mean that local reflexes are not important for human balance control. We only showed that our model, based on linear feedback from ankle angle and velocity cannot explain the reactive muscle activity and joint moments respectively 190ms and 230ms after perturbation onset. The reason for this may be twofold: First, alternative local feedback models, for example based on feedback from muscle fiber kinematics or force, might improve the variance explained by the local feedback model. Second, 200ms after perturbation onset, the reactive muscle activity observed in the EMG signals and joint moments could be the result from both local reflexes and long latency reflexes in response to the ongoing perturbations. The high variance explained by the COM kinematics feedback model indeed suggests that the response, at the time instance we evaluated it, does not merely result from local reflexes. Hence, our results indicate that, similar as in standing balance [3], long latency responses reflect delayed feedback of task-level variables and not linear feedback of joint kinematics errors.

3.4 Contribution of reactive muscle activity

The relation between COM kinematics and reactive muscle activity shows that the reactive ankle joint moments do not solely result from visco-elastic properties of active muscles, but result from active feedback control (Fig 4). Based on the relation between joint moments and delayed COM kinematics, it is unclear if the modulation of ankle moment is indeed an active control mechanism. Given that a similar correlation was observed at the level of reactive activity of the calf muscles confirmed the important contribution of active feedback control.

Despite the clear contribution of active control mechanisms, it is hard to identify the relative importance of active feedback control and passive dynamic contributions to the ankle moment. Musculoskeletal modelling could be used to identify muscle stiffness and damping using muscle models [31]. This would enable estimation of joint stiffness and damping. However, this relies on accurate methods to estimate the muscle state from motion capture data (e.g. [32]) and on accurate models of muscle contraction dynamics [33].

3.5 Derive neuromechanical models for balance control during walking

Our findings have important implications for modelling neuromechanics of walking. In current models, balance is mainly controlled by foot placement [15, 34]. Here, we showed that subjects also adjust the ankle joint moment in response to perturbation during walking and that this strategy can be modelled using delayed COM position and velocity feedback. Hence, we propose an additional supra-spinal feedback loop to model the ankle strategy during walking, similar to the neuromechanical models of perturbed standing [29]. Since COM kinematics feedback largely captures reactive ankle joint moment in response to different types of balance perturbations across walking speeds, we believe that this is an important extension to current state-of-the art neuromechanical models. One of the future challenges is to investigate how this additional COM-feedback loop interacts with local feedback control, for example positive force feedback of the calf muscles as modeled by [35]. We believe that a combination of local and supra-spinal feedback to control the ankle muscles is especially useful in the control of wearable robotic devices. We hypothesize that an additional supra-spinal feedback loop will facilitate shared control of balance when using neuromechanical models in the control of wearable robotic devices.

4 Materials and methods

We used existing data from multiple perturbation experiments in healthy young and older subjects to derive sensorimotor transformations for the ankle strategy during standing and walking (see Fig 1 for an overview).

4.1 Experimental methods

Integrated 3D motion capture was used in four datasets to measure the human response to perturbations during standing and walking. An overview of the different datasets and measurements is given in Fig 1.

  • Standing balance was perturbed using support surface translations triggered at a discrete time instance. Integrated motion capture data was collected in 8 young (age 21 ± 2std) and 10 older adults (age 67 ± 3std). The perturbation protocol and data collection is extensively described in [26]. In short, standing balance was perturbed by means of a sudden forward and backward platform translation with respectively fast, medium and slow acceleration profiles (Fig 1), as well as medio-lateral platform translations, but only forward and backward perturbations were analyzed in this study. Perturbations were applied in a random order. Whole body motion was recorded using 3D motion capture with an extended plug in gait marker (Vicon, Oxford Metrics). Ground reaction forces were collected from two AMTI force plates (AMI, Watertown, USA) embedded in the platform. This dataset has already been used to analyse kinematic strategies in response to backward surface translations in [17]. Here, we additionally analyzed the response to forward support surface translations and reactive muscle activity of the tibialis anterior, soleus and gastrocnemius, which was collected using surface electromyography (Cometa).

  • Steady state walking was also perturbed by means of an external force (push) at the pelvis. Integrated motion capture data was collected in 9 subjects (age 25 ± 2 std years). The perturbation protocol and data collection is extensively described in [9]. In short, the perturbations consisted of a forward of backward directed push at the pelvis with four different magnitudes. Forward and backward push perturbations were applied in a random while the subjects walked at 0.62 m/s and 1.25 m/s. Whole body motion was recorded using 3D motion capture with an full-body marker protocol (VisualEyez-II). Ground reaction forces were collected on a split-belt treadmill (MotekForceLink). Muscle activity of the tibialis anterior and gastrocnemius was measured using electromyography (Delsys).

  • Steady state walking was perturbed by means of a sudden change in speed of the treadmill belt, triggered at specific phases of the gait cycle. Integrated motion capture data was collected in 18 young (age 21 ± 2 std years) and ten older adults (age 71 ± 4 years). The perturbation protocol and data collection in extensively described in [10]. In short, the perturbations consisted of a sudden increase or decrease in speed of the treadmill belt with two different magnitudes. These perturbations were applied at four different phases of the gait cycle. Medio-lateral treadmill translations were also part of the perturbation protocol but were not analysed in this study. All perturbations were applied in a random order. All subjects walked at 1.1 m/s on a split-belt treadmill. Whole body motion was recorded using 3D motion capture with an extended plug in gait marker set (Vicon, Oxford Metrics). Ground reaction forces were collected on a split-belt treadmill (MotekForceLink). Muscle activity of the gastrocnemius, soleus, tibialis anterior was measured using surface electromyography (Bortec Octopus 8 channel electromyography).

  • Steady state walking was also perturbed by means of continuous changes in the speed of the treadmill belts. Integrated motion capture data was collected in 15 young subjects (age 24 ± 4 std years). The perturbation protocol and data collection is extensively described in [27]. In short, balance was perturbed continuously using three pseudo-random belt speed control signals, with mean velocities of 0.8 m/s, 1.2 m/s and 1.6 m/s. Each trial with continuous perturbations consists of 480 seconds walking, resulting in approximately 500 gait cycles of perturbed walking for each subject and each walking speed. Whole body motion was recorded using 3D motion capture with an full-body marker protocol. Ground reaction forces were collected on a split-belt treadmill (MotekForceLink). No electromyography data was collected in this study.

4.2 Inverse kinematic and dynamic analysis of the experimental data

Joint kinematics and kinetics were computed using a scaled generic musculoskeletal model with 23 degrees of freedom (gait 2392) in OpenSim [36]. This model was scaled to the anthropometry and mass of the subject based on the marker positions and ground reaction forces in the static trials. Joint kinematics of the scaled model were computed from the recorded marker trajectories using a Kalman smoothing algorithm [37]. Joint kinetics were computed based on the equations of motion of the model with OpenSim’s inverse dynamics tool. The motion capture dataset with continuous perturbations was processed using the open source software to compute joint kinematics and kinetics provided with the manuscript [27]. Whole body COM position and velocity was computed from the joint kinematics and the model of the skeleton (i.e. kinematic approach), and was expressed relative to the position and velocity of the base of support (i.e. foot in contact with the ground).

4.3 Normalization of data

Joint kinematics, kinetics and muscle activity data were normalized to facilitate comparison between subjects and datasets to enable estimation of feedback gains based on data pooled over all subjects. Similar as in [28], COM positions were normalized by lmax, speeds by glmax, torques by mglmax and muscle activity by MVC values for the standing data and by the peak values of the gait-cycle-average activity observed during unperturbed walking for the walking data. We noticed that normalizing the EMG data was not sufficient to have a reliably comparison of muscle activity between subjects, which might be related to the lack of MVC data the in walking experiments [38]. We therefore fitted the feedback model only on EMG data of individual subjects, and not on the data pooled over all subjects.

4.4 Linear regression

We used least squares regression to obtain linear models between the deviations in ankle and COM kinematics (inputs) and reactive ankle joint moment and muscle activity (outputs) after perturbation. We first computed the mean of the inputs and outputs in the unperturbed standing and walking data. Subsequently, we computed the deviation from the means in the perturbed standing and walking data. For example, in standing balance, we first used the data at 0.5 seconds before perturbation onset, averaged over the multiple perturbation trials, to determine the mean of the unperturbed COM kinematics, joint moments and muscle activity. We then computed the deviation from this mean in each perturbed and unperturbed trial. These deviations were used as input for the least squares regression. The same method was used in perturbed walking, but the inputs and outputs were expressed as a percentage of the stance and swing phase. The linear regression was evaluated at 150ms after perturbation onset in all datasets because a large deviation in COM kinematics was observed in all datasets. For the continuous treadmill perturbations [27], we divided the stance duration into 16 bins and computed a linear model for the ankle moment at each of those phases, all with delayed COM kinematics as inputs.

To evaluate the relation between inputs, i.e., COM or ankle kinematics and the ankle joint moment, we performed the linear regression independent of the perturbation direction (i.e. one single regression for both perturbation directions). This decision was mainly based on the observation that the variance explained by a single regression model was similar to the variance explained by separate regression models for each perturbation direction (Fig 4). In contrast, we found that separate feedback gains for the forward and backward directed perturbations were needed to explain the reactive muscle activity (Figs 3H–3J and 4H–4J).

For each dataset, we pooled the data for the different perturbation magnitudes, perturbation direction (forward and backward), repetitions (i.e. trials) to perform a single least squares regression. The uncentered coefficient of determination (R2) (Eq 7) and root mean square error (RMSE) between the measured and reconstructed joint moments and muscle activity are reported to quantify the fit of the linear regression (Eqs 1 and 2).

R2=1-(ymeas-ymod)2ymeas2 (7)

with ymeas the measured ankle moments or muscle activity and ymod the reconstructed ankle moments and muscle activity with delayed linear feedback of COM kinematics.

4.5 Gains normalized by COP position or vertical ground reaction force

The hypothesis that gain modulation might be driven by cutaneous information from the foot was evaluated by fitting one model with constant feedback gains through all data points of the continuous perturbations (i.e. combing the different gait phases). We computed the R2 and RMSE when the feedback gains were proportional to the vertical ground reaction force (Eq 5) or by the minimal distance between the COP and the bounds of the foot (Eq 6) in the fore-aft direction. The bounds of the foot were computed based on the motion capture data.

4.6 Statistical analysis

We used statistical tests to evaluate (1) if gain modulation might be driven by cutaneous information from the foot and (2) if the fit with the task-level feedback model is better than the joint-level feedback model. To test the first hypothesis, we used a repeated measures ANOVA to evaluate if the R value is significantly different in models with constant gains modulated by the vertical ground reaction force or COP position compared to a model with constant gains Table in S1 Table. A Fisher z-transformation was used to transform the correlation coefficient in normally distributed z-scores. Bonferroni correction was used to correct for multiple comparisons in the post-hoc testing. To test the second hypothesis, we also used a paired t-test, with R-values after Fisher z-transformation, to evaluate if the R value is significantly different in the model with task-level feedback compared to a model with joint-level feedback. A two-sided confidence interval with an alpha level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Sensitivity of the time delay.

We evaluated whether the variance in ankle moment explained by the linear regression is sensitive to the time delay. This additional analysis was important to verify that the observed relation between COM kinematics and ankle moment is a feedback control process, and does not simply reflect the coupling due to the dynamics of the skeleton. We did this sensitivity analysis on the pelvis push perturbations and found that the relation between ankle moment and COM kinematics is indeed sensitive to the neural delay. The variance explained by the linear regression was optimal with a physiological plausible delay of 100ms and decreased strongly for delays smaller than 50 ms or larger than 120 ms. Note that time delay in this study was selected based on literature [29] and was not based on this sensitivity analysis.

(EPS)

S2 Fig. Deviations in COM position and velocity in the different types of perturbations.

To evaluate if the magnitude of the perturbations are similar between datasets, we compared the deviation in COM position and velocity at 150ms after perturbation onset for the four datasets. We found that the deviation in COM position and velocity is similar in the different datasets of perturbed walking (B-D) and is larger in perturbed walking compared to perturbed standing (A). A similar deviation in COM kinematics is observed when analysing the data 100ms (E), 150ms (F) and 200ms (G) after perturbation onset in the pelvis push perturbations.

(TIF)

S1 Table. Sensitivity of the time delay.

We evaluated whether the variance in ankle moment explained by the linear regression is sensitive to the time delay. This additional analysis was important to verify that the observed relation between COM kinematics and ankle moment is a feedback control process, and does not simply reflect the coupling due to the dynamics of the skeleton. We did this sensitivity analysis on the pelvis push perturbations and found that the relation between ankle moment and COM kinematics is indeed sensitive to the neural delay. The variance explained by the linear regression was optimal with a physiological plausible delay of 100ms and decreased strongly for delays smaller than 50 ms or larger than 120 ms. Note that time delay in this study was selected based on literature [29] and was not based on this sensitivity analysis.

(PDF)

S1 File. DataBeltPerturb.mat.

Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with discrete perturbations in speed of the treadmill belts [10].

(MAT)

S2 File. DataContinuousPerturb.mat.

Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with continuous perturbations in speed of the treadmill belts [27].

(MAT)

S3 File. DataPelvisPush.mat.

Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with pelvis push perturbations [9].

(MAT)

S4 File. DataStandingBalance.mat.

Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with support surface translations during standing [26].

(MAT)

S5 File. ExamplePlotFigureAfschrift2019.m.

Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the discrete belt speed perturbations.

(M)

S6 File. ExamplePlotFigureVlutters2018.m.

Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the pelvis push perturbations.

(M)

S7 File. ExamplePlotFigureMoore2014.m.

Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the continuous belt speed perturbations.

(M)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the authors of the public available datasets for sharing their data. In addition, we would like to thank Herman van der Kooij and Edwin van Asseldonk for their insightful discussion related to the modulation of COM feedback during the gait cycle and if this modulation can be explained by altered input from tactile sensors.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

MA was funded by Fonds Wetenschappelijke Onderzoek - Vlaanderen (FWO): 12ZP120N. (https://www.fwo.be/) The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Reimann H, Fettrow T, Grenet D, Thompson ED, Jeka JJ. The onset time of balance control during walking is phase-independent, but the magnitude of the response is not. bioRxiv. 2019; p. 655928. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Ting LH. Dimensional reduction in sensorimotor systems: a framework for understanding muscle coordination of posture. Progress in Brain Research. 2007;165:299–321. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)65019-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Safavynia SA, Ting LH. Long-latency muscle activity reflects continuous, delayed sensorimotor feedback of task-level and not joint-level error. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110(6):1278–1290. doi: 10.1152/jn.00609.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Ting LH, van Antwerp KW, Scrivens JE, McKay JL, Welch TDJ, Bingham JT, et al. Neuromechanical tuning of nonlinear postural control dynamics. Chaos (Woodbury, NY). 2009;19(2):026111. doi: 10.1063/1.3142245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Lockhart DB, Ting LH. Optimal sensorimotor transformations for balance. Nature neuroscience. 2007;10(10):1329–36. doi: 10.1038/nn1986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Welch TDJ, Ting LH. A feedback model explains the differential scaling of human postural responses to perturbation acceleration and velocity. Journal of neurophysiology. 2009;101(6):3294–309. doi: 10.1152/jn.90775.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hof AL, Duysens J. Responses of human hip abductor muscles to lateral balance perturbations during walking. Experimental Brain Research. 2013;230(3):301–310. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3655-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Hof AL, Duysens J. Responses of human ankle muscles to mediolateral balance perturbations during walking. Human Movement Science. 2018;57:69–82. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Vlutters M, van Asseldonk EHF, van der Kooij H. Lower extremity joint-level responses to pelvis perturbation during human walking. Scientific Reports. 2018;8(1):14621. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32839-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Afschrift M, Deursen RV, Groote FD, Jonkers I. Increased use of stepping strategy in response to medio-lateral perturbations in the elderly relates to altered reactive tibialis anterior activity. Gait & Posture. 2019;68(May 2018):575–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Hof aL, Vermerris SM, Gjaltema Wa. Balance responses to lateral perturbations in human treadmill walking. The Journal of experimental biology. 2010;213(Pt 15):2655–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Reimann H, Fettrow TD, Thompson ED, Agada P, McFadyen BJ, Jeka JJ. Complementary mechanisms for upright balance during walking. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(2). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Vlutters M, van Asseldonk EHF, van der Kooij H. Center of mass velocity-based predictions in balance recovery following pelvis perturbations during human walking. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2016;219(10):1514–1523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Rankin BL, Buffo SK, Dean JC. A neuromechanical strategy for mediolateral foot placement in walking humans. Journal of neurophysiology. 2014;112(2):374–83. doi: 10.1152/jn.00138.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Wang Y, Srinivasan M. Stepping in the direction of the fall: The next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biology Letters. 2014;10(9):1–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Stimpson KH, Heitkamp LN, Horne JS, Dean JC. Effects of walking speed on the step-by-step control of step width. Journal of Biomechanics. 2018;68:78–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Afschrift M, Pitto L, Aerts W, van Deursen R, Jonkers I, De Groote F. Modulation of gluteus medius activity reflects the potential of the muscle to meet the mechanical demands during perturbed walking. Scientific Reports. 2018;8(1):11675. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30139-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Bruijn SM, Van Dieën JH. Control of human gait stability through foot placement; 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Joshi V, Srinivasan M. A controller for walking derived from how humans recover from perturbations. Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2019;16(157):20190027. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2019.0027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Vlutters M, van Asseldonk EHF, van der Kooij H. Reduced center of pressure modulation elicits foot placement adjustments, but no additional trunk motion during anteroposterior-perturbed walking; 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Neptune RR, Kautz Sa, Zajac FE. Contributions of the individual ankle plantar flexors to support, forward progression and swing initiation during walking. Journal of biomechanics. 2001;34(11):1387–98. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(01)00105-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Makihara Y, Segal RL, Wolpaw JR, Thompson AK. H-reflex modulation in the human medial and lateral gastrocnemii during standing and walking. Muscle and Nerve. 2012;45(1):116–125. doi: 10.1002/mus.22265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Edamura M, Yang JF, Stein RB. Factors that determine the magnitude and time course of human H-reflexes in locomotion. Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;11(2):420–427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-02-00420.1991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Nakajima T, Suzuki S, Futatsubashi G, Ohtsuska H, Mezzarane RA, Barss TS, et al. Regionally distinct cutaneous afferent populations contribute to reflex modulation evoked by stimulation of the tibial nerve during walking. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;116(1):183–190. doi: 10.1152/jn.01011.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Zehr EP, Nakajima T, Barss T, Klarner T, Miklosovic S, Mezzarane RA, et al. Cutaneous stimulation of discrete regions of the sole during locomotion produces “sensory steering” of the foot. BMC Sports Science, Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2014;6(1). doi: 10.1186/2052-1847-6-33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Afschrift M, Jonkers I, De Schutter J, De Groote F. Mechanical effort predicts the selection of ankle over hip strategies in nonstepping postural responses. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;116(4):1937–1945. doi: 10.1152/jn.00127.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Moore JK, Hnat SK, van den Bogert AJ. An elaborate data set on human gait and the effect of mechanical perturbations. PeerJ. 2015;2015(3):e918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Seethapathi N, Srinivasan M. Step-to-step variations in human running reveal how humans run without falling. eLife. 2019;8:1–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Welch TDJ, Ting LH. A Feedback Model Reproduces Muscle Activity During Human Postural Responses to Support-Surface Translations. J Neurophysiol. 2007;99:1032–1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Magnani RM, Bruijn SM, vanDieën JH, Forbes PA. Stabilization demands of walking modulate the vestibular contributions to gait. bioRxiv. 2020; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Sartori M, Pizzolato C, Farina D, Maculan M, Reggiani M. Modeling and simulating the neuromuscular mechanisms regulating ankle and knee joint stiffness during human locomotion. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;114(4):2509–2527. doi: 10.1152/jn.00989.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. De Groote F, Kinney AL, Rao AV, Fregly BJ. Evaluation of Direct Collocation Optimal Control Problem Formulations for Solving the Muscle Redundancy Problem. Annals of Biomedical Engineering. 2016;44(10):2922–2936. doi: 10.1007/s10439-016-1591-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. De Groote F, Allen JL, Ting LH. Contribution of muscle short-range stiffness to initial changes in joint kinetics and kinematics during perturbations to standing balance: A simulation study. Journal of Biomechanics. 2017;55:71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Song S, Geyer H. Evaluation of a Neuromechanical Walking Control Model Using Disturbance Experiments. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2017;11(March):15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Geyer H, Seyfarth A, Blickhan R. Compliant leg behaviour explains basic dynamics of walking and running. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2006;273(1603):2861–2867. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3637 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Delp SL, Anderson FC, Arnold AS, Loan P, Habib A, John CT, et al. OpenSim: open-source software to create and analyze dynamic simulations of movement. IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering. 2007;54(11):1940–50. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2007.901024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. De Groote F, De Laet T, Jonkers I, De Schutter J. Kalman smoothing improves the estimation of joint kinematics and kinetics in marker-based human gait analysis. Journal of biomechanics. 2008;41(16):3390–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2008.09.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Besomi M, Hodges PW, Clancy EA, Van Dieën J, Hug F, Lowery M, et al. Consensus for experimental design in electromyography (CEDE) project: Amplitude normalization matrix. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology. 2020;53:102438. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2020.102438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008369.r001

Decision Letter 0

Wolfgang Einhäuser, Adrian M Haith

17 Dec 2020

Dear Dr. Afschrift,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology.

As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. I apologize for the lengthy review period for this paper. This was due to waiting for comments from a reviewer who was ultimately unable to complete their reviews. Nevertheless, the remaining two reviewers were quite positive about the manuscript. They did, however, raise several important points related to the logic of the conclusions and presentation of the methods and results. In light of the reviews (below this email), we would like to invite the resubmission of a significantly-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments.

We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent to reviewers for further evaluation.

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to the review comments and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 60 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. Please note that revised manuscripts received after the 60-day due date may require evaluation and peer review similar to newly submitted manuscripts.

Thank you again for your submission. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Adrian M Haith

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Wolfgang Einhäuser

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: Review: PCOMPBIOL-D-20-01750

Title: Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking

This manuscript describes a series of analyses on existing datasets to study how balance is controlled during standing and walking. The authors use these analyses to show that during walking, as during standing, COM state feedback (rather than ankle feedback) is used to control ankle moments (and ankle muscle activities). Overall, this is a nice manuscript, that brings together datasets from several previous datasets to come to an interesting result. I have one somewhat more major comment, and several smaller minor comments.

One of the things I’m kind of left confused about is the role of simple stretch reflexes in all of this; we know they exist (see all the work on H-reflexes that the authors also mention), and that they should have a more or less 1-to-1 mapping between ankle state and muscle activity. Yet, the data of the authors shows that these models fit less well, which, in a way, is surprising to me. Probably, this is due to the fact that the models that are fitted here do not only contain information on short-latency reflexive activity, but also on longer latency mechanisms. Or is there something else that I am missing here? These kind of thoughts could be expressed somewhat more clear in the manuscript.

Minor comments:

1) Figure 2; what are the green lines in this (and other) figures? It seems as if they are fits to just the unperturbed data for a, but for b, it seems for all data (or is that due to the multiple regression that was performed? Also, I was wondering why for the standing, only the COM feedback model is shown, and not the data for the ankle feedback model?

2) Figure 3; from the numbers, the RMSE in G is twice that of D, but if I would have to guess, I would say it’s much larger? (or is this just my eyes being off?

3) It may be nice to show some example traces for one perturbation of COM deviations, and then ankle moment deviations, and estimated ankle moment deviations, just for the reader to get some idea of what’s going on.

4) Figure 4. In figures 2-3 you used arrows with arrowheads pointing toward the feature, and at the tail the thing it indicated. Here, you do the opposite (super-minor, but may be nice to be consistent in this).

5) Page 6, line 183; here, it is stated that the perturbations happened around toe-off, yet the figure that is refereed to has perturbations in many phases of the gait cycle?

6) Figures 6 and 7; I think the figures miss a delta in both the x and y labels, as these are deviations from the nominal (pattern/ activity/etc), right?

7) Page 9, line 277, one of our recent preprints may be of interest here; basically, we showed phase dependent contributions of vestibular info, which also was context dependent. See https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.09.30.319434v1

8) Page 10, line 234; references that subjects might rely more on stepping (at least in the ML plane) are:

Stimpson, K.H., et al., Effects of walking speed on the step-by-step control of step width. J Biomech, 2018. 68: p. 78-83.

And

https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.06.10.143867v1

9) Methods; R^2 values are typically not normally distributed, and some sort of a (modified) fisher r-to-z transform is needed before averaging them, or doing stats on them.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript evaluates a linear feedback control models for balance control during standing and walking. The authors compared task-level feedback models that use COM position and velocity changes to control ankle moment or muscles and local feedback models that use ankle angle and angular velocity changes. In all four (one standing and three walking) experiments evaluated in the study, the task-level feedback models showed higher correlation and smaller errors with human data than the local feedback model did. The author conclude that ankle responses to disturbances during standing and walking can be explained by talk-level feedback but not by local feedback.

Strengths:

- The study shows that an ankle control strategy that has been proposed to explain standing balance can also be applied for walking to some extent.

- It is shown that explain standing and walking balance control is not trivial by evaluating a local feedback control model that fails to explain.

- The models are evaluated with multiple (independently conducted) human experiments.

- The findings (that a standing balance control model also can explain balance control during walking) have interesting and important implications for human walking control.

Weaknesses:

- The manuscript lacks the nuanced presentation that, in my opinion, is necessary for such modeling studies. For instance, it is stated in the abstract that “We found that delayed feedback of center of mass position and velocity, but not local feedback of joint positions and velocities, can explain…” and in the results section that “… we showed that … can largely be explained by task-level feedback of COM kinematics and not with local feedback of joint kinematics” However, the study does not show that ankle responses cannot be explained by local feedback; it shows that the local feedback model the authors selected cannot, and it is still possible that a different local feedback model could explain the ankle responses observed in humans. I think this is a critical limitation of the current manuscript and suggest the following modifications:

1) Do not say “local feedback cannot …” as your conclusion. Instead, say “the local feedback model cannot …”

2) In the introduction, discuss the nature of modeling studies. Again, showing that a local feedback model you selected cannot fit/correlate/etc. observed data does not show that local feedback cannot explain observed data.

3) You could/should introduce and discuss that there can be and there are other (task-based and) local models than the ones you selected. For instance, [Song & Geyer, A neural circuitry that emphasizes spinal feedback generates diverse behaviours of human locomotion. The Journal of physiology, 2015] shows that local force feedback for ankle control can contribute to producing walking and maintaining balance. While the model includes a foot placement control (as you cited as “balance is mainly controlled by foot placement […,33]”) it also has local ankle control.

4) In the introduction, you should also justify the models you selected/proposed in your study. I think the linear models you selected are justifiable as it is simple to be considered as a baseline model and have been used in previous studies on standing balance. (In that, if the models have been used in or have been adapted from previous studies, you should clearly state and cite them in the introduction and also when presenting eq (1)~(4).

5) The discussion can be stronger by covering relevant musculoskeletal modeling studies as (there are not too many and) can be suitable for more nuanced discussions:

5-1) Section 3.2 can include the discussion of “the changes in muscle responses do not necessarily indicate modulation of reflex gains” as stated in [33], as the changes in the responses could result from the musculoskeletal configuration/dynamics. I think this is worth emphasizing to point out the limitation of your studies using simple linear models that do not incorporate the complex musculoskeletal dynamics.

5-2) Section 3.4 can mention the potential contribution of feedforward control in balancing as suggested in [Haeufle, Schmortte, Geyer, Müller & Schmitt, The benefit of combining neuronal feedback and feed-forward control for robustness in step down perturbations of simulated human walking depends on the muscle function. Frontiers in computational neuroscience, 2018].

- The presentation is not in its best form. Please find my comments below for more details.

Detailed Comments:

Line 52~53: It is written that the ankle strategy is the “dominant” strategy in the sagittal balance control. However, the following content does not seem enough to support that statement. Either rephrase the sentence, elaborate on what you mean by “dominant”, or add evidence that the ankle strategy is dominant over the stepping strategy.

Lines 89~91: “We therefore hypothesize that tactile sensors also modulate task-level feedback gains and thereby contribute to the observed modulation of reactive muscle activity.” -> Consider rephrasing the sentence so that this hypothesis is secondary to your main hypothesis of task-level vs. local feedback.

Lines 136~137: “Inputs (COM kinematics or ankle kinematics) and outputs (ankle moment) …” -> I suggest you rephrase the sentence so that it holds the main points without the parts in parentheses. In other words, “ankle moment” should not be in parentheses.

The order of the experiments in Fig. 1, 2 Results, and 4.1 Experimental methods are not consistent, thus confusing.

Fig. 1, Continuous translation walking: Is the y-axis treadmill velocity or Com velocity? Please clarify.

Lines 155~156: “reactive ankle joint moments and muscle activity …” -> “reactive ankle joint moments and muscle activity during standing…”

Please explain what you mean by “limitations related to normalizing EMG data” in section 4.3.

Fig 3. Subplot labels are missing A) and are not consistent with the caption.

I would recommend presenting Fig. 2~4 in a consistent manner. For instance, include joint feedback data in Fig 2, and combine Fig 3 and 4. Fig 2 and the combined Fig3+4 could have the same layout.

Table 2. RMS of TA is smaller for the Joint feedback model. p-RMS of 0.004 means that the Joint feedback model is significantly better? Please clarify.

Line 182~186: Is this paragraph only talking about the data around 60 %stride in Fig 5? If so, please clarify it by, for example, marking the 60 % stride data with shaded boxes and referring it to it as “(shaded area in Fig. 5)”. If I am misunderstanding this paragraph, then please clarify what you mean by “(also applied around toe-off of the contralateral leg)” at line 183.

Fig. 5: Are the data for one speed?

Lines 195~196: Indicate what gait phases you are indicating to by initial, mid-, and terminal stance. E.g. terminal stance: 62.5%, etc.

Fig 6, Row 2~4: Why are the regressions done together for forward and background perturbations? Or, why are they done separately for data in Fig 2 and 4?

Fig 6&7: Are the x-axes COM or Del_COM? If they are COM please explain.

Why is there no Table that summarizes the R^2 and RMS values for the continuous speed perturbation experiment?

Line 242: “(Fig. 9)” -> “(Fig. 9-F)”

Line 245: “(Fig. 9)” -> “(Fig. 9-B and F)” or remove

Line 259: “(Fig. 9)” -> “(Fig. 9-C, D, G, and H)”

Please check the writing/editorial mistakes throughout the manuscript. A few examples are:

- Line 39: “are less well studied” -> “are less studied” or “has received less …”

- Line 54: “fore-after” -> “fore-aft”

- Line 59: “Is is” -> “It is”

- Lines 201~202: “(one representative subject: figure 6A, all subjects figure: 5)” -> “(Fig. 5: all subjects, Fig 6: one representative subject)”

- Fig. 6, Row 1: Mark another value in the y-axis to show the scale.

- Fig. 7: Indicate % stance/stride as in Fig 6.

- Fig. 9: Place the subfigure labels and y-axis labels (e.g., D and H) to not invade the other subfigures.

- Line 326: “pcontact” -> “contact”

- “Fig 11.” -> “Fig. S1”

- “Fig 12.” -> “Fig. S12”

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Sjoerd M. Bruijn

Reviewer #2: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions, please see http://journals.plos.org/compbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008369.r003

Decision Letter 1

Wolfgang Einhäuser, Adrian M Haith

19 Mar 2021

Dear Dr. Afschrift,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology. As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. The reviewers were mostly satisfied with the revisions to the paper. However, Reviewer 2 raised some outstanding concerns related to presentation. Based on the reviews, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication. But first please consider the criticisms and suggestions from Reviewer 2 as you may find their comments instructive for further strengthening the paper. I leave it to your judgement, however, as to the extent of the revisions that you wish to make before resubmitting.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 30 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email.

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to all review comments, and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Adrian M Haith

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Wolfgang Einhäuser

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

A link appears below if there are any accompanying review attachments. If you believe any reviews to be missing, please contact ploscompbiol@plos.org immediately:

[LINK]

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: The authors have done a great job in revising the manuscript.

Reviewer #2: I appreciate the authors’ work in addressing most of my comments. I especially acknowledge the added discussion on other factors that could potentially explain the results of this study.

However, I unfortunately need to point out that the writing is not in its best form. Some parts, especially the introduction, even became more difficult for me to read than the original manuscript. Perhaps this happened in the process of modifying the manuscript to address the reviewers’ comments while not considering the overall flow of the paragraphs. While I will leave a few specific comments below, I strongly recommend the authors to proofread the entire manuscript, especially the introduction section, to improve the clarity and readability.

Lines 30~31: “When standing balance is perturbed, the central nervous system estimates the movement of the whole body center of mass to activate muscles and control balance” This is too assertive in my opinion. Rather suggest strong enough citations or modify to something like “Previous studies suggest that …”

It was very difficult to read the introduction. I needed to stop a lot to understand how some parts make sense in the broader context. I think a major revision of the entire section is necessary, so it is not so easy to suggest specific small changes. Thus, rather than giving specific suggestions, I will list a few specific points I think made the section difficult to read:

- Regarding the overall structure of the introduction, currently, many paragraphs consist of both the background and the specific ideas of this study (e.g., Lines 53~54, Lines 87~90, Line96~98, Lines 110~112). I think jumping back and forth between background and what is done in this study harms the readability. It could be easier if the introduction first covers the relevant background then describe what and how this study would address the questions. Maybe this can be done by rephrasing some sentences. For instance, instead of writing “… we hypothesize …” (Line 96) in the middle of a background paragraph, it could be stated as a general idea (and later in the “In this study, …” paragraph you can pick up on that idea).

- Lines 41~42: “seems to be simple” and “This is remarkable…” do not read naturally together, although I understand what it means. Maybe you can change the first sentence to something like “Humans can stand and walk … without difficulty …”

- Lines 83~85: The sentence does not flow well in the paragraph. You should provide some flow/reasoning why you are making this statement here.

- Line 126~128: I think you should merge this paragraph with the previous one (with a good flow rather than simply connecting them).

I still could spot some typos:

- Line 38: “can be can be” -> “can be”

- Line 377: “season” -> “reason”

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

References:

Review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008369.r005

Decision Letter 2

Wolfgang Einhäuser, Adrian M Haith

24 May 2021

Dear Dr. Afschrift,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Adrian M Haith

Associate Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Wolfgang Einhäuser

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008369.r006

Acceptance letter

Wolfgang Einhäuser, Adrian M Haith

22 Jun 2021

PCOMPBIOL-D-20-01750R2

Similar sensorimotor transformations control balance during standing and walking

Dear Dr Afschrift,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Katalin Szabo

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Sensitivity of the time delay.

    We evaluated whether the variance in ankle moment explained by the linear regression is sensitive to the time delay. This additional analysis was important to verify that the observed relation between COM kinematics and ankle moment is a feedback control process, and does not simply reflect the coupling due to the dynamics of the skeleton. We did this sensitivity analysis on the pelvis push perturbations and found that the relation between ankle moment and COM kinematics is indeed sensitive to the neural delay. The variance explained by the linear regression was optimal with a physiological plausible delay of 100ms and decreased strongly for delays smaller than 50 ms or larger than 120 ms. Note that time delay in this study was selected based on literature [29] and was not based on this sensitivity analysis.

    (EPS)

    S2 Fig. Deviations in COM position and velocity in the different types of perturbations.

    To evaluate if the magnitude of the perturbations are similar between datasets, we compared the deviation in COM position and velocity at 150ms after perturbation onset for the four datasets. We found that the deviation in COM position and velocity is similar in the different datasets of perturbed walking (B-D) and is larger in perturbed walking compared to perturbed standing (A). A similar deviation in COM kinematics is observed when analysing the data 100ms (E), 150ms (F) and 200ms (G) after perturbation onset in the pelvis push perturbations.

    (TIF)

    S1 Table. Sensitivity of the time delay.

    We evaluated whether the variance in ankle moment explained by the linear regression is sensitive to the time delay. This additional analysis was important to verify that the observed relation between COM kinematics and ankle moment is a feedback control process, and does not simply reflect the coupling due to the dynamics of the skeleton. We did this sensitivity analysis on the pelvis push perturbations and found that the relation between ankle moment and COM kinematics is indeed sensitive to the neural delay. The variance explained by the linear regression was optimal with a physiological plausible delay of 100ms and decreased strongly for delays smaller than 50 ms or larger than 120 ms. Note that time delay in this study was selected based on literature [29] and was not based on this sensitivity analysis.

    (PDF)

    S1 File. DataBeltPerturb.mat.

    Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with discrete perturbations in speed of the treadmill belts [10].

    (MAT)

    S2 File. DataContinuousPerturb.mat.

    Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with continuous perturbations in speed of the treadmill belts [27].

    (MAT)

    S3 File. DataPelvisPush.mat.

    Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with pelvis push perturbations [9].

    (MAT)

    S4 File. DataStandingBalance.mat.

    Matlab file with raw data of the experiment with support surface translations during standing [26].

    (MAT)

    S5 File. ExamplePlotFigureAfschrift2019.m.

    Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the discrete belt speed perturbations.

    (M)

    S6 File. ExamplePlotFigureVlutters2018.m.

    Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the pelvis push perturbations.

    (M)

    S7 File. ExamplePlotFigureMoore2014.m.

    Matlab script to plot the relation between COM kinematics and changes in ankle moment in the continuous belt speed perturbations.

    (M)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponseToReviewers.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: AnswerToReviewers.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES