Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Jul 13;10:e65582. doi: 10.7554/eLife.65582

Neural correlates of ingroup bias for prosociality in rats

Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal 1,2,3,4,, Jocelyn M Breton 3,4,, Huanjie Sheng 3,, Kimberly LP Long 3,4,§, Stella Chen 3,4, Aline Halliday 3, Justin W Kenney 5,#, Anne L Wheeler 5,6, Paul Frankland 5,6,7, Carrie Shilyansky 8, Karl Deisseroth 9,10,11, Dacher Keltner 4,12, Daniela Kaufer 3,4,7
Editors: Shelly B Flagel13, Kate M Wassum14
PMCID: PMC8277352  PMID: 34253289

Abstract

Prosocial behavior, in particular helping others in need, occurs preferentially in response to distress of one’s own group members. In order to explore the neural mechanisms promoting mammalian helping behavior, a discovery-based approach was used here to identify brain-wide activity correlated with helping behavior in rats. Demonstrating social selectivity, rats helped others of their strain (‘ingroup’), but not rats of an unfamiliar strain (‘outgroup’), by releasing them from a restrainer. Analysis of brain-wide neural activity via quantification of the early-immediate gene c-Fos identified a shared network, including frontal and insular cortices, that was active in the helping test irrespective of group membership. In contrast, the striatum was selectively active for ingroup members, and activity in the nucleus accumbens, a central network hub, correlated with helping. In vivo calcium imaging showed accumbens activity when rats approached a trapped ingroup member, and retrograde tracing identified a subpopulation of accumbens-projecting cells that was correlated with helping. These findings demonstrate that motivation and reward networks are associated with helping an ingroup member and provide the first description of neural correlates of ingroup bias in rodents.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

Humans are an intensely social species, with complex social interactions and an ability to know and share others’ emotional states (Wilson, 2012). We often behave prosocially, acting with the intention of benefiting others or improving their well-being (Cronin, 2012). Yet, prosocial behavior tends to be extended preferrentially between group members and is less likely to be offered to others outside the group (Eisenberg et al., 2010). Reduced prosocial motivation towards outgroup members poses a major challenge for a diverse society, where members of different groups, such as racial and religious ones, coexist (Dovidio, 2010). Yet overcoming biases can be difficult, even for highly motivated individuals (Fiske, 2002), and multiple strategies for bias reduction have proved only partly successful in the long term (Paluck et al., 2021). Empathy, the ability to perceive and share the emotions of others, coupled with a motivation to care for their well-being (Batson, 2009; Decety et al., 2016), is considered to be a major driver for prosocial behavior in humans (Batson, 2011; Eisenberg, 2007). An "empathy gap" towards outgroup members is well documented in humans (Cheon et al., 2011; Chiao et al., 2008; Cikara et al., 2011; Eisenberg, 1989; Gutsell and Inzlicht, 2012) and is thought to lie at the root of reduced prosocial motivation towards other groups (Amodio et al., 2004; Echols and Correll, 2012; Levine et al., 2005; Stürmer et al., 2006). In line with this idea, humans are willing to pay a greater personal cost to prevent pain to ingroup members (Hein et al., 2010) and display a dampened neural response to outgroup members’ pain (Avenanti et al., 2010; Ruckmann et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2009).

While the extent and complexity of human prosocial behavior is unique, basic empathic responses and prosocial behavior are common in social species across the phylogenetic spectrum (Brosnan, 2020), and rely on evolutionarily conserved biological mechanisms (Decety et al., 2012). Empathy is thought to have evolved in the context of parental care and subsequently integrated into the natural behavioral repertoire of social species (de Waal, 2008; Preston and de Waal, 2002). As in humans, other animals, including rodents, determine behavior towards others in large part according to social identity, with prosocial actions typically extended towards ingroup members rather than unaffiliated others (Anacker and Beery, 2013; Campbell and de Waal, 2011; Fu et al., 2012; Hamilton, 1964; Mahajan et al., 2011; Nowak et al., 2010). The terms ‘ingroup’ and ‘outgroup,’ while often used in regard to cultural processes underlying social identity in humans, are also used for describing socially selective affiliation in non-human animals (Masuda and Fu, 2015; Nakamura and Masuda, 2012; Robinson and Barker, 2017), and are adopted here, despite likely differences in their neurobiological structure across species.

In recent years, evidence has shown that rodents experience stress and fear in response to observing others in distress (Cox and Reichel, 2021; Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2020; Meyza et al., 2017), console distressed mates (Burkett et al., 2016), and act for the benefit of others (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011; Cox and Reichel, 2020; Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2014; Márquez et al., 2015; Rice and Gainer, 1962; Sato et al., 2015). These studies provide robust evidence that rats, a highly social species, are moved to action by others’ distress and strive to help them, thereby demonstrating the basic components of empathic helping.

In the ‘helping behavior test’ (HBT) used below, it has been previously demonstrated that rats help conspecifics by releasing them from a restrainer, without any prior training or reward and even in the absence of social contact after helping (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011). Importantly, rats demonstrate an ingroup bias, releasing ‘ingroup members’ (rats of the same strain, whether familiar or not), but not ‘outgroup members’ (rats of an unfamiliar strain) (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2014).

Here we employed a discovery-driven approach to compare brain-wide activation patterns in response to trapped ingroup and outgroup members following the HBT. This investigation, which aimed at providing a broad and unbiased overview, led to the identification of central hubs specifically active during the ingroup condition, where rats demonstrated prosocial intent.

Understanding the neural mechanisms at the root of these phenomena is imperative for the advancement of novel interventions aimed at eliminating social bias. We seek to understand how the brain represents group membership categories, and how this information determines the cascade of evaluative and affective responses to others’ distress, ultimately influencing the motivation to approach and help others in need. Rodents provide an ideal model for exploring neural activity in this context. Thus, here we set out to describe the neural activity associated with rats’ ingroup bias for prosocial behavior.

Results

Rats demonstrate an ingroup bias for prosocial behavior

In the first set of experiments, male Sprague–Dawley (SD) rats were tested for helping behavior with a trapped cagemate of the same strain (‘HBT ingroup’ condition, n=8) or a stranger of the black-caped Long–Evans strain (‘HBT outgroup’ condition, n=8; Figure 1A). Along the 2 weeks of testing, most rats in the HBT ingroup condition learned to open the restrainer and consistently released the trapped rat on following sessions (n=6/8 rats became ‘openers,’ see Materials and methods). Along the days of testing, the percent of door-openings increased (Cochran’s Q = 31.43, df = 11, p=0.001, Figure 1B) and the mean latency to door-opening decreased (Friedman, Χ= 20.12, df = 11, p=0.043, Figure 1C). In contrast, and in line with prior findings, rats tested with a trapped outgroup member rarely released the trapped rat (n=0/8 became ‘openers’), and door-opening did not increase along testing sessions (Cochran’s Q and Friedman, p>0.05, Figure 1B, C). Rat’s movement patterns during testing were recorded by video and analyzed offline (Figure 1D–F). Across the 12 days, rats in the HBT ingroup condition were more active in total (t(14) = 3.8, p=0.002, Figure 1G), as well as across days (mixed model analysis [MMA], F(1,1037) = 372.3, p<0.001; Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc comparisons, p<0.05 for all days, Figure 1H) and minutes (MMA, F(1,1304) = 556.1, p<0.001; Bonferroni, p<0.05, Figure 1I) of testing. Rats in the ingroup condition also spent more time in the area around the restrainer (t(14) = 2.35, p=0.03, Figure 1J). This difference emerged in later days (MMA, F(1,1650) = 94.2, p<0.001; Bonferroni, p<0.05 for days 4–6, 8–12, Figure 1K) and minutes of testing (MMA, F(1,1690) = 103.85, p<0.001; Bonferroni, p<0.05 for minutes 30–40, Figure 1L). During times in the session when the restrainer door was closed, this pattern was maintained yet less pronounced both for velocity (MMA, F(1,486) = 172, p<0.001; Bonferroni, p<0.05, barring day 3, and F(1,1140) = 211.3, p<0.01, Bonferroni p < 0.05 for all minutes) and time around the restrainer (MMA, F(1,1037) = 11.5, p<0.01; Bonferroni p<0.05 for days 11–12, and F(1,1520) = 12.6, p<0.01; Bonferroni p<0.05 for minutes 30–40). In sum, rats released trapped cagemates of the same strain, but not strangers of an unfamiliar strain, demonstrating an ingroup bias for prosocial behavior.

Figure 1. Helping behavior for adult rats tested with ingroup and outgroup members.

Figure 1.

Adult rats selectively helped ingroup members. (A) Diagram of the helping behavior test (HBT) with a trapped rat. The trapped rat was either a cagemate of the same strain (ingroup, left) or a stranger of an unfamiliar strain (outgroup, right). (B, C) Rats released ingroup members but not outgroup members, as expressed by % door-openings (B) and mean (± SEM) latency to open (C) across testing sessions. The dashed line indicates the half-way door-opening by the experimenter. (D–F) Representative movement patterns of rats tested with an ingroup or outgroup member, depicted by a heatmap of the rat’s location along the session. Rats were more active (G–I) and spent more time around the restrainer (J–L) in the presence of a trapped ingroup member than an outgroup member. Lighter dots indicate non-openers, dark dots are openers.

In order to map the brain-wide activation involved in this selective prosocial response, the immediate early-gene marker c-Fos was quantified as an index of neural activity (Guzowski et al., 2005) across the brain of rats tested in the HBT conditions (n = 84 samples per rat, Figure 2A–C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A, B, Supplementary file 1). c-Fos expression reflects neural activity during the final testing session, during which restrainers were latched shut. Thus, rats in both HBT groups were in the presence of a trapped rat for the session’s entire duration and had an objectively similar experience on the c-Fos imaging day, despite their different history of door-opening. c-Fos was also quantified for several control conditions (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A–D). As a non-social control task, rats were tested in the HBT with a restrainer containing chocolate chips (‘chocolate,’ n = 8). In another condition, rats were tested with either an ingroup or outgroup member trapped in a latched, unopenable restrainer for three daily sessions in order to capture the neural response to a trapped rat without the experience of door-opening (‘brief ingroup,’ n = 7 and ‘brief outgroup,’ n = 8). To extract neural activity due to social interaction, non-trapped rats were exposed to a free rat across a wire mesh over three daily sessions (‘2 free ingroup,’ n = 8 and ‘2 free outgroup,’ n = 7). In this control condition, both animals could freely explore the conspecific in a context that mimicked the level of contact with a trapped rat across the holes of the restrainer. Additionally, the neural activity of rats trapped inside the restrainer (‘trapped,’ n = 8) and non-tested rats (‘baseline,’ n = 10) was examined.

Figure 2. Neural activity associated with the helping test.

Brain-wide patterns of neural activity associated with the helping behavior test (HBT). (A) Diagram of brain regions sampled for c-Fos expression. (B) A representative image of c-Fos signal sampled in the nucleus accumbens. (C) Legend of brain region categories coded by color. (D) Number of c-Fos+ cells per region (mean ± SEM) for rats tested with ingroup members, outgroup members, and an untested baseline. (E) Partial least square (PLS) task analysis of all social conditions. On left, the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions showed a common pattern of neural activity, which contrasts with the other social conditions, including a brief exposure to a trapped rat (brief), and exposure to a non-trapped rat separated by a wire mesh (2 free). On right, regions that contributed to this contrast display increased activity in the HBT compared to the other conditions. The black line marks a significance threshold at p<0.01. (F) Diagram of rat brains showing regions significantly active (in color) for the HBT ingroup (left) or outgroup (right) conditions compared to baseline.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. c-Fos acquisition.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Regions of interest (ROIs) for c-Fos analysis. Rat Atlas map with 84 sampled brain regions (according to the Paxinos and Watson Rat Brain Atlas). ROIs are color coded based on the legend in Figure 2. Some ROIs (e.g., multiple samples of the same region on different slices) were merged into the final list of 45 areas depicted in Figure 2. (B) Representative c-Fos samples (250 × 250 µm) from brain ROIs. Quantification of c-Fos+ cell numbers (in fluorescent green) was performed by ImageJ (see image on bottom), followed by visual validation.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Outline of control conditions.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Rats in the ‘chocolate’ condition were tested with five chocolate chips in the restrainer for 12 daily sessions. (B) Rats in the ‘brief’ condition were exposed to a trapped ingroup or outgroup rat for three daily sessions without being able to open the restrainer. (C) Rats in the ‘2 free’ condition were exposed to a freely moving ingroup or outgroup rat across a wire mesh. (D) Rats in other control conditions included the trapped rat, and untested rats left in their homecage as a ‘baseline’ group.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Box plots of c-Fos data in all brain regions across all test groups.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Center bars mark the median. Lower and upper edges correspond to the 25th and 75th percentiles. Descriptions of the brain region abbreviations can be found in Supplementary file 1. Data points are jittered along the x-axis to avoid overlaps. X: experimental groups; Y: c-Fos+ cell numbers.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Comparison of the helping behavior test (HBT) to the brief conditions.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

Task partial least square (PLS) was used to compare rats in the ingroup (A) and outgroup (B) conditions. Regions contributing significantly to the contrast (left) are shown on the right. The solid black line marks the significance threshold (p<0.01). (C) The average number of c-Fos+ cells was greater in the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions compared to their respective brief controls, and ingroup rats showed higher c-Fos than the outgroup in the HBT but not in the brief condition. Movement on the final session (D, E) showed higher mean velocity in the brief conditions (D) and no difference in time spent around the restrainer for any group (E).
Figure 2—figure supplement 5. c-Fos-associated movement patterns on the final testing session.

Figure 2—figure supplement 5.

Correlation between neural activity and time spent around the restrainer. For rats tested with ingroup or outgroup members, velocity (A, C) and time around the restrainer (B, D) were similar across the final testing session on which c-Fos was measured. Velocity was not correlated with c-Fos numbers (E). Time spent in the area around the restrainer was significantly correlated with c-Fos+ cell numbers (F). Light dots indicate non-openers, dark dots are openers.
Figure 2—figure supplement 6. Rats tested in the helping behavior test (HBT) with chocolate chips display little door-opening.

Figure 2—figure supplement 6.

(A) Diagram of the chocolate chip control condition. (B, C) Example image and representative heatmap of activity across one session. (D, E) Rats showed low levels of door-opening behavior as expressed by percent of door-openings and mean latency to door-opening across testing sessions. The dashed line indicates the half-way door-opening by the experimenter. (F, G) Rats in the chocolate condition showed lower levels of movement across the 12 days of testing relative to the other two HBT conditions, as presented by the average velocity and average time rats spent near the restrainer.

A common neural response to the HBT independent of group identity

To identify patterns of neural activity associated with each condition in a minimally biased way, multivariate task partial least square analysis (PLS, see Materials and methods) was conducted on the six social conditions (Figure 2D–F). This analysis found a neural pattern that distinguished the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions from the other conditions. A significant latent variable (LV) emerged (LV1, p<0.001), which was characterized by a contrast between brain-wide c-Fos expression in the HBT conditions and all other social control conditions (Figure 2E, left). This finding reflects increased overall neural activity in both HBT conditions compared to the control conditions (ANOVA F(5,40) = 21.04, p<0.001, Bonferroni, p<0.05, Supplementary file 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Permutation and bootstrapping tests were used to identify brain regions that maximally contributed to this contrast between conditions. The relative contribution of each brain region (termed here ‘salience,’ Figure 2E, right), provides a profile of neural activation across tasks. Activity in multiple brain regions emerged as significantly salient for the HBT conditions (regions crossing the threshold line denote a p<0.01, Figure 2E, right). The frontal cortex and anterior insula (AI) received particularly high salience scores, indicating that high levels of neural activity in these regions were driving the contrast between the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions, and the controls.

Dissociating neural activity from environmental and motor confounds

Neural activity measured on the final testing session reflects both exposure to a trapped conspecific as well as an intention or lack thereof to open the restrainer, indicated by each rat’s history of door-opening. To isolate the neural response associated with the HBT from neural activity associated with exposure to a trapped rat, each HBT condition (ingroup and outgroup) was compared with the corresponding brief condition (Figure 2—figure supplement 4). For animals tested with ingroup members, this analysis indicated increased brain-wide neural activity in the HBT compared to the brief condition, with numerous regions significantly contributing towards the contrast (Figure 2—figure supplement 4A). Yet, for animals tested with outgroup members, only a few regions significantly differed between the HBT and brief conditions, such as areas in the sensory cortices (Figure 2—figure supplement 4B). Furthermore, there was higher mean c-Fos for the ingroup relative to the outgroup in the HBT but not the brief condition (Bonferroni, p=0.02, Figure 2—figure supplement 4C). Thus, for rats tested with ingroup members, experiencing the HBT was more salient than brief exposure to a trapped rat. However, for rats tested with outgroup members, these two conditions were not dramatically different. As velocity and time spent around the restrainer were similar in the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions on the final session (MMA, p>0.05, Bonferroni, p>0.05, Figure 2—figure supplements 4D, E and 5A–D), the increased neural activity in the HBT ingroup condition is not likely due to motor movements. While c-Fos was not correlated with velocity on the final session (Figure 2—figure supplement 5E), it was found to be significantly correlated with time spent in the area near the restrainer (Pearson’s r, p<0.05, Figure 2—figure supplement 5F). This finding provides further indication that motivational state, rather than increased motor activity, better explains the observed brain-wide neural activity.

The distinct activity in the HBT ingroup condition may reflect the rewarding nature of opening the restrainer. To distinguish this from the neural activity associated with a non-social reward, a separate group of rats was tested in the HBT with a restrainer containing five chocolate chips (Figure 2—figure supplement 6A–C). Unexpectedly, most rats did not learn to open the restrainer (two of eight became openers, Figure 2—figure supplement 6D, E), and rats tested with chocolate displayed significantly lower velocity (Bonferroni, p<0.001) and time around the restrainer (Bonferroni, p<0.01) across testing sessions compared to the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions (Figure 2—figure supplement 6F, G). Moreover, brain-wide neural activity was significantly lower for this group than for the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions (Bonferroni, p=0.01, p=0.03, respectively, Supplementary file 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 3). As door-opening was scarce in this condition, it is not informative about the neural activity associated with a non-social reward. However, it provides evidence that the neural activity observed in the HBT conditions was a response to the social context, rather than simply the experience of undergoing the full-length paradigm with a restrainer.

A distinct neural signature of prosocial intent

The analysis above provided a broad overview of all social conditions, but was not informative about the neural activity associated specifically with either the HBT ingroup or outgroup conditions. To identify regions uniquely active in each HBT condition, c-Fos expression in the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions was compared to the baseline of non-tested rats for each brain region. This analysis (Figure 2F) identified a common set of regions significantly more active than baseline (Bonferroni, p<0.05, Supplementary file 3) for both HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions, including the sensory cortex, AI, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and orbitofrontal cortex (ventral and lateral; VO and LO), CA2 of the hippocampus and the habenula (Hab). These regions are thus interpreted as participating in the response to a trapped rat, regardless of prosocial motivation.

A distinct set of regions was more active than baseline only in the HBT ingroup condition (Bonferroni, p<0.05, Supplementary file 3, Figure 2F). These included the medial orbitofrontal cortex (MO), prelimbic cortex (PrL), dorsal endopiriform cortex (DEn), nucleus accumbens (Nac), lateral septum (LS), claustrum (Cl), and the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH). Of these areas, the MO, PrL, Nac, and LS were also significantly more active in the HBT ingroup compared to the HBT outgroup condition (Bonferroni, p<0.05, Figure 3A). These regions, which comprise part of the brains’ reward and motivation network, showed low levels of activity in the control conditions (Figure 3A), indicating that the activity observed in the HBT was not due to social exposure or to the presence of a trapped rat. Rather, their specific activation in the HBT ingroup condition suggests that they play a role in the observed prosocial response to trapped ingroup members.

Figure 3. The nucleus accumbens (Nac) is selectively active for the helping behavior test (HBT) ingroup condition.

The Nac was activated selectively for trapped ingroup members. (A) Several brain regions (in orange) were significantly more active in the HBT ingroup compared to the HBT outgroup condition (p<0.05). c-Fos numbers are also shown for the brief, 2 free, chocolate (ch), trapped (trp), and baseline (base) conditions. (B, C) Network graph depicting the top 10% inter-region correlations for the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions. Positive correlations are shown in solid lines, negative correlations in dashed lines. Central hubs were determined as the top 20% of regions with highest in both degree and betweenness (yellow). In bold, regions that were more active in the HBT ingroup condition than the outgroup. Circle color represents clusters identified via a Louvain algorithm, circle size represents the number of degrees for each region. (D) A series of multiple logistical regression tests on all test conditions identified clusters of brain regions that aligned with the distinct brain activity in the helping test conditions. The figure contrasts regions uniquely observed for the ingroup condition (x-axis) with regions observed for both ingroup and outgroup conditions (y-axis). The nucleus accumbens shell (NacSh) and nucleus accumbens core (NacC) were present uniquely in the ingroup condition in 85 and 67.5% of tests, respectively. Dashed line represents the boundary for the regions that are required to identify the ingroup condition based on brain activity. Diagram describes how the graph was derived. (E) Activity in the NacC and NacSh was positively correlated with door-opening behavior. No other regions were significantly correlated with helping.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Network analyses.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Louvain clustered heatmap of pairwise correlation values for the helping behavior test (HBT) ingroup and outgroup conditions. Bar on left visualizes the identified clusters. (C–E) Network parameters used for selecting a threshold. The 10% top-ranking correlation values were used in the network map. This threshold was determined according to the network parameters. Increasing the threshold results in a fragmented network, that is, overly scale-free and reduced in connectivity. Decreasing the threshold undermines the small-worldness of the network. (C) Scale-free topology index shows the correlation threshold cutoff of the transition to scale-free network. (D) The percent connectivity graph represents the correlation threshold for 10% connectivity. (E) The small-worldness of the network for the HBT ingroup and outgroup condition is displayed for each correlation threshold. (F, G) Central hubs of the HBT ingroup and outgroup networks. To identify central hubs, brain regions were ranked by degree and betweenness. The top 20% of regions are shown for each parameter. Brain areas appearing in the top 20% of both parameters were classified as central hubs (in yellow).

Identifying central network hubs

To explore how different brain regions interact in the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions, and to identify central hubs, functional connectivity was assessed based on c-Fos+ cell counts. This strategy has previously proved useful for outlining the neural networks involved in complex behaviors like fear learning and other social behaviors (Oliveira, 2013; Rogers-Carter et al., 2018; Vetere et al., 2017; Wheeler et al., 2013). To this end, a covariance matrix based on c-Fos numbers was generated (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A, B) and clustered using a Louvain algorithm for community detection (Blondel et al., 2008). Network graphs thresholded at 10% of the top correlations were generated from these matrices to visualize the pairwise correlations, as well as provide input about the importance, or centrality, of each region to the network (Sporns, 2013Figure 3B, C). This threshold was determined based on optimization of the network parameters such that it was as scale-free as possible without compromising connectivity and small-worldness (Figure 3—figure supplement 1C–E, see Materials and methods). Central hubs were identified as regions scoring in the top 20% for both degree (the number of connections) and betweenness (representing how many regions connect to others through this region; Figure 3B, C, Figure 3—figure supplement 1F, G). The nucleus accumbens shell (NacSh), PrL, and MO, areas identified above as selectively active in the HBT ingroup condition, also emerged as central hubs for the HBT ingroup network, suggestive of a functional role for these regions in the HBT ingroup condition.

As network graphs are prone to change based on selected parameters, a further analysis was conducted to enhance the validity of these findings. A series of 40 multinomial logistic regression tests compared the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions to a reference group using varying parameters for threshold, clustering, and weighting of the network (see Materials and methods, Figure 3D, Supplementary file 4). With this alternative analysis, several regions emerged as significant only in the HBT ingroup condition. Specifically, the NacSh appeared in >80% of tests in clusters that were significant for the ingroup condition, but not the outgroup condition (Figure 3D).

In sum, neural activity in the Nac appeared to most distinctly characterize the HBT ingroup condition. Additionally, out of all brain regions tested, the Nac was the sole measured region where neural activity was significantly correlated with helping behavior, as expressed by the history of door-opening across testing days (Figure 3E). These findings led us to further explore the role of the Nac in vivo as rats experienced the HBT.

Nac population activity in vivo demonstrates group selectivity

Across analyses, the Nac emerged as a central region for the HBT ingroup condition, both in activity levels and connectivity. In order to further explore Nac activity during the HBT, and to compare the Nac’s response to a trapped ingroup and outgroup member within rats, calcium signal was recorded in vivo as an index of neural activity during the entire HBT for SD rats tested with trapped strangers of the same strain (ingroup, n = 8) via fiber photometry. Within-rat sessions included exposure to a trapped outgroup member (stranger of the LE strain), an empty restrainer, and an open arena as a baseline (see Materials and methods). As previously demonstrated (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2014), rats were motivated to help strangers of their own strain, and 5/8 of these rats became ‘openers.’ To tag firing neurons, an adeno-associated virus (AAV) driving the expression of the genetically coded calcium indicator, GCaMP6m, under the hSyn promoter was unilaterally injected into the right Nac, and an optic fiber was implanted at the same location (Figure 4A, B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, see Materials and methods). Calcium signal was recorded by a photoreceptor and fluorescence intensity was analyzed as previously described (Lerner et al., 2015Figure 4C). To measure activity during prosocial approach, instances of the free rat’s entry into the area around the restrainer were identified as events of interest (Figure 4D). Nac activity significantly increased when rats approached a restrainer containing an ingroup member (expressed as Δf/F, n = 83 sessions, Wilcoxon ranked-sum test, p<0.05, Figure 4E, Video 1). In contrast, activity was not changed when these same rats approached a trapped outgroup member (n = 5 sessions), an empty restrainer (n = 47 sessions, Wilcoxon, p>0.05, Figure 4E), or when the rat was free to roam in an empty arena (‘baseline,’ n = 45 sessions, Figure 4F). As this measure is defined by a specific movement (entry into the zone around the restrainer), the motor movements associated with all events should be similar and are not a likely cause for the different neural signals across conditions. In evidence of this idea, velocity at the moment of entry into the restrainer was similar for the outgroup session and the following ingroup session (first 10 min, n = 7 per group, Figure 4G), and no differences were found in velocity (5.39 ± 0.28; 5.42 ± 0.34 cm/s), number of entries (19.1 ± 1.6; 19.6 ± 3.1), or time spent in the area around the restrainer (198.9 ± 18.7; 223.7 ± 29.1 s) between these ingroup and outgroup sessions, respectively (paired t-tests, p>0.05, mean ± SEM).

Figure 4. In vivo neural activity in the nucleus accumbens (Nac) corresponds with approach towards an ingroup member.

Approaching a trapped ingroup member was associated with increased calcium signal in the Nac. (A) A diagram depicting location of virus injection and optic-fiber implant used for fiber photometry recordings. (B) Example of virus spread in one animal (left) and overlay summary of all animals (right). (C) Diagram of setup used for photometry recordings. (D) Top view of testing arena. The area around the restrainer is depicted by the red rectangle. Entry into the area, defined as when the rat’s body was detected moving from outside this zone to inside this zone, was the point used in the analyses below. (E) Mean neural activity (Δf/F) across rats and testing sessions increased when rats approached a trapped ingroup member (red) but not a trapped outgroup member (blue) or empty restrainer (green). (F) Activity increased both during sessions before (cyan) and after (magenta) rats learned to open the restrainer. No increase was observed while rats were recorded in an empty arena (yellow). (G) Velocity during entry into the zone was not different across conditions (mean ± STDEV). Point of entry into the area around the restrainer is indicated by the dashed line. (H) Neural activity averaged across the whole session (mean ± SEM) was higher for the ingroup when the rat was in the area around the restrainer compared to outside this zone. Activity is also shown around the moment of door-opening (I) or when rats started eating a cranberry on the final session (J).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Summary of fiber photometry injections and implants.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Virus infection, spread, and optical fiber placements are shown for all eight animals, across the anterior-posterior (AP) extent of the nucleus accumbens (AP coordinates are from Paxinos and Watson, 1998). Representative images are shown on the left, while the summaries are shown on the right. DAPI is in blue, GCaMP6m virus in green. The ends of the fibers are represented by gray ovals. Scale bar: 2 mm.

Video 1. Video demonstrating fiber photometry recording of a rat tested with an ingroup member.

Download video file (4.6MB, mp4)

The video shows nucleus accumbens (Nac) activity increases when the free rat approaches a trapped ingroup member.

The above event-related analysis focused on the signal in the few seconds around the act of approaching the restrainer. Next, activity was analyzed over the duration of the entire session. For ingroup members, Nac activity was significantly higher when the free rat was located in the area around the restrainer compared to when they were outside this zone (repeated-measures ANOVA, p<0.01, Bonferroni p<0.01, Figure 4H). This effect was not observed for outgroup members. As the outgroup data represents activity recorded over a single session, the sampling is lower for this condition. Yet, each session included multiple approaches to the restrainer (total n = 248 entries into the zone), rendering this statistical analysis feasible.

To understand if the observed increase in signal was related to door-opening or to the intention to open the restrainer, activity at the moment of door-opening was analyzed. A peak in activity was observed at the moment of door-opening, indicating that door-opening itself was a salient event (Figure 4I). Yet, increased activity during approach was observed even before rats learned to open the restrainer (Figure 4F), indicating that Nac activity was implicated more generally in approach rather than in the act of door-opening itself. Lastly, to examine the Nac’s neural response to a non-social reward, cranberries were placed in the restrainer on the last session. We found that activity significantly decreased when rats ate a cranberry, evidence that the Nac was active during seeking, rather than reward acquisition (n = 15 eating events, Wilcoxon, p<0.05, Figure 4J). These data provide further support for selective Nac activation for ingroup members and suggest that prosocial approach is associated with increased Nac activity.

A subpopulation of cells projecting from the ACC to the Nac participates in prosocial approach

The data described above identified the frontal cortex as participating in the HBT, with the PrL and MO more active for ingroup members. To identify inputs from the frontal cortex to the Nac that promote prosocial behavior, neurons were co-labeled for c-Fos and a retrograde tracer. Structural projections were marked by injecting the retrograde tracer Fluoro-Gold (FG) into the Nac prior to participation in the HBT with a trapped ingroup member (n = 13, Figure 5A, B). Most animals learned to open the restrainer (8/13 became ‘openers’). Co-labeling of FG+ and c-Fos+ cells identified cells that were active during the HBT and structurally projected to the Nac (Figure 5C, D). The frontal cortex, insula and BLA, all areas with known projections to the Nac, were sampled for co-labeling. The PrL and MO in particular showed substantial FG+ labeling (Figure 5E). The frontal cortex also showed significantly more co-labeling for c-Fos and FG than the insula and BLA (ANOVA, F(2,34) = 20.1, p<0.001, Figure 5F), indicating that these inputs to the Nac were more active during the HBT. A significant positive correlation (Pearson’s r2 = 0.37, p=0.03) between the co-labeled cells and door-opening was uniquely observed in the projection from the ACC to the Nac (Figure 5G), whereas ACC c-Fos+ cell numbers as a whole did not correlate with door-opening (Figure 5H).

Figure 5. Analysis of structural projections to the nucleus accumbens (Nac) co-labeled for c-Fos.

Figure 5.

Structural inputs to the Nac and their corresponding activity during the helping behavior test (HBT). (A) Schematic of a retrograde injection into the Nac and the input regions that were analyzed. (B) Diagram of retrograde tracing labeling cells in the prelimbic cortex (PrL) that project to the Nac. (C) On left: example Fluoro-Gold (FG) injection. FG is in blue, c-Fos in magenta. On right: summary of all FG injections. Scale bar: 2 mm. Coordinates are anterior-posterior from Bregma Paxinos and Watson, 1998. (D) On left: an example coronal section containing regions of interest (ROIs) that to project to the Nac. On right: a fluorescent image of a PrL ROI containing FG+ cells (blue) and c-Fos+ cells (magenta). ROI scale bar: 50 µm. (E) Average number of FG+ cells for each brain ROI. (F) Percent of FG+ cells that were co-localized with c-Fos, for each region. (G) The percent of FG+ cells co-localized with c-Fos in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) positively correlated with the percent of door-openings across testing sessions. (H) The average number of c-Fos+ cells in the ACC did not correlate with the percent of door-openings.

A comparison between the openers (n = 8) and non-openers (n = 5) revealed overall increased c-Fos levels across sampled regions (ANOVA, F(1, 88) = 27.8, p<0.001, Figure 6A), with significantly higher c-Fos levels in the LO and MO of openers (Sidak correction, p<0.01), suggesting that the neural activity in the OFC may vary according to this behavior. Analysis of FG+ cells also showed overall increased numbers for the openers (ANOVA, F(7,88) = 7.9, p<0.001, Figure 6B), yet no significant differences emerged per region. We noted the BLA as an interesting region for further exploration, with a markedly increased activity in non-openers (Sidak correction, p=0.07). This is suggestive of an increased structural connectivity between the BLA and the Nac in rats that did not show helping behavior towards ingroup members. Finally, no significant difference was identified in the percent of co-labeled FG+/c-Fos+ cells (ANOVA, p>0.05, Figure 6C). In conclusion, these findings, while descriptive and based on a small sample size, point to the ACC-Nac projection as a target for future investigation into the circuitry involved in prosocial behavior.

Figure 6. Comparison between openers and non-openers.

Figure 6.

Higher c-Fos levels in the orbitofrontal cortex were observed for openers than for non-openers. (A) A comparison of total c-Fos numbers between five non-openers and eight openers in the retrograde experiment for rats tested with ingroup members. No difference was significant for the total FG+ numbers signifying structural connections (B) or the percent of co-labeled c-Fos+/FG+ cells (C) for these populations.

Discussion

In these experiments, rats were tested for helping behavior with trapped rats from their ingroup, who typically elicit prosocial motivation, or with outgroup members, who do not. Increased neural activity was observed across multiple brain regions for rats in the HBT conditions compared to the control conditions. Regardless of the identity of the trapped rat, neural activity in the sensory, orbitofrontal, anterior cingulate, and insular cortices was observed, evidence that these regions participate in processing the distress of others but are not predictive of prosocial behavior. The frontal regions MO and PrL, as well as striatal regions LS and Nac, were significantly more active in the ingroup than the outgroup HBT conditions. The Nac, a region associated with reward seeking and social reward in rodents (Aragona et al., 2006; Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2013; Dölen et al., 2013; Gunaydin et al., 2014), emerged as a central hub of the ingroup network and correlated with prosocial approach and helping behavior, pointing to its recruitment in a prosocial context.

The activation of the Nac observed in this study mirrors a broad literature of the Nac’s role in social reward-related behaviors. The Nac is mainly composed of GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs) that fall into two classes: those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D1 (D1 MSNs) and those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D2 (D2 MSNs). Broadly, these pathways are thought to lead to opposing outcomes, with D1 MSNs and their pathways activated during reward, and D2 MSNs and their pathways activated during aversion (Klawonn and Malenka, 2018, but see Soares-Cunha et al., 2016). For example, Nac MSNs are associated with stress-induced anhedonia (Bessa et al., 2013), and mice displaying depressive-like behaviors following chronic stress have decreased excitatory input in D1 MSNs and increased excitatory input in D2 MSNs (Francis et al., 2015). Further, increased D1 MSN activity is predictive of animals that are resilient to stress-induced changes in social behavior (Muir et al., 2018). While the experiments presented here do not distinguish Nac cell types, given the dichotomy of D1 and D2 activity – that is, that D2 MSNs are typically involved in aversive behaviors, including avoidance (Boschen et al., 2011), and D1 MSNs are typically involved in rewarding behaviors including aggression seeking (Aleyasin et al., 2018; Golden et al., 2019) – we speculate that dopamine receptor D1 MSN cells are involved in prosocial intent. This hypothesis would require further testing and will be an interesting area for future study.

One interpretation of these findings is that the observed neural activity for ingroup members reflects ‘empathic helping,’ prosocial behavior motivated by empathic arousal in response to the other’s distress. This interpretation is based on the prevalent view of empathy as a major driver of prosocial behavior (Decety et al., 2016; Eisenberg and Miller, 1987). Importantly, the term empathic arousal encompasses an emotional response to the other’s distress as well as ‘caring,’ or concern for their well-being (Batson, 2009). Here, we observed common elements in the neural response to trapped ingroup and outgroup members regardless of helping. This finding provides evidence that rats sense and respond to outgroup distress, even in the lack of prosocial intent.

An alternative explanation of door-opening behavior is that rats are acting primarily for social contact. While accumulating evidence shows that social contact is not required for helping in rats (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011; Cox and Reichel, 2020; Sato et al., 2015), this interpretation cannot be ruled out by the present dataset. Undoubtedly, mammalian behavior is motivated by social reward, and rats experience social reward during this paradigm. However, the helping test is a stressful situation: restraint is a well-validated chronic stressor (Glavin et al., 1994; Ottenweller et al., 1994; Paré and Glavin, 1986; Servatius et al., 2000), and testing induces a stress response in both the free and the trapped rat, including secretion of the stress hormone corticosterone, defecation, and freezing (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011; Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2016). It is thus important to consider that the behavior of the free rat is influenced by the stress of the trapped rat. Even if social reward was not the primary motivation, it is likely that rats found social contact and door-opening itself rewarding, resulting in a reward anticipation signal. Another possibility is that since restrainers are latched on the final test day, neural activity of successful helpers reflects frustration at failure to open the door. Based on the data presented here, it is impossible to disentangle these motivations and the accompanying neural elements of reward anticipation and prediction error from the prosocial response. Yet the very presence of such a signal would be inherently prosocial, reflecting an emotional response selectively elicited for ingroup members: a positive response to social contact, and an aversive response to the inability to release the trapped rat. Whatever the motivational components may include, they are all part of the response underlying ingroup bias.

An alternative explanation of the increased neural activity observed in the ingroup condition is that it may reflect primarily motor activity associated with door-opening, stemming from increased movement in these conditions. Yet, it is important to keep in mind that c-Fos expression reflects neural activity only on the final testing session, when no door-opening occurred and the restrainers were latched. Notably, no differences in velocity or time around the restrainer were observed on the last session between the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions. The measure of entry into the area around the restrainer used in the fiber photometry experiment by definition controls for movement as it depicts the same movement across conditions as the event of interest. Thus, while motor-related processing is part of the overall neural activity displayed by these rats, it is unlikely to be the primary explanation for the differences observed between groups.

Methodological limitations should also be considered. c-Fos is an indirect index of neural activity that does not provide direct access to neural firing and is suspected to be influenced by other neural events, such as plasticity (Kovács, 2008; Minatohara et al., 2015). Moreover, this index provides high spatial but low temporal resolution, which may be critical in a complex social situation where different events occur over an hour-long period. Finally, although c-Fos was sampled from 84 regions covering most major brain areas, it is still but a fraction of the entire brain. The results presented above are correlational in nature and descriptive only. Correlation data in and of itself is not sufficient evidence, especially given the limited sample size and possibility of false discoveries. Thus, these data provide a partial picture of the rats’ neural response and should be interpreted with this caveat in mind. It is nonetheless encouraging that the Nac emerged as a key region across several measures. The increase in c-Fos+ cells observed in the ingroup condition was mirrored by the in vivo calcium signal, and both measures were associated with behavior (helping and approach respectively). It is important to note that as the data presented here represents activity in male rats only, conclusions are limited to males. The collection of data from female rats is currently ongoing and will be important for generalizing these results. Overall, this article aims to provide a broad overview of the neural circuitry associated with prosocial intent and provides a base for future work that will aim to dissect these circuits and understand their causal contribution to behavior.

Regarding the motivational state underlying door-opening, consider the following: when rats open the restrainer quickly on consecutive days and become ‘openers,’ it is clearly an intentional behavior. However, when rats fail to open the restrainer, it could be explained by either lack of will or lack of ability to open the door. Here, some rats tested with ingroup members did not become ‘openers.’ Previous experiments similarly found that while most rats help ingroup members, around 30% of SD rats do not become openers. The current dataset does not provide enough information to support any conclusions regarding the motivational state of the ‘non-openers’ to surmise whether they were unable or unwilling to open the restrainer. Analysis presented above suggests that the OFC and BLA are potential targets for future investigation of these two behavioral phenotypes. Individual variability of helping behavior is of great interest and needs to be investigated in depth in future studies.

Several cortical regions exhibited graded activity in the HBT. While they were most active in the ingroup condition, they were also notably more active for HBT outgroup members compared to the baseline conditions. This may indicate that a threshold of activation must be crossed to initiate prosocial approach. Alternatively, it may point to specific neural populations within these regions that are involved in helping. In the ACC, total c-Fos levels were not correlated with helping, yet the subset of c-Fos+ cells projecting to the Nac was positively correlated with helping. This suggests that at least for the ACC the increased activity observed in the ingroup condition is due to the recruitment of a specific neural population. Emerging technologies enabling cell-specific recordings are opening a path for future studies to further identify circuit and cell-type-specific activation involved in the behavior we observed. These technologies, along with further investigation, will allow researchers to expand upon the current findings to a point that may permit artificial manipulation of prosocial motivation.

It is a major goal for society to understand the empathy gap for outgroup members: why do we help some, but remain impervious to the suffering of others? The rats we tested demonstrated social selectivity; they did not help a rat of an unfamiliar strain. The study of rodents allows us to examine the neural activity that leads to this behavior. A growing body of work demonstrates that empathy-like behavior in rodents, including vicarious fear and contextual fear learning, is associated with neural activity in the ACC and insula (Carrillo et al., 2019; Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2020; Jeon et al., 2010; Karakilic et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2018; Raam et al., 2017; Rogers-Carter et al., 2018; Sakaguchi et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2015; Schaich Borg et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2020). The similarity of these findings with the human research is notable, where empathy to others’ pain has been linked with ACC and insular activity (Preston and de Waal, 2002; Shamay-Tsoory and Lamm, 2018). The findings presented here, showing Nac activation during a prosocial context, are congruent with demonstration of Nac activity during helping and compassion in humans (Hackel et al., 2017; Harbaugh et al., 2007; Inagaki et al., 2016; Klimecki et al., 2014; Lamm et al., 2011; Morelli et al., 2018). In conclusion, this study provides the first evidence for a common neurobiological mechanism driving empathic helping across mammalian species and highlights a distinct neural response to the distress of affiliated others. These findings provide insight into the way the brain determines the value of others’ outcomes based on their social identity and open a path towards predicting and influencing prosocial decisions.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Strain, strain background (Rattus norvegicus) Sprague–Dawley Rat Charles River Labs Charles River 001; RRID:RGD_10395233
Strain, strain background (Rattus norvegicus) Long–Evans Rat Envigo Envigo: HsdBlue:LE; RRID:RGD_5508398
Recombinant DNA reagent (virus strains) AAV-hSyn-GCaMP6m Addgene Addgene: 131004; RRID:Addgene_131004
Antibody Rabbit anti-cFos primary antibody Millipore Sigma Millipore: ABE457; RRID:AB_2631318 IHC (1:1000)
Antibody Donkey anti-rabbit IgG Alexa Fluor 488 secondary antibody Jackson ImmunoResearch Labs Cat#: 711-545-152; RRID:AB_2313584 IHC (1:500)
Chemical compound, drug Fluoro-Gold Fluorochrome RRID:AB_2314408
Software, algorithm MATLAB MathWorks RRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithm Zen Zeiss RRID:SCR_013672
Software, algorithm Fiji NIH RRID:SCR_002285

Experimental design

Animals

Rat studies were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California, Berkeley. Rats were kept on a 12-hr light-dark cycle and received food and water ad libitum. In total, 83 rats were tested across all experiments. Adult male SD rats ('SD,' age p60–p90 days) were used as the free and trapped ingroup rats (Charles River, Portage, MI). Adult male Long–Evans rats were used as trapped outgroup rats ('LE,' Envigo, CA). All rats that were ordered were allowed a minimum of 5 days to acclimate to the facility prior to beginning testing.

Helping behavior test (HBT)

The HBT was performed as described previously (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011). Animals were handled for 5 days and tested for boldness four times. For 3 days, animals received 30 min habituation sessions to the arena, followed by a 15 min session of open-field testing in the same arena on the fourth day. The boldness and open-field testing were conducted in order to remain consistent with previous experiments, but were not analyzed and are thus not reported.

During the HBT, a free rat was placed in an open arena containing a rat trapped inside a restrainer (Figure 1A, D–F). The free rat could help the trapped rat by opening the restrainer door with its snout, thereby releasing the trapped rat. One-hour-long sessions were repeated daily over a 2-week period. At 40 min, the door was opened half-way by the experimenter; this was typically followed by the exit of the trapped rat and was aimed at preventing learned helplessness. Door-opening was counted as such when performed by the free rat before the half-way opening point. Rats that learned to open the restrainer and consistently opened it on the final three days of testing were labeled as ‘openers.’

To prevent individual familiarity with the outgroup member, strangers were swapped daily. Note that we previously found rats will release trapped strangers of their own strain, and that familiarity with the strain determines prosocial behavior rather than individual familiarity (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2014). On the last day of testing, the restrainer was latched shut throughout the 60 min session and rats were perfused immediately following behavioral testing. Sessions were video recorded with a CCD color camera (KT&C Co, Seoul, Korea) connected to a video card (Geovision, Irvine, CA) that linked to a PC. Movement data were analyzed using Ethovision video tracking software (Noldus Information Technology, Inc, Leesburg, VA). Housing conditions: rats were housed in pairs, with a cagemate of the same strain, and were allowed 2 weeks to habituate to their cagemate prior to any testing. In the c-Fos experiment, ingroup members were SD cagemates. For the photometry experiments, ingroup members were SD strangers. The trapped outgroup member was a different LE stranger daily.

Control conditions

For the ‘chocolate’ condition, rats underwent same procedure as the HBT described above, but instead of a rat, five chocolate chips were placed inside the restrainer. To prevent novelty-induced hypophagia, rats were exposed to chocolate chips in their homecage (and consistently ate them) a few days before start of testing. In the ‘brief’ condition, rats were tested for three daily 1-hr-long sessions with a rat trapped inside a latched, unopenable restrainer. In the ‘2 free’ condition, a wire mesh was placed in the arena with the test rat on one side and the partner on the other side. Neither rat was trapped. For the ‘trapped’ condition, rats trapped inside the restrainer were used following the last session with ingroup members. For the ‘baseline,’ adult SD rats were used. These rats did not undergo behavioral testing and, thus, c-Fos measurements reflect time in the homecage.

HBT for the fiber photometry experiment

A variation on the HBT was performed to allow a within-subject comparison to the control conditions. Following a week of handling, rats were tested with a trapped rat of the same strain for daily sessions while connected to the optic fiber. The door was opened half-way by the experimenter after 30 min. Rats usually escaped the restrainer within a few minutes of half-way door-opening. After the trapped rat exited the restrainer, rats were allowed 10 minutes of free roaming, before the trapped rat's removal. Then, 10 minutes of recording were conducted while the free rat was in the arena with the empty, closed restrainer. Finally, the restrainer was removed and another 5 min baseline was recorded for the free rat in the empty arena. Additionally, rats were tested with a trapped outgroup member, an LE rat, on day 7. None of the rats had learned to open the restrainer at this point. We hypothesized that the optic cable was an obstacle to door-opening. Thus, the paradigm was run without the optic fiber for a week, allowing rats to learn the opening behavior. For this, rats were given two 30 min sessions of habituation time in the arena without the fiber, followed by 4–5 days with the trapped rat, as described above. As the fiber was not connected, no neural signal was collected during these sessions, which were recorded by video. Rats were then tested with the fiber for another 3 days with the trapped rat. We found that rats that had learned the door-opening behavior continued to do so on the later sessions when the wire was now connected.

On the last session, five rats were tested with a dried cranberry placed inside the closed restrainer in order to record the neural activity involving a non-social reward. Once rats ate the cranberry (within a few minutes), another cranberry was placed inside the restrainer by the investigator and the door was closed again. This was repeated three times per animal. For one rat that did not open the restrainer, the cranberries were placed in the arena outside the restrainer. Rats always ate all cranberries offered.

Immunohistochemistry

On the last day of testing, animals were sacrificed within 90 min from the beginning of the session, at the peak expression of the early immediate gene product c-Fos. Rats were transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline and freshly made 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline (PBS). Brains were sunk in 30% sucrose as a cryoprotectant and frozen at –80°C. They were later sliced at 40 μm and stained for c-Fos. Sections were washed with 0.1 M tris-buffered saline (TBS), incubated in 3% normal donkey serum (NDS) in 0.3% TritonX-100 in TBS (TxTBS), then transferred to rabbit anti-c-Fos antiserum (ABE457; Millipore, 1:1000; 1% NDS; 0.3% TxTBS) overnight. Sections were then incubated in Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit antiserum (AF488; Jackson, 1:500; 1% NDS; 0.3% TxTBS). Sections were briefly washed in 0.1 M TBS again. Sections were further stained in DAPI (1:40,000) for 10 min if they did not contain the retrograde tracer FG and were then washed for a further 15 min (3 × 5′). Tissue that contained FG could not be stained for DAPI as both dyes are excited by UV fluorescence and their spectra overlap. Lastly, all slides were coverslipped with DABCO, dried overnight, and stored at 4°C until imaged.

Immunostained tissue was imaged at 10× using a wide-field fluorescence microscope (Zeiss AxioScan) and was processed in Zen software. Regions of interest (ROIs) (250 × 250 µm squares) were placed across the whole brain (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) and closely followed the methods performed in Wheeler et al., 2013; Sadananda et al., 2008. A custom written script in ImageJ V2.0.0 (National Institute of Health, Bethesda, MD) was used to quantify immunoreactive nuclei (either c-Fos+ and/or FG+ cells), followed by manual checks and counting by multiple individuals who were blind to condition; consistency for counts across individuals was verified by a subset of samples. The threshold for detection of positive nuclei was set at a consistent level for each brain region, and only targets within the size range of 25–125 mm2 in area were counted as cells. Manual verification was targeted at identifying gross errors in the ImageJ scripts. For instance, in some cases the script falsely identified >100 cells within the counting square; this usually occurred when there was high background staining. This type of error occurred in ~15% of the samples, which were then manually corrected. All means are reported as mean ± SEM. Furthermore, 39 values for cell counts were removed from the dataset as outliers. The outliers were defined as those that were more than 2 standard deviations higher or lower than the group mean and further fell outside of the observed range for all conditions.

Network analysis

Generating network graphs

The network graph was obtained via a correlation matrix of c-Fos activity between all brain regions (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A, B). The top correlations are presented in a graphic form (Figure 3B, C). The cutoff threshold of 10% top correlations was determined based on scale-free network characteristics (Figure 3—figure supplement 1C–E). To obtain network graphs of the c-Fos data, pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients were determined between the number of c-Fos+ cells for all pairs of brain regions. The top 10% correlations from these matrices were used to generate network maps (Figure 3B, C). Correlation values higher than the cutoff were set to 1, and the corresponding brain regions >1 were considered connected for that threshold cutoff. This resulted in a brain region network for each correlation threshold cutoff.

Calculating the correlation threshold

To obtain this threshold, all the possible correlation thresholds were enumerated and applied to the correlation matrices of the c-Fos data. Scale-free topology index and percent connectivity were computed for each network based on the WGCNA tutorial (Langfelder and Horvath, 2008). The results were plotted as functions of correlation threshold cutoffs. A positive value of the scale-free topology index indicates a scale-free network, a property of diverse types of non-random networks (Barabási, 2009). A correlation threshold cutoff of 10% was the transition point where the network demonstrated scale-free network characteristics (with a positive scale-free topology index), whereas a higher cutoff resulted in an unstable network with few connections. Additionally, the small-worldness of the brain networks was examined for the ingroup and outgroup maps. For this, the entire range of possible correlation threshold cutoffs was enumerated to compute two metrics: small-worldness and network density. The small-worldness was calculated in the same way as is discussed in Humphries and Gurney, 2008; the network density as is defined in Wasserman and Faust, 1994.

Identification of central hubs

Central hubs were determined by ranking all brain regions according to two parameters: the betweenness value, representing the number of times all regions must pass through the ROI in order to reach other regions via the shortest path, and the number of connections (degrees) with other regions. The 20% top-ranking regions were then identified; brain areas that were in the top 20% for both categories were considered to be central hubs of the network (Figure 3—figure supplement 1F, G).

Additional strategy for identifying central regions

The network presented is based on one set of parameters (threshold, clustering algorithm, weights), and as such, has limited validity. In order to increase the robustness of our conclusions, an additional analysis was conducted using an alternative strategy. The aim was to identify regions important for the ingroup condition according to which areas were unique actively in that region according to a pool of different statistical tests based on different parameters and configurations. To this end, a series of 40 multinomial logistic regression tests (Figure 3D) compared the HBT adult ingroup and outgroup conditions to a reference group (which was itself varied) using varying parameters for threshold, clustering, and weighting of the network for each test (Supplementary file 4). This method identified clusters of regions that repeatedly emerged as different than the baseline (reference group) clusters. Across all these tests, clusters varied slightly as the clustering algorithms are sensitive to the manipulated parameters. The number of times each brain region in the clusters significantly differed from baseline was quantified for each condition. To be more specific, in our customized ensemble clustering algorithm based on methods described in Vega-Pons and Ruiz-Shulcloper, 2011, each run of the 40 tests resulted in a computation of principal components. The first principal component (eigenvector) of each cluster was recorded. These were considered as the representative ‘eigen region’ for the clusters they were derived from. Subsequently, the eigen regions, representative of each cluster, were fed into a multinomial logistic regression (Hilbe, 2009; Zalaquett and Thiessen, 1991) to determine whether they were significantly different between the ingroup and the reference group or the outgroup and the reference group. The reference group itself varied between using the untested condition and a broader reference group containing the c-Fos data from rats in the brief condition, rats in the ‘2 free’ condition, the trapped rats themselves, and rats in the ‘baseline’ condition (total n = 48). The standard p-value cutoff of 0.05 was used after Bonferroni correction. Louvain clustering (Blondel et al., 2008) was done with the igraph package in R (Csardi and Nepusz, 2006), and Dynamic tree cut of hierarchical clustering was done with the WGCNA package in R (Langfelder and Horvath, 2008). ‘Soft power’ was defined as described in Langfelder and Horvath, 2008. Each cluster was classified as significantly different than the reference for the ingroup, outgroup, both, or none. The number of occurrences per region in each category was quantified. A high ratio of occurrences in the ingroup-only category indicates that this region was uniquely important for the ingroup condition.

Fiber photometry calcium signal recordings

Rats underwent unilateral injections of 1 μL of virus (AAV-hsyn-GCaMP6m) into the right hemisphere of the Nac (AP: +2.0, ML: +1.0, DV: –7.2) and were implanted with an optic fiber patch cord with a numerical aperture of 0.48, and 400 µm core (Doric Lenses). Six weeks were allowed for virus infection, and upon signal detection, rats began testing in the HBT while neural activity was recorded, as described below. Rats were removed from the experiment in cases where no signal was detected or due to failed implants (n = 10). The protocol followed that previously performed in Lerner et al., 2015. An LED emitting 470 nm light was used for the Ca2+ signal and 405 nm light was used as a control signal to remove movement artifacts. GCaMP fluorescence was collected by the same fiber; light passed through a dichroic lens with a GFP emission filter and was registered by a photoreceiver. Synapse software (TDT) and MATLAB code were used to demodulate the brightness from the 470 nm and 405 nm excitation and synchronize it with the video data. For analysis, signal was normalized to the median of the session and was represented as Δf/F after a least-square linear fit was applied via a custom MATLAB code. After testing was complete, rats were transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline and 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. Brains were sunk in 30% sucrose as a cryoprotectant, frozen at –80°C, and sliced at 40 μm. Representative sections containing the Nac were mounted on slides and imaged in order to determine the location of the implant and virus spread. In order to line up entry to the point around the restrainer, the synchronized videos were analyzed in Ethovision (Noldus Information Technology, Inc). Using MATLAB code, each frame of entry (where 'in the zone' changed from 0 to 1) was identified, matched to the neural data via the synchronized time stamps, and used as the 0 point for Figure 4E, F, indicating entry into the zone around the restrainer. The time stamp of door-opening events was identified manually and used as input for the MATLAB code. Movement data was analyzed for the first 10 min of two subsequent sessions, the outgroup session and an ingroup session. This time frame was selected since for all sessions the restrainer was closed during this time.

Retrograde tracing

Rats were anesthetized with 3–5% isoflurane and mounted onto a stereotaxic frame. The skull was exposed and a small hole was made above the determined stereotactic coordinates on the right hemisphere (AP: +2.0, ML: +1.0, DV: –7.2; from Bregma; Paxinos and Watson, 1998). A Hamilton Syringe containing the retrograde tracer Fluoro-Gold (FG, Fluorochrome, 4% in saline) was used to administer 200 nL of dye into the Nac. Rats were allowed 1–2 weeks to recover from surgery prior to starting the behavioral task. One animal failed to recover following surgery due to an infection and was removed from the experiment, bringing the total number of animals to 13. Following behavior, histology was performed as described above. The number of immunostained cells co-labeled for FG and c-Fos was manually counted in ImageJ (NIH), as well as the overall number of FG+ cells. Co-labeled cells represent neurons that were active during the task and that project to the Nac. The number of c-Fos+ cells was counted using an ImageJ script and then manually checked by hand. A subset of images was counted by two or more experimenters in order to ensure that cell counting was consistent across all observers.

Statistical analysis

Task PLS analysis

Task PLS is a multivariate statistical technique that was used to identify optimal patterns of neural activity that differentiate between the experimental conditions (McIntosh, 1999; McIntosh et al., 1996). It searches for LVs that can explain a large percent of the variability in the data by maximizing the contrast between test conditions. Through singular value decomposition, PLS produces a set of mutually orthogonal LV pairs. One element of the LV depicts the contrast, which reflects a commonality or difference between conditions. The other element of the LV, the relative contribution of each brain region (termed here ‘salience’), identifies brain regions that show the activation profile across tasks, indicating which brain areas are maximally expressed in a particular LV. Statistical assessment of PLS was performed by using permutation testing for LVs and bootstrap estimation of standard error for the brain region saliences. For the LV, significance was assessed by permutation testing: resampling without replacement by shuffling the test condition. Following each resampling, the PLS was recalculated. This was done 500 times in order to determine whether the effects represented in a given LV were significantly different than random noise. For brain region salience, reliability was assessed using bootstrap estimation of standard error. Bootstrap tests were performed by resampling 500 times with replacement, while keeping the subjects assigned to their conditions. This reflects the reliability of the contribution of that brain region to the LV. Brain regions with a bootstrap ratio >2.57 (roughly corresponding to a confidence interval of 99%) were considered as reliably contributing to the pattern. Missing values were interpolated by the average for the test condition. An advantage to using this approach over univariate methods is that no corrections for multiple comparisons are necessary because the brain region saliences are calculated on all of the brain regions in a single mathematical step. MATLAB code for running the Task PLS analysis is available for download from the McIntosh lab website.

Other statistical tests

In addition to the PLS analysis described above, a one-way ANOVA was conducted on the c-Fos data to compare the HBT ingroup and outgroup conditions and baseline for each brain region. Unpaired t-tests and two-way ANOVAs were used to compare the pattern of animals’ movements. Bonferroni or Sidak post-hoc corrections were used following all ANOVAs. The comparison of velocity and time around the restrainer per day or minute was conducted via a MMA with an autoregressive covariance structure, with 'day' and ‘minute’ as the repeated measure for calculations of velocity and time around the restrainer. Movement was binned per minute. Data lines with velocity >30 cm/s were excluded as these values are not physically plausible and likely represent acquisition errors. To compare the photometry signal in the time before and after entry to the area around the restrainer, the signal in the second before entry to the zone (the 0 point) was compared to the signal in the second after entry to the zone around the restrainer for the ingroup, outgroup, and empty restrainer data. Wilcoxon signed-rank, a non-parametric test, was used in order to avoid assumptions about signal distribution. The comparisons of total session time spent in and outside the zone around the restrainer were conducted with a repeated-measures ANOVA, where the repeated measure was time in the zone and time outside the zone, as these were dependent samples for each animal. Note that the dataset used for the time-series analysis and the total time analysis are not the same, and thus the y-axis values are different. The time-series PSTH graph includes only the 10 s before and after each point of entry, whereas the total time bar graphs include the entire dataset for each session, split by location. Changes across days to helping behavior, including percent door-opening and latency to door-opening, were examined using the non-parametric Cochran’s Q test and Friedman test respectively. Pearson’s correlations were used for all correlation analyses; corrections for multiple comparisons were not run so as to avoid type 2 errors correcting for correlations with 45 brain regions.

Number of animals

We aimed for eight animals per testing group. This number is based on our previous experience with the HBT. We generally observe ~70% of rats become ‘openers’ when tested with ingroup members (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2011) and expect no ‘openers’ for rats tested with outgroup members (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., 2014); eight rats per group provide a power of 95% for incidence of ‘openers,’ with α and β of 0.05.

Number of brain regions sampled

The list of brain regions was based on previous work that followed the same methodology as used here (Vetere et al., 2017; Wheeler et al., 2013).

Acknowledgements

We thank the following people for their help with this manuscript: Erin Aisenberg, Vanessa Alschuler, Noopur Amin, Anjile An, Kelsey Clausing, Dr. Robert Froemke, Pooya Ganjali, Nikita Gourishetty, Kelsey Hu, Kylie Huang, Ben Kantor, Claire Lee, Catriona Lewis, Dr. Chris Morrissey, Charu Ramakrishnan, Dr. Peter Sudmant, Yuanqui Sun, Charlie Walters, and Shannon Wong-Michalak. Imaging was conducted at the CRL Molecular Imaging Center, supported by the UC Berkeley Biological Faculty Research Fund. We would like to thank Holly Aaron and Feather Ives for their microscopy training and assistance. This manuscript was supported by the Miller Institute for Basic Science, Israel Science Foundation, Azrieli Foundation (IBB), and CIFAr (DKa).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal, Email: inbalbe@tauex.tau.ac.il.

Shelly B Flagel, University of Michigan, United States.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Adolph C. and Mary Sprague Miller Institute for Basic Research in Science, University of California Berkeley to Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal.

  • CIFAR to Daniela Kaufer.

  • Israel Science Foundation 0610719821 to Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal.

  • Azrieli Foundation to Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Visualization.

Investigation.

Investigation.

Investigation.

Formal analysis, Methodology.

Formal analysis, Methodology.

Supervision, Methodology.

Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

Resources, Software, Supervision, Methodology.

Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Rat studies were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California, Berkeley. protocol number AUP-2014-11-6943.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Detailed list of brain regions used in the figures.
elife-65582-supp1.docx (13.3KB, docx)
Supplementary file 2. Means and confidence intervals for brain-wide c-Fos numbers across conditions.
elife-65582-supp2.docx (23.7KB, docx)
Supplementary file 3. Means and confidence intervals for c-Fos numbers per brain region.
elife-65582-supp3.docx (611.9KB, docx)
Supplementary file 4. Set of parameters used in a series of multinomial regression tests.

Combinations of clustering algorithms, parameters, and subsets of data used in Figure 3D. ‘Cluster group’ was used to obtain the cluster assignments for each brain region. ‘Reference group’ was the control group in the multinomial logistic regression. The ‘weighted’ column indicates whether weights (correlations) between brain regions were taken into account while performing clustering. ‘All’: all groups; ‘rest’: all conditions except ingroup and outgroup; ‘baseline’: untested baseline condition. The thresholds of percentile rank, p-value, and absolute r value in the parameter column were applied to the covariance matrix of the c-Fos data in the cluster group before feeding it into the selected clustering algorithm.

elife-65582-supp4.docx (30.1KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Data of c-Fos numbers, calcium signal and movement are uploaded on the Open Science Framework depository (https://osf.io/5qtcx/).

The following dataset was generated:

Bartal B-AI. 2021. Neural correlates of ingroup bias for pro-sociality in rats. Open Science Framework. osf.io/5qtcx/

References

  1. Aleyasin H, Flanigan ME, Golden SA, Takahashi A, Menard C, Pfau ML, Multer J, Pina J, McCabe KA, Bhatti N, Hodes GE, Heshmati M, Neve RL, Nestler EJ, Heller EA, Russo SJ. Cell-Type-Specific role of ??fosb in nucleus accumbens in modulating intermale aggression. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:5913–5924. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0296-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amodio DM, Harmon-Jones E, Devine PG, Curtin JJ, Hartley SL, Covert AE. Neural signals for the detection of unintentional race Bias. Psychological Science. 2004;15:88–93. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anacker AM, Beery AK. Life in groups: the roles of oxytocin in mammalian sociality. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;7:185. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Yu YJ, Curtis JT, Detwiler JM, Insel TR, Wang Z. Nucleus accumbens dopamine differentially mediates the formation and maintenance of monogamous pair bonds. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:133–139. doi: 10.1038/nn1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Avenanti A, Sirigu A, Aglioti SM. Racial Bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with Other-Race pain. Current Biology. 2010;20:1018–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Báez-Mendoza R, Schultz W. The role of the striatum in social behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:233. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barabási A-L. Scale-Free networks: a decade and beyond. Science. 2009;325:412–413. doi: 10.1126/science.1173299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Batson DC. These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena. In: Ickes J. D, editor. The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. The MIT Press; 2009. pp. 3–15. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Batson CD. Altruism in Humans. Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  10. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Decety J, Mason P. Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science. 2011;334:1427–1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1210789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Rodgers DA, Bernardez Sarria MS, Decety J, Mason P. Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. eLife. 2014;3:e01385. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ben-Ami Bartal I, Shan H, Molasky NM, Murray TM, Williams JZ, Decety J, Mason P. Anxiolytic treatment impairs helping behavior in rats. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:850. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bessa JM, Morais M, Marques F, Pinto L, Palha JA, Almeida OF, Sousa N. Stress-induced anhedonia is associated with hypertrophy of medium spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens. Translational Psychiatry. 2013;3:e266. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blondel VD, Guillaume J-L, Lambiotte R, Lefebvre E. Fast unfolding of communities in large networks. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment. 2008;2008:P10008. doi: 10.1088/1742-5468/2008/10/P10008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boschen SL, Wietzikoski EC, Winn P, Da Cunha C. The role of nucleus accumbens and dorsolateral striatal D2 receptors in active avoidance conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2011;96:254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brosnan SF. Comparative Evolutionary Psychology. In: Shackelford T, editor. The Sage Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. SAGE; 2020. pp. 503–504. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FB, Young LJ. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science. 2016;351:375–378. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Campbell MW, de Waal FB. Ingroup-outgroup Bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e18283. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carrillo M, Han Y, Migliorati F, Liu M, Gazzola V, Keysers C. Emotional mirror neurons in the rat's Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Current Biology. 2019;29:2104. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.05.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheon BK, Im DM, Harada T, Kim JS, Mathur VA, Scimeca JM, Parrish TB, Park HW, Chiao JY. Cultural influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage. 2011;57:642–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chiao JY, Iidaka T, Gordon HL, Nogawa J, Bar M, Aminoff E, Sadato N, Ambady N. Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:2167–2174. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cikara M, Botvinick MM, Fiske ST. Us versus them: social identity shapes neural responses to intergroup competition and harm. Psychological Science. 2011;22:306–313. doi: 10.1177/0956797610397667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cox SS, Reichel CM. Rats display empathic behavior independent of the opportunity for social interaction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:1097–1104. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0572-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cox SS, Reichel CM. Current rodent models for the study of empathic processes. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2021;32:96–111. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cronin KA. Prosocial behaviour in animals: the influence of social relationships, communication and rewards. Animal Behaviour. 2012;84:1085–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Csardi G, Nepusz T. The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJournal, Complex Systems. 2006;1695:1–9. [Google Scholar]
  27. de Waal FB. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Decety J, Norman GJ, Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT. A neurobehavioral evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy. Progress in Neurobiology. 2012;98:38–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Decety J, Bartal IB, Uzefovsky F, Knafo-Noam A. Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2016;371:20150077. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dölen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature. 2013;501:179–184. doi: 10.1038/nature12518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dovidio JG. Intergroup bias. In: Indzey S. F, editor. Handbook of Social Psychology. Wiley; 2010. pp. 1084–1121. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Echols S, Correll J. It's More than skin deep: Empathy and helping behavior across social groups. In: Decety J, editor. Empathy: From Bench to Bedside. MIT Press; 2012. pp. 55–71. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eisenberg N. Empathy and related vicarious emotional responses. New Directions for Child Development. 1989;44:1–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Eisenberg N. Empathy-related responding and prosocial behaviour. Novartis Foundation Symposium; 2007. pp. 71–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Eisenberg N, Eggum ND, Di Giunta L. Empathy-related responding: associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2010;4:143–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Eisenberg N, Miller PA. The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101:91–119. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fiske ST. What we know now about Bias and intergroup conflict, the problem of the century. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:123–128. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00183. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Francis TC, Chandra R, Friend DM, Finkel E, Dayrit G, Miranda J, Brooks JM, Iñiguez SD, O'Donnell P, Kravitz A, Lobo MK. Nucleus accumbens medium spiny neuron subtypes mediate depression-related outcomes to social defeat stress. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;77:212–222. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fu F, Tarnita CE, Christakis NA, Wang L, Rand DG, Nowak MA. Evolution of in-group favoritism. Scientific Reports. 2012;2:460. doi: 10.1038/srep00460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Glavin GB, Paré WP, Sandbak T, Bakke HK, Murison R. Restraint stress in biomedical research: an update. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1994;18:223–249. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)90027-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Golden SA, Jin M, Heins C, Venniro M, Michaelides M, Shaham Y. Nucleus accumbens Drd1-Expressing neurons control aggression Self-Administration and aggression seeking in mice. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:2482–2496. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2409-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gunaydin LA, Grosenick L, Finkelstein JC, Kauvar IV, Fenno LE, Adhikari A, Lammel S, Mirzabekov JJ, Airan RD, Zalocusky KA, Tye KM, Anikeeva P, Malenka RC, Deisseroth K. Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell. 2014;157:1535–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gutsell JN, Inzlicht M. Intergroup differences in the sharing of emotive states: neural evidence of an empathy gap. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:596–603. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Guzowski JF, Timlin JA, Roysam B, McNaughton BL, Worley PF, Barnes CA. Mapping behaviorally relevant neural circuits with immediate-early gene expression. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15:599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hackel LM, Zaki J, Van Bavel JJ. Social identity shapes social valuation: evidence from prosocial behavior and vicarious reward. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2017;12:1219–1228. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hamilton WD. The genetical evolution of social behaviour I. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1964;7:1–16. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Harbaugh WT, Mayr U, Burghart DR. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science. 2007;316:1622–1625. doi: 10.1126/science.1140738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hein G, Silani G, Preuschoff K, Batson CD, Singer T. Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron. 2010;68:149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hernandez-Lallement J, van Wingerden M, Marx C, Srejic M, Kalenscher T. Rats prefer mutual rewards in a prosocial choice task. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8:443. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hernandez-Lallement J, Attah AT, Soyman E, Pinhal CM, Gazzola V, Keysers C. Harm to others acts as a negative reinforcer in rats. Current Biology. 2020;30:949–961. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hilbe JM. Logistic Regression Models. Chapman and hall/CRC; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  52. Humphries MD, Gurney K. Network 'small-world-ness': a quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLOS ONE. 2008;3:e0002051. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Inagaki TK, Bryne Haltom KE, Suzuki S, Jevtic I, Hornstein E, Bower JE, Eisenberger NI. The neurobiology of giving versus receiving support: the role of Stress-Related and social Reward-Related neural activity. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2016;78:443–453. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jeon D, Kim S, Chetana M, Jo D, Ruley HE, Lin SY, Rabah D, Kinet JP, Shin HS. Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:482–488. doi: 10.1038/nn.2504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Karakilic A, Kizildag S, Kandis S, Guvendi G, Koc B, Camsari GB, Camsari UM, Ates M, Arda SG, Uysal N. The effects of acute foot shock stress on empathy levels in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2018;349:31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.04.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Klawonn AM, Malenka RC. Nucleus accumbens modulation in reward and aversion. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology; 2018. pp. 119–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Ricard M, Singer T. Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2014;9:873–879. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kovács KJ. Measurement of immediate-early gene activation- c-fos and beyond. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2008;20:665–672. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01734.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2492–2502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Langfelder P, Horvath S. WGCNA: an R package for weighted correlation network analysis. BMC Bioinformatics. 2008;9:559. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-9-559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lerner TN, Shilyansky C, Davidson TJ, Evans KE, Beier KT, Zalocusky KA, Crow AK, Malenka RC, Luo L, Tomer R, Deisseroth K. Intact-Brain analyses reveal distinct information carried by SNc dopamine subcircuits. Cell. 2015;162:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Levine M, Prosser A, Evans D, Reicher S. Identity and emergency intervention: how social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31:443–453. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lin YT, Hsieh TY, Tsai TC, Chen CC, Huang CC, Hsu KS. Conditional deletion of hippocampal CA2/CA3a oxytocin receptors impairs the persistence of Long-Term social recognition memory in mice. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:1218–1231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1896-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mahajan N, Martinez MA, Gutierrez NL, Diesendruck G, Banaji MR, Santos LR. The evolution of intergroup Bias: perceptions and attitudes in rhesus macaques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;100:387–405. doi: 10.1037/a0022459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Márquez C, Rennie SM, Costa DF, Moita MA. Prosocial choice in rats depends on Food-Seeking behavior displayed by recipients. Current Biology. 2015;25:1736–1745. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Masuda N, Fu F. Evolutionary models of in-group favoritism. F1000Prime Reports. 2015;7:27. doi: 10.12703/P7-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. McIntosh AR, Bookstein FL, Haxby JV, Grady CL. Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images using partial least squares. NeuroImage. 1996;3:143–157. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McIntosh AR. Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. Memory. 1999;7:523–548. doi: 10.1080/096582199387733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Meyza KZ, Bartal IB, Monfils MH, Panksepp JB, Knapska E. The roots of empathy: through the Lens of rodent models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;76:216–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Minatohara K, Akiyoshi M, Okuno H. Role of Immediate-Early genes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal ensembles underlying the memory trace. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2015;8:78. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2015.00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Morelli SA, Knutson B, Zaki J. Neural sensitivity to personal and vicarious reward differentially relates to Prosociality and well-being. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2018;13:831–839. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Muir J, Lorsch ZS, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K, Nestler EJ, Calipari ES, Bagot RC. In vivo fiber photometry reveals signature of future stress susceptibility in nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:255–263. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Nakamura M, Masuda N. Groupwise information sharing promotes ingroup favoritism in indirect reciprocity. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2012;12:213. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Nowak MA, Tarnita CE, Wilson EO. The evolution of eusociality. Nature. 2010;466:1057–1062. doi: 10.1038/nature09205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Oliveira RF. Mind the fish: zebrafish as a model in cognitive social neuroscience. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:131. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ottenweller JE, Servatius RJ, Natelson BH. Repeated stress persistently elevates morning, but not evening, plasma corticosterone levels in male rats. Physiology & Behavior. 1994;55:337–340. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90143-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Paluck EL, Porat R, Clark CS, Green DP. Prejudice reduction: progress and challenges. Annual Review of Psychology. 2021;72:533–560. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-071620-030619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Paré WP, Glavin GB. Restraint stress in biomedical research: a review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1986;10:339–370. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(86)90017-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press, San Diego; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  80. Preston SD, de Waal FB. Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2002;25:1–20. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X02000018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Raam T, McAvoy KM, Besnard A, Veenema AH, Sahay A. Hippocampal oxytocin receptors are necessary for discrimination of social stimuli. Nature Communications. 2017;8:2001. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02173-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Rice GE, Gainer P. "Altruism" in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1962;55:123–125. doi: 10.1037/h0042276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Robinson EJ, Barker JL. Inter-group cooperation in humans and other animals. Biology Letters. 2017;13:20160793. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Rogers-Carter MM, Varela JA, Gribbons KB, Pierce AF, McGoey MT, Ritchey M, Christianson JP. Insular cortex mediates approach and avoidance responses to social affective stimuli. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:404–414. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0071-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Ruckmann J, Bodden M, Jansen A, Kircher T, Dodel R, Rief W. How pain empathy depends on ingroup/outgroup decisions: a functional magnet resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2015;234:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Sadananda M, Wöhr M, Schwarting RK. Playback of 22-kHz and 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations induces differential c-fos expression in rat brain. Neuroscience Letters. 2008;435:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sakaguchi T, Iwasaki S, Okada M, Okamoto K, Ikegaya Y. Ethanol facilitates socially evoked memory recall in mice by recruiting pain-sensitive anterior cingulate cortical neurons. Nature Communications. 2018;9:3526. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05894-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sato N, Tan L, Tate K, Okada M. Rats demonstrate helping behavior toward a soaked conspecific. Animal Cognition. 2015;18:1039–1047. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0872-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schaich Borg J, Srivastava S, Lin L, Heffner J, Dunson D, Dzirasa K, de Lecea L. Rat intersubjective decisions are encoded by frequency-specific oscillatory contexts. Brain and Behavior. 2017;7:e00710. doi: 10.1002/brb3.710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Servatius RJ, Natelson BH, Moldow R, Pogach L, Brennan FX, Ottenweller JE. Persistent neuroendocrine changes in multiple hormonal axes after a single or repeated stressor exposures. Stress. 2000;3:263–274. doi: 10.3109/10253890009001132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shamay-Tsoory S, Lamm C. The neuroscience of empathy - from past to present and future. Neuropsychologia. 2018;116:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Soares-Cunha C, Coimbra B, Sousa N, Rodrigues AJ. Reappraising striatal D1- and D2-neurons in reward and aversion. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;68:370–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Sporns O. Making sense of brain network data. Nature Methods. 2013;10:491–493. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Stürmer S, Snyder M, Kropp A, Siem B. Empathy-motivated helping: the moderating role of group membership. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:943–956. doi: 10.1177/0146167206287363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Vega-Pons S, Ruiz-Shulcloper J. A survey of clustering ensemble algorithms. International Journal of Pattern Recognition and Artificial Intelligence. 2011;25:337–372. doi: 10.1142/S0218001411008683. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vetere G, Kenney JW, Tran LM, Xia F, Steadman PE, Parkinson J, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. Chemogenetic interrogation of a Brain-wide fear memory network in mice. Neuron. 2017;94:363–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wasserman S, Faust K. Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Cambridge University Press; 1994. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wheeler AL, Teixeira CM, Wang AH, Xiong X, Kovacevic N, Lerch JP, McIntosh AR, Parkinson J, Frankland PW. Identification of a functional connectome for Long-Term fear memory in mice. PLOS Computational Biology. 2013;9:e1002853. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wilson EO. The Social Conquest of Earth. Liveright Publishing Corporation; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  100. Xu X, Zuo X, Wang X, Han S. Do you feel my pain? racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8525–8529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zalaquett C, Thiessen D. The effects of odors from stressed mice on conspecific behavior. Physiology & Behavior. 1991;50:221–227. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90524-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zheng C, Huang Y, Bo B, Wei L, Liang Z, Wang Z. Projection from the anterior cingulate cortex to the lateral part of mediodorsal thalamus modulates vicarious freezing behavior. Neuroscience Bulletin. 2020;36:217–229. doi: 10.1007/s12264-019-00427-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Shelly B Flagel1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This manuscript provides a comprehensive combination of behavioral, cellular, and circuit-based analyses that support a role for the nucleus accumbens in social motivation. The results provide novel insight regarding the neural mechanisms governing prosociality, and will therefore be of broad interest to the scientific community.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Neural Correlates of Ingroup Bias for Pro-sociality in Rats" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Kate Wassum as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential Revisions:

In addition to the reviewers' detailed comments below, the editors and reviewers have agreed that the following essential revisions must be addressed:

1) A more rigorous analysis of behavioral data (as outlined in the reviewers' comments below).

2) An enhanced description of the cFos data and calcium transients as they relate to behavior.

3) Further consideration of the natural variation inherent in the reported behaviors.

4) Inclusion of data on cell counts and cell density, as well as representative Fos images that generated those data.

5) Reconsideration of the terminology "ingroup" vs. "outgroup".

6) While we agreed that the addition of causal experiments could help expose which (if any) network is critical for helping behavior, we also agreed that the discovery driven nature of these experiments has high value and that such additional causal experiments are not crucial. Rather we think the revision could be reframed as a discovery-based, exploratory investigation and should include a stronger discussion of the correlational nature of the results, putting these correlational observations within the context of what is already known about the NAc in social reward in the discussion.

Please also include a key resource table, if you have not already done so.

Reviewer #1:

The submission by Ben-Ami Bartal et al., reported the neural correlates of helping behavior in rats. The authors trained rats to release a peer from the same strain (ingroup) or different strain (outgroup). Then the authors performed a brain-wide neural activity analysis, using Fos as a neuronal activity marker. The behavioral results showed that rats helped ingroup, but not outgroup members, by releasing them from a restrainer. The Fos data identified a shared network including frontal and insular cortices, active independently of group membership. Interestingly, the striatum region was selectively active for ingroup rats. The author further identified the nucleus accumbens (NAc) as a central network hub correlated with the releasing a peer behavior. Additionally, in vivo imaging investigation revealed NAc activity when a rat was approaching a trapped ingroup member. Finally, by using a retrograde tracer plus Fos, the authors identified a subpopulation of activated anterior cingulate cortex --> NAc projection correlated with helping behavior in the ingroup rats. Overall, the manuscript provides informative data on the neural correlates of helping behavior providing some inputs for mechanistic investigations of this phenomenon.

The main limitation is that at its current stage, the work is just descriptive and includes only correlational analyses. While the network brain-wide approach is intriguing, it is not clear which network (if any) is critical for helping behavior (relative to the other controls reported here). These are critical experiments for this type of studies. The exclusion of female subjects from the study significantly decreases the impact of the study and its generalizability. It is difficult to reconcile the in vivo data with Fos. For Fos it is difficult to identify the increased activity during the approaching of a trapped ingroup member, just because the timeline of Fos expression is extremely different than the in vivo investigation.

1. The main limitation is that at its current stage, the work is just descriptive and includes only correlational analyses. While the network brain-wide approach is intriguing, it is not clear which network (if any) is critical for helping behavior (relative to the other controls reported here). These are critical experiments for this type of studies.

2. The exclusion of female subjects from the study significantly decreases the impact of the study and its generalizability.

3. It is difficult to reconcile the in vivo data with Fos. For Fos it is difficult to identify the increased activity during the approaching of a trapped ingroup member, just because the timeline of Fos expression is extremely different than the in vivo investigation. The author should consider to either remove the in vivo data or provide a rationale/justification for this technical concern.

4. The retrograde tracing experiment seems to be disconnected from the rest of the paper and, as reported by the authors, under developed. Initially the investigation is about the comparison between ingroup and outgroup (together with the other control groups). Then for the retrograde tracing experiment the authors focused only on the ingroup rats. This is confusing because the retrograde tracing experiment should be useful to confirm the networks identified with the Fos analysis.

5. Page 7 refers to Figure 2G but this figure is missing in the panel.

Reviewer #2:

In their study "neural correlates of ingroup bias for pro-sociality in rats", the authors conduct analyses of IEG induction and calcium imaging during a helping behavior test (HBT) to characterize networks of core brain regions that are functionally responsive to helping behavior towards "ingroup" vs. "outgroup" rats. Rats demonstrate a bias towards helping trapped individuals of the same strain (ingroup) compared to releasing rats of a different strain (outgroup). The results identify a collection of functionally correlated regions that appear to be selectively activated under ingroup, but not outgroup, HBT conditions. Of these regions, the nucleus accumbens arises as a central node whose cFos induction and calcium transients' correlate with approaching a trapped ingroup conspecific. The authors argue that these novel results highlight NAc activity as a neural correlate of ingroup bias and may serve as a substrate mediating basic empathic responses in a lower vertebrate. The authors further suggest that such network activity may reflect evolutionarily conserved mechanisms that give rise to more complex empathic, and at times maladaptively biased, social phenotypes in humans.

The authors sampled an impressive number of brain regions for their cFos analyses, and the convergence with the fiber photometry data provided compelling evidence that the NAc is indeed involved in these complex prosocial behaviors. The findings are novel, if correlative, and the behavioral paradigm used in conjunction with calcium imaging techniques provide a rich opportunity to understand the neural mechanisms governing prosociality, which is of broad interest to the scientific community.

The experimental paradigm provides a rich behavioral context to examine the neural correlates of helping behavior. However, collapsing the behavioral dependent variable into a binary measure of opening vs. not opening, or using just the percentage of rats that opened, ignores the potentially nuanced nature of these prosocial behaviors and their neural substrates. Quantifying what the free rats (and trapped rats) were doing over the course of the HBT would help more rigorously contextualize what the cFos data correspond to at a finer behavioral scale. Such an analysis is also needed to help to evaluate whether the trapped mouse is in distress after 12 days of training (habituation), which is critical to the empathic response explanation for the corresponding neural data. I would caution against the authors' use of ingroup/outgroup terminology as a framework for the experiments as it invokes potentially anthropocentric interpretations and explanations of the rats' behavior. As these terms emerge from the social psychology literature and typically regard cultural processes underlying social identity, there is not enough evidence that parallel cognitive processes are driving the behavioral phenotypes in the rats.

1. After 12 days of training, in which the trapped rat appears to be trapped for at least 20 mins for the ingroup condition, can we safely assume that the paradigm is still distressing for the trapped rat? The empathic response explanation for the corresponding neural data is contingent upon the notion that the rat in the trap is experiencing distress. As the trapped rat becomes more habituated to being trapped, does this pose as a confound for the authors interpretations? Does the trapped rat re-enter the trap after release? This could help characterize whether or not the trap was still negatively valenced.

2. The experimental paradigm provides a rich behavioral context to examine the neural correlates of helping behavior. However, collapsing the behavioral dependent variable into a binary measure of opening vs. not opening, or using just the percentage of rats that opened, ignores the potentially nuanced nature of these prosocial behaviors and their neural substrates. Quantifying what the free rats (and trapped rats) were doing over the course of the HBT would help more rigorously contextualize what the cFos data correspond to at a finer behavioral scale. Are they still trying to open the restrainer throughout? Do they give up part way? The cFos totals and calcium transients relate to a combination of the experimental conditions and the subject's behavior during the HBT. Without extensive behavioral analysis it's not clear what the neural activity truly reflects (i.e. internal motivation to open the previously openable trap? A response to whatever signals are being emitted from the trapped conspecific? Etc. ). One could also correlate or regress number of opening attempts with the cFos counts to see if there's a significant relationship between them.

3. Further, there is very interesting natural variation in the propensity of rats to learn to become "openers". A non-trivial number of free rats did not seem to become openers, but the implications of these outcomes were not discussed. Were the 2/8 rats that failed to become openers in the ingroup condition included in the cFos analyses? Or the 5/13 in the retrograde cFos analyses? Combining these would result in a reasonable sample size for analysis. Incorporating the status of the free rat as an opener or non-opener may be critical to interpreting the neural data.

4. I would caution against the authors' use of ingroup/outgroup terminology as a framework for the experiments as it invokes potentially anthropocentric interpretations and explanations of the rats' behavior. As these terms emerge from the social psychology literature and typically regard cultural processes underlying social identity, there is not enough evidence that parallel cognitive processes are driving the behavioral phenotypes in the rats. The authors included the phrase "social selectivity", which I believe better captures the phenomenon without invoking unnecessary higher order associations. There are other concerns with using ingroup/outgroup to refer to strain and how this may be construed with respect to ethnicity/race.

5. Is the outgroup trapped rat always the same animal? Differences in stimulus ID exposure during training for the HBT may mean cFos responses are tied to stimulus familiarity.

6. Line 382- are these boldness results available? Or the OFT results? And why run a test of exploration anxiety-like behavior in an arena in which the animals have been habituated? Why run these tests and not report the results?

7. Line 139 – The brief condition is used to isolate the neural response of helping vs. exposure to a trapped rat, but the differences in the experiences of the trapped rat (12 days, 1 completely trapped vs. 3 days completely trapped) could also contribute to the variation in neural activity of the free rat that is not easily decomposed into a helping response + noise.

8. Line 155- The baseline condition rats were presumably not handled, they remained in their home cage instead of moving to an arena and were not exposed to a trap. Therefore, it's not entirely clear what experimental components the differential neural responses are related to, and this should be mentioned.

9. Line 252 – It's curious that activity would decrease, given that other studies show significant increases in NAc activity upon reward consumption (water, sucrose, social interaction) and this not what is observed upon acquisition of social reward when opening the restrainer. A short discussion of this discrepancy is warranted.

10. Differences in locomotion could contribute to the differences in NAc Ca2+ activity. The authors partially address this in line 320, but this explanation does not take into account potential differences in velocity upon entry to the area around the restrainer – a particular concern given that differences in velocity are noted in Figure 1: "The measure of entry into the area around the restrainer used in the fiber photometry experiment by definition controls for movement, as it depicts the same movement across conditions as the event of interest. Thus, while motor processing is part of the neural processing displayed by these rats, it is unlikely to be the primary explanation for the differences observed between groups."

11. Ca2+ signals in the NAc commonly exhibit attenuation over time as novelty seems to be a strong driver of activity. This can lead to false interpretations of the data if the behavior is not balanced across groups/condition. Did the animals show equivalent number of approach bouts for in- and outgroup members? If so, were there significant changes in fluorescence across approach bouts?

Reviewer #3:

The manuscript under review, by Bartal et al. titled "Neural Correlates of Ingroup Bias for Pro-Sociality in Rats" provides a comprehensive combination of behavioral, cellular, and circuit-based analysis. Briefly, the authors first show that male rats have an "ingroup" bias for pro-social behavior using their previously introduced helping behavior task (HBT). Specifically, rats preferred to help trapped rats of the same (ingroup), but not different (outgroup), strain. Using this procedure, in conjunction with an exhaustive set of control conditions(!), the authors next performed Fos activity mapping in 45 brain regions spanning bregma +4.2mm to -5.64mm. The statistical approaches for this Fos mapping experiment were Partial Least Square analysis with permutation and boot strapping tests. The data show that HBT conditions are overall elevated in contrast comparted to controls, with the ingroup condition greater than the outgroup condition. Subsequent analysis of only the ingroup vs outgroup conditions compared to base-line non-tested rats shows that there are brain regions in both conditions that are commonly or uniquely activated by either HBT condition. Of specific interest, the MO, PrL, NAc and LS were significantly more active in the ingroup than outgroup condition. These differences were not found in a group of rats trained to retrieve chocolate pellets from the HBT apparatus (although few rats acquired this task). The authors propose this neural correlate therefore is specific to social reward. A second set of analysis were then performed on the same datasets using network graph analysis, which further refined the list of brain regions of interest to the NAc. To determine how the NAc encodes HBT behavior, the authors used non-conditional GCaMP in the NAc in conjunction with fiber photometry, revealing the NAc is most active during the HBT within the ingroup condition. Lastly, the authors use retrograde tracing to show that a sub-population of NAc projecting ACC neurons are activated by pro-social approach.

The authors do an excellent job of highlighting the potential methodological and interpretational caveats of their results within the main text, as well as detail unexpected methodological issues within the methods section. Thank you for this transparency. Below I provide comments regarding these caveats.

1) It appears there is very little behavioral plasticity between the ingroup and outgroup rats. That is, across 2 weeks of repeated testing none of the outgroup rats transition to receiving empathy-like behavior from their partner. From the methods, it is unclear to me what the housing conditions are with the ingroup and outgroup conditions. I think the housing is that ingroup rats are pair-housed with their partner for the duration of the experiment, and outgroup rats are introduced to their (consistently same) partner during the 1 hour test periods? Is this correct or incorrect? Regardless, please clarify the housing conditions for ingroup and outgroup conditions across the full experimental time-line.

2) As a follow-up, since I am not sure of the housing conditions, could the authors speculate on if there is behavioral plasticity for empathy in rats and if they can transition between "ingroup" and "outgroup" conditions? How much exposure does this require, or does it never happen? Since I am unsure of the exact methods, I am curios if this ingroup bias is more a result of the methods (familiarity with a housed partner vs being a rat of another strain). If a Sprague-Dawley was housed with a Long-Evans cage-mate as their ingroup, would the ingroup bias still exist? In rats, is ingroup/outgroup a function of social familiarity or genetic background?

3) This is a herculean fos mapping effort. The authors provide cell count data but gloss over analysis of cell counts or cell density, jumping directly to more complex PLS and bootstrapping. It is unclear to me of any brain regions, as group means, are significantly different in Fos+ cell counts between HBT conditions.

4) I am not an expert in PLS and bootstrapping analysis of fos mapping data, so I cannot speak directly these results. However, I would like to see representative Fos images of the brain regions that were identified by these approaches as a main or supplemental figure, between conditions. I believe these would be the regions referenced in Figure 2F.

6) Similar to point 4, it would be helpful if the authors provided representative Fos images of the key brain regions identified in by graph network theory. Since some of these overlap with the PLS analysis, perhaps a figure could be provided that has the relevant Fos images between conditions from these two analysis?

7) The NAc has a long and prolific literature surroundings its role in various social reward behaviors, most recently highlighted by work in the stress anhedonia and aggression reward fields identifying cell-type specific roles for NAc medium spiny neurons in controlling social reward-related behaviors. The photometry data is very compelling. Can the author speculate on the NAc cell types that are underlying their photometry observations?

8) The NAc has been the focus of intense scrutiny across numerous social behaviors, ranging from stress-related social anhedonia to sexual motivation to aggression motivation. The discussion would notably strengthen by relating the current results within the context of previously identified roles for the NAc in other forms of social motivation behaviors.

Overall, the authors have presented a well-crafted series of experiments that are appropriately controlled.

5) There is no Figure 2G, referenced in line 156.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Neural Correlates of Ingroup Bias for Pro-sociality in Rats" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Kate Wassum (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

The concerns regarding the original submission have been addressed. The revised manuscript provides novel insight regarding the role of the nucleus accumbens and surrounding circuitry in prosociality, and will therefore be of broad interest to the scientific community. The reviewers have a few remaining concerns regarding the Figures that need to be addressed:

1) The representative cFos image shown in Figure 2B should be replaced with a cFos image from the nucleus accumbens, as this is the brain region that was used for recording.

2) To better illustrate the co-labels with cFos in the merged image in Figure 5D, the authors should consider pseudo-coloring the flurogold label.

eLife. 2021 Jul 13;10:e65582. doi: 10.7554/eLife.65582.sa2

Author response


Essential Revisions:

In addition to the reviewers' detailed comments below, the editors and reviewers have agreed that the following essential revisions must be addressed:

1) A more rigorous analysis of behavioral data (as outlined in the reviewers' comments below).

An in-depth analysis of movement pattern of rats along the session days and minutes has been added, in order to better understand the rats’ motivational state during the helping test. The added analyses are detailed below. Broadly, we found that:

While rats were generally more motivated to move and act around a trapped ingroup member than an outgroup member along the 12 days of testing in the helping behavior test, differences in motor activity were not significant on the cFos day itself. Rats did spend more time near the restrainer, suggesting that motivational state rather than velocity was associated with increased c-Fos activity. In the photometry experiment, an analysis of the velocity as rats entered the area around the restrainer found no differences for the ingroup and outgroup sessions, providing further evidence that differences in the recorded calcium signal represent primarily the social context.

2) An enhanced description of the cFos data and calcium transients as they relate to behavior.

Several figures have been added to provide insight into the association between c-Fos levels and movement data, in a new figure (Figure 2 —figure supplement 5 in the current manuscript). An analysis of movement in the photometry experiment has been added both as summary statistics in the results and an additional graph (Figure 4G).

3) Further consideration of the natural variation inherent in the reported behaviors.

A new analysis has been added comparing neural activity in openers and non-openers in the retrograde experiment (see new Figure 6). Additionally, individual variation can be observed across the manuscript in figure 1G-H; Figure 2 —figure supplement 3, 4C-E, and 5.

4) Inclusion of data on cell counts and cell density, as well as representative Fos images that generated those data.

Data on cell counts is provided on the public repository OSF, a visual presentation in Figure 2 —figure supplement 3, and a new figure presenting representative c-Fos images has been added to the supplementary methods (new Figure 2 —figure supplement 1B).

5) Reconsideration of the terminology "ingroup" vs. "outgroup".

A detailed discussion of this concern is provided below, in the detailed response to the reviewers’ comments. In brief, the terms ‘ingroup’ and ‘outgroup’ are commonly used in the sociobiology literature to refer to non-human social groups and to describe behavior between these groups. However, to emphasize the difference between this meaning and the more cultural usage in human social psychology, a clearer definition has been added to the introduction, including the limitation of this terminology in comparison with human behavior. The terms are now introduced in parenthesis, followed by the operational definition.

Furthermore, it’s worth noting that these terms are used in particular based on previous results demonstrating that rats (a) determine pro-social behavior based on group identity rather than individual familiarity and (b) these groups are flexible and determined by social experience (Bartal et al., eLife 2014). In this manuscript, the ‘ingroup’ members included both cage-mates and strangers of the same strain, rendering either the term ‘strain’ or ‘familiarity’ partially accurate for describing the relationship between the free and trapped rat. Thus, despite the obvious differences between rats and humans, we request to be allowed to retain these terms, as they best reflect the effect we observe.

6) While we agreed that the addition of causal experiments could help expose which (if any) network is critical for helping behavior, we also agreed that the discovery driven nature of these experiments has high value and that such additional causal experiments are not crucial. Rather we think the revision could be reframed as a discovery-based, exploratory investigation and should include a stronger discussion of the correlational nature of the results, putting these correlational observations within the context of what is already known about the NAc in social reward in the discussion.

We are in agreement. The abstract has been modified accordingly, and the following text has been added to the introduction:

“Here we employed a discovery-driven approach to compare the brain-wide activation pattern in response to trapped ingroup and outgroup members, following the helping behavior test. This investigation, which aimed at providing a broad and unbiased overview, led to the identification of central hubs specifically active during the ingroup condition, where rats demonstrated prosocial intent.”

Furthermore, the following discussion of the correlational nature of the manuscript has been added to the discussion:

“The results presented above are correlational in nature and descriptive only. Correlation data in and of itself is not sufficient evidence, especially given the limited sample size and possibility of false discoveries. Thus, these data provide a partial picture of the rats’ neural response and should be interpreted with this caveat in mind. It is nonetheless encouraging that the Nac emerged as a key region across several measures. The increase in c-Fos+ cells observed in the ingroup condition was mirrored by the in-vivo calcium signal, and both measures were associated with behavior (helping and approach respectively)…Overall, this manuscript aims to provide a broad overview of the neural circuitry associated with pro-social intent and provides a base for future work that will aim to dissect these circuits and understand their causal contribution to behavior.”

Please also include a key resource table, if you have not already done so.

A key resource table has been added to the methods.

Reviewer #1:

The submission by Ben-Ami Bartal et al., reported the neural correlates of helping behavior in rats. The authors trained rats to release a peer from the same strain (ingroup) or different strain (outgroup). Then the authors performed a brain-wide neural activity analysis, using Fos as a neuronal activity marker. The behavioral results showed that rats helped ingroup, but not outgroup members, by releasing them from a restrainer. The Fos data identified a shared network including frontal and insular cortices, active independently of group membership. Interestingly, the striatum region was selectively active for ingroup rats. The author further identified the nucleus accumbens (NAc) as a central network hub correlated with the releasing a peer behavior. Additionally, in vivo imaging investigation revealed NAc activity when a rat was approaching a trapped ingroup member. Finally, by using a retrograde tracer plus Fos, the authors identified a subpopulation of activated anterior cingulate cortex --> NAc projection correlated with helping behavior in the ingroup rats. Overall, the manuscript provides informative data on the neural correlates of helping behavior providing some inputs for mechanistic investigations of this phenomenon.

The main limitation is that at its current stage, the work is just descriptive and includes only correlational analyses. While the network brain-wide approach is intriguing, it is not clear which network (if any) is critical for helping behavior (relative to the other controls reported here). These are critical experiments for this type of studies. The exclusion of female subjects from the study significantly decreases the impact of the study and its generalizability. It is difficult to reconcile the in vivo data with Fos. For Fos it is difficult to identify the increased activity during the approaching of a trapped ingroup member, just because the timeline of Fos expression is extremely different than the in vivo investigation.

1. The main limitation is that at its current stage, the work is just descriptive and includes only correlational analyses. While the network brain-wide approach is intriguing, it is not clear which network (if any) is critical for helping behavior (relative to the other controls reported here). These are critical experiments for this type of studies.

2. The exclusion of female subjects from the study significantly decreases the impact of the study and its generalizability.

We agree. Experiments with female rats are currently underway. We have added the following text to the discussion:

“It is important to note that as the data presented here represents activity in male rats only, conclusions are limited to males. The collection of data from female rats is currently ongoing and will be important for generalizing these results.”

3. It is difficult to reconcile the in vivo data with Fos. For Fos it is difficult to identify the increased activity during the approaching of a trapped ingroup member, just because the timeline of Fos expression is extremely different than the in vivo investigation. The author should consider to either remove the in vivo data or provide a rationale/justification for this technical concern.

The advantage of using both the c-Fos and photometry data together is to provide converging evidence from two different methods with different limitations. As noted, c-Fos provides low temporal resolution but high spatial resolution, whereas the photometry provides high temporal resolution but low spatial resolution. These two methods are meant to complement one another. cFos is used to contrast the behavioral experience over the entire session between the ingroup and outgroup conditions. Photometry provides a time series signal of activity change along the task, and provides an opportunity to compare the different conditions within subject. The two datasets propose to capture different aspects of the neural response, and provide supporting evidence for the role of the Nac in the helping test.

4. The retrograde tracing experiment seems to be disconnected from the rest of the paper and, as reported by the authors, under developed. Initially the investigation is about the comparison between ingroup and outgroup (together with the other control groups). Then for the retrograde tracing experiment the authors focused only on the ingroup rats. This is confusing because the retrograde tracing experiment should be useful to confirm the networks identified with the Fos analysis.

The rationale behind the retrograde experiment was to test which structural projections may be playing a role in pro-social approach. The network data derived from the c-Fos experiments provide information about the functional connectivity, and here we are investigating structural connectivity. The specific question answered in this experiment was which structural projections into the Nac are also active during the helping test, as determined by c-Fos+ cells. As the other findings suggested the Nac is a central hub in the ingroup network, this region was chosen for this experiment. We agree that adding a condition with the outgroup would be informative. However, as there is no helping in the outgroup, and the Nac is not active according to our findings, this comparison would not necessarily add useful information. This experiment provides a lead for future investigation narrowing in on structural projections that are necessary for helping to occur.

5. Page 7 refers to Figure 2G but this figure is missing in the panel.

Fixed.

Reviewer #2:

In their study "neural correlates of ingroup bias for pro-sociality in rats", the authors conduct analyses of IEG induction and calcium imaging during a helping behavior test (HBT) to characterize networks of core brain regions that are functionally responsive to helping behavior towards "ingroup" vs. "outgroup" rats. Rats demonstrate a bias towards helping trapped individuals of the same strain (ingroup) compared to releasing rats of a different strain (outgroup). The results identify a collection of functionally correlated regions that appear to be selectively activated under ingroup, but not outgroup, HBT conditions. Of these regions, the nucleus accumbens arises as a central node whose cFos induction and calcium transients' correlate with approaching a trapped ingroup conspecific. The authors argue that these novel results highlight NAc activity as a neural correlate of ingroup bias and may serve as a substrate mediating basic empathic responses in a lower vertebrate. The authors further suggest that such network activity may reflect evolutionarily conserved mechanisms that give rise to more complex empathic, and at times maladaptively biased, social phenotypes in humans.

The authors sampled an impressive number of brain regions for their cFos analyses, and the convergence with the fiber photometry data provided compelling evidence that the NAc is indeed involved in these complex prosocial behaviors. The findings are novel, if correlative, and the behavioral paradigm used in conjunction with calcium imaging techniques provide a rich opportunity to understand the neural mechanisms governing prosociality, which is of broad interest to the scientific community.

The experimental paradigm provides a rich behavioral context to examine the neural correlates of helping behavior. However, collapsing the behavioral dependent variable into a binary measure of opening vs. not opening, or using just the percentage of rats that opened, ignores the potentially nuanced nature of these prosocial behaviors and their neural substrates. Quantifying what the free rats (and trapped rats) were doing over the course of the HBT would help more rigorously contextualize what the cFos data correspond to at a finer behavioral scale.

This is right. Perhaps because of the previous emphasis given to behavior in the last publications, the behavioral analysis was somewhat lean in this manuscript. We have now added a thorough analysis of the movement patterns, including a new figure (Figure 2 —figure supplement 4) and new analysis for the photometry movement data. These analyses provide insight about the capacity of a trapped ingroup member to motivate rats to persist in the typical pattern of circling the restrainer containing the trapped rat and attempting to reach the rat inside, or release him, that was previously described in Ben-Ami Bartal, 2011, 2014, 2016.

Such an analysis is also needed to help to evaluate whether the trapped mouse is in distress after 12 days of training (habituation), which is critical to the empathic response explanation for the corresponding neural data.

While this dataset does not inform the question of whether the animals were still stressed at the end of testing, corticosterone levels were tested in a previous manuscript in 50 pairs of rats during the first and last days of the helping test. Then it was found that both the free and trapped rats demonstrated an increase in cort levels compared to baseline and compared to control rats on the first days of testing. In opener pairs this response was abolished by the last day of testing, but in non-opener pairs it remained high, probably since rats remained trapped. (this data is only partially published, in Ben-Ami Bartal et al., Front. Psych, 2016) In the current experiments, it is likely that rats were similarly stressed on the final day. Even opener pairs, as the restrainer was latched, and no one was released. While a direct test of this remains to be done, please consider as well that restraint is a highly validated stressor and has been used for chronic stress paradigms, where classically rats are trapped daily for 2-3 weeks. Habituation likely does not reduce stress in this context. Habituation is effective in reducing stress when animals learn the new situation is not aversive. As rats experience restraint as aversive, they continue to be negatively aroused across testing. The following text has been added to the discussion:

“Undoubtedly, social reward is an important driver of mammalian behavior, and rats experience social reward during this paradigm. However, the helping test is a stressful situation. Restraint is a well validated chronic stressor (Glavin et al., 1994; Ottenweller, Servatius, and Natelson, 1994; Pare and Glavin, 1986; Servatius et al., 2000), and testing induces a stress response in both the free and the trapped rat, including secretion of the stress hormone corticosterone, defecation and freezing(Ben-Ami et al., 2011; Ben-Ami et al., 2016). It is thus important to consider that the behavior of the free rat is influenced by the stress of the trapped rat.”

I would caution against the authors' use of ingroup/outgroup terminology as a framework for the experiments as it invokes potentially anthropocentric interpretations and explanations of the rats' behavior. As these terms emerge from the social psychology literature and typically regard cultural processes underlying social identity, there is not enough evidence that parallel cognitive processes are driving the behavioral phenotypes in the rats.

While we agree that the terms ‘ingroup’ and ‘outgroup’ have been typically used to describe a cultural construct rather than a biological one, we would argue that the meaning of this terms best captures the animals’ behavior. As we have previously demonstrated, rats help others who belong to a certain group (strain) based on their previous social experience with other members of this group. As such, Sprague-Daweley rats fostered at birth with Long-evans litters help only members (even strangers) of the adoptive strain as adults, and do not help strangers of their own strain. Even as adults, after 2 weeks of co-housing with a Long-evans cage-mate, rats help Long-evans strangers (Ben-Ami Bartal et al., eLife 2014). This finding shows that pro-social behavior is determined according to experience with a member of the strain, but not the biological identity of the trapped rat. Thus pro-social behavior is flexible, and determined by the experience with the group, rather than individual familiarity, or biological similarity. This behavior is similar to ingroup bias in humans, who show a preference for ingroup members even if the ingroup is determined by minimal cues. Furthermore, a diverse environment (e.g. in school) has been shown to reduce ingroup bias in humans and increases empathic responding to members of other ethnic groups experiencing pain. As rats will release Long-evans strangers following co-housing with a long-evans cage-mate, it would be inaccurate to use neither ‘strain’ nor ‘familiarity’ as the critical factor determining the behavioral phenotype. Rather, it is a “strain with which the rat is familiar” and, we suspect, has had positive social experience with (has bonded with) a member of the strain. In-group bias is a term used to describe a preference for pro-social behavior among members of the same group, where groups can be determined by innate characteristics such as sex, or formed by the environment. Usage of the term “ingroup bias” or “ingroup favoritism” to describe social behavior in non-human animals is frequent and accepted. Ingroup bias has been widely documented both in humans and non-human animals, from ants to dolphins and birds. The terms ingroup and outgroup are now first presented in parentheses (“ingroup” and “outgroup”) followed with an operational definition. The following has been added to the introduction:

“Importantly, rats demonstrate an ingroup bias, releasing “ingroup members” (rats of the same strain, whether familiar or not), but not “outgroup members” (rats of an unfamiliar strain).”

“The terms “ingroup” and “outgroup”, while often used in regard to cultural processes underlying social identity in humans, are also used for describing socially-selective affiliation in non-human animals (Masuda and Fu, 2015; Nakamura and Masuda, 2012; Robinson and Barker, 2017), and are adopted here, despite likely differences in their neurobiological structure across species.”

1. After 12 days of training, in which the trapped rat appears to be trapped for at least 20 mins for the ingroup condition, can we safely assume that the paradigm is still distressing for the trapped rat? The empathic response explanation for the corresponding neural data is contingent upon the notion that the rat in the trap is experiencing distress. As the trapped rat becomes more habituated to being trapped, does this pose as a confound for the authors interpretations? Does the trapped rat re-enter the trap after release? This could help characterize whether or not the trap was still negatively valenced.

See answer for this as above.

Regarding the point of re-entry into the restrainer, it is a commonly observed behavior. However, it does not provide evidence that being trapped is not aversive. On the contrary, It’s likely the restrainer itself is appealing for being a small protected place. But when trapped inside, it becomes aversive. This is evidenced by attempts to escape, defecations, and occasional breakouts, especially as the experiment progresses. Furthermore, when the door was turned around in previous unpublished experiments, trapped rats quickly released themselves. The analogy might be to sitting comfortably in your room, but if suddenly you were to discover the door was locked from the outside, you’d start feeling uncomfortable and try to find a way out. This does not mean that you find the room itself aversive, it is the situation of being locked inside that is stressful.

2. The experimental paradigm provides a rich behavioral context to examine the neural correlates of helping behavior. However, collapsing the behavioral dependent variable into a binary measure of opening vs. not opening, or using just the percentage of rats that opened, ignores the potentially nuanced nature of these prosocial behaviors and their neural substrates. Quantifying what the free rats (and trapped rats) were doing over the course of the HBT would help more rigorously contextualize what the cFos data correspond to at a finer behavioral scale. Are they still trying to open the restrainer throughout? Do they give up part way? The cFos totals and calcium transients relate to a combination of the experimental conditions and the subject's behavior during the HBT. Without extensive behavioral analysis it's not clear what the neural activity truly reflects (i.e. internal motivation to open the previously openable trap? A response to whatever signals are being emitted from the trapped conspecific? Etc. ). One could also correlate or regress number of opening attempts with the cFos counts to see if there's a significant relationship between them.

We have added an extensive analysis in response to this point. First, an analysis of movement pattern along the 12 testing days of the HBT. This is presented in Figure 1. Next, an analysis of movement on the c-Fos day, which can be associated with the c-Fos levels. This analysis shows the activity levels and location near the restrainer of the free rats, indicating the motivational state was more persistent in the ingroup condition. This is marked by a consistently high amount of time spent near the restrainer on the later days and times in the session by the free rat in the ingroup condition, whereas this approach decreases as time goes by in the outgroup condition. This finding also lines up with previously published data showing an increased ratio of activity in the first and second halves of the session compared to control conditions (Ben-Ami Bartal, 2011). On the c-Fos day, the movement differences were not significant, yet there was a significant correlation between time spent around the restrainer and brain-wide c-Fos levels, indicating that neural activity was higher the more rats remained near the trapped rat. while we did not conduct a manual coding of opening attempts (pretty tricky to do this in a standard, objective way, working on it!), this measure would be a derivative of time near the restrainer, as rats would have to be in the area around the restrainer to attempt door-opening.

3. Further, there is very interesting natural variation in the propensity of rats to learn to become "openers". A non-trivial number of free rats did not seem to become openers, but the implications of these outcomes were not discussed. Were the 2/8 rats that failed to become openers in the ingroup condition included in the cFos analyses? Or the 5/13 in the retrograde cFos analyses? Combining these would result in a reasonable sample size for analysis. Incorporating the status of the free rat as an opener or non-opener may be critical to interpreting the neural data.

In this manuscript, for all original analyses, all animals from the tested condition were included, both openers and non-openers. At your suggestion, a new analysis of openers vs. non-openers has been included (Figure 6). This is based off the 5vs8 comparison in the retrograde experiment. In addition, the two non-openers in the c-Fos experiment are marked in lighter gray in the dot movement graphs in Figure 1.

Historically, 30% of rats tested with ingroup members never become openers. We are very interested in understanding these individual differences. Were the non-openers unable to learn the task? Or perhaps less motivated? From observations, it appears they tend to be of a more anxious profile. As yet, we have not gathered a convincing body of evidence to support either alternative. We are currently working on developing algorithms for a more sophisticated analysis of the rats’ movement data to address these questions. A few differences were identified in this analysis, as detailed in the results, providing interesting leads for future studies with a larger sample. In addition to the new analysis, the following text has been added to the discussion:

“Regarding the motivational state underlying door-opening, consider the following: when rats open the restrainer quickly on consecutive days and become ‘openers’, it is clearly an intentional behavior. However, when rats fail to open the restrainer, it could be explained by either lack of will or lack of ability to open the door. Here, some rats tested with ingroup members did not become ‘openers’. Previous experiments similarly found that while most rats help ingroup members, around 30% of Sprague-Dawley rats don’t become openers. The current dataset does not provide enough information to support any conclusions regarding the motivational state of the ‘non-openers’ to surmise whether they were unable or unwilling to open the restrainer. Analysis presented above suggest the OFC and BLA are potential targets for future investigation of these two behavioral phenotypes. Individual variability of helping behavior is of great interest and needs to be investigated in depth in future studies.”

4. I would caution against the authors' use of ingroup/outgroup terminology as a framework for the experiments as it invokes potentially anthropocentric interpretations and explanations of the rats' behavior. As these terms emerge from the social psychology literature and typically regard cultural processes underlying social identity, there is not enough evidence that parallel cognitive processes are driving the behavioral phenotypes in the rats. The authors included the phrase "social selectivity", which I believe better captures the phenomenon without invoking unnecessary higher order associations. There are other concerns with using ingroup/outgroup to refer to strain and how this may be construed with respect to ethnicity/race.

Please see our response above.

5. Is the outgroup trapped rat always the same animal? Differences in stimulus ID exposure during training for the HBT may mean cFos responses are tied to stimulus familiarity.

This is an excellent question. In the past, I’ve tried both alternating the trapped strangers daily so that the free rat never met the same rat twice (as published in eLife 2014), and keeping the same trapped stranger over the entire HBT (unpublished). In both setups, SD rats did not release the trapped LE, leading us to the conclusion that the 1-hr exposure afforded during the helping test is not sufficient to induce a shift in pro-social motivation in the free SD rat (as published in Bartal, 2014). On the other hand, we know that 2 weeks of cohabitation does cause such a shift. keep in mind that it is the motivation towards the group, and not the individual, which changes, such that rats will help stranger LEs after the co-habitation. This suggests the necessary familiarity is with the strain as a category and not with an individual rat. Future studies will need to pinpoint the exact timeframe and hopefully the neurobiological changes that occur to support this pro-social shift. The following has been added to the methods to clarify this point:

“Housing conditions: rats were housed in pairs, with a cage-mate of the same strain, and were allowed 2 weeks to habituate to their cage-mate prior to any testing. In the c-Fos experiment, ingroup members were SD cage-mates. For the photometry experiments, ingroup members were SD strangers. The trapped outgroup member was a different LE stranger daily.”

“To prevent individual familiarity with the outgroup member, strangers were swapped daily. Note that we previously found rats will release trapped strangers of their own strain, and that familiarity with the strain determines pro-social behavior rather than individual familiarity.”

6. Line 382- are these boldness results available? Or the OFT results? And why run a test of exploration anxiety-like behavior in an arena in which the animals have been habituated? Why run these tests and not report the results?

The boldness and OFT were run because historically that’s the protocol we’ve followed for the HBT. While we did not analyze these results for this experiment, we found it appropriate to report that these procedures were conducted before the start of testing in the HBT. The following has been added to the methods:

“The boldness and open-field testing were conducted in order to remain consistent with previous experiments, but were not analyzed and are thus not reported.”

7. Line 139 – The brief condition is used to isolate the neural response of helping vs. exposure to a trapped rat, but the differences in the experiences of the trapped rat (12 days, 1 completely trapped vs. 3 days completely trapped) could also contribute to the variation in neural activity of the free rat that is not easily decomposed into a helping response + noise.

We agree that the situation is not identical. The reasoning behind this control condition is that rats tested in the brief condition have experienced witnessing a trapped rat, but they don’t have the knowledge that the restrainer can be opened. For them it is merely witnessing a trapped rat. We chose 3 days instead of a single session in order to allow some habituation to the experimental setup. But the c-Fos day reflects for all rats neural activity on that day only, in which rats witness a trapped rat for the entire session. We agree that this control on its own does not purely isolate the response to a trapped animal, as these rats do have different previous experience that influences their response on that day. However, we are contrasting the 12 day condition with the 3 day condition for ingroup vs. outgroup, which does allow a good comparison of the changes between these conditions, and the effect of group membership can thus be dissociated. Another important consideration, note that even in the HBT group rats were usually trapped at least 40 min for several days, before the free rats learned to open the restrainer (and even more so in the HBT outgroup).

8. Line 155- The baseline condition rats were presumably not handled, they remained in their home cage instead of moving to an arena and were not exposed to a trap. Therefore, it's not entirely clear what experimental components the differential neural responses are related to, and this should be mentioned.

Rats in the baseline condition underwent the same habituation as the other conditions. On the day of sacrifice these rats did not undergo behavioral testing. This is now added to the methods. This untested baseline is meant to contrast out unspecific neural activity observed in our c-Fos stains.

Comparing against baseline for both groups tells us also what the exposure to the restrainer/new arena/ experience of the HBT captures above baseline.

9. Line 252 – It's curious that activity would decrease, given that other studies show significant increases in NAc activity upon reward consumption (water, sucrose, social interaction) and this not what is observed upon acquisition of social reward when opening the restrainer. A short discussion of this discrepancy is warranted.

Activity in the Nac has been associated with different aspects of motivated behavior, such as reward consumption, as well as seeking behavior and avoidance of aversive outcome, and relief of pain. The following has been added to the discussion:

“The activation of the Nac observed in this study mirrors a broad literature of the Nac’s role in social reward-related behaviors. The Nac is mainly composed of GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs) that fall into two classes: those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D1 (D1 MSNs) and those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D2 (D2 MSNs). Broadly, these pathways are thought to lead to opposing outcomes, with D1 MSNs and their pathways activated during reward, and D2 MSNs and their pathways activated during aversion (Klawonn and Malenka, 2018) but see (Soares-Cunha et al., 2016). For example, Nac MSNs are associated with stress-induced anhedonia (Bessa et al., 2013), and mice displaying depressive-like behaviors following chronic stress have decreased excitatory input in D1 MSNs and increased excitatory input in D2 MSNs (Francis et al., 2015). Further, increased D1 MSN activity is predictive of animals that are resilient to stress-induced changes in social behavior (Muir et al., 2018). Whereas the experiments presented here do not distinguish Nac cell types, Given the dichotomy of D1 and D2 activity – i.e. that D2 MSNs are typically involved in aversive behaviors, including avoidance (Boschen et al., 2011), and D1 MSNs are typically involved in rewarding behaviors including aggression seeking (Aleyasin et al., 2018; Golden et al., 2019) –we speculate that dopamine receptor D1 MSN cells are involved in prosocial intent. This hypothesis would require further testing and will be an interesting area for future study.”

10. Differences in locomotion could contribute to the differences in NAc Ca2+ activity. The authors partially address this in line 320, but this explanation does not take into account potential differences in velocity upon entry to the area around the restrainer – a particular concern given that differences in velocity are noted in Figure 1: "The measure of entry into the area around the restrainer used in the fiber photometry experiment by definition controls for movement, as it depicts the same movement across conditions as the event of interest. Thus, while motor processing is part of the neural processing displayed by these rats, it is unlikely to be the primary explanation for the differences observed between groups."

This analysis has been added to the manuscript, in Figure 4, comparing the velocity of rats upon entry into the zone around the restrainer. No differences were identified. We also ran the same analysis for the c-Fos movement data with a very similar outcome, which strengthens this finding. We did not include it because the c-Fos measure is not temporally sensitive and this is not a very informative measure for that data set, but it did help reassure us of the photometry data. Additional comparisons of velocity and time around the restrainer also revealed no differences in the ingroup and outgroup session compared from the photometry data. The following text has been added to the results:

“As this measure is defined by a specific movement (entry into the zone around the restrainer), the motor movements associated with all events should be similar, and are not a likely cause for the different neural signals across conditions. In evidence of this idea, velocity at the moment of entry into the restrainer was similar for the outgroup session and the following ingroup session (n=8 per group, Figure 4G), and no differences were found in velocity (5.39±0.28; 5.42±0.34 cm\s), number of entries (19.1±1.6; 19.6±3.1), or time spent in the area around the restrainer (198.9±18.7; 223.7±29.1 s) between the ingroup and outgroup sessions respectively (p>0.05, mean ± SEM).”

11. Ca2+ signals in the NAc commonly exhibit attenuation over time as novelty seems to be a strong driver of activity. This can lead to false interpretations of the data if the behavior is not balanced across groups/condition. Did the animals show equivalent number of approach bouts for in- and outgroup members? If so, were there significant changes in fluorescence across approach bouts?

No difference was found in the mean number of bouts in the analysis of the ingroup vs outgroup session here (see text above).

Reviewer #3:

The manuscript under review, by Bartal et al. titled "Neural Correlates of Ingroup Bias for Pro-Sociality in Rats" provides a comprehensive combination of behavioral, cellular, and circuit-based analysis. Briefly, the authors first show that male rats have an "ingroup" bias for pro-social behavior using their previously introduced helping behavior task (HBT). Specifically, rats preferred to help trapped rats of the same (ingroup), but not different (outgroup), strain. Using this procedure, in conjunction with an exhaustive set of control conditions(!), the authors next performed Fos activity mapping in 45 brain regions spanning bregma +4.2mm to -5.64mm. The statistical approaches for this Fos mapping experiment were Partial Least Square analysis with permutation and boot strapping tests. The data show that HBT conditions are overall elevated in contrast comparted to controls, with the ingroup condition greater than the outgroup condition. Subsequent analysis of only the ingroup vs outgroup conditions compared to base-line non-tested rats shows that there are brain regions in both conditions that are commonly or uniquely activated by either HBT condition. Of specific interest, the MO, PrL, NAc and LS were significantly more active in the ingroup than outgroup condition. These differences were not found in a group of rats trained to retrieve chocolate pellets from the HBT apparatus (although few rats acquired this task). The authors propose this neural correlate therefore is specific to social reward. A second set of analysis were then performed on the same datasets using network graph analysis, which further refined the list of brain regions of interest to the NAc. To determine how the NAc encodes HBT behavior, the authors used non-conditional GCaMP in the NAc in conjunction with fiber photometry, revealing the NAc is most active during the HBT within the ingroup condition. Lastly, the authors use retrograde tracing to show that a sub-population of NAc projecting ACC neurons are activated by pro-social approach.

The authors do an excellent job of highlighting the potential methodological and interpretational caveats of their results within the main text, as well as detail unexpected methodological issues within the methods section. Thank you for this transparency. Below I provide comments regarding these caveats.

1) It appears there is very little behavioral plasticity between the ingroup and outgroup rats. That is, across 2 weeks of repeated testing none of the outgroup rats transition to receiving empathy-like behavior from their partner. From the methods, it is unclear to me what the housing conditions are with the ingroup and outgroup conditions. I think the housing is that ingroup rats are pair-housed with their partner for the duration of the experiment, and outgroup rats are introduced to their (consistently same) partner during the 1 hour test periods? Is this correct or incorrect? Regardless, please clarify the housing conditions for ingroup and outgroup conditions across the full experimental time-line.

The LE stranger was changed out every day. However, in other experiments (not presented here), we’ve kept the LE stranger’s identity constant over the entire HBT, and did not see a difference in helping behavior. In both setups, SD rats did not release the trapped LE, leading us to the conclusion that the 1hr exposure afforded during the helping test is not sufficient to induce a shift in pro-social motivation in the free SD rat (as published in Bartal, 2014). On the other hand, we know that 2 weeks of co-habitation does cause such a shift. keep in mind that it is the motivation towards the group, and not the individual, which changes, such that rats will help stranger LEs after the cohabitation. This suggests the necessary familiarity is with the strain as a category and not with an individual rat. Future studies will need to pinpoint the exact timeframe and hopefully the neurobiological changes that occur to support this pro-social shift. The following has been added to the methods to clarify this point:

“Housing conditions: rats were housed in pairs, with a cage-mate of the same strain, and were allowed 2 weeks to habituate to their cage-mate prior to any testing. In the c-Fos experiment, ingroup members were SD cage-mates. For the photometry experiments, ingroup members were SD strangers. The trapped outgroup member was a different LE stranger daily.”

“To prevent individual familiarity with the outgroup member, strangers were swapped daily. Note that we previously found rats will release trapped strangers of their own strain, and that familiarity with the strain determines pro-social behavior rather than individual familiarity.”

2) As a follow-up, since I am not sure of the housing conditions, could the authors speculate on if there is behavioral plasticity for empathy in rats and if they can transition between "ingroup" and "outgroup" conditions? How much exposure does this require, or does it never happen? Since I am unsure of the exact methods, I am curios if this ingroup bias is more a result of the methods (familiarity with a housed partner vs being a rat of another strain). If a Sprague-Dawley was housed with a Long-Evans cage-mate as their ingroup, would the ingroup bias still exist? In rats, is ingroup/outgroup a function of social familiarity or genetic background?

The question of group membership (how it’s defined, sensed, modulated) is fascinating. We have previously found that 2 weeks of co-housing will promote helping for the LE cage-mate, and even for LE strangers (eLife, 2014). In the same paper, another experiment looked at SD rats fostered at birth with an LE litter, and we found that as adults, these fostered rats helped only strangers of the foster LE strain, not SD strangers. These findings led to the conclusion that pro-social motivation is determined by social experience, rather than the biological identity. In new experiments we will try to identify what neurobiological changes occur over the 2 weeks of co-housing that could account for this difference. It is also our target to understand how group identity is coded and how it modulates activity in the networks associated with empathy and helping.

3) This is a herculean fos mapping effort. The authors provide cell count data but gloss over analysis of cell counts or cell density, jumping directly to more complex PLS and bootstrapping. It is unclear to me of any brain regions, as group means, are significantly different in Fos+ cell counts between HBT conditions.

Thank you! The analysis of a direct comparison of group means is included in the results. Group means were significantly different between HBT conditions. These findings are visualized in Figure 2F and Figure 3, and Supplementary Files 2,3. In Figure 2F, the brain diagrams are based off ANOVAS with the HBT ingroup, HBT outgroup, and baseline used for the (Bonferonni corrected) posthoc comparisons. The four regions identified in Figure 3A are based off a direct comparison of the ingroup and outgroup HBT means. Supplementary File 2 and 3 contain the means, SEMs and CI per condition and brain region.

4) I am not an expert in PLS and bootstrapping analysis of fos mapping data, so I cannot speak directly these results. However, I would like to see representative Fos images of the brain regions that were identified by these approaches as a main or supplemental figure, between conditions. I believe these would be the regions referenced in Figure 2F.

This is now included in Figure 2 —figure supplement 1B. In addition, Figure 2—figure supplement 3 provides a useful visualization of the counts across all test conditions.

6) Similar to point 4, it would be helpful if the authors provided representative Fos images of the key brain regions identified in by graph network theory. Since some of these overlap with the PLS analysis, perhaps a figure could be provided that has the relevant Fos images between conditions from these two analysis?

We included several regions, though not all, in Figure 2 —figure supplement 1B. As there are many interesting areas, it was hard to choose!

7) The NAc has a long and prolific literature surroundings its role in various social reward behaviors, most recently highlighted by work in the stress anhedonia and aggression reward fields identifying cell-type specific roles for NAc medium spiny neurons in controlling social reward-related behaviors. The photometry data is very compelling. Can the author speculate on the NAc cell types that are underlying their photometry observations?

The following has been added to the discussion:

“The activation of the Nac observed in this study mirrors a broad literature of the Nac’s role in social reward-related behaviors. The Nac is mainly composed of GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs) that fall into two classes: those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D1 (D1 MSNs) and those expressing primarily dopamine receptor D2 (D2 MSNs). Broadly, these pathways are thought to lead to opposing outcomes, with D1 MSNs and their pathways activated during reward, and D2 MSNs and their pathways activated during aversion (Klawonn and Malenka, 2018) but see (Soares-Cunha et al., 2016). For example, Nac MSNs are associated with stress-induced anhedonia (Bessa et al., 2013), and mice displaying depressive-like behaviors following chronic stress have decreased excitatory input in D1 MSNs and increased excitatory input in D2 MSNs (Francis et al., 2015). Further, increased D1 MSN activity is predictive of animals that are resilient to stress-induced changes in social behavior (Muir et al., 2018). Whereas the experiments presented here do not distinguish Nac cell types, Given the dichotomy of D1 and D2 activity – i.e. that D2 MSNs are typically involved in aversive behaviors, including avoidance (Boschen et al., 2011), and D1 MSNs are typically involved in rewarding behaviors including aggression seeking (Aleyasin et al., 2018; Golden et al., 2019) – we speculate that dopamine receptor D1 MSN cells are involved in prosocial intent. This hypothesis would require further testing and will be an interesting area for future study.”

8) The NAc has been the focus of intense scrutiny across numerous social behaviors, ranging from stress-related social anhedonia to sexual motivation to aggression motivation. The discussion would notably strengthen by relating the current results within the context of previously identified roles for the NAc in other forms of social motivation behaviors.

We hope the new paragraph in the discussion (quoted above) provides a resolution of this concern.

5) There is no Figure 2G, referenced in line 156.

This typo has been fixed, thank you!

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

The concerns regarding the original submission have been addressed. The revised manuscript provides novel insight regarding the role of the nucleus accumbens and surrounding circuitry in prosociality, and will therefore be of broad interest to the scientific community. The reviewers have a few remaining concerns regarding the Figures that need to be addressed:

1) The representative cFos image shown in Figure 2B should be replaced with a cFos image from the nucleus accumbens, as this is the brain region that was used for recording.

2) To better illustrate the co-labels with cFos in the merged image in Figure 5D, the authors should consider pseudo-coloring the flurogold label.

We have modified the two figures as requested.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Bartal B-AI. 2021. Neural correlates of ingroup bias for pro-sociality in rats. Open Science Framework. osf.io/5qtcx/

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Detailed list of brain regions used in the figures.
    elife-65582-supp1.docx (13.3KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 2. Means and confidence intervals for brain-wide c-Fos numbers across conditions.
    elife-65582-supp2.docx (23.7KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 3. Means and confidence intervals for c-Fos numbers per brain region.
    elife-65582-supp3.docx (611.9KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 4. Set of parameters used in a series of multinomial regression tests.

    Combinations of clustering algorithms, parameters, and subsets of data used in Figure 3D. ‘Cluster group’ was used to obtain the cluster assignments for each brain region. ‘Reference group’ was the control group in the multinomial logistic regression. The ‘weighted’ column indicates whether weights (correlations) between brain regions were taken into account while performing clustering. ‘All’: all groups; ‘rest’: all conditions except ingroup and outgroup; ‘baseline’: untested baseline condition. The thresholds of percentile rank, p-value, and absolute r value in the parameter column were applied to the covariance matrix of the c-Fos data in the cluster group before feeding it into the selected clustering algorithm.

    elife-65582-supp4.docx (30.1KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Data of c-Fos numbers, calcium signal and movement are uploaded on the Open Science Framework depository (https://osf.io/5qtcx/).

    The following dataset was generated:

    Bartal B-AI. 2021. Neural correlates of ingroup bias for pro-sociality in rats. Open Science Framework. osf.io/5qtcx/


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES