Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
. 2021 Apr 15;44(7):zsab048. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab048

Changing school start times: impact on sleep in primary and secondary school students

Lisa J Meltzer 1,, Kyla L Wahlstrom 2, Amy E Plog 3, Matthew J Strand 1
PMCID: PMC8271142  PMID: 33855446

Abstract

Study Objectives

To examine the impact of changing school start times on sleep for primary (elementary school: ES) and secondary (middle and high school: MS/HS) students.

Methods

Students (grades 3–12) and parents (grades K-12) were surveyed annually, before and for 2 years after school start time changes (ES: 60 min earlier, MS: 40–60 min later; HS: 70 min later). Student sleep and daytime sleepiness were measured with school-administered student surveys and parent-proxy online surveys.

Results

Approximately 28,000 students annually completed surveys (~55% White, ~21% free/reduced lunch [FRL]). One-year post-change, weekday bedtimes and wake times were slightly earlier for ES students, with an 11-min decrease in sleep duration. MS and HS students reported slightly later weekday bedtimes, significantly later wake times, and significantly longer sleep duration (MS: 29 min; HS: 45 min). The percent of ES students reporting sufficient sleep duration, poor sleep quality, or daytime sleepiness did not change, but the percent of MS and HS students reporting sufficient sleep duration significantly increased and clinically significant daytime sleepiness decreased. All results were maintained at the 2-year follow-up. Benefits of later start times were similar across racial and free/reduced lunch groups.

Conclusions

This is the first large scale, longitudinal, and representative study to concurrently examine the impact of changing school start times across students in primary/secondary school. Findings suggest a minimal impact of earlier start times on ES students’ sleep or daytime sleepiness, while further supporting the significant benefits of delaying MS and HS start times on student sleep and daytime sleepiness.

Keywords: school start times, sleep, health policy, longitudinal, adolescent, elementary


Statement of Significance.

This study highlights the significant benefit of later school start times for middle school and high school students, while also demonstrating no significant negative effects of earlier elementary school start times. The study is novel due to the large sample size, the 2-year follow-up period, and the relatively diverse sample. The implementation of healthy school start times (at or after 8:30 am for middle and high school students) is a critical health policy that can quickly and effectively address significant adolescent sleep debt, with minimal impact on younger students, who often are required to start earlier in order to accommodate later secondary school start times. Future studies should examine how early is too early to start school across development.

Introduction

Sleep is essential for optimal health and development, academic achievement, and social and emotional functioning [1–3], yet insufficient sleep is common among children and adolescents. To address this public health concern, the goal of increasing sleep duration for not only adolescents, but for children of all ages, was recently identified as a target for Healthy People 2030 [4]. For secondary school students, healthy school start times (no earlier than 8:30 am) have been identified by most major professional associations, including the American Academy of Pediatrics [5], American Academy of Sleep Medicine [6], American Medical Association [7], American Psychological Association [8], National Association of School Nurses [9], and National Parent Teacher Association [10], as a modifiable policy to improve adolescent health and well-being. The need for, and benefits of, later secondary school start times is well documented [1, 11, 12]. However, it is notable that <21% of middle schools and <18% of US high schools start at 8:30 am or later [13, 14]. Thus, further evidence supporting later start times for adolescents is needed.

In order to achieve later secondary school start times, it is often necessary for primary school students to start earlier, especially if the district has a staggered transportation schedule and has only one set of buses that transports all students [15]. Based on biological changes to human circadian rhythms during puberty, early sleep onset is difficult for adolescents, whose sleep is then truncated with early school start times, resulting in deficient sleep [16]. Primary school students, on the other hand, typically go to bed and wake earlier than secondary school students [17, 18]. Furthermore, biological changes in the circadian rhythm and self-reported delays in circadian preference (i.e. time one feels most alert and awake) typically begin between 11 and 13 years, when students in the United States are transitioning from elementary to middle school [19, 20]. Studies have shown an increase in sleep duration for primary school-age students based on changing to an earlier bed time and maintaining a consistent wake time [21–23]. Together, these studies suggest primary school students are able to fall asleep earlier if they have an earlier bedtime set for them, allowing them the opportunity to obtain sufficient sleep duration, even with an earlier school start time. However, bedtimes are often set by parents, and are part of greater family routines and schedules. Thus, even though primary school students are biologically able to fall asleep earlier than adolescents, few studies have considered whether the implementation of earlier primary school start times (as a result of delayed secondary school start times) results in changes to sleep routines and sleep duration.

Only two studies in the United States have prospectively examined how district changes to start times impact primary school students’ sleep [24, 25]. Qualitative findings from a large, diverse urban district in Minnesota reported that moving to later start times (8:40–9:40 am) negatively impacted transportation, student behavior, staff meetings, morning teaching and learning, and afternoon student fatigue. Schools that moved earlier (9:40 am to either 8:40 am or 7:40 am) reported students benefitting from fewer transitions before school, having fewer behavior problems, and being more alert and focused during the day [24]. In the northeast United States, minimal changes in sleep were found after moving start times earlier (3rd grade: 9:10–7:45 am, 4th and 5th grade: 8:20–7:45 am). Students reported earlier bedtimes and wake times, increased sleep duration for 3rd graders (24 min), minimally decreased sleep duration for 4th and 5th graders (4 and 9 min, respectively), and no change in daytime sleepiness [25]. However, the study sample included only one school that was predominantly white (97.8%) and not economically diverse. No studies have concurrently considered the impact on changing start times on sleep for students across grades from Kindergarten through 12th grade.

Finally, questions also remain about whether the impact of changing school start times is consistent across different racial and economic groups of students. Many studies that have looked at the impact of changes to school start times on sleep have focused on schools where the majority of students were non-Hispanic white and/or did not qualify for free or reduced lunch (FRL), a proxy schools often use for poverty (e.g. Refs. [25–28]). Across studies, even those with somewhat more diverse samples, the impact of changing start times on sleep by race or FRL status has not been considered. However, both racial minority and low socioeconomic status students at all levels may be disproportionately impacted by early start times (e.g. unable to get to school if they oversleep and miss the bus) [29], thus, it is important to consider these variables.

As a result, there remains a significant need for rigorous, longitudinal research that includes large, diverse samples, to demonstrate how this important health policy impacts all students from Kindergarten through 12th grade. In August 2017, the Cherry Creek School District (CCSD), a diverse district of ~55,000 students in suburban Denver, Colorado, changed school start times, delaying high schools (HS, grades 9–12, typical ages 14–18 years) by 70 min (to 8:20 am) and middle schools (MS, grades 6–8, typical ages 11–14 years) by 40–60 min (to 8:50 am), while moving elementary schools (ES, grades K-5, typical ages 5–11 years) 60 min earlier (to start at 8:00 am) [30]. To address knowledge gaps noted above, the Changing Start Times: Longitudinal Effects Study (CaSTLES) was developed to evaluate outcomes in the CCSD before and for 2 years after the start time change. The aim of this paper is to examine the impact of changing school start times on student sleep, including bedtime, wake time, sleep duration, sleep quality, and daytime sleepiness on students K-12.

Methods

Study design and participants

Students enrolled in the CCSD and their parents were invited to participate in CaSTLES in spring 2017 (pre-change, ~4 months before start times changed), spring 2018 (post-change, ~6 months post-change), and once again in spring 2019 (follow-up, ~18 months post-change). Pre-change surveys included students in grades 3 through 11 (and parents of students in Kindergarten [K] through 11th grade), and both the post-change and follow-up surveys included students in grades 3 through 12 (and parents of students in K through 12th grade). There were no exclusion criteria. Approximately 24,000–30,000 students chose to participate in the study each year. Study surveys and procedures were approved by the CCSD Research Review Committee, and all applicable ethical standards were followed. Parents were informed of the upcoming survey through multiple email and phone notifications from CCSD in their preferred language (i.e. English, Amharic, Arabic, Chinese, Korean, Russian, Spanish, and Vietnamese), with the option to have their student not participate; less than one-half of one percent of all parents in the district opted out of the study. Student surveys were administered on laptops or tablets during designated class periods. Prior to starting the survey, students were informed both verbally and within the survey that participation was voluntary. If needed, survey questions/responses were read aloud to students. Parent surveys were sent via email (in the parent’s preferred language), with multiple email and phone reminders to complete the survey. Some families had two parents or caregivers complete surveys, but only the primary caregiver report is included in this paper. Survey data were collected with SurveyGizmo (Boulder, CO). Demographic information was provided by the school district, including race and FRL status.

Outcome measures

Sleep timing and duration

Student and parent surveys separately asked about students’ typical bedtime and wake time on both weekdays and weekends. Times were selected from drop down menus in 5 min increments, providing the following outcomes for weekdays and weekends: (1) bedtime; (2) wake time; (3) sleep duration (hours between bedtime and wake time); and (4) weekend oversleep (difference between weekday and weekend sleep duration).

Sleep quality and daytime sleepiness

Three items from the PROMIS Pediatric Sleep Disturbances item bank and two items from the PROMIS Pediatric Sleep Related Impairment item bank were included to measure sleep quality and daytime sleepiness [31]. All questions had a 7-day recall period (In the past 7 days…), and used a 5-point Likert response scale (“Always” to “Never”). T-scores (mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10) were derived based on national normative data through the Health Measures Scoring Service (assessmentcenter.net/ac_scoringservice).

Data analysis

As this was an anonymous survey, it was not possible to link the full sample of students across years. Therefore, we utilized three approaches for examining the data. The ecological approach was our primary analysis, with data averaged within level, school, race, and year. Sleep outcomes were then fit as a function of race, year, and race-by-year, using linear mixed models, stratified by levels (ES, middle school [MS], high school [HS]). To account for correlated data within schools, a Kronecker Product structure was used for the error covariance structure, with an Unstructured covariance structure for race (5 levels: White, Black, Hispanic, Asian, Mixed Race/American Indian or Alaskan Native/Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander [MR/AIAN/NHOPI]) and a first-order autoregressive (AR(1)) structure for year (pre, post, follow-up). This model allowed us to examine whether changes over time in mean sleep outcomes were modified by race. Since some schools had more participants than others, each outcome mean was weighted by the number of subjects used in the average. A second set of models, using a similar approach, were used to test for free and reduced lunch status (FRL, 2 levels: yes or no) as a predictor. Using the methods described above, we carried out tests to determine whether there were significant race-by-time or FRL-by-time interactions.

We also examined the data using a unit-level approach, with models fit as previously described, but using individual subjects in the model fit, and correlated data were accounted for by including a random intercept for school (rather than a non-simple error covariance structure). Results were similar to the ecological approach, and unless otherwise noted, are not reported. The third approach was descriptive, with means and asymptotic confidence intervals computed for each year (by level); however, this approach does not allow for statistical comparisons between years.

In addition, descriptive statistics were used to identify the proportion of students each year who (1) were obtaining sufficient sleep, (2) had poor sleep quality, or (3) had significant daytime sleepiness. Sufficient weekday sleep duration for ES and MS students was defined as at least 9 h, and for HS students as at least 8 h [4]. Poor sleep quality and significant daytime sleepiness were defined as a T-score ≥ 60 (1 SD above the mean) on the PROMIS measures [31, 32]. When reporting student-reported outcomes, parent-proxy data for younger ES students (grades K-2) are provided. Parent-proxy data for older ES students (grades 3–5), MS, and HS students are reported separately. Given the large study sample size and number of analyses, findings were considered significant if p < 0.001.

Results

Sample characteristics

Sample demographics are shown in Table 1. Older ES (grades 3–5) student participation was consistent across years (~77%), with increased participation over time in MS (~70%–79%) and HS students (52%–60%). Notably fewer parents of younger ES students (grades K-2) participated (~28%–37%). The sample was similar to the district in terms of gender, although some groups were less representative of students who qualify for FRL (district average 29.0%) or Hispanic students (district average 20.1%). However, this was not consistent across years or levels.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of students

Younger elementary school (Grades K-2) Older elementary school (Grades 3–5)
Pre-change Post-change Follow-up Pre-change Post-change Follow-up
Total N 4,207 3,159 3,149 9,604 9,812 9,720
 % enrolled 37.2 27.9 27.5 77.0 77.3 77.8
 % female 52.6 53.8 52.0 47.6 48.6 48.7
 % FRL 18.4 16.4 15.6 22.3 27.4 18.7
Grade
 % K 31.3 30.4 31.1
 % 1st 34.1 35.3 34.8
 % 2nd 34.6 34.3 34.1
 % 3rd 34.0 31.7 30.3
 % 4th 33.7 34.8 33.6
 % 5th 32.4 33.5 36.1
Race/ethnicity
 % White 63.1 63.6 63.9 57.9 55.0 51.8
 % Black 5.2 4.4 4.4 9.5 10.4 10.2
 % Hispanic 14.4 14.4 14.4 16.1 17.5 20.4
 % Asian 8.6 8.3 8.6 8.1 8.1 8.5
 % MR/AIAN/NHOPI 8.8 9.3 8.7 8.4 9.0 9.1
Middle school (Grades 6–8) High school (Grades 9–12)
Pre-change Post-change Follow-up Pre-change Post-change Follow-up
Total N 8,414 9,619 9,915 6,275 9,849 10,516
 % Enrolled 69.6 79.1 75.7 51.9 59.8 60.4
 % Female 51.0 49.8 49.1 52.6 51.2 51.3
 % FRL 20.2 24.6 18.1 17.8 21.3 15.3
Grade
 % 6th 35.3 31.8 33.3
 % 7th 32.8 34.3 33.8
 % 8th 31.9 34.0 32.9
 % 9th 39.7 30.1 26.9
 % 10th 33.0 28.6 26.2
 % 11th 27.3 24.6 26.0
 % 12th 16.7 20.9
Race/ethnicity
 % White 58.7 56.6 53.1 57.0 56.4 52.0
 % Black 10.3 11.2 10.4 11.2 11.3 11.3
 % Hispanic 15.1 16.7 20.5 15.4 16.9 19.9
 % Asian 8.9 8.2 8.8 9.8 9.7 10.1
 % MR/AIAN/NHOPI 6.9 7.2 7.3 6.5 6.6 6.8

FRL, free or reduced lunch status; MR/AIAN/NHOPI, mixed race/American Indian or Alaskan Native/Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander.

Sleep outcomes: full sample

Complete results using descriptive analyses for sleep outcomes by level are found in Table 2, with graphic representation of weekday bedtime, wake time, and sleep duration by level found in Figure 1. After the start time change (ES starting 60 min earlier), younger ES students had earlier average weekday bedtimes (11 min) and wake times (22 min), and decreased average weekday sleep duration (11 min). On weekends, younger ES students had average bedtimes and wake times that were earlier (11 and 10 min, respectively), with no difference in weekend duration. Average weekday and weekend sleep timing and duration remained relatively consistent at follow-up (change ≤4 min). Weekend oversleep increased by an average of 12 min at post-change. The percent of younger ES students obtaining sufficient sleep duration or having poor sleep quality did not differ at post-change or follow-up, with only a small increase in the percent of students reporting daytime sleepiness (4%) at post-change. However, at follow-up, the percent of students reporting daytime sleepiness was lower than pre-change.

Table 2.

Means (95% CIs) and differences for sleep outcome variables across years using descriptive analytical methods

Difference
Pre-change Post-change Follow-up Post–Pre Follow-up–Post
Younger elementary school (Grades K-2)
Bedtime*
 Weekday 20:32
(20:31–20:34)
20:21
(20:19–20:22)
20:19
(20:17–20:20)
–11 min –2 min
 Weekend 21:10
(21:09–21:12)
20:59
(20:57–21:01)
20:56
(20:54–20:58)
–11 min –3 min
Wake time*
 Weekday 7:04
(7:03– 7:06)
6:42
(6:41–6:43)
6:41
(6:40–6:43)
–22 min –1 min
 Weekend 7:42
(7:41–7:44)
7:32
(7:30–7:34)
7:28
(7:26–7:30)
–10 min –4 min
Duration*
 Weekday 10.53 h
(10.51–10.56)
10.35 h
(10.33–10.38)
10.38 h
(10.36–10.41)
–11 min +2 min
 Weekend 10.54 h
(10.51–10.56)
10.55 h
(10.52–10.58)
10.53 h
(10.49–10.56)
+ <1 min –2 min
Weekend oversleep*,† 0.2 min
(–1.3 to 1.6)
12.1 min
(10.3–13.9)
8.7 min
(7.0–10.5)
+11.9 min –3.4 min
% sufficient sleep 98.6% 98.2% 98.7% –0.4% +0.5%
% poor sleep quality§ 22.3% 21.0% 19.7% –1.3% –1.3%
% daytime sleepiness|| 34.8% 38.8% 33.0% +4.0% –5.8%
Older elementary school (Grades 3–5)
Bedtime*
 Weekday 21:13
(21:11–21:14)
21:01
(21:00–21:02)
21:02
(21:01–21:03)
–12 min +1 min
 Weekend 22:21
(22:19–22:23)
22:19
(22:17–22:21)
22:20
(22:18–22:22)
–2 min +1 min
Wake time*
 Weekday 7:05
(7:04–7:06)
6:42
(6:41–6:43)
6:42
(6:41–6:43)
–23 min No change
 Weekend 8:33
(8:31–8:35)
8:33
(8:31–8:35)
8:33
(8:31–8:35)
No change No change
Duration*
 Weekday 9.87 h
(9.85–9.90)
9.69 h
(9.67–9.72)
9.67 h
(9.64–9.69)
–11 min –1 min
 Weekend 10.19 h
(10.16–10.23)
10.23 h
(10.19–10.26)
10.22 h
(10.18–10.25)
+2 min – <1 min
Weekend Oversleep*,† 14 min
(12–16)
26 min
(24–28)
28 min
(26–30)
+12 min +2 min
% Sufficient sleep 83.5% 81.2% 80.2% –2.3% –1.0%
% Poor sleep quality§ 24.1% 25.8% 23.8% +1.7% –2.0%
% Daytime sleepiness|| 22.4% 26.5% 25.8% +4.1% –0.7%
Middle school (Grades 6–8)
Bedtime*
 Weekday 21:49
(21:48–21:51)
21:58
(21:56–21:59)
21:59
(21:57–22:00)
+9 min +1 min
 Weekend 23:22
(23:20–23:24)
23:32
(23:30–23:34)
23:30
(23:28–23:32)
+10 min –2 min
Wake time*
 Weekday 06:27
(06:26–06:28)
07:04
(07:04–07:05)
07:02
(07:02–07:03)
+37 min –2 min
 Weekend 09:12
(09:10–09:14)
09:20
(09:18–09:22)
09:16
(09:14–09:18)
+8 min –4 min
Duration*
 Weekday 8.63 h
(8.60–8.65)
9.11 h
(9.09–9.14)
9.06 h
(9.04–9.09)
+29 min –3 min
 Weekend 9.83 h
(9.79–9.86)
9.79 h
(9.76–9.82)
9.78 h
(9.75–9.81)
–2 min – <1 min
Weekend oversleep*,† 1.13 h
(1.10–1.17)
0.60 h
(0.57–0.63)
0.64 h
(0.61–0.68)
–32 min +2 min
% Sufficient sleep 40.5% 61.0% 59.6% +20.5% –1.4%
% Poor sleep quality§ 30.3% 27.7% 28.9% –2.6% +1.2%
% Daytime sleepiness|| 48.7% 37.1% 38.5% –11.6% +1.4%
High school (Grades 9–12)
Bedtime*
 Weekday 22:23
(22:21–22:25)
22:37
(22:36–22:39)
22:46
(22:44–22:47)
+14 min +9 min
 Weekend 23:47
(23:45–23:50)
00:02
(00:01–00:04)
00:06
(00:05–00:08)
+15 min +4 min
Wake time*
 Weekday 05:46
(05:45–05:47)
06:46
(06:45–06:47)
06:45
(06:44–06:46)
+60 min –1 min
 Weekend 09:16
(09:14–09:19)
09:27
(09:25–09:29)
09:27
(09:25–09:29)
+11 min No change
Duration*
 Weekday 7.39 h
(7.36–7.41)
8.14 h
(8.12–8.17)
7.99 h
(7.97–8.01)
+45 min –9 min
 Weekend 9.48 h
(9.44–9.52)
9.41 h
(9.38–9.44)
9.34 h
(9.31–9.37)
–4 min –4 min
Weekend oversleep*,† 2.05 h (2.01–2.09) 1.21 h
(1.18–1.24)
1.28 h
(1.25–1.31)
–77 min +4 min
% Sufficient sleep 30.4% 62.7% 57.6% +32.3% –5.1%
% Poor sleep quality§ 43.1% 31.2% 35.3% –11.9% +4.1%
% Daytime sleepiness|| 76.2% 55.2% 62.0% –21.0% +6.8%

h = hours, min = minutes.

*Data are presented as mean (95% CI), with military time used for bedtimes and wake times.

Weekend oversleep is the difference between weekday and weekend sleep duration.

Sufficient sleep defined as an average of at least 9 h for ES and MS, and at least 8 h for HS4.

§Poor sleep quality defined as T ≥ 60 on PROMIS Pediatric Sleep Disturbance items.

||Daytime sleepiness defined as T ≥ 60 on PROMIS Pediatric Sleep Related Impairment items.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Changes in weekday sleep outcomes by school level using descriptive analyses.

For older ES students, a similar change was reported post-change for both average weekday bedtime (12 min earlier) and wake time (23 min earlier), resulting in a decrease in average weekday sleep duration of 11 min. There were no significant differences post-change in average weekend bedtimes, wake times, or sleep duration. Both average weekday and weekend sleep timing and duration remained consistent at follow-up. Average weekend oversleep increased by 12 min at post-change. The percent of older ES students reporting sufficient sleep duration slightly decreased (~2%) at post-change, while the percent of students reporting poor sleep quality or daytime sleepiness slightly increased (~2% and ~4%, respectively).

Post-change (MS starting 40–60 min later), MS students reported a slightly later average weekday bedtime (9 min), but a much later average weekday wake time (37 min), resulting in a clinically meaningful increase in average weekday sleep duration of 29 min. Average weekend bedtimes and wake times were slightly later post-change (10 and 8 min, respectively), resulting in no notable change in mean weekend sleep duration. Both average weekday and weekend timing and duration changes were maintained at follow-up. Weekend oversleep decreased post-change by an average of 32 min, with this difference maintained at follow-up. The percent of MS students obtaining sufficient sleep increased by ~21% post-change. Although there was only a small decrease in the percent of students with poor sleep quality (~3%), there was a notable decrease (~12%) in MS students reporting daytime sleepiness post-change. These differences were maintained at follow-up.

Finally, post-change (HS starting 70 min later) HS students reported a slightly later average weekday bedtime (14 min), but a later average weekday wake time (60 min), resulting in a clinically meaningful increase in average weekday sleep duration of 45 min. Average weekend bedtimes and wake times were also later (15 and 11 min, respectively), resulting in a relatively consistent average weekend sleep duration. Average weekday and weekend sleep timing and duration changes were maintained at follow-up. Average weekend oversleep was significantly reduced (77 min), with the percent of HS students with clinically significant oversleep (≥2 h) on weekends reduced from 54.6% pre-change to 31.4% at post-change and 33.2% at follow-up. The percent of HS students obtaining sufficient weekday sleep increased ~32% at post-change, with fewer students reporting poor sleep quality (~12%) and daytime sleepiness (21%) post-change.

For older ES, MS, and HS students, parent-proxy sleep outcomes are found in Supplemental Table 1 (ST-1). Overall at post-change, parent-reported sleep was similar to student-reported sleep, with a decrease in average weekday sleep duration for older ES (17 min) and a clinically significant increase in average weekday sleep duration for both MS (30 min) and HS (43 min), with no significant change in average weekend sleep duration for all three groups. Parents also reported a slightly earlier average weekday bedtime at post-change for ES students (9 min), with a slightly later mean weekday bedtime for MS students (6 min) and HS students (12 min). Average weekday wake times were earlier at post-change for older ES students (26 min), and later for MS students (36 min) and HS students (56 min). Finally, similar to student-report, parents reported an increase in average weekend oversleep for older ES students (18 min), but a decrease in average weekend oversleep for MS students (29 min) and HS students (46 min).

Sleep outcomes by race and FRL status

The fitted linear mixed models using the ecological approach were used to examine whether differences in sleep outcome variables were modified by race or FRL status. Models for weekday and weekend sleep duration by level are found in Table 3, with weekday and weekend bedtime and wake time models by level found in Supplemental Table 2 (ST-2). Least square means for weekday and weekend sleep duration main effects are found in Table 4 (Race models) and Table 5 (FRL models), with means for weekday and weekend bedtime and wake time presented in Supplemental Table 3 (ST-3, bedtime) and Supplemental Table 4 (ST-4, wake time). Figures 2–4 present year-by-race and year-by-FRL interactions for all sleep variables by level, which are based on the ecological analytical approach unless otherwise noted (see Table 3 and ST-2 notes).

Table 3.

F-Statistics and p-values for key predictors in linear mixed models for weekday and weekend sleep duration

F* P F P
Later elementary school (Grades 3–5)
Weekday sleep duration Weekday sleep duration
 Year 55.55 <0.0001 Year 43.76 <0.0001
 Race 28.31 <0.0001 FRL 32.28 <0.0001
 Race × year 2.41 0.0155 FRL × year 1.50 0.2304
Weekend sleep duration Weekend sleep duration
 Year 0.32 0.7265 Year 0.01 0.9922
 Race 2.00 0.0967 FRL 1.39 0.2457
 Race × year 2.18 0.0290 FRL × year 1.73 0.1835
Middle school (Grades 6–8)
Weekday sleep duration Weekday sleep duration
 Year 121.55 <0.0001 Year 325.02 <0.0001
 Race 6.12 0.0006 FRL 0.00 0.9812
 Race × year 1.29 0.2607 FRL × year 2.72 0.0657
Weekend sleep duration Weekend sleep duration
 Year 0.45 0.6468 Year 0.97 0.3969
 Race 1.21 0.3201 FRL 3.36 0.0968
 Race × year 2.09 0.0475 FRL × year 0.57 0.5756
High school (Grades 9–12)
Weekday sleep duration Weekday sleep duration
 Year 252.36 <0.0001 Year 202.31 <0.0001
 Race 18.82 <0.0001 FRL 7.39 0.0418
 Race × year 1.31 0.2656 FRL × year 1.15 0.3546
Weekend sleep duration Weekend sleep duration
 Year 3.07 0.0911 Year 11.89 0.0023
 Race 1.28 0.3124 FRL 4.94 0.0768
 Race × year 3.22 0.0064 FRL × year 4.35 0.0436

Results for models with race are shown on left, those with FRL status on right. The ecological modeling approach was used to obtain results, unless otherwise noted.

*Numerator DF were as follows: Year = 2, Race = 4, Year × Race = 8; denominator DF ranged from 80 to 314.

Numerator DF were as follows: Year = 2, FRL = 1, Year x FRL = 1; denominator DF ranged from 41 to 80.

Standard models did not converge, so for these conditions, so the unit modeling approach described in the methods section is reported.

Table 4.

Means and 95% CI for weekday and weekend sleep duration main effects in race models (hours; year means averaged across race, race means averaged across years)

Later elementary (Grades 3–5) Middle school (Grades 6–8) High school (Grades 9–12)
Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend
Year
 Pre-change 9.84
(9.80–9.89)
10.21
(10.16–10.27)
8.61
(8.56–8.67)
9.80
(9.72–9.87)
7.34
(7.27–7.41)
9.46
(9.36–9.57)
 Post-change 9.62
(9.58–9.66)
10.19
(10.13–10.24)
9.09
(9.04–9.14)
9.75
(9.69–9.82)
8.12
(8.07–8.17)
9.40
(9.31–9.48)
 Follow-up 9.59
(9.55–9.62)
10.18
(10.13–10.24)
9.05
(9.00–9.09)
9.78
(9.72–9.85)
7.96
(7.91–8.01)
9.37
(9.29–9.44)
Race
 White 9.81
(9.79–9.84)
10.22
(10.19–10.25)
8.98
(8.94–9.01)
9.81
(9.78–9.85)
7.86
(7.83–7.90)
9.43
(9.37–9.50)
 Black 9.54
(9.48–9.59)
10.14
(10.05–10.22)
8.87
(8.81–8.92)
9.76
(9.68–9.84)
7.89
(7.79–7.99)
9.45
(9.32–9.58)
 Hispanic 9.70
(9.66–9.75)
10.20
(10.14–10.26)
8.91
(8.86–8.96)
9.75
(9.68–9.83)
7.94
(7.88–8.01)
9.43
(9.33–9.53)
 Asian 9.69
(9.63–9.74)
10.26
(10.18–10.34)
8.92
(8.87–8.97)
9.81
(9.72–9.90)
7.56
(7.49–7.63)
9.40
(9.27–9.54)
 MR/AIAN/NHOPI 9.68
(9.62–9.74)
10.17
(10.10–10.24)
8.91
(8.86–8.97)
9.75
(9.64–9.87)
7.78
(7.72–7.85)
9.33
(9.22–9.43)

The ecological modeling approach was used to obtain results.

Table 5.

Means and 95% CI for weekday and weekend sleep duration main effects in FRL status models (hours; year means averaged across FRL status, FRL status means averaged across years)

Later elementary (Grades 3–5) Middle school (Grades 6–8) High school (Grades 9–12)
Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend
Year
 Pre-change 9.84
(9.80–9.88)
10.22
(10.18–10.27)
8.63
(8.58–8.68)
9.84
(9.77–9.91)
7.41
(7.34–7.49)
9.51
(9.38–9.63)
 Post-change 9.65
(9.61–9.68)
10.22
(10.18–10.27)
9.14
(9.09–9.19)
9.82
(9.76–9.88)
8.17
(8.12–8.23)
9.49
(9.40–9.58)
 Follow-up 9.62
(9.58–9.66)
10.23
(10.17–10.28)
9.06
(9.01–9.11)
9.79
(9.73–9.85)
8.01
(7.95–8.07)
9.38
(9.28–9.48)
FRL status
 FRL 9.63
(9.59–9.68)
10.24
(10.19–10.29)
8.95
(8.90–8.99)
9.84
(9.78–9.91)
7.89
(7.83–7.96)
9.50
(9.37–9.64)
 Not FRL 9.77
(9.75–9.80)
10.21
(10.18–10.23)
8.94
(8.90–8.99)
9.79
(9.74–9.84)
7.84
(7.80–7.88)
9.41
(9.31–9.51)

The ecological modeling approach was used to obtain results.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Sleep outcomes (mean and 95% CI) for later elementary school students (grades 3–5); (A) by race and year; (B) by FRL status and year using ecological analyses.

For older ES students, no statistically significant interactions were observed for either race-by-year (Figure 2, A) or FRL-by-year (Figure 2, B) across all sleep outcomes on weekdays and weekends (Table 3 and ST-2). In both race and FRL models, a main effect was found for year (comparing findings by year, averaged across race, or FRL status), with significant changes similar to those previously reported in the descriptive findings for all weekday sleep outcome variables (i.e., earlier bedtime, earlier wake time, shorter sleep duration, all p < 0.0001), but no significant change in weekend sleep outcome variables across years (all p > 0.5). A main effect for race (comparing races averaged over years) was also found for all sleep variables (all p < 0.0001), except weekend sleep duration. Similarly, a main effect of FRL (comparing FRL status averaged over years) was found for weekday and weekend bedtimes, weekday sleep duration, and weekend wake times (all p < 0.0001). Means for all main effects can be found in Tables 4 and 5, ST-3, and ST-4.

For MS students, no statistically significant interactions were observed for either race-by-year (Figure 3, A) or FRL-by-year (Figure 3, B) across all sleep outcomes on weekdays and weekends (Table 3 and ST-2). A main effect for year, showing significant changes across the 3 years of the study, was found for all weekday sleep variables in both race and FRL models, as well as for weekend bedtime in the FRL model (all p < 0.0001). The direction of these changes was similar to previously reported descriptive results. A significant main effect for race was found for all sleep variables, except weekend sleep duration, while a significant main effect for FRL status was found only for weekend bedtime and wake time (all p < 0.0001). Main effect means are found in Tables 4 and 5, ST-3, and ST-4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Sleep outcomes (mean and 95% CI) for middle school students (grades 6–8); (A) by race and year; (B) by FRL status and year.

For HS students, no statistically significant interactions were observed for either race-by-year (Figure 4, A) or FRL-by-year (Figure 4, B) across all sleep outcomes on weekdays and weekends (Table 3 and ST-2). Interactions for race-by-year were borderline significant for weekend bedtime (p = 0.0019) and weekend sleep duration (p = 0.0064). Mean weekend bedtimes were later across all groups, with the smallest delay from pre-change to post-change in Black students (~11 min) and the largest delay in White students (~18 min). Weekend sleep duration changes were greatest for MR/AIAN/NHOPI students (14 min longer), with no change for Asian students. Significant main effects for year (all p ≤ 0.0001) were found for all weekday sleep variables and weekend bedtime in both the race and FRL status models, with borderline significant main effects for year found for weekend wake time in both models (p ≤ 0.004), and weekend sleep duration (p = 0.0023). The direction of changes in sleep outcomes were similar to those previously reported in the descriptive results. A significant main effect for race was found for weekday bedtime and weekday sleep duration (p < 0.0001), with no significant main effects for FRL status found. See Tables 4 and 5, ST-3, and ST-4 for all main effect means.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Sleep outcomes (mean and 95% CI) for high school students (grades 9–12); (A) by race and year; (B) by FRL status and year.

Discussion

This is the first large scale study to concurrently examine the impact of changing school start times on sleep for primary and secondary school students in grades K-12, to provide follow-up data for 2 years post-change, to include both student and parent-proxy report, and to consider the impact on sleep by sociodemographic variables. Similar to previous studies [28, 33–39], results from CaSTLES confirm the significant benefit of later secondary school start times for MS and HS students’ sleep and daytime functioning, with benefits maintained for up to 2 years. Also similar to the one published study of US 3rd to 5th graders [25], CaSTLES found earlier bedtimes and wake times after changing to an earlier ES start time. As the earlier bedtimes were not proportional to earlier wake times, a small decrease in sleep duration was found. However, it is important to note that this 11-min difference is not considered clinically meaningful [22, 40], and the changes in the percent of ES students who obtained sufficient sleep duration or experienced daytime sleepiness was small. Furthermore, the reported post-change sleep duration remained consistent with previous studies of child sleep [18, 41, 42]. That said, it is critical for districts who are transitioning to earlier ES start times to provide education to families on the importance of sleep. As differences between racial and economic groups was found for weekday sleep variables, and with an increased recognition of sleep disparities that influence children’s sleep patterns [43, 44], steps should be taken to identify and ameliorate systemic factors that contribute to these differences. In addition, education programs should be developed in partnership with families to ensure that recommendations for adjusting bedtimes and increasing sleep opportunities are sensitive to sociocultural and environmental factors.

Sleep duration showed the greatest change for HS students, who obtained an extra 3.8 h per week, with MS students getting an extra 2.4 h of sleep per week. This resulted in a significant increase in the percent of students who obtained sufficient sleep after implementation of later start times (MS: pre 40.5%, post 61.0%, HS: pre 30.4%, post 62.7%). With ~15,000–20,000 MS and HS students per year in CaSTLES, this represents a large and significant number of students who are now obtaining sufficient sleep. The finding of this magnitude is in stark contrast to the more conservative goals and outcomes of Healthy People 2020 [45], which aimed to increase the percent of students obtaining sufficient sleep from 30.9% to 33.1% over 10 years. Unfortunately, the most recent national Youth Risk Behavior Survey reported that the percent of US adolescents obtaining sufficient sleep had actually decreased to 25.4% [46].

For MS and HS students, later weekday wake times were the most significant factor contributing to increased weekday sleep duration. One notable benefit of increased weekday sleep duration was the significant decrease in weekend oversleep, where a 2+ hour difference between weekday and weekend sleep duration is used as a marker of significant clinical sleep deprivation [1]. “Social jetlag” results from significantly shifting sleep–wake schedules on weekends, and has been associated with negative adolescent outcomes, including poorer mental health [16, 47, 48]. For HS students, the average weekend oversleep dropped from just over 2 h pre-change to 1.2 h post-change, suggesting that with sufficient weekday sleep, students are no longer clinically sleep deprived and needing to “catch up” on sleep on weekends. For adolescents with delayed sleep–wake phase disorder, another potential benefit of later start times, and thus later wake times, is better alignment with their natural circadian rhythm. Future reports should examine changes in sleep by chronotype (circadian preference).

Consistent with previous studies [27, 28, 33, 34, 36, 37, 39, 49, 50], weekday bedtimes for MS and HS students were not significantly or proportionately delayed with later start times, yet weekday wake times were significantly later. As weekend bedtimes were also minimally delayed, later weekday bedtimes were more likely a result of non-start time factors (e.g. developmental changes). Similar to ES students, racial differences in weekday bedtimes were found for both MS and HS students, suggesting again the importance of working with all families to identify and provide strategies, both structural and educational, for improving sleep health across populations of students.

For ES students, it was notable that parent-proxy and student-reported weekday wake times were nearly identical, highlighting the association between school start times and weekday sleep. However, compared to parent reports about their students, older ES students (grades 3–5) reported later weekday bedtimes (~15 min), later weekend bedtimes (~35–45 min), and later weekend wake times (~30 min). These findings highlight that parents may become less involved with bedtime and sleep routines as children get older [51–54]. In addition, the bedtime discrepancies may suggest that children do not fall asleep as early as parents believe.

While parents of MS and HS students also reported an increase in student sleep duration, parent-reported sleep duration was longer than student self-reported sleep duration. As parents and students reported similar student wake times, this discrepancy resulted from earlier parent-reported bedtimes for students (20–28 min difference). Similar to ES students, these findings support the fact that parental involvement with bedtime decreases with child age; however, parents often remain actively involved in waking adolescents on weekday mornings [55, 56].

Unlike previous studies, CaSTLES examined changes in sleep by socio-demographic variables (race and FRL status). Although there were significant differences between racial and economic groups for some sleep outcome variables, no significant interactions were found between sleep and socio-demographic variables over the 3 years of the study, suggesting that the change in start times did not impact racial and economic groups differently. For example, it is important to note that across racial groups, MS students reported an average increase of 24–33 min in weekday sleep duration, regardless of racial group, while high school students reported an average increase in sleep duration of 43–52 min. Although these data suggest benefits of later secondary school start times across student groups, it is important for future studies to continue examining potential sleep disparities.

Beyond sleep continuity (bedtime, wake time, duration), CaSTLES also examined the percent of students with clinically significant poor sleep quality and daytime sleepiness. For ES students, there was only a minimal decrease in sleep duration with earlier start times, with only a small percent of students reporting changes to sleep quality or daytime sleepiness. For MS students, no change in sleep quality was reported, however, there was a notable decrease in students reporting clinically significant daytime sleepiness (translating to ~1,100 students). For HS students, later school start times positively impacted both sleep quality and daytime sleepiness, with more than 1 in 10 students reporting improved sleep quality and 1 in 5 students reporting reduced daytime sleepiness post-change.

While this study has many strengths, several limitations should also be noted. First, all results are survey based, with no objective measures of sleep. However, having both student and parent reporters provides reasonable validity to the outcomes. Second, although data were collected across 3 years, only one survey per year (spring) was collected. It is possible that sleep patterns were different in the fall/winter, but using the same time frame window each year to collect survey data may mitigate the possible seasonal differences. Third, similar to previous studies, students in this study were not tracked across years, thus data were aggregated within a higher unit and it was not possible to tease out potential developmental factors that contributed to changes in outcomes. Fourth, this study did not include a control group. Although it is possible that sleep outcomes changed for reasons other than or in addition to different start times, the lack of change in sleep duration across the United States in the same time frame would suggest that later start times for secondary school students was the primary factor driving changes in sleep outcomes. Fifth, this paper focused only on sleep outcomes, thus no conclusions can be made at this time about the impact of changing school start times on academic, cognitive, or psychological outcomes. Finally, this study was conducted in suburban Denver, Colorado, and although the sample was somewhat diverse in terms of both race and socioeconomic status, the results may not be generalizable to either urban inner city school districts or rural school districts. That said, the examination of differences by sociodemographic variables suggests no significant negative sleep outcomes for changing to earlier start times across groups for ES students, and positive benefits of later start times across groups of MS and HS students.

Conclusion

CaSTLES, a longitudinal study that includes a large, diverse sample, provides both complementary and novel findings to the literature on changing school start times. For secondary school students, recommended healthy school start times (at or after 8:30 am) result in increased sleep duration and decreased daytime sleepiness. In order to achieve these outcomes however, for many districts, “flipping” primary and secondary school start times is required to accommodate transportation schedules. CaSTLES findings support the option of moving primary school start times earlier, although future studies are needed to determine the optimal start time for younger students. Furthermore, when logistically and financially feasible, a uniform later school start time would be ideal for students and families.

Although recently passed legislation (CA Senate Bill 328) will implement healthy start times for all California secondary school students (starting fall 2022), many districts across the United States are still determining whether they should also change their bell schedules. This study provides critical evidence of how a single policy initiative, healthy school start times, is a significant and effective way to improve sleep duration and daytime sleepiness for large numbers of secondary school students. However, additional rigorous studies are needed to continue answering the many questions raised by changing school start times, most notably, how early is too early for primary school students to start their school day. Finally, as a sufficient sleep opportunity is critical for all students, education about the importance of healthy sleep patterns needs to be developed and disseminated to all families. In order to ensure that education programs are sensitive to factors that may contribute to sleep disparities (e.g. activities, parent or student work schedules, shared bedrooms), these programs should be developed in partnership with diverse students and parents.

Supplementary Material

zsab048_suppl_Supplementary_Material

Acknowledgments

We thank all students and parents who participated in CaSTLES, the Cherry Creek School District and Superintendent Dr. Scott Siegfried for their support for conducting this research, the Assessment and Performance Analytics team and the Information Systems team, for assistance with recruitment and distribution of surveys, Ann Dosen for assistance with compiling district demographic data, and Janise McNally for all of her assistance throughout the study.

Disclosure Statements

Financial Disclosure. This study was supported by the Robert Wood Johnson’s Evidence for Action program (grant #75277). The study does not necessarily reflect the opinions or views of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Dr. Meltzer had full access to all data for the study and had the final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Non-financial Disclosure. Although Dr. Plog was involved with the study design, data collection, and preparation of the manuscript, all analyses were completed by Drs. Strand and Meltzer, independent of the Cherry Creek School District.

Data were collected through the Cherry Creek School District. All analyses were conducted at National Jewish Health.

Conflicts of interest statement. The authors have no financial conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1. Owens J, et al. . Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):e921–e932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Beebe DW. Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2011;58(3):649–665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Gregory AM, et al. . Sleep, emotional and behavioral difficulties in children and adolescents. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(2):129–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Healthy people 2030. https://www.healthypeople.gov/sites/default/files/ObjectivesPublicComment508.pdf. Accessed February 17, 2020.
  • 5. American Academy of Pediatrics. School start times for adolescents. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):642–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Watson NF, et al. . Delaying middle school and high school start times promotes student health and performance: an American academy of sleep medicine position statement. J Clin Sleep Med. 2017;13(4):623–625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. American Medical Association. Insufficient sleep in adolescents. https://policysearch.ama-assn.org/policyfinder/detail/school%20start%20time?uri=%2FAMADoc%2FHOD.xml-0-5024.xml. Accessed May 15, 2017.
  • 8. American Psychological Association. Later school start times promote adolescent well-being. https://www.apa.org/pi/families/resources/school-start-times.pdf. Accessed May 15, 2017.
  • 9. National Association of School Nurses, Society of Pediatric Nurses. Consensus statement: early school start times. https://www.startschoollater.net/uploads/9/7/9/6/9796500/position_statement_2016.10_society_of_pediatric_nurses.pdf. Accessed May 15, 2017.
  • 10. National PTA. Resolution: healthy sleep for adolescents. https://www.pta.org/docs/default-source/files/advocacy/2017-approved-convention-resolutions/healthy-sleep-for-adolescents-r.pdf. Accessed May 15, 2017.
  • 11. Wheaton AG, et al. . School start times, sleep, behavioral, health, and academic outcomes: a review of the literature. J Sch Health. 2016;86(5):363–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Wahlstrom KL, et al. . School start time effects on adolescent learning and academic performance, emotional health and behaviour. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2017;30(6):485–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Wheaton AG, et al. . School start times for middle school and high school students—United States, 2011-12 school year. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2015;64(30):809–813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. National Center for Education Statistics. Average school start time and percentage distribution of public school start time, by school type and selected school characteristics: 2017–18. https://nces.ed.gov/surveys/ntps/tables/ntps1718_2019082305_s12n.asp. Accessed April 14, 2019.
  • 15. Owens JA, et al. . School start time change: an in-depth examination of school districts in the United States. Mind Brain Educ. 2014;8(4):182–213. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Crowley SJ, et al. . An update on adolescent sleep: new evidence informing the perfect storm model. J Adolesc. 2018;67:55–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Gruber R, et al. . An actigraphic study of the sleep patterns of younger and older school-age children. Sleep Med. 2018;47:117–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Galland BC, et al. . Establishing normal values for pediatric nighttime sleep measured by actigraphy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep. 2018; 41(4). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Crowley SJ, et al. . A longitudinal assessment of sleep timing, circadian phase, and phase angle of entrainment across human adolescence. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e112199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Russo PM, et al. . Sleep habits and circadian preference in Italian children and adolescents. J Sleep Res. 2007;16(2):163–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Gruber R, et al. . Impact of sleep extension and restriction on children’s emotional lability and impulsivity. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):e1155–e1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Sadeh A, et al. . The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003;74(2):444–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Vriend J, et al. . Emotional and cognitive impact of sleep restriction in children. Sleep Med Clin. 2015;10(2):107–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Wahlstrom K. Elementary feedback on changed start times.https://conservancy.umn.edu/handle/11299/214078. Accessed November 11, 2018.
  • 25. Appleman ER, et al. . School start time changes and sleep patterns in elementary school students. Sleep Health. 2015;1(2):109–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Owens JA, et al. . A quasi-experimental study of the impact of school start time changes on adolescent sleep. Sleep Health. 2017;3(6):437–443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Thacher PV, et al. . Longitudinal outcomes of start time delay on sleep, behavior, and achievement in high school. Sleep. 2016;39(2):271–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Widome R, et al. . Association of delaying school start time with sleep duration, timing, and quality among adolescents. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):697–704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Carskadon MA. The Time has come to expand our studies of school timing for adolescents. J Biol Rhythms. 2020;35(4):323–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Meltzer LJ, et al. . Engaging the community in the process of changing school start times: experience of the Cherry Creek School District. Sleep Health. 2017;3(6):472–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Forrest CB, et al. . Development and validation of the PROMIS pediatric sleep disturbance and sleep-related impairment item banks. Sleep. 2018;41(6). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Meltzer LJ, et al. . Clinical validity of the PROMIS pediatric sleep measures across populations of children with chronic illnesses and neurodevelopment disorders. J Pediatr Psychol. 2020;45( 3): 319–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Wahlstrom K. Changing times: findings from the first longitudinal study of later high school start times. NASSP Bull. 2002; 86(633):3–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Boergers J, et al. . Later school start time is associated with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35(1):11–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Wahlstrom K, et al. . Examining the Impact of Later School Start Times on the Health and Academic Performance of High School Students: A Multi-Site Study. St. Paul, MN: Center for Applied Research and Educational Improvement: University of Minnesota. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Wolfson AR, et al. . Middle school start times: the importance of a good night’s sleep for young adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5(3):194–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Owens JA, et al. . Impact of delaying school start time on adolescent sleep, mood, and behavior. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(7):608–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Lo JC, et al. . Sustained benefits of delaying school start time on adolescent sleep and well-being. Sleep. 2018;41(6). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Dunster GP, et al. . Sleepmore in Seattle: later school start times are associated with more sleep and better performance in high school students. Sci Adv. 2018;4(12):eaau6200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Gringras P, et al. . Efficacy and safety of pediatric prolonged-release melatonin for insomnia in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56(11):948–957.e4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Short MA, et al. . Cognition and objectively measured sleep duration in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Health. 2018;4(3):292–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Galland BC, et al. . Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: a systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(3):213–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Jackson CL, et al. . A workshop report on the causes and consequences of sleep health disparities. Sleep. 2020;43(8). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Johnson DA, et al. . Environmental determinants of insufficient sleep and sleep disorders: implications for population health. Curr Epidemiol Rep. 2018;5(2):61–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Healthy People 2020. https://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topics-objectives/topic/sleep-health/objectives. Accessed May 15, 2018.
  • 46. Kann L, et al. . Youth risk behavior surveillance - United States, 2017. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;67(8):1–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Wittmann M, et al. . Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23(1–2):497–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Henderson SEM, et al. . Associations between social jetlag and mental health in young people: a systematic review. Chronobiol Int. 2019;36(10):1316–1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Carskadon MA, et al. . Adolescent sleep patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days. Sleep. 1998;21(8):871–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. O’Malley EB, et al. . School start time and its impact on learning and behavior. In: Ivanenko A, ed. Sleep and Psychiatric Disorders in Children and Adolescents. New York, NY: Information Healthcare Publisher; 2008:28–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Mindell JA, et al. . Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Med. 2009;10(7):771–779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Paavonen EJ, et al. . Sleep problems of school-aged children: a complementary view. Acta Paediatr. 2000;89(2):223–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Dayyat EA, et al. . Sleep estimates in children: parental versus actigraphic assessments. Nat Sci Sleep. 2011;3:115–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Poulain T, et al. . Parent-child agreement in different domains of child behavior and health. PLoS One. 2020;15(4):e0231462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Short MA, et al. . Time for bed: parent-set bedtimes associated with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescents. Sleep. 2011;34(6):797–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Gangwisch JE, et al. . Earlier parental set bedtimes as a protective factor against depression and suicidal ideation. Sleep. 2010;33(1):97–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

zsab048_suppl_Supplementary_Material

RESOURCES