Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021 Jan 26;6(7):706–715. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.01.004

Mapping disease course across the mood disorder spectrum through a Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework

Alexis E Whitton 1,2, Poornima Kumar 1, Michael T Treadway 3, Ashleigh V Rutherford 1, Manon L Ironside 1, Dan Foti 4, Garrett Fitzmaurice 1, Fei Du 1, Diego A Pizzagalli 1
PMCID: PMC8273113  NIHMSID: NIHMS1692832  PMID: 33508498

Abstract

Background:

The National Institute of Mental Health Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative aims to establish a neurobiologically valid framework for classifying mental illness. Here, we examined whether the RDoC construct of Reward Learning and three aspects of its underlying neurocircuitry predicted symptom trajectories in individuals with mood pathology.

Methods:

Aligning with the RDoC approach, we recruited individuals [n=80 with mood disorders (58 unipolar, 22 bipolar) and n=32 controls; 63.4% female] based on their performance on a laboratory-based reward learning task, rather than clinical diagnosis. We then assessed (1) anterior cingulate cortex prediction errors using electroencephalography, (2) striatal reward prediction errors using functional magnetic resonance imaging, and (3) medial prefrontal cortex glutamatergic function (mPFC Gln/Glu) using 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Severity of anhedonia, (hypo)mania and impulsivity were measured at baseline, 3 months and 6 months.

Results:

Greater homogeneity in aspects of brain function (mPFC Gln/Glu) was observed when individuals were classified according to reward learning ability rather than diagnosis. Furthermore, mPFC Gln/Glu levels predicted more severe (hypo)manic symptoms cross-sectionally, worsening (hypo)manic symptoms longitudinally, and explained greater variance in future (hypo)manic symptoms than diagnostic information. However, rather than being transdiagnostic, this effect was specific to individuals with bipolar disorder. Prediction error indices were unrelated to symptom severity.

Conclusions:

Although findings are preliminary and require replication, they suggest that heightened mPFC Gln/Glu warrants further consideration as a predictor of future (hypo)mania. Importantly, this work highlights the value of an RDoC approach that works in tandem with, rather than independent of, traditional diagnostic frameworks.

Keywords: depression, bipolar disorder, reward learning, reward prediction error, dopamine, glutamate

Introduction

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (1) and International Classification of Diseases (2) classify Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) and Bipolar Disorder (BD) as separate conditions distinguishable by a history of (hypo)mania, with evidence supporting a disease-specific treatment approach (3, 4). Although these nosological systems provide a useful common language for clinicians and researchers, their value for understanding mood disorder pathophysiology remains limited. Accordingly, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) (5, 6) was proposed as a strategic change in scientific inquiry, and seeks to classify psychiatric disorders according to measurable variability within and across different domains of functioning. Subsequently, the Positive Valence Systems domain – in particular, the subdomain of ‘Reward Learning’ – has emerged as an especially promising target for understanding the mechanisms underpinning mood symptoms.

Reward learning refers to the ability to adaptively modulate behavior as a function of positive reinforcement. Abnormalities in reward learning and underlying neurocircuitry have been strongly implicated in mood disorders (7, 8). For example, performance on behavioral reward learning paradigms has been shown to (i) differentiate patients with MDD or BD from controls during symptomatic and asymptomatic states (9, 10), (ii) predict anhedonia severity and treatment outcome (11), (iii) change following pharmacological dopaminergic manipulations (12, 13), (iv) be linked to striatal dopamine transporter function and frontostriatal functional connectivity (14), and (v) be heritable (15). Decades of research in laboratory animals has identified the neurobiological processes underpinning reward learning (16). Therefore, examining how these processes vary across the mood disorder spectrum represents a fruitful avenue for identifying the neurobiological basis underpinning mood disorder heterogeneity.

Imaging and computational studies suggest that the brain employs distinct hierarchical systems to support learning (17, 18) and to date, the neural circuitry involved in learning from positive reinforcement has been especially well characterized (1921). Importantly, individuals with MDD and BD have been found to exhibit dysregulation in three key aspects of this neurocircuitry. First, a fundamental mechanism that supports reward learning is the reward prediction error (RPE), which is a striatal dopamine-based signal that encodes violations of reward expectancies (22). Individuals with MDD have been found to have blunted striatal RPE signals during learning (2325) and this blunting has been linked to a more recurrent depressive illness course (23). Similar abnormalities have been observed in individuals with BD, although the direction of effects often diverges from those observed in studies of unipolar MDD. Relative to healthy controls, euthymic individuals with BD or individuals with subthreshold hypomania have been found to have elevated striatal activation during reward anticipation (26) and reward outcome (27). Similarly, manic individuals with BD show striatal responses that fail to differentiate between receipt and omission of rewards, suggestive of abnormal RPE signaling (28).

Second, event-related potential (ERP) studies highlight the reward positivity (RewP) as another important reward circuit component linked to mood pathology (29). The RewP is a frontocentral electroencephalographic (EEG) deflection that is elicited by RPEs, and is thought to originate from the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and striatum (30). Smaller RewP amplitudes, as well as weaker RewP-related ACC activation, predict poorer reward learning (31, 32). Furthermore, abnormal RewP amplitudes have been observed in individuals with hypomania (33) and those with MDD (34), and have been found to predict future depression onset in healthy individuals (35). Critically, the source of these RewP signals is believed to be distinct from that of striatal dopaminergic RPEs (36), hence, they offer complementary information to fMRI-based RPE studies in terms of understanding the biological basis of reward learning dysfunction.

Finally, while the reward learning literature has historically emphasized the role of dopamine, the hedonic effects of dopamine are thought to be partially mediated by its interactions with glutamatergic signals originating in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (37). In line with this notion, in animal studies, disrupted glutamate signaling between mPFC and striatal regions impairs reward motivation (38), and in psychiatrically healthy humans, mPFC glutamate levels (measured using magnetic resonance spectroscopy; MRS) predict reward-based decision-making (39). Human MRS studies often focus on the glutamine/glutamate ratio (Gln/Glu) because glutamate is released into the synaptic cleft, taken up by glial cells, converted into glutamine, and cycled back into neurons (40), making mPFC Gln/Glu a proxy measure of the integrity of the glutamatergic synapse. Of note, meta-analyses of MRS studies have highlighted mPFC glutamate abnormalities in MDD and BD, albeit in opposite directions, with glutamatergic transmission being reduced in MDD (41), but elevated in BD (42) across manic (43), depressive (44) and euthymic (45) mood states.

Taken together, these studies suggest that striatal and ACC-mediated PE signals, along with mPFC Gln/Glu, are promising biomarkers of reward learning that may be implicated in mood pathology. Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine whether variation in reward learning neurocircuit function predicts variability in symptom trajectories in individuals with mood disorders. In line with the grant mechanism supporting this study (RFA-MH-14-050, Dimensional Approaches to Research Classification in Psychiatric Disorders), we recruited individuals based on their performance on a well-validated behavioral reward learning task, rather on the basis of specific DSM diagnoses. We then examined whether neurobiological indices of reward learning predicted cross-sectional and longitudinal variation in three reward-relevant symptom domains, namely anhedonia, (hypo)mania and impulsivity. We predicted that potentiated striatal and ACC-mediated PEs, and elevated mPFC Gln/Glu, would predict worsening (hypo)mania and impulsivity. In contrast, we predicted that blunted striatal and ACC-mediated PEs, and reduced mPFC Gln/Glu, would predict worsening anhedonia. Importantly, we assessed whether these reward learning biomarkers provided superior predictive validity in determining symptom trajectories, relative to clinical diagnostic information alone.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The mood pathology group were required to have depressive, mixed or hypomanic symptoms severe enough to cause distress/impairment. Participants could pursue treatment but were excluded from further testing if they initiated one of the exclusionary treatments (Supplemental Methods). Psychotropic medication load was quantified using previously established procedures (see Supplemental Methods). The control group had no lifetime psychiatric disorders or psychotropic medication use. This study was approved by the Partners Human Research Committee. Participants provided written, informed consent prior to participating.

Study design and recruitment

Fig. 1A shows the study design. Recruitment occurred as follows: healthy controls and treatment-seeking individuals with mood disorders were screened on a Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT) (10, 46). Screening continued until two conditions were met: (i) until a sample of 32 healthy controls with valid PRT data, and who met study eligibility criteria, was recruited, and (ii) until a sample of 80 individuals with mood pathology whose PRT performance spanned the full range of a normative distribution, and who met study eligibility criteria, was recruited. For the 80 individuals with mood pathology, we focused on equally populating quintiles of reward learning (i.e., n=~16 per quintile; Fig. 2) that were defined using cut-offs derived from a prior normative sample of 572 controls who had performed the PRT in prior studies. In total, n=272 individuals had to be screened on the PRT to reach these two criteria (see Fig. S1 for study flow diagram).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Study methods overview

Panel A shows a summary of the study flow. Participants were screened on a Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT) and the patient group was recruited so that their scores on the PRT spanned the entire range of possible scores on a pre-existing normative distribution. If eligible, a clinical assessment was conducted, then participants returned for two baseline neuroimaging visits (EEG and fMRI/MRS sessions), as well as 3- and 6-month follow-up assessments. Panel B displays source localization analyses demonstrating that scalp-recorded RewP amplitude correlated with current source density in the dorsal ACC (p<0.005 uncorrected; x=−3), validating RewP amplitude as a marker of ACC-mediated activation. Panel C shows the bilateral NAc region-of-interest (y=10) from which striatal RPEs were extracted. Panel D shows the 2 × 2 × 2cm voxel placed in the mPFC (x=0), from which Gln/Glu metabolites were extracted.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Recruitment based on behavioral reward learning

Panel A shows the number of participants with mood pathology whose PRT performance fell in each quintile of reward learning performance according to the normative distribution (the normative distribution was based on a separate existing sample of N=572 healthy controls). The dotted line indicates the a priori target of n=16 per quintile that was set to ensure that we recruited individuals who spanned the entire range of reward learning performance. This target was met in all but the lowest quintile, however, this quintile was still adequately represented with a sample of n=13. Panel B shows frequency histograms of reward learning performance across the control, unipolar and bipolar groups.

For participants who were screened on the PRT and had valid data, study eligibility criteria and clinical diagnoses were further evaluated via a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (47) (conducted by Masters- or PhD-level interviewers). Participants were also screened with the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (48) to ensure that at least one third of the mood pathology sample exhibited (hypo)manic symptoms. Eligible participants completed five study visits: (i) behavioral testing and clinical assessment; (ii) a baseline EEG/ERP recording; (iii) a baseline MRI scan; (iv) a 3-month follow-up clinical assessment; and (v) a 6-month follow-up clinical assessment. Participants received $15/hr in compensation plus earnings on the behavioral and imaging tasks.

Primary outcomes

Anhedonia was measured using the Anhedonic Depression subscale of the 62-item Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ-AD) (49) and impulsivity was assessed using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) (50). (Hypo)mania was measured using the Bipolar Inventory of Symptoms Scale Mania subscale (BISS-mania), which was chosen over the YMRS as it measures an extended range of (hypo)manic symptoms (51) and showed greater variance across both unipolar and bipolar groups. These measures were completed at baseline, and again at 3- and 6-month follow-up assessments. All three scales demonstrated good internal consistency (Supplementary Methods).

Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT) – quantifying reward learning

Reward learning was assessed using a well-validated computer-based PRT (46). On each trial, a fixation cross (500ms) was followed by a schematic, mouthless face (500ms). Next, a short (11.5mm) or a long (13mm) mouth appeared (100ms). Participants indicated whether the mouth was long or short. There were 3 blocks of 100 trials, and for each block, 40 correct trials were rewarded (“Correct!! You won 20 cents”). Although long and short mouths were presented at equal frequency, unbeknownst to participants, correct identification of one mouth (the ‘rich stimulus’) was rewarded three times more than the other (the ‘lean stimulus’).

Following quality control (Supplemental Methods), we used signal detection analysis (52) to compute response bias (the tendency to bias responding to the rich stimulus), using the formula:

logb=12log( Riccorrect* Lean incorrect Ricincorrect* Lean correct)

To allow calculation of response bias for cases that included a zero in the formula, 0.5 was added to each cell of the matrix (53). Reward learning was defined as the increase in response bias from block 1 to block 3.

Scalp-recorded RewP amplitude – quantifying ACC PEs

The RewP was computed from 128-channel scalp-recorded EEG, acquired while participants performed a counterbalanced version of the PRT. After preprocessing, temporo-spatial principal components analysis was used to decompose the time-domain ERP (54). Temporal variance in the averaged ERP waveforms was examined using temporal PCA and Infomax rotation. Based on the scree plot used to determine the factors to retain in a PCA analysis, 12 temporal factors were retained for rotation. The spatial distribution of these temporal factors was then examined using spatial PCA and Infomax rotation, with a spatial PCA being conducted for each temporal factor. Eight spatial factors were retained for each temporal factor. Analyses focused on the PCA component with a timing and topography most consistent with the RewP (TF8/SF2; Supplemental Methods). Furthermore, source localization (55) confirmed that the RewP had a source in the dorsal ACC (Fig. 1B). Our primary variable of interest was the difference in RewP amplitude following feedback on lean versus rich trials (ΔRewP), which captures the degree to which the ACC tracks reward probability across different contexts.

fMRI-based learning task – quantifying striatal RPEs

Striatal RPE signals were assessed using a well-validated explicit reinforcement learning paradigm (19, 56) that required participants to learn reward contingencies through trial-and-error. On each trial, participants were asked to choose between two symbols and each symbol in the pair was associated with an 80%/20% probability of a given outcome (gain: $1/$0; loss: $0/−$1; neutral: grey square/’nothing’). We used Q-learning to calculate the RPE (19) from participants’ behavioral data, and imaging analyses focused on a parametric modulation contrast for RPE signals (Supplementary Methods).

Anatomically-defined regions-of-interest (ROIs) in the left and right NAc were selected from prior research showing links between dopamine transporter function and reward learning (14) (Fig. 1C). Beta weights from RPE contrasts were extracted from these ROIs. A one-sample t-test confirmed that the RPE in both ROIs was >0 [left: t(106)=3.07, p=0.003; right: t(106)=4.12, p<0.001], so beta values were averaged to create a single NAc RPE beta weight that was used for subsequent analyses. A positive RPE beta value signified higher activation for unexpected reward and lower activation for unexpected omission of rewards during gain trials.

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) – quantifying mPFC glutamate

1H-MRS was used to assess mPFC Gln/Glu. A 2 × 2 × 2cm voxel was placed in the mPFC, midsagittally, anterior to the genu of the corpus collosum (Fig. 1D). The voxel was automatically shimmed, with further manual shimming performed as needed. A modified J-resolved protocol (2D-JPRESS) (57) was used to resolve glutamatergic metabolites. This sequence involved the collection of 22 TE-stepped spectra with an echo time ranging from 35ms to 250ms in 15ms increments (TR=2s, f1 acquisition bandwidth=67Hz, spectral bandwidth=2kHz, readout duration=512ms, NEX=16/TE-step, approximate scan duration=12min).

To quantify glutamate and glutamine with the JPRESS data, the 22 TE-stepped free-induction decay series (FIDS) was zero-filled out to 64 points, Gaussian-filtered, and Fourier-Transformed using GAMMA-simulated J-resolved basis sets modeled for 2.89 T. Every J-resolved spectral extraction within a bandwidth of 67Hz was fit with the spectral-fitting package LCModel and its theoretically-correct template. The integrated area under the entire 2D surface for each metabolite was calculated by summing the raw peak areas across all 64 J-resolved extractions (Supplemental Methods). Our primary outcome was the glutamine to glutamate ratio (Gln/Glu).

Statistical analysis

Multivariable regression analyses examined whether ΔRewP, NAc RPE or mPFC Gln/Glu predicted anhedonia, (hypo)mania or impulsivity in the clinical sample cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Separate regression models were run for each outcome (MASQ-AD, BISS-mania, BIS). Models included covariates (age, sex, medication load), mood polarity/diagnosis (Group: dummy-coded 0=unipolar, 1=bipolar), the three neural predictors (ΔRewP, NAc RPE, mPFC Gln/Glu), and a Group × Predictor interaction term for each neural predictor. Models predicting follow-up symptom severity also controlled for baseline symptom severity.

Results

Sample characteristics

The sample was 63.4% female (n=71), with a mean age of 28.6 (SD=9.1, range 18–60). Of the patient group, 72.5% (n=58) had unipolar mood pathology (MDD/dysthymia, or MDD in partial remission), 27.5% (n=22) had bipolar mood pathology (BD-I/II, depressed, mixed or hypomanic), and 40% (n=32) took medication (see Table 1 and Supplemental Methods for details). Sample sizes for each of the analyses varied when a participant had missing data on one or more of the neural indices and/or follow-up measures. Accordingly, sample sizes ranged from 25–32 for the control group, 38–58 for the unipolar group, and 12–22 for the bipolar group (sample sizes for each analysis are specified below).

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of sample

HC (n=32) Unipolar (n=58) Bipolar (n=22) Test P value
 Age, M (SD) 28.4 (7.7) 28.0 (8.6) 30.5 (12.1) F=0.59 0.56
 Female, N (%) 17 (53.1) 41 (71.7) 13 (59.1) χ2=2.96 0.23
 Years education, M (SD) 17.0 (3.2) 16.0 (2.8) 15.6 (3.1) F=1.77 0.18
 White, N (%) 21 (65.6) 40 (69.0) 19 (86.4) χ2=10.02 0.26
 Hispanic, N (%) 2 (6.3) 6 (10.3) 2 (9.1) χ2=0.43 0.81
Clinical diagnoses
 Current MDD, N (%) - - 49 (84.5) - - - -
 Current dysthymia, N (%) - - 1 (1.7) - - - -
 MDD in partial remission, N (%) - - 8 (13.8) - - - -
 BD-I depressed, N (%) - - - - 7 (31.8) - -
 BD-I mixed, N (%) - - - - 0 (0.0) - -
 BD-I hypomanic, N (%) - - - - 2 (9.1) - -
 BD-II depressed, N (%) - - - - 9 (40.9) - -
 BD-II mixed, N (%) - - - - 1 (4.6) - -
 BD-II hypomanic, N (%) - - - - 3 (13.6) - -
Comorbidities
 Alcohol abuse, N (%) - - 0 (0.0) 2 (9.1) χ2=5.41 0.02
 EDNOS or BED, N (%) - - 2 (3.4) 2 (9.1) χ2=1.07 0.30
 GAD, N (%) - - 3 (5.2) 2 (9.1) χ2=0.42 0.52
 Panic disorder, N (%) - - 1 (1.7) 0 (0.0) χ2=0.38 0.54
 PTSD, N (%) - - 3 (5.2) 2 (9.1) χ2=0.42 0.52
 Social Phobia, N (%) - - 8 (13.8) 3 (13.6) χ2=0.00 0.99
 Specific Phobia, N (%) - - 3 (5.2) 2 (9.1) χ2=0.42 0.52
Medication
 Antidepressants, N (%) - - 19 (32.8) 4 (18.2) χ2=1.65 0.20
 Mood stabilizer or anticonvulsant, N (%) - - 1 (1.7) 7 (31.8) χ2=16.05 <0.001
 Anticonvulsants, N (%) - - 0 (0.0) 1 (4.5) χ2=2.67 0.10

Note. HC=healthy control; MDD=major depressive disorder; BD=bipolar disorder; EDNOS=eating disorder not otherwise specified; BED=binge eating disorder; GAD=generalized anxiety disorder; PTSD=post-traumatic stress disorder. All tests are two-tailed.

Correlations among units

Pearson correlations were used determine the degree to which the three neural indices mapped onto behavioral reward learning (see Tables S1 & S2; differences in units of analysis between diagnostic groups are reported in the Supplementary Results and Figure S2). Across the sample, higher mPFC Gln/Glu correlated with better reward learning (r=0.27, p=0.007, n=102; Fig. S3A). This was consistent with the linear trend shown in Fig. S3A, where mPFC Gln/Glu values increased across the learning quintiles. Furthermore, the quintiles explained a greater proportion of the variance in mPFC Gln/Glu relative to diagnosis (5% vs. 2%; R2 change=0.05, F change=5.25, p=0.02).

Although the ΔRewP and NAc RPE were not correlated with our a priori-defined learning measure (block 3 minus block 1 response bias), they were correlated with the total overall response bias. Specifically, heightened NAc RPE (r=0.37, p=0.04, n=32; Fig. S3B) and ΔRewP (r=0.41, p=0.04, n=25; Fig. S3C) correlated with greater overall response bias in controls but not in patients (p>0.10, n=75). Furthermore, across the whole sample, heightened NAc RPE was associated with faster learning in block 1 (r=0.23, p=0.02, n=107).

Elevated mPFC Gln/Glu correlates with more severe (hypo)manic symptoms cross-sectionally

Standardized values for each outcome measure across the reward learning quintiles are shown in Figure S4 (patients only). Multimodal regression models assessed whether the three reward circuit markers were associated with symptom severity cross-sectionally.

A significant Group × mPFC Gln/Glu interaction (β=0.28, p=0.04, n=57) emerged from the model predicting baseline (hypo)mania severity (BISS-mania), indicating that the effect of mPFC Gln/Glu on baseline (hypo)mania severity differed across the unipolar and bipolar groups (Table 2). To unpack this interaction, we examined the correlation between mPFC Gln/Glu and baseline BISS-mania scores (both residualized for other variables in the model) in each group. mPFC Gln/Glu was associated with higher BISS-mania scores in the bipolar (r=0.56, p=0.045, n=13) but not the unipolar group (r=−0.24, p=0.12, n=45).

Table 2.

Models predicting (hypo)manic symptom severity on the BISS-mania scale

Dependent variable: Baseline (hypo)manic symptom severity
B SE β t p
(constant) 9.14 2.33 3.92 <0.001
Age −0.09 0.08 −0.13 −1.18 0.24
Sex −2.77 1.52 −0.20 −1.82 0.08
Medication load −0.71 0.42 −0.19 −1.71 0.09
Group 10.46 1.60 0.69 6.52 <0.001
ΔRewP −0.70 1.24 −0.07 −0.56 0.58
NAc RPE 0.03 0.57 0.01 0.05 0.96
mPFC Gln/Glu −9.33 16.39 −0.07 −0.57 0.57
Group × ΔRewP −1.48 2.27 −0.09 −0.65 0.52
Group × NAc RPE 2.11 1.90 0.13 1.11 0.27
Group × mPFC Gln/Glu 68.06 31.51 0.28 2.16 0.04
Dependent variable: 3-month (hypo)manic symptom severity
B SE β t p
(constant) 3.49 1.93 1.81 0.08
Age 0.06 0.06 0.15 0.99 0.33
Sex −2.14 1.19 −0.27 −1.80 0.08
Medication load −0.29 0.32 −0.12 −0.90 0.37
Baseline BISS-mania 0.16 0.11 0.28 1.54 0.13
Group −1.15 1.61 −0.13 −0.71 0.48
ΔRewP −1.02 0.99 −0.16 −1.03 0.31
NAc RPE −0.04 0.43 −0.01 −0.10 0.92
mPFC Gln/Glu −38.43 14.30 −0.46 −2.69 0.01
Group × ΔRewP 1.03 1.93 0.08 0.53 0.60
Group × NAc RPE −0.07 1.35 −0.01 −0.05 0.96
Group × mPFC Gln/Glu 96.92 24.90 0.70 3.89 <0.001

Note. Group was dummy-coded 0=unipolar, 1=bipolar; BISS-Mania=Bipolar Inventory of Symptoms Scale mania subscale score; RewP=reward positivity; NAc RPE=nucleus accumbens reward prediction error; mPFC Gln/Glu=medial prefrontal cortex ratio of glutamine to glutamate.

In contrast, none of the neural indices predicted anhedonia severity (MASQ-AD), or impulsivity (BIS; all ps>0.05) cross-sectionally.

Elevated mPFC Gln/Glu correlates with more severe (hypo)manic symptoms longitudinally

Next, we examined whether, after controlling for baseline (hypo)manic severity, the reward circuit markers were associated with 3- and 6-month follow-up symptom severity (Figure S5 shows mean symptom severity across time). A Group × mPFC Gln/Glu interaction (β=0.70, p<0.001, n=49) emerged for the model predicting 3-month BISS-mania scores (Table 2). To unpack this interaction, we again examined the correlation between mPFC Gln/Glu and 3-month BISS-mania scores (residualized for other variables in the model) in each group. Increased mPFC Gln/Glu was associated with less severe hypomanic symptoms in the unipolar group (r=−0.35, p=0.03, n=38) but more severe hypomanic symptoms in the bipolar group (r=0.85, p<0.001, n=12; Fig. 3) at 3 months.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Group × mPFC Gln/Glu interaction for longitudinal (hypo)manic symptom severity

Residualized scatter plots showing the relationship between mPFC Gln/Glu and (hypo)manic symptom severity (BISS-mania subscale scores) at baseline (A, B) and at the 3-month follow-up assessment (C, D) in the unipolar and bipolar mood disorder groups. Residualized values on each axis control for the other variables in the model, which were: age, sex, baseline BISS-mania subscale scores, ΔRewP amplitude, NAc RPE beta weights.

In contrast, the reward learning markers did not predict 6-month follow-up BISS-mania scores, or 3- or 6-month MASQ-AD or BIS scores (all ps>0.05; see Supplemental Results for exploratory unimodal analyses).

Predictive value of mPFC Gln/Glu

Next, we compared a simple model containing covariates (age, sex, medication load, baseline BISS-mania) and diagnostic information (Group), to a model containing terms for mPFC Gln/Glu and Group × mPFC Gln/Glu. The simple model explained 15.8% of the variance in 3-month (hypo)manic symptom severity, F(5,44)=1.65, p=0.17. However, adding the mPFC Gln/Glu terms explained an additional 24.3% of the variance in 3-month hypomanic symptom severity, F(7,42)=4.01, p=0.002, and this change in R2 was significant (F change=8.49, R2 change=0.24, p=0.001). This indicates that mPFC Gln/Glu explained greater variance in future hypomanic symptom severity relative to baseline diagnosis alone. Furthermore, we confirmed that mPFC Gln/Glu explained a greater proportion of the variance in 3-month (hypo)manic symptom severity relative to behavioral reward learning alone (F change=3.91, R2 change=0.09, p=0.03; Table S3), indicating that adding this biomarker enhanced predictive power over and above behavioral data.

Discussion

Using a novel recruitment method, a transdiagnostic sample, and a multimodal longitudinal design, we examined whether variation along the RDoC Positive Valence Systems domain of Reward Learning and the underlying neurocircuitry predicted variability in three reward-related mood symptoms – anhedonia, (hypo)mania and impulsivity. In doing so, we focused on three components of reward learning neurocircuitry linked to mood disorder pathophysiology that span distinct units of analysis across physiology (ACC-mediated PEs), circuits (striatal RPEs), and molecules (mPFC Gln/Glu).

As predicted, the three neural components correlated with aspects of behavioral reward learning on the PRT. In terms of symptoms, elevated mPFC Gln/Glu predicted more severe cross-sectional and longitudinal (hypo)manic symptoms in those with bipolar pathology. Importantly, baseline mPFC Gln/Glu levels explained a greater proportion of the variance in (hypo)manic symptoms at 3 months relative to diagnosis alone. These findings extend prior case-control MRS studies (41, 42) by showing that elevated mPFC Gln/Glu is also associated with (hypo)mania severity dimensionally.

We replicated prior findings linking blunted ΔRewP amplitude with greater anhedonia in exploratory unimodal analyses (see Supplement), however, neither ΔRewP nor NAc RPE signals were associated with symptom severity when entered into a multimodal model with mPFC Gln/Glu. Although the lack of a relationship between NAc RPE and anhedonia in our unimodal analyses contrasts with recent findings showing that striatal RPEs predicted improvement in anhedonic symptoms (58), we used a more complex instrumental fMRI learning paradigm designed to assess striatal RPEs in the context of learning, as opposed to a more traditional guessing-type paradigm (which maximizes the RPE signal yet involves minimal learning).

It is important to consider what these findings mean for an RDoC approach to mood disorder classification that remains agnostic to DSM diagnoses. On the one hand, mPFC Gln/Glu correlated with reward learning across diagnoses, providing converging evidence that mPFC Gln/Glu is a transdiagnostic marker of this RDoC domain. Additionally, in a heterogeneous sample, more homogeneity in neurobiology (mPFC Gln/Glu levels) was observed within groups when groups were defined on the basis of reward learning than by diagnostic categories. This is consistent with RDoC’s assumption that dimensions of functioning are more proximal to neurobiology than diagnostic categories. Furthermore, dimensional increases in this Reward Learning biomarker (i.e., mPFC Gln/Glu levels) predicted dimensional increases in symptoms characterized by excessive reward responsiveness (i.e., hypomania), rather than membership to a specific diagnostic category. This echoes one of RDoC’s central theses that abnormalities in circuits and associated constructs likely underpin specific features of mental illness, rather than explaining any single disorder in its entirety. Together, these findings partly align with a diagnosis-agnostic approach to mood disorder classification.

However, our results also highlight the considerable value of diagnostic information in predicting symptom trajectories. Specifically, although mPFC Gln/Glu correlated with reward learning transdiagnostically, the link between mPFC Gln/Glu and (hypo)manic symptom severity was disease-specific, and diagnostic information remained an integral component of the final predictive model. If we assume that these findings could inform novel interventions based on neurobiological underpinnings (a key driver of the RDoC approach), then targeting mPFC Gln/Glu may affect reward learning in a similar manner across disorders, but have different effects on symptoms in distinct mood disorder subtypes. The degree to which the RDoC framework predicts purely dimensional variability across disorders versus a blend of transdiagnostic and disorder-specific effects, remains an important topic of debate. Our findings indicate that while information about reward circuit function could improve the prediction of risk for reward-related clinical symptoms (an important finding in its own right), it would do so in tandem with, rather than independent of, existing diagnostic frameworks.

This study has several strengths. By examining neurobiological mechanisms of reward learning across multiple units of analysis, we could probe reward learning circuitry with superior spatiotemporal resolution and at both micro and macro scales, which cannot be achieved with a single unit or modality alone. Furthermore, we tested whether these units of analysis enhanced the ability to predict clinical course, over and above information already used in routine clinical care (diagnosis, baseline symptom severity). Since mPFC Gln/Glu levels can be obtained using MRS in as little as 6 minutes with good test-retest reliability (ICC=0.803) (59), mPFC Gln/Glu warrants further investigation as a potential screening method for individuals at suspected risk for bipolar disorder.

However, some limitations must also be noted. First, mPFC Gln/Glu predicted worse (hypo)manic symptoms specifically in individuals with bipolar mood pathology. As the instance of (hypo)manic symptoms was lower in the unipolar group at follow-up, this may have restricted the variance in symptoms that could be explained by mPFC Gln/Glu. Second, although our three neural indices were selected based on their established association with reward learning, only mPFC Gln/Glu was associated with our a priori reward learning measure, and the three neural indices were not correlated with one another. Although stronger associations may have been evident in a larger sample, the lack of association could also reflect an issue with the construct validity of these units of analysis. For example, it is possible that similar impairments in reward learning may have distinct etiologies (often referred to as ‘equifinality’), particularly when considering individuals with very divergent forms of psychopathology. How equifinality is accounted for remains an important conceptual issue for the RDoC framework. Finally, reductions in sample size for longitudinal analyses (resulting from participant attrition and the need to obtain good quality data across all three neural indices) mean that reduced statistical power is a limitation of our study, and may explain several null findings. The replicability of these results must be interpreted in light of concerns around the generalizability and reproducibility of neuroimaging findings obtained using small samples (60). Accordingly, rather than being definitive, we interpret these findings as novel yet preliminary insights that warrant replication in larger samples.

In sum, we showed that a key component of reward learning neurocircuitry – mPFC Gln/Glu – predicted worse (hypo)manic symptoms. This marker enhanced the ability to predict future (hypo)mania risk over and above diagnostic information alone. Using this marker to improve precision in the diagnosis and treatment of mood pathology therefore represents an important avenue for future research, with a focus on larger, well-powered samples.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgements

Funding:

This project was supported by R01 MH101521 and R37 MH068376 (to DAP) from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. AEW received support from the National Health and Medical Research Council, Grant No. APP1110773.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor:

The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Prior Poster and Conference Presentations: The findings from this study have been presented in part as posters or invited talks at the 2019 and 2015 Social of Biological Psychiatry Annual Meetings, the 2017 and 2015 Anxiety and Depression Association of America Annual Meetings, and the 2017 Association for Psychological Science Annual Meeting.

Disclosures

Over the past three years, Dr. Pizzagalli has received funding from NIMH, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, the Dana Foundation, and Millennium Pharmaceuticals; consulting fees from BlackThorn Therapeutics, Boehreinger Ingelheim, Compass Pathway, Engrail Therapeutics , Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, and Takeda Pharmaceuticals; one honorarium from Alkermes; stock options from BlackThorn Therapeutics. Dr. Pizzagalli has a financial interest in BlackThorn Therapeutics, which has licensed the copyright to the Probabilistic Reward Task through Harvard University. Dr. Pizzagalli’s interests were reviewed and are managed by McLean Hospital and Partners HealthCare in accordance with their conflict of interest policies. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.World Health Organization (2018). International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics, 11th Revision. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Yatham LN, Kennedy SH, Parikh SV, Schaffer A, Bond DJ, Frey BN, Sharma V, Goldstein BI, Rej S, Beaulieu S (2018). Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments (CANMAT) and International Society for Bipolar Disorders (ISBD) 2018 guidelines for the management of patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 20: 97–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Goodwin G, Haddad P, Ferrier I, Aronson J, Barnes T, Cipriani A, Coghill D, Fazel S, Geddes J, Grunze H (2016). Evidence-based guidelines for treating bipolar disorder: revised third edition recommendations from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol 30: 495–553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cuthbert BN, Insel TR (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 11:126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cuthbert BN (2014). The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry 13: 28–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Whitton AE, Treadway MT, Pizzagalli DA (2015). Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry 28: 7–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Alloy LB, Nusslock R, Boland EM (2015). The development and course of bipolar spectrum disorders: an integrated reward and circadian rhythm dysregulation model. Ann Rev Clin Psychol 11: 213–250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pizzagalli DA, Goetz E, Ostacher M, Iosifescu DV, Perlis RH (2008). Euthymic patients with bipolar disorder show decreased reward learning in a probabilistic reward task. Biol Psychiatry 64: 162–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatric Res 43: 76–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Vrieze E, Pizzagalli DA, Demyttenaere K, Hompes T, Sienaert P, de Boer P, Schmidt M, Claes S (2013). Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 73: 639–645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pizzagalli DA, Evins AE, Schetter EC, Frank MJ, Pajtas PE, Santesso DL, Culhane M (2008). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: behavioral evidence from a laboratory-based measure of reward responsiveness. Psychopharmacology 196: 221–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Santesso DL, Evins AE, Frank MJ, Schetter EC, Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA (2009). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: Evidence from event-related potentials and computational modeling of striatal-cortical function. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 1963–1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kaiser RH, Treadway MT, Wooten DW, Kumar P, Goer F, Murray L, Beltzer M, Pechtel P, Whitton A, Cohen AL (2018). Frontostriatal and dopamine markers of individual differences in reinforcement learning: A multi-modal investigation. Cereb Cortex 28: 4281–4290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA (2009). The heritability of hedonic capacity and perceived stress: a twin study evaluation of candidate depressive phenotypes. Psychol Med 39: 211–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Schultz W (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiological Rev 95: 853–951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Balleine BW, O’doherty JP (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 48–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nat Neurosci 8: 1704–1711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442: 1042–1045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275: 1593–1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gläscher J, Daw N, Dayan P, O’Doherty JP (2010). States versus rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron 66: 585–595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schultz W (2016). Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nat Rev Neurosci 17: 183–195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kumar P, Goer F, Murray L, Dillon DG, Beltzer ML, Cohen AL, Brooks NH, Pizzagalli DA (2018). Impaired reward prediction error encoding and striatal-midbrain connectivity in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 43: 1581–1588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Whitton AE, Reinen JM, Slifstein M, Ang Y-S, McGrath PJ, Iosifescu DV, Abi-Dargham A, Pizzagalli DA, Schneier FR (2020). Baseline reward processing and ventrostriatal dopamine function are associated with pramipexole response in depression. Brain 143: 701–710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Milders M, Reid I, Steele J (2008). Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain 131: 2084–2093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Nusslock R, Almeida JR, Forbes EE, Versace A, Frank E, LaBarbara EJ, Klein CR, Phillips ML (2012). Waiting to win: elevated striatal and orbitofrontal cortical activity during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder adults. Bipolar Disord 14: 249–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.O’Sullivan N, Szczepanowski R, El-Deredy W, Mason L, Bentall RP (2011). fMRI evidence of a relationship between hypomania and both increased goal-sensitivity and positive outcome-expectancy bias. Neuropsychologia 49: 2825–2835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Abler B, Greenhouse I, Ongur D, Walter H, Heckers S (2008). Abnormal reward system activation in mania. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 2217–2227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Proudfit GH (2015). The reward positivity: From basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology 52: 449–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Foti D, Weinberg A, Dien J, Hajcak G (2011). Event-related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: response to commentary. Hum Brain Mapp 32: 2267–2269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Santesso DL, Dillon DG, Birk JL, Holmes AJ, Goetz E, Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA (2008). Individual differences in reinforcement learning: behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging correlates. Neuroimage 42: 807–816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Whitton AE, Kakani P, Foti D, Van’t Veer A, Haile A, Crowley DJ, Pizzagalli DA (2016). Blunted neural responses to reward in remitted major depression: a high-density event-related potential study. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 1: 87–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mason L, O’Sullivan N, Blackburn M, Bentall R, El-Deredy W (2012). I want it now! Neural correlates of hypersensitivity to immediate reward in hypomania. Biol Psychiatry 71: 530–537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Foti D, Hajcak G (2009). Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biol Psychol 81: 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bress JN, Foti D, Kotov R, Klein DN, Hajcak G (2013). Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology 50: 74–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Holroyd CB, Coles MG (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109: 679–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Geisler S, Derst C, Veh RW, Zahm DS (2007). Glutamatergic afferents of the ventral tegmental area in the rat. J Neurosci 27: 5730–5743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hauber W, Sommer S (2009). Prefrontostriatal circuitry regulates effort-related decision making. Cereb Cortex 19: 2240–2247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jocham G, Hunt LT, Near J, Behrens TE (2012). A mechanism for value-guided choice based on the excitation-inhibition balance in prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 15: 960–961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pellerin L, Magistretti PJ (2004). Let there be (NADH) light. Science 305:50–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Yüksel C, Öngür D (2010). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies of glutamate-related abnormalities in mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 68: 785–794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gigante AD, Bond DJ, Lafer B, Lam RW, Young LT, Yatham LN (2012). Brain glutamate levels measured by magnetic resonance spectroscopy in patients with bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Bipolar Disord 14: 478–487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Öngür D, Jensen JE, Prescot AP, Stork C, Lundy M, Cohen BM, Renshaw PF (2008). Abnormal glutamatergic neurotransmission and neuronal-glial interactions in acute mania. Biol Psychiatry 64: 718–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Frye MA, Watzl J, Banakar S, O’Neill J, Mintz J, Davanzo P, Fischer J, Chirichigno JW, Ventura J, Elman S (2007). Increased anterior cingulate/medial prefrontal cortical glutamate and creatine in bipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 2490–2499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kubo H, Nakataki M, Sumitani S, Iga J-i, Numata S, Kameoka N, Watanabe S-y, Umehara H, Kinoshita M, Inoshita M (2017). 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of glutamate-related abnormality in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 208: 139–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pizzagalli DA, Jahn AL, O’Shea JP (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biol Psychiatry 57: 319–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. SCID-I/P New York, NY; New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA (1978). A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 133: 429–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Watson D, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, Clark LA, Strauss ME, McCormick RA (1995). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. J Abnorm Psychol 104: 3–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 51: 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Gonzalez JM, Bowden CL, Katz MM, Thompson P, Singh V, Prihoda TJ, Dahl M (2008). Development of the Bipolar Inventory of Symptoms Scale: concurrent validity, discriminant validity and retest reliability. Int J Meth Psychiatr Res 17: 198–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Macmillan N, Creelman C (1991). Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. Cambridge University Press, UK. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hautus MJ (1995). Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values of d′. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 27: 46–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dien J (2010). The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data. J Neurosci Methods 187: 138–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Pascual-Marqui RD (2002). Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 24: 5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Treadway MT, Admon R, Arulpragasam AR, Mehta M, Douglas S, Vitaliano G, Olson DP, Cooper JA, Pizzagalli DA (2017). Association between interleukin-6 and striatal prediction-error signals following acute stress in healthy female participants. Biol Psychiatry 82: 570–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brennan BP, Tkachenko O, Schwab ZJ, Juelich RJ, Ryan EM, Athey AJ, Pope HG, Jenike MA, Baker JT, Killgore WD (2015). An examination of rostral anterior cingulate cortex function and neurochemistry in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:1866–1876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Eckstrand KL, Forbes EE, Bertocci MA, Chase HW, Greenberg T, Lockovich J, Stiffler R, Aslam HA, Graur S, Bebko G (2019). Anhedonia reduction and the association between left ventral striatal reward response and 6-month improvement in life satisfaction among young adults. JAMA Psychiatry 8: 958–965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jensen JE, Auerbach RP, Pisoni A, Pizzagalli DA (2017). Localized MRS reliability of in vivo glutamate at 3 T in shortened scan times: a feasibility study. NMR in Biomed 30: e3771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Sui J, Jiang R, Bustillo J, Calhoun VJBP (2020). Neuroimaging-based individualized prediction of cognition and behavior for mental disorders and health: Methods and promises. Biol Psychiatry 88: 818–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES