Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Jul 8;11:14168. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93714-7

Differential effects of body mass index on domain-specific cognitive outcomes after stroke

Minwoo Lee 1, Mi Sun Oh 1, San Jung 2, Ju-Hun Lee 3, Chul-Ho Kim 4, Min Uk Jang 5, Young Eun Kim 1, Hee-Joon Bae 6, Jaeseol Park 1, Yeonwook Kang 7, Byung-Chul Lee 1, Jae-Sung Lim 8,, Kyung-Ho Yu 1,
PMCID: PMC8266804  PMID: 34239011

Abstract

Although the obesity paradox is an important modifiable factor in cardiovascular diseases, little research has been conducted to determine how it affects post-stroke cognitive function. We aimed to investigate the association between body mass index (BMI) and domain-specific cognitive outcomes, focusing on the subdivision of each frontal domain function in post-ischemic stroke survivors. A total of 335 ischemic stroke patients were included in the study after completion of the Korean-Mini Mental Status Examination (K-MMSE) and the vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards neuropsychological protocol at 3 months after stroke. Frontal lobe functions were analyzed using semantic/phonemic fluency, processing speed, and mental set shifting. Our study participants were categorized into four groups according to BMI quartiles. The z-scores of K-MMSE at 3 months differed significantly between the groups after adjustment for initial stroke severity (p = 0.014). Global cognitive function in stroke survivors in the Q1 (the lowest quartile) BMI group was significantly lower than those in Q2 and Q4 (the highest quartile) BMI groups (K-MMSE z-scores, Q1: − 2.10 ± 3.40 vs. Q2: 0.71 ± 1.95 and Q4: − 1.21 ± 1.65). Controlled oral word association test findings indicated that phonemic and semantic word fluency was lower in Q4 BMI group participants than in Q2 BMI group participants (p = 0.016 and p = 0.023 respectively). BMI might differentially affect cognitive domains after ischemic stroke. Although being underweight may negatively affect global cognition post-stroke, obesity could induce frontal lobe dysfunctions, specifically phonemic and semantic word fluency.

Subject terms: Dementia, Stroke

Introduction

Obesity is a growing public health concern and a well-recognized risk factor for cardiovascular disorders and overall mortality1. However, an increasing body of evidence suggests that obesity has a protective effect on cardiovascular disease or increases the probability of survival in certain patients such as the elderly2,3. Referred to as the ‘obesity paradox’, this paradoxical phenomenon has been observed not only in cardiovascular diseases4, but also in cerebrovascular disorders, peripheral arterial disease5, diabetes mellitus6, malignancy7, and dementia8,9. However, it is yet controversial whether the ‘obesity paradox’ also exists in patients with cognitive disorders. While being underweight (BMI < 20 kg/m2) is known to be associated with an increased risk of dementia8, the effects of higher BMI on cognitive outcomes are inconsistent throughout the studies. Some studies have suggested that inflammation and vascular changes accompanied by obesity lead to deterioration of cognitive function10, while others reported that a higher BMI in middle-to-late life predicts a lower risk of developing dementia9.

The obesity paradox has also been observed in stroke population, however, the relationship is somewhat complex11. In some studies, high BMI was associated with reduced stroke severity, favorable functional outcomes, and lower mortality after stroke1214. However another study showed that overweight or obesity did not confer a favorable effect while underweight could have poor outcomes after stroke15. Furthermore, these reports did not explore the independent association between BMI and post-stroke cognition. One Swedish study reported that BMI did not influence cognitive impairment at 20 months after stroke in a sample of 149 stroke survivors16. On the other hand, pre-stroke frailty was independently associated with poor post-stroke cognitive fucnction17. However, those studies used only the screening tools to assess cognition, which cannot be considered appropriate given the higher incidence of frontal dysfunction among stroke survivors18. To date, few studies have used detailed neuropsychological evaluations to determine how obesity affects post-stroke cognitive impairment19. It has already been reported that obesity primarily affects metabolic activity and cognitive function of the frontal lobe, but the effects of obesity on each cognitive domain in stroke patients have not yet been sufficiently studied20,21. This is also clinically important in properly screening patients with cognitive impairment and evaluating their treatment effectiveness.

In this context, we aimed to investigate the differential effects of BMI on domain-specific and global cognitive function at 3 months post-ischemic stroke using the Korean Vascular Cognitive Impairment Harmonization Standards-Neuropsychological Protocol (K-VCIHS-NP), a comprehensive neuropsychological test developed for post-stroke survivors. We hypothesized that BMI might have distinct roles in each neural substrate associated with each cognitive domain, as previous studies have shown that high BMI is associated with executive dysfunction and frontal lobe atrophy in a generally healthy population20,22,23.

Methods

Study participants

Participants were enrolled from the Korean Vascular Cognitive Impairment Harmonization Standards (K-VCIHS) study, which has been previously described in detail24. The K-VCIHS cohort enrolled patients with ischemic stroke who had been consecutively admitted to 12 university hospitals from October 2007 to August 2008. Inclusion criteria for our study comprised the following: (i) a diagnosis of acute ischemic stroke with neurological deficits persisting for ≥ 24 h, (ii) a relevant ischemic lesion observed on magnetic resonance imaging, (iii) admission within 7 days after symptom onset, and (iv) available data on admission concerning BMI and the results of K-VCIHS-NP performed at 3 months post-ischemic stroke. In total, 335 participants from the K-VCIHS cohort who fulfilled the inclusion criteria were finally analyzed. We collected clinical variables and the results of K-VCIHS-NP with the consent of the principal investigator of the K-VCIHS-NP study group and the Institutional Review Board of Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital. BMI was calculated as a participant’s weight (kg) divided by their height (m) squared, measured on the first day of admission according to the institutional protocol with an automatic weight and height machine. Body weight and height were measured to the nearest 0.1 kg and 0.1 cm respectively using BSM-330 (InBody, South Korea). Those who were not able to stand were weighed using an electronic bed scale, SCB330-7 (SOHWA Inc, South Korea), in the supine position. The participants were then stratified into BMI quartiles25, namely, Q1, < 21.87 kg/m2; Q2, 21.88–23.87 kg/m2; Q3, 23.88–25.96 kg/m2, and; Q4, > 25.97 kg/m2. From the cohort registry data, we collected clinical data concerning age, sex, and stroke subtype according to the Trial of ORG 10,172 in Acute Stroke Treatment (TOAST) classification26. Moreover, initial stroke severity represented as the National Institute of Health Stroke Scale and vascular risk factors were also collected. The study was approved by the Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital Institutional Review Board and conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. The requirement for informed consent was waived the Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital Institutional Review Board because of the retrospective nature of this study as well as the minimal risk that it posed to participants.

Cognitive function assessment

Participants completed the Korean Mini-Mental Status Examination (K-MMSE) and a 60-min neuropsychological test as described in the K-VCIHS study protocol27. The K-VCIHS assesses four cognitive domains: (i) executive/activation function, using the Korean version of the controlled oral word association test (COWAT) for phonemic and semantic fluency, digit symbol coding, and trail making tests A and B; (ii) language, using the Korean-Boston naming test; (iii) visuospatial, using the Rey complex figure test (copy), and; (iv) memory, using delayed recall scores from the Seoul verbal learning test. The K-VCIHS study protocol also included the Informant Questionnaire of Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE) for the premorbid history of cognitive dysfunctions. The specific tests and scales which comprise the K-VCIHS protocol and relevant references are described in Supplemental Table I. We obtained standardized Z-scores for each domain and for the K-MMSE after adjustment for age, sex, and educational level to directly compare groups according to BMI quartiles.

Brain imaging

All participants underwent brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), performed using a 3 T whole-body MRI system. Diffusion-weighted imaging was used to obtain the apparent diffusion coefficient and assess acute cerebral infarction. The location and number of acute ischemic stroke lesions were rated and quantified. We subdivided the stroke lesions as follows: (i) cortical vs. subcortical-only, (ii) left-sided vs. right-sided, and; (iii) single vs. multiple12.

Statistical analysis

In order to detect an effect η2p = 0.04 with 80% power in a between-subjects ANCOVA (four groups, numerator df = 3, alpha = 0.05), a priori power calculation of G*Power 3.1.9.428 suggested that we would need 67 subjects in each group (n = 266). Pearson’s chi-square test and an analysis of variance (ANOVA) were used, as appropriate, to compare demographic characteristics between the groups according to BMI quartiles. For the univariate analysis, an ANOVA was used to compare the Z-scores of each cognitive domain between the groups. The Levene’s test and Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were performed to check for equality of variances and normality of dependent variables, respectively. The prespecified covariates included in the main analysis were age, sex, education years, and initial stroke severity (NIHSS) that showed a strong association with post-stroke cognitive impairment in the previous studies29. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed adjusting for the initial stroke severity (NIHSS), as age, sex, and education levels were considered in the z-score transformation process. The effect size of BMI quartiles on dependent variables in the ANCOVA was presented with partial eta squared (η2p). We further conducted a sensitivity analysis using cutoff points for BMI according to the Asia–Pacific classification30 (Underweight, < 18.5 kg/m2; Normal, 18.5–22.9 kg/m2; Overweight, 23–24.9 kg/m2; Obese ≥ 25 kg/m2) to determine the generalizability of our study. Two-sided p-values < 0.05 were considered to indicate statistical significance. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS, version 26.

Results

The mean age of the participants was 64.8 ± 12.4 years, and women comprised 38.9% of the study population. The mean BMI was 23.89 ± 3.10 kg/m2 (range, 15.82–32.93 kg/m2). The BMI quartile groups did not differ in terms of age, sex, years of education, previous stroke history, or pre-stroke cognitive decline. There were no significant differences in initial stroke severity, stroke etiology according to the TOAST classification, or the number and location of ischemic lesions. However, the prevalence of hypertension was higher in the Q4 BMI group (the highest) than in the Q1 BMI group (the lowest). The prevalence of other vascular risk factors did not differ significantly between the groups (Table 1).

Table 1.

Clinical Characteristics according to the quartiles of body mass index.

Q 1 (< 21.87, n = 84) Q 2 (21.88–23.87, n = 84) Q 3 (23.88–25.96, n = 84) Q 4 (> 25.97, n = 83) P values
Age, mean ± SD 66.2 ± 14.6 65.8 ± 12.8 63.8 ± 11.1 63.3 ± 10.9 0.329
Women, %(N) 46.4(39) 42.9 (36) 27.4 (23) 39.8 (33) 0.064
Vascular risk factors
 Hypertension, %(N) 54.8 (46) 63.1 (53) 58.3 (49) 74.7 (62) 0.045*
 Diabetes, %(N) 31.0 (26) 23.8 (20) 33.3 (28) 39.8 (33) 0.172
 Dyslipidemia, %(N) 31.0 (26) 23.8 (20) 33.3 (28) 39.8 (33) 0.085
 History of Stroke, %(N) 23.8 (20) 14.3 (12) 20.2 (17) 27.4 (23) 0.194
 Smoking, %(N) 44.0 (37) 42.9 (36) 50.0 (42) 43.4 (36) 0.772
 Atrial fibrillation, %(N) 13.1 (11) 16.7 (14) 16.7 (14) 14.5 (12) 0.896
Education 0.148
 Illiterate, %(N) 9.5 (8) 6.0 (5) 1.2 (1) 6.0 (5)
 0–6 years, %(N) 31.0(26) 48.8(41) 38.1(32) 28.9(24)
 7–12 years, %(N) 39.2 (33) 31.0(26) 38.1(32) 47.0(39)
 13 years and more, %(N) 20.2 (17) 14.3 (12) 22.6 (19) 18.1 (15)
Prestroke Cognitive Impairmenta, %(N) 8.3 (7) 3.6 (3) 7.1 (6) 8.4 (7) 0.565
NIHSS score, median [IQR] 2.5[1.0;5.5] 3.0[2.0;5.0] 3.0[1.5;6.0] 3.0[1.0;5.0] 0.942
Ischemic stroke subtype 0.813
 Large artery atherosclerosis, %(N) 39.3 (33) 34.5 (29) 42.9 (36) 47.0 (39)
 Small vessel occlusion, %(N) 26.2 (22) 33.3 (28) 25.0 (21) 26.5 (22)
 Cardioembolism, %(N) 14.3 (12) 15.5 (13) 19.0 (16) 12.0 (10)
 Other determined causes, %(N) 2.4 (2) 2.4 (2) 1.2 (1) 0.0 (0)
 Undetermined causes, %(N) 17.9 (15) 14.3 (12) 11.9 (10) 14.5 (12)
Brain MRI findings
 Left/Right/Both, % 52.4/40.5/7.1 40.5/53.6/6.0 41.7/51.2/7.1 53.0/42.2/4.8 0.624
 Cortical/Subcortical only, % 46.4/53.6 39.3/60.7 45.2/54.8 47.0/53.0 0.734
 Single/Multiple, % 51.2/48.8 58.3/41.7 53.6/46.4 49.4/50.6 0.680

aPre-stroke cognitive impairment was evaluated with the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE).

Bold values followed by * are significant at α = 0.05.

NIHSS National Institute of Health Stroke Scales, SD Standard deviation, IQR Interquartile range, MRI Magnetic Resonance Image.

The Z-scores of the K-MMSE differed significantly between the quartile groups at 3 months post-ischemic stroke onset after adjustment for a prespecified covariate, initial stroke severity(NIHSS). (p = 0.014, η2p = 0.031, ANCOVA, Fig. 1 and Table 2). A multiple comparison analysis indicated that stroke survivors in the Q1 BMI group showed significantly lower global cognitive functions (K-MMSE Z-score, − 2.1 ± 3.4) than those in the Q2 and Q4 BMI groups (K-MMSE Z-score, Q2: − 0.71 ± 1.95; Q4: − 1.21 ± 1.65). The Z-scores of the COWAT also significantly differed between the quartile groups after adjusting for initial stroke severity. A multiple comparison analysis of the COWAT results indicated that participants in the Q4 BMI group had significantly lower phonemic and semantic word fluencies than their counterparts in the Q2 BMI group, despite similar levels of global cognitive function.(p = 0.016, η2p = 0.032 and p = 0.023, η2p = 0.029 respectively; ANCOVA). Other neuropsychological test findings relating to frontal lobe functions, including processing speed and mental set-shifting, and other cognitive domains, did not differ significantly between the groups. The sensitivity analysis using Asian-Pacific BMI classification resulted in the loss of statistical significance in comparison of z-scores of K-MMSE and COWAT tests. Only the K-MMSE raw score differences remained significant. (Supplemental Table 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Violin plots indicating the association between the quartiles of BMI and z-scores of both K-MMSE and frontal cognitive domain in patients with ischemic stroke. The z-scores of K-MMSE, COWAT-semantic, and COWAT-phonemic were significantly different between the groups after adjusting for initial stroke severity. (p values, 0.014, 0.034 and 0.023 respectively; ANCOVA) *Abbreviations: BMI,body mass index; K-MMSE Korean version of mini-mental status examination; COWAT controlled oral word association test, ANCOVA analysis of covariance.

Table 2.

Comparison of Z scores of cognitive tests according to the quartiles of body mass index.

Q 1 (< 21.87, n = 84) Q 2 (21.88–23.87, n = 84) Q 3 (23.88–25.96, n = 84) Q 4 (> 25.97, n = 83) Unadjusted P values Adjusted P values*
K-MMSE, Z score  − 2.1 ± 3.4  − 0.71 ± 1.95  − 1.68 ± 3.03  − 1.21 ± 1.65 0.007* 0.014*
Raw score 22.99 ± 6.7 25.23 ± 5.21 24.89 ± 5.32 24.56 ± 5.52 0.025* 0.034*
COWAT semantic  − 0.87 ± 1.41  − 0.58 ± 1.18  − 1.03 ± 1.24  − 1.14 ± 1.00 0.019* 0.023*
COWAT phonemic  − 0.88 ± 1.30  − 0.47 ± 1.57  − 0.96 ± 1.44  − 1.18 ± 1.16 0.012* 0.016*
DSC  − 1.06 ± 1.22  − 0.70 ± 1.06  − 1.05 ± 1.37  − 1.11 ± 0.96 0.112 0.159
TMT-A  − 1.44 ± 3.41  − 1.03 ± 2.29  − 1.36 ± 2.84  − 0.62 ± 1.50 0.205 0.315
TMT-B  − 1.13 ± 2.32  − 0.89 ± 1.96  − 1.06 ± 2.09  − 1.09 ± 2.20 0.920 0.954
K-BNT  − 0.95 ± 2.12  − 0.69 ± 1.98  − 1.33 ± 2.76  − 0.67 ± 1.58 0.181 0.236
RCFT Copy  − 1.53 ± 2.44  − 0.88 ± 1.48  − 1.63 ± 2.51  − 1.35 ± 2.06 0.128 0.224
SVLT-E  − 0.83 ± 1.11  − 0.89 ± 1.14  − 1.08 ± 1.29 -0.95 ± 1.19 0.557 0.603

Adjusted for the initial stroke severity (National Institute of Health Stroke Scale).

Bold values followed by * are significant at α = 0.05.

K-MMSE Korean Mini-Mental Status Examination; COWAT Controlled Oral Word Association Test; DSC Digit Symbol Coding; TMT Trail Making Test; K-BNT Korean version-Boston Naming Test; RCFT Rey Complex Figure Test; SVLT-E Seoul Verbal Learning Test-Elderly’s version.

Discussion

In this study, we assessed the association between obesity phenotypes defined using BMI quartiles at admission and cognitive function at 3 months post-ischemic stroke onset. To our knowledge, this study is the first to evaluate BMI and domain-specific cognitive outcomes using a comprehensive, standardized, neuropsychological protocol in relation to a multicenter cohort of stroke patients24. Our data analysis indicated that a lower BMI at admission was associated with a higher risk of global cognitive impairment, while a higher BMI was associated with significantly worse frontal dysfunction post-ischemic stroke. These findings suggest that BMI has differential effects on global cognitive function and frontal domain performance following an ischemic stroke.

Previous epidemiologic studies concerning the relationship between BMI and cognitive impairment have yielded controversial findings. The relationship between obesity and long-term cognitive outcomes has alternately been identified as direct, inverse, U-shaped, or even absent31,32. However, as previous studies only performed the MMSE to assess cognition, they might have overlooked the influence of BMI on frontal/executive function, given that the MMSE is not sensitive to evaluating frontal lobe function33. Our results are in line with previous studies showing that a low BMI was significantly associated with a high risk of cognitive decline8.

Despite uncertainly concerning the precise mechanism underlying post-stroke worsening of cognitive function, decreased body weight has been identified as an early indication of declining health and even neurodegeneration34. Moreover, several studies have proposed that leptin, an adipokine produced by adipose tissue, exerts neuroprotective effects through anti-oxidative activity and its promotion of hippocampal progenitor cell proliferation. As underweight patients may have decreased levels of leptin, a lower BMI may result in less neuroprotection after neurological insult35. It is also possible that low BMI may have enhanced neurodegeneration via nutritional deficiencies including decreased level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor36 and vitamin B levels with a reactive elevation of homocysteine37.

Our results also showed that a higher BMI was significantly associated with worse frontal/executive function, specifically in phonemic and semantic fluencies. This finding is in agreement with those of previous studies that have linked obesity to temporal atrophy22. Moreover, one study that used high-resolution 3D MRI scans reported that obese individuals had a significantly lower density of gray matter in the frontal lobe, post-central gyrus, and middle frontal gyrus than control group participants23. Obesity has also been reportedly related to executive dysfunction with other cognitive functions preserved, even in neurologically healthy adults without cognitive impairment20. Specifically, obese adults perform worse on executive function tests, especially those testing verbal interference, than their counterparts with a lower BMI. These findings may indicate vulnerability of the frontotemporal lobe in obese patients to acute stroke, regardless of lesion location.

Taken together, lower BMI and higher BMI may have differential pathophysiological roles in cognitive impairment after stroke. These differential effects of BMI on post-stroke cognitive impairment observed in the study may strengthen the clinical utilization of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) over MMSE in stroke population38,39. The MoCA is known to pick up substantially more cognitive abnormalities after stroke than MMSE by demonstrating deficits in frontal function including verbal fluencies38. Our data showed that those with the higher BMI (Q4, BMI > 25.97) had higher K-MMSE scores but lower phonemic and semantic fluency scores compared to the Q1, implying that the stroke population with higher BMI require MoCA or domain-specific neuropsychological battery for optimal screening and diagnosis of cognitive impairment after stroke.

On the other hand, the sensitivity analysis using the Asian-Pacific BMI classification30 revealed that statistical significances regarding the differences in z-scores of K-MMSE and each cognitive domain disappear, although both underweight and obese group shows relatively poor cognitive prognosis. The Asian-Pacific classification resulted in only 4% of our patients included in the underweight group (BMA < 18.5). Given this skewed distribution of BMI in study population, it may not be appropriate to assess the risk of post-stroke cognitive impairment according to the Asia–Pacific WHO classification. Our results suggest that if the patients belong to the group with a lower normal range of BMI according to the Asian-Pacific classification30, they may be at risk for post-stroke cognitive impairment. These findings are in line with the previous study that having BMI lower than 20 kg/m2 was associated with an increased risk of dementia8. Those corresponding to our Q2 group (BMI 21.88–23.87 kg/m2) are the most advantageous in terms of cognitive prognosis after stroke. This observation should be replicated in future prospective studies and is an important research topic that should be of interest as a potentially modifiable factor.

Our study had some limitations. First, only participants’ height and weight measurements were used to determine the BMI. Other adiposity data, such as abdominal circumference or waist-hip ratio, were unavailable; therefore, we could not address possible differences between leanness and being underweight and their relationships with long-term cognitive outcomes40. Future studies should consider replicating the approach of studies that have investigated the relationship between obesity and Alzheimer’s disease through using the waist-to-hip ratio and waist circumference at multiple sites as a measure of central obesity. Second, we were unable to determine any causal relationships between BMI and cognition due to the nature of the cross-sectional study design. Thus, there is room for other interpretations in the association between BMI and differential cognitive outcomes in that they may be due to pre-existing differential cognitive dysfunction. However, the pre-morbid cognitive decline status evaluated with IQCODE was not different between the BMI groups. Third, restrictions were inevitable concerning the number of participants studied and some of the explanatory variables because this study was the result of a secondary analysis of a multicenter study that aimed to investigate the prevalence of cognitive disorders post-stroke. Consequently, we did not include several image variables such as cerebral atrophy or white matter hyperintensities, which may have affected cognitive function post-stroke. Furthermore, we did not assess temporal changes in BMI from stroke onset. Nonetheless, the main strength of our study is that it is by far the first study to investigate the effects of BMI on each cognitive domain after stroke. These findings may provide additional evidence for pathophysiological aspects of BMI on each neural substrate after ischemic stroke.

Conclusions

Our research suggests that BMI may interact variably with cognitive domains post-ischemic stroke. Although being underweight might negatively affect global cognition post-stroke, obesity might also induce frontal lobe dysfunctions, specifically in terms of phonemic and semantic word fluency. Though exact pathomechanisms being unclear, these findings may be attributed to the distinct roles of BMI on neural substrates, specifically the frontal lobe area. A large prospective cohort study with a focus on neural substrates evaluated with brain imaging is necessary to elucidate further relationships between BMI and post-stroke cognitive outcomes and to validate our findings.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

Korean VCIHS study group members include Hee-Jun Bae, Jae-Kwan Cha, Ki-Hyun Cho, Soo-Jin Cho, San Jung, Yeonwook Kang, Dong-Eog Kim, Hahn-Young Kim, Oeun-Kyu Kim, Yong-Jae Kim, Im-Suck Koh, Sun-Uck Kwon, Ju-Hun Lee, Seung- Hoon Lee, Soo-Ju Lee, Mi Sun Oh, Jong-Moo Park, Joon-Hyun Shin, Kyung-Ho Yu and Byung-Chul Lee.

Author contributions

M.L, J.S.L and K.H.Y made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work; M.S.O, S.J, J.H.L, C.H.K, M.U.J, Y.E.K, H.J.B, B.C.L made contributions to the acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data; J.P and Y.K contributed to the interpretation of data. M.L, J.S.L, and K.H.Y have drafted and revised the manuscript.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Jae-Sung Lim, Email: jaesunglim@amc.seoul.kr.

Kyung-Ho Yu, Email: ykh1030@hallym.or.kr.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-93714-7.

References

  • 1.Collaborators GBDO, et al. Health effects of overweight and obesity in 195 countries over 25 years. N. Engl. J. Med. 2017;377:13–27. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1614362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Curtis JP, et al. The obesity paradox: Body mass index and outcomes in patients with heart failure. Arch. Intern. Med. 2005;165:55–61. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Oreopoulos A, Kalantar-Zadeh K, Sharma AM, Fonarow GC. The obesity paradox in the elderly: Potential mechanisms and clinical implications. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2009;25:643–659. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Elagizi A, et al. An overview and update on obesity and the obesity paradox in cardiovascular diseases. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2018;61:142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2018.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ludhwani D, Wu J. Obesity paradox in peripheral arterial disease: Results of a propensity match analysis from the national inpatient sample. Cureus. 2019;11:e4704. doi: 10.7759/cureus.4704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Han SJ, Boyko EJ. The evidence for an obesity paradox in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Metab. J. 2018;42:179–187. doi: 10.4093/dmj.2018.0055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lee DH, Giovannucci EL. The obesity paradox in cancer: Epidemiologic insights and perspectives. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2019;8:175–181. doi: 10.1007/s13668-019-00280-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fitzpatrick AL, et al. Midlife and late-life obesity and the risk of dementia: Cardiovascular health study. Arch. Neurol. 2009;66:336–342. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2008.582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Suemoto CK, Gilsanz P, Mayeda ER, Glymour MM. Body mass index and cognitive function: The potential for reverse causation. Int. J. Obes. 2015;39:1383–1389. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Nguyen JC, Killcross AS, Jenkins TA. Obesity and cognitive decline: Role of inflammation and vascular changes. Front. Neurosci. 2014;8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Oesch L, Tatlisumak T, Arnold M, Sarikaya H. Obesity paradox in stroke—Myth or reality? A systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0171334. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0171334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kim BJ, et al. Dynamics of obesity paradox after stroke, related to time from onset, age, and causes of death. Neurology. 2012;79:856–863. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318266fad1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim Y, Kim CK, Jung S, Yoon BW, Lee SH. Obesity-stroke paradox and initial neurological severity. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2015;86:743–747. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-308664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ryu WS, Lee SH, Kim CK, Kim BJ, Yoon BW. Body mass index, initial neurological severity and long-term mortality in ischemic stroke. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2011;32:170–176. doi: 10.1159/000328250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sun W, et al. Association of body mass index with mortality and functional outcome after acute ischemic stroke. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:2507. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02551-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pascoe M, Ski CF, Thompson DR, Linden T. Serum cholesterol, body mass index and smoking status do not predict long-term cognitive impairment in elderly stroke patients. J. Neurol. Sci. 2019;406:116476. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2019.116476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Taylor-Rowan M, et al. Pre-stroke frailty is independently associated with post-stroke cognition: A cross-sectional study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2019;25:501–506. doi: 10.1017/S1355617719000092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stephens S, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics of mild vascular cognitive impairment and dementia after stroke. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2004;19:1053–1057. doi: 10.1002/gps.1209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zhang Y, et al. Incidence and risk factors of cognitive impairment 3 months after first-ever stroke: A cross-sectional study of 5 geographic areas of China. J. Huazhong Univ. Sci. Technol. Med. Sci. 2012;32:906–911. doi: 10.1007/s11596-012-1056-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Gunstad J, et al. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr. Psychiatry. 2007;48:57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Volkow ND, et al. Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2009;17:60–65. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gustafson D, Lissner L, Bengtsson C, Bjorkelund C, Skoog I. A 24-year follow-up of body mass index and cerebral atrophy. Neurology. 2004;63:1876–1881. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000141850.47773.5f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pannacciulli N, et al. Brain abnormalities in human obesity: A voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1419–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Yu KH, et al. Cognitive impairment evaluated with vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards in a multicenter prospective stroke cohort in Korea. Stroke. 2013;44:786–788. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.668343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tanne D, Medalie JH, Goldbourt U. Body fat distribution and long-term risk of stroke mortality. Stroke. 2005;36:1021–1025. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000162584.39366.1c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Adams, H. P., Jr. et al. Classification of subtype of acute ischemic stroke. Definitions for use in a multicenter clinical trial. TOAST. Trial of Org 10172 in Acute Stroke Treatment. Stroke24, 35–41. 10.1161/01.str.24.1.35 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 27.Hachinski V, et al. National institute of neurological disorders and stroke-Canadian stroke network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke. 2006;37:2220–2241. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000237236.88823.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pendlebury ST, Rothwell PM. Prevalence, incidence, and factors associated with pre-stroke and post-stroke dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:1006–1018. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70236-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.World Health Organization. Regional Office for the Western, P. The Asia-Pacific perspective : redefining obesity and its treatment. 55p. Coordinated by the International Diabetes Institute; co-sponsored jointly by the Regional Office for the Western Pacific (WPRO), World Health Organization, the International Association for the Study of Obesity and the International Obesity Task Force (Sydney: Health Communications Australia, 2000).
  • 31.Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Wang Y. Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2008;9:204–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00473.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Singh-Manoux A, et al. Obesity phenotypes in midlife and cognition in early old age: The Whitehall II cohort study. Neurology. 2012;79:755–762. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182661f63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fu C, et al. Comparison of the mini-mental state examination and montreal cognitive assessment executive subtests in detecting post-stroke cognitive impairment. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2017;17:2329–2335. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lee EB. Obesity, leptin, and Alzheimer's disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011;1243:15–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06274.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tang BL. Leptin as a neuroprotective agent. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2008;368:181–185. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2008.01.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zuccato C, Cattaneo E. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2009;5:311–322. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Coin A, et al. Nutritional predictors of cognitive impairment severity in demented elderly patients: the key role of BMI. J. Nutr. Health Aging. 2012;16:553–556. doi: 10.1007/s12603-012-0052-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Pendlebury ST, Cuthbertson FC, Welch SJ, Mehta Z, Rothwell PM. Underestimation of cognitive impairment by mini-mental state examination versus the montreal cognitive assessment in patients with transient ischemic attack and stroke: A population-based study. Stroke. 2010;41:1290–1293. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.579888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pendlebury ST, et al. Differences in cognitive profile between TIA, stroke and elderly memory research subjects: A comparison of the MMSE and MoCA. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2012;34:48–54. doi: 10.1159/000338905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sakakura K, et al. Association of body mass index with cognitive function in elderly hypertensive Japanese. Am. J. Hypertens. 2008;21:627–632. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2008.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES