Abstract
Background:
Spanking is associated with detrimental outcomes for young children. Research shows that spanking is more commonly used in low-income households.
Objective:
To examine whether economic hardship, measured by household income-to-poverty ratio at the time of the child’s birth, moderated the longitudinal associations between maternal spanking and child externalizing behavior problems during the first nine years of childhood.
Participants and setting:
Mother-child pairs (N = 4,149) from a cohort study of urban families in 20 US cities.
Methods:
Cross-lagged path models examined associations between maternal spanking and externalizing behavior when children were between the ages of 1 and 9. Multigroup analyses examined whether income-to-poverty ratio moderated these associations.
Results:
Bivariate analyses showed that income-to-poverty ratio was associated with child externalizing behavior problems at each time point; income-to-poverty ratio was associated with maternal spanking at age 3 only. Longitudinal path model results indicated that, for low- and middle-income groups, maternal spanking at each age had significant associations with child externalizing behavior at each subsequent age. For the high-income group, maternal spanking at age 1 and age 3 had significant associations with child externalizing behavior at each subsequent age; however, spanking at age 5 was not associated with child externalizing behavior at age 9.
Conclusions:
Spanking is disadvantageous for children at all income levels, with more persistent effects in low- and middle-income families. For higher-income families, the associations of maternal spanking with child externalizing behavior problems may be attenuated as child age increases. Regardless of income level, parents should be advised against spanking.
Keywords: Physical punishment, Spanking, Corporal punishment, Discipline, Child behavior problems, Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, Poverty, Mother child relationship
1. Introduction
Spanking, also known as physical punishment, physical discipline, or corporal punishment, is defined as the use of physical force “with the intention of causing a child to experience pain, but not injury, for the purpose of correcting or controlling the child’s behavior” (Straus & Donnelly, 2005, p. 3). A recent study indicated that longitudinal trends in parental endorsement and use of spanking have declined in the last 30 years (Ryan, Kalil, Ziol-Guest, & Padilla, 2016). Even so, the majority of U.S. parents of young children still use physical punishment (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a, 2016b). Nationally representative data shows that the rates of spanking of young children (ages 2–4) have largely remained unchanged over the last 2 decades, declining 11% from 1995 to 2014 (Finkelhor, Turner, Wormuth, Vanderminden, & Hamby, 2019). Specifically, in 1995, approximately 76% of children ages 2–4 were spanked; in 2014, approximately 65% of children ages 2–4 were spanked (Finkelhor et al., 2019).
Declines in the use of spanking are not uniform across socioeconomic groups. There have been significantly greater declines in attitudes endorsing physical punishment among middle-income parents compared to lower-income parents. In 1988, 46% of middle-income parents and 50% of lower-income parents endorsed use of physical punishment. By 2011, 21% of middle-income parents and 30% of lower-income parents endorsed the practice (Ryan et al., 2016). While research suggests that low income, economic hardship, and the use or endorsement of spanking are correlated (e.g., Berger, 2004; Giles-Sims, Straus, & Sugerman, 1995; Wissow, 2001), few studies have examined whether the effects of spanking on child behavioral outcomes may vary as a function of economic hardship. The current study examines whether household economic hardship, measured by household income-to-poverty ratio at the time of the child’s birth, moderates the associations between maternal spanking and child externalizing behavior problems in the first nine years of life. In this study, we focus on maternal spanking because prior research has shown that mothers use spanking at higher rates than fathers (Kim, Lee, Taylor, & Guterman, 2014). In addition, longitudinal path model analysis of mothers’ and fathers’ spanking showed that, whereas mothers’ spanking was associated with elevated child externalizing behavior problems over time, fathers’ spanking did not show longitudinal associations with elevated levels of child behavior problems over the same time period (Lee, Altschul, & Gershoff, 2015).
1.1. Association between maternal spanking and economic hardship
Researchers have long posited that low income, poverty, and economic hardship are associated with harsh parenting practices, including higher levels of spanking and physical punishment. Family stress theory (Boss, 1987) suggests that fluctuations in income (e.g., moving from middle or high income to low income, or experiencing unanticipated economic strain) contribute negatively to parental psychological functioning (e.g., higher levels of parenting stress and depression), thus increasing risk for harsh parenting (Conger, Rueter, & Conger, 2000, 2002). Similar mechanisms may be at play for low-income families who experience chronic levels of economic distress (McLoyd, 1998). Families living in chronic conditions of poverty and economic strain are more likely to experience high levels of parenting stress and depression, which in turn are detrimental to parenting processes (Berger, 2004).
The basic supposition that family economic deprivation is associated with parental physical punishment is largely - but not uniformly - supported by empirical evidence. Studies show that low income and economic hardship are correlated with parents’ greater use of physical punishment (e.g., Berger, 2004; Berlin et al., 2009; Giles-Sims et al., 1995; Lee, Brooks-Gunn et al., 2013; Linver, Brooks-Gunn, & Kohen, 2002; Straus & Stewart, 1999; Wissow, 2001). For example, one study using data from the Early Head Start National Research and Evaluation Project found that lower family income predicted more frequent maternal spanking in early childhood (Berlin et al., 2009). Another study that used data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth found that higher levels of family income were associated with a decrease in the likelihood of children being spanked four or more times per week (Berger, 2004). However, some studies do not report a relation between family economic deprivation and physical punishment (Socolar, Savage, Keyes-Elstein, & Evans, 2005; Zolotor, Robinson, Runyan, Barr, & Murphy, 2011), suggesting the need for further research.
Ryan et al. (2016) provide compelling evidence for the correlation between income, parental socioeconomic status, and spanking in analysis of trends over four decades using nationally representative data. At all four time points, when compared to middle- and upper-income mothers, lower-income mothers were more likely to endorse the use of spanking and also more likely to report that they used spanking recently (Ryan et al., 2016). Beginning in 1997, mothers in the lowest 10th percentile of income were at least twice as likely to report spanking their child in the last week when compared to mothers in the top 10th percentile of income. One explanation for this correlation is that the stresses associated with parenting in a low-income context contribute to spanking (Masarik & Conger, 2017). For example, higher-income mothers may have childcare and other resources that decrease parental stress, thus decreasing the likelihood of spanking. In sum, although the association between economic deprivation and spanking cannot be viewed as causal, there is evidence from a number of large representative data sets that low income is associated with spanking.
1.2. Using income-to-poverty ratio as an indicator of economic deprivation
A wide variety of measures have been used in family research to assess household economic deprivation, with no widely accepted “gold standard” measure. Commonly used measures include annual household income, maternal socioeconomic status, the federal poverty threshold, indices capturing receipt of benefits, or some combination of those measures. In the current study, we used the income-to-poverty ratio as a measure of economic hardship for several reasons. The income-to-poverty ratio indicates how far above or below a family is from the poverty threshold (McLoyd, 1998). As a point of reference, in 2019 the federal poverty threshold for a family of three (e.g., single mother with two children) was $21,330; for a family of four (e.g., two parents and two children), it was $25,750 (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2019). An income-to-poverty ratio of 1.0 (or 100%) conveys that a family’s income is equal to the poverty threshold for their particular family size and composition. The income‐to‐poverty ratio is a useful representation of family economic hardship, as this ratio indicates how far above or below the federal poverty line a family is, based on household income and accounting for family size (Dearing, McCartney, & Taylor, 2001; Mayer & Jenks, 1989). In addition, because of the way the income-to-poverty ratio is calculated, sources of household income (not including capital gains, noncash benefits, and tax credits) as well as the number of adults and children in the household are accounted for in analyses. Another advantage of using a measure that references the federal poverty guidelines is that the federal poverty guidelines serve as the benchmark on which benefits eligibility is determined for many programs in the U.S. Finally, prior studies of spanking (Berlin et al., 2009) and studies that have used the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) to examine issues related to family income and wellbeing (Gottlieb, Pilkauskas, & Garfinkel, 2014) have elected to use the income-to-poverty ratio, viewing it as a measure of overall economic deprivation.
1.3. The current study
A recent meta-analysis established that parental physical punishment is linked to child externalizing behavior problems and higher levels of child aggression (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016b). Building on this, our primary research question was whether the associations between maternal spanking and child externalizing behavior vary by household economic hardship. One hypothesis is that children in low-income households may be more likely to exhibit negative consequences related to maternal physical punishment, in that parents and children have fewer resources to protect them from the effects of physical punishment. From this perspective, it is possible to imagine that spanking compounds other risks that children face in low-income contexts that are also associated with heightened levels of externalizing behavior problems (Evans, 2004). However, a second hypothesis is that spanking may be less problematic in poorer families, in part because spanking is more normative in those contexts (see similar arguments as presented in prior research, e.g., Benjet & Kazdin, 2003). Finally, one could imagine that spanking is equally detrimental in poor and non-poor families because spanking strains the parent-child relationship in a manner that contributes to greater risk of child externalizing behavior problems, independent of other factors. This view would be consistent with prior research examining moderating effects of the longitudinal associations between maternal spanking and child behavior problems. These studies have shown that race and ethnicity (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a), maternal warmth (Lee, Altschul, & Gershoff, 2013), and maternal-child attachment relationship (Ward, Lee, Pace, Grogan-Kaylor, & Ma, 2020) do not moderate the association of maternal spanking with child behavior problems.
In the current study, we tested whether economic hardship, measured by the household income-to-poverty ratio, moderated the associations between maternal spanking and child externalizing behavior during the first nine years of life. We focused on the first nine years of life for several reasons. It is well established that parental spanking often begins at early ages, with approximately one-third of parents in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) using physical punishment around the time their child is 1 year of age. Parental spanking is used most frequently during the toddler and preschool years (e.g., approximately ages 3–5) (Finkelhor et al., 2019), with FFCWS data showing that 56% of 3-year-olds and 51% of 5-year-olds were spanked in the past month (Maguire-Jack, Gromoske, & Berger, 2012). By including data of associations up to age 9, we were able to examine the longitudinal associations of early experiences of physical punishment and behavior problems into middle childhood. We controlled for key variables that are associated with maternal use of spanking and child externalizing behavior problems such as race and ethnicity (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a), maternal depression (Berlin et al., 2009; Chung, McCollum, Elo, Lee, & Culhane, 2004), and maternal education level (Ryan et al., 2016). We conducted analyses to assess whether the study results were robust to inclusion of a variable assessing neighborhood poverty context, as well as robust to a variable assessing family poverty category mobility during the waves of the study.
2. Method
2.1. Data and participants
This study used data from mothers who participated in FFCWS core interviews and supplemental In-Home studies. The FFCWS is a birth-cohort study that sampled children born between 1998–2000 to mothers in 20 U.S. cities with populations over 200,000. Unmarried mothers were purposively oversampled. Respondents were recruited at hospitals at the time of their child’s birth. Interviews were conducted either over the phone or in person. Verbal and/or written informed consent was obtained from participants at each interview, and respondents were informed of the interviewers’ obligation to report observations of child abuse. The compensation participants received varied across multiple waves of data collection. A detailed description of the sampling strategy, and participant compensation is detailed in Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel, and McLanahan (2001). Core FFCWS interviews were conducted with parents at baseline, shortly after the focal child’s birth, and at 1 year, 3 years, 5 years, and 9 years following the focal child’s birth. Families who completed the core interviews when children were age 3 were invited to participate in an add-on study called the In-Home Longitudinal Study of Pre-School Aged Children, which collected child behavioral assessments via a primary care-giver survey, who was usually the child’s mother (age 3, n = 3,288; age 5, n = 2,989; age 9, n = 3,630). To generate the analytic sample for this study, we began with the full sample (N = 4,898 children), then dropped 109 cases who were part of FFCWS through the Time, Love, and Cash in Couples with Children Study. These cases were not selected at random and were not eligible to participate at year 9. We also dropped 640 observations with no data on externalizing behavior at any wave between age 3 and age 9. Our final analytic sample was 4,149.
2.2. Study participants
See Table 1 for all sample descriptive statistics. Approximately half of the mothers indicated their race or ethnicity was non-Hispanic Black (49%), roughly one quarter of the sample reported being Hispanic (27%), nearly one quarter reported being non-Hispanic White (21%), and 3% reported being of another race. Most mothers (77%) were not married to their child’s biological father at the time of the birth. Relatively few mothers had a college degree or higher (10%); one third had less than a high school degree (34%), followed by a high school degree or equivalent (31%) and some college or technical school (24%). At the three-year core interview, focal children were on average 35.7 months old (SD = 2.5 months). At the five-year core interview, focal children were on average 61.8 months old (SD = 2.8 months). At the nine-year core interview, focal children were on average 112.6 months old (SD = 4.5 months).
Table 1.
Total Sample | Poverty Group | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(N = 4,149) | 0–99% (n = 1,510) | 100–199% (n = 1,100) | 200+% (n = 1,539) | ||
Maternal spanking, age 1, % | .278 | ||||
0 times in the past month | 72.39 | 70.57 | 72.45 | 74.09 | |
Once or twice or a few times in the past month | 21.28 | 23.04 | 21.18 | 19.66 | |
Few times a week or every day in the past month | 6.33 | 6.39 | 6.37 | 6.25 | |
Maternal spanking, age 3, % | .001 | ||||
0 times in the past month | 46.80 | 50.11 | 48.24 | 42.66 | |
Once or twice or a few times in the past month | 41.49 | 39.19 | 39.12 | 45.34 | |
Few times a week or every day in the past month | 11.71 | 10.70 | 12.65 | 12.00 | |
Maternal spanking, age 5, % | .130 | ||||
0 times in the past month | 53.01 | 54.57 | 52.55 | 51.82 | |
1–3 times in the past month | 41.69 | 39.28 | 42.74 | 43.28 | |
Few times or every day in the past month | 5.31 | 6.16 | 4.70 | 4.90 | |
Maternal spanking, age 9, % | .234 | ||||
0 times in the past year | 52.32 | 50.05 | 51.77 | 54.77 | |
Low frequency spanking in the past year | 43.94 | 45.95 | 44.20 | 41.94 | |
High frequency spanking in the past year | 3.74 | 4.01 | 4.03 | 3.29 | |
Child externalizing behavior, age 3 | 0.65 | 0.70 | 0.68 | 0.58 | <.001 |
Child externalizing behavior, age 5 | 0.53 | 0.58 | 0.55 | 0.48 | <.001 |
Child externalizing behavior, age 9 | 0.24 | 0.28 | 0.26 | 0.21 | <.001 |
Maternal race or ethnicity, % | |||||
White | 20.85 | 7.91 | 16.39 | 36.67 | <.001 |
Black | 48.67 | 60.37 | 48.91 | 37.06 | <.001 |
Hispanic | 27.03 | 29.65 | 31.60 | 21.20 | <.001 |
Other | 3.45 | 2.06 | 3.10 | 5.07 | <.001 |
Maternal relationship status with biological father, baseline, % | |||||
Married | 23.43 | 7.68 | 17.09 | 43.40 | <.001 |
Cohabitating | 36.62 | 38.24 | 39.64 | 32.88 | <.001 |
Not married or cohabitating | 39.95 | 54.08 | 43.27 | 23.72 | <.001 |
Maternal age, baseline | 25.09 | 23.88 | 24.19 | 26.91 | <.001 |
Maternal education, baseline, % | |||||
Less than high school | 34.48 | 52.92 | 35.97 | 15.33 | <.001 |
High school | 31.22 | 33.29 | 35.88 | 25.86 | <.001 |
Some college | 24.20 | 13.20 | 25.50 | 34.05 | <.001 |
College graduate | 10.11 | 0.60 | 2.64 | 24.76 | <.001 |
Maternal depression, age 1, % | <.001 | ||||
Yes | 15.54 | 17.05 | 17.83 | 12.48 | |
No | 84.46 | 82.95 | 82.17 | 87.52 | |
Child sex, % | .930 | ||||
Male | 51.96 | 51.79 | 52.45 | 51.79 | |
Female | 48.04 | 48.21 | 47.55 | 48.21 | |
Child temperament, age 1 | 1.84 | 1.95 | 1.89 | 1.69 | <.001 |
Results are presented as means, unless otherwise specified. Child externalizing behavior ranged from 0 to 2. Maternal age ranged from 15 to 43 years. Child temperament ranged from 0 to 4. Chi-square tests were used for categorical variables. ANOVAs were used for continuous variables, and Bonferroni corrections were used to adjust for multiple testing.
3. Measures
3.1. Income-to-poverty ratio
FFCWS staff constructed the income-to-poverty ratio, which is defined as the ratio of total household income reported by the mother to the U.S. Census Bureau’s poverty thresholds for the size of the household (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2019). We categorized families into 3 groups: “low-income” with a 0–99% poverty threshold (median = $7500; range = $0–$35,649), “middle-income” with a 100–199% poverty threshold (median = $22,500; range = $10,759–$67,590), and “high-income” including all families with a 200% or higher poverty threshold (median = $42,500; range = $22,009–$133,750). A lower income-to-poverty ratio is indicative of having lower income for a household’s needs, and is synonymous with poverty status. Families in the 0–99% poverty group are considered to be in poverty by the U.S. federal government. Because the experience of hardship in early childhood sets up the long-term trajectory for child wellbeing (Duncan, Yeung, Brooks-Gunn, & Smith, 1998; Duncan, Ziol-Guest, & Kalil, 2010), we focus on the income-to-poverty ratio at the time of the child’s birth. We use the income-to-poverty ratio at subsequent waves as part of robustness checks.
3.2. Maternal spanking
When the focal child was age 1, age 3, and age 5, mothers responded to two questions: “Sometimes children behave pretty well and sometimes they don’t. In the past month, have you spanked (child) because (he/she) was misbehaving or acting up?” (1 = no, 2 = yes). If the mother reported spanking the child in the past month, the parent was then asked, “Did you do this… 1 = every day or nearly every day, 2 = a few times a week, 3 = a few times this past month, or 4 = only once or twice)?” Consistent with prior FFCWS studies, mother’s responses to these two questions were combined to create an ordinal variable of spanking (0 = never in the past month, 1 = only once or twice or a few times this past month, 2 = a few times a week or every day or nearly every day) (Altschul, Lee, & Gershoff, 2016; Lee et al., 2015; Lee, Altschul et al., 2013; Ma, 2016; Ma, Grogan-Kaylor, & Klein, 2018; Taylor, Manganello, Lee, & Rice, 2010).
At year 9, the spanking data were found in the Conflict Tactics Scale, which had different response categories than previous waves with spanking data. Parents were asked how many times in the past year they had spanked the child on the bottom with a bare hand. The response categories were once, twice, 3–5 times, 6–10 times, 11–20 times, more than 20 times, yes but not in past year, and this has never happened. We created an ordinal variable (0 = no spanking in the past year, 1 = low frequency spanking defined as 1–10 times in the past year, and 2 = high frequency spanking defined as 11 or more times in the past year).
3.3. Child externalizing behavior problems
Externalizing behavior problems were measured during the In-Home survey with the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). The CBCL/2–3 was administered to primary caregivers when the child was age 3 (15 items; α = .86). Sample items include: “(He/she) is defiant” and “(He/she) gets in many fights.” The CBCL/4–18 was administered to primary caregivers when the child was age 5 (20 items; α = .84). Sample items include: “Child is cruel, bullies, or shows meanness to others”, “Child destroys his/her own things”, and “Child physically attacks people.” The CBCL/6–18 was administered to primary caregivers when the child was age 9 (18 items; α = .89). Sample items include: “Child is cruel, bullies, or shows meanness to others”, “Child has temper tantrums or a hot temper”, and “Child physically attacks people.” Response categories were consistent across waves (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true) and items were averaged with higher values indicating higher child externalizing behavior problems.
Studies have shown that behavior problems in childhood have significant stability over time (e.g., Gray, Indurkhya, & McCormick, 2004), and the CBCL items were slightly modified to reflect developmental changes in childhood. The CBCL items and procedures are outlined in the FFCWS user’s guide for year 3 data (Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, 2018a, pg. 65), year 5 data (Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, 2019, pg. 61), and year 9 data (Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, 2018b, pg. 47).
3.4. Child characteristics
Child temperament at age 1 was used as an early proxy for whether mothers found the child’s behavior difficult and was assessed with the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability (EAS) Temperament Survey for Children (Mathiesen & Tambs, 1999). Mothers indicated (1 = not at all like my child to 5 = very much like my child) the extent to which their child “often fusses and cries,” “gets upset easily,” and “reacts strongly when upset” (α = .60). Mothers also reported child sex at baseline (indicated by 0 = girl, 1 = boy).
3.5. Maternal and family characteristics
The following demographic control variables were assessed at the time of the child’s birth: maternal age, maternal education level (less than high school, high school degree or GED, some college/technical school, college degree or higher), maternal race and ethnicity (non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, other race/ethnicity), and maternal relationship status with child’s father at baseline (married, cohabiting, not married or cohabiting). We controlled for a binary indicator for maternal depression (liberal caseness) when the child was age one (Kessler, Andrews, Mroczek, Ustun, & Wittchen, 1998).
4. Analysis plan
Stata 15.1 was used for descriptive statistics. Path modeling was conducted in Mplus 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017,Muthén and Muthén, 1998,Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017) using maximum likelihood estimation with the robust standard errors estimator (MLR). A path model was preferred over a structural equation model with latent variable modeling because each CBCL outcome measure (at 3 time points) has between 15–20 items. Although the study sample size was relatively large, we would not have sufficient sample size or corresponding degrees of freedom to be able to model the CBCL as a latent variable at each time point. Further, many other spanking studies to date utilize the mean CBCL score in an autoregressive cross-lagged path analysis to examine the effects of spanking on externalizing behavior (e.g., Gromoske & Maguire-Jack, 2012; Maguire-Jack et al., 2012). For these reasons, we utilized a path model approach.
We compared model fit between constrained and unconstrained models. In order to compare models across groups, we used the Satorra-Bentler Scaled χ2 test (Satorra & Bentler, 2010). The comparative fit index (CFI) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were used to evaluate fit between the hypothesized models and observed data, with values of .95 for CFI and .06 for RMSEA suggesting good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The cluster option within Mplus was used to account for the city-based sampling design of the FFCWS.
Forty-six percent of the analytic sample had no missing data. Missing data occurred when participants were not in that particular wave of data collection, or the participant refused to answer the question, or the participant indicated that they did not know the answer to the question. Mothers’ spanking variables were non-missing for 63.8% of cases, missing at one wave for 27.0% of cases, and missing at 2 or more waves for 9.3% of cases. Externalizing behavior problems was non-missing for 48.9% of cases, missing at one wave for 28.4% of cases, and missing at two or more waves for 22.7% of cases. There were more missing data for child behavior problems because they were drawn from the In-Home studies, which was not administered to all families at every wave. Across our control variables, only 6.73% of cases were missing on one or more control variables, primarily maternal depression.
In order to maximize sample size and to avoid biasing the sample by removing all cases with any missing data, we used full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) in Mplus. Rather than dropping all cases that have missing data on any variable that is included in the model (as occurs in Listwise deletion), FIML utilizes all available information about each case to be incorporated in the estimation of the model. Simulation studies that compare FIML to numerous other methods of handling missing data in the context of longitudinal and multilevel modeling – including listwise deletion, pairwise deletion, similar response pattern imputation, stochastic regression imputation, multiple imputation, and expectation-maximization imputation algorithm – suggest that FIML is superior to these analytic approaches to handling missing data (Enders & Bandalos, 2001; Larsen, 2011; Lee, Harring, & Stapleton, 2019; Newman, 2003). Furthermore, FIML is a common strategy to account for missing data in FFWCS (e.g., Carlson, McLanahan, & Brooks-Gunn, 2008; Gard, McLoyd, Mitchell, & Hyde, 2020, In Press; McLeod, Johnson, Cryer-Coupet, & Mincy, 2019; Meadows, McLanahan, & Knab, 2009; Waller et al., 2019).
We conducted two sets of robustness checks. First, we added a control variable of neighborhood poverty, e.g., the census tract measure for percent of families below the poverty threshold, to examine whether our results focusing on proximal level indicators of poverty (family income-to-poverty ratio) were robust to the inclusion of an indicator assessing neighborhood-level poverty. Second, we assessed whether results were robust to mobility in family poverty status. In other words, since our moderator of family income-to-poverty ratio was assessed at the time of the child’s birth, we created a variable that captured whether the family income-to-poverty ratio changed at another wave in the study (e.g., when the child was age 1, 3, 5 or 9). We conducted an additional set of analyses that included this control variable for whether family’s poverty category differed from baseline (0 = no mobility; 1 = mobility).
5. Results
5.1. Consistency in income-to-poverty ratio over time
We examined the consistency of families’ income-to-poverty ratio category over time and found that 75% of families’ (n = 3,127) modal poverty category over time was the same as their poverty category at baseline. Also, 30% (n = 1,227) of our sample had the same income-to-poverty ratio category at each wave. For the remainder of the sample with a later poverty category that differed from baseline, 22% (n = 897) had one wave that differed, 20% (n = 821) had two, 19% (n = 771) had three, and 10% (n = 423) had all four. These results show the poverty categories were relatively consistent across waves of the study. Thus, we use the baseline income-to-poverty ratio category, measured at the time of the child’s birth, as the study moderator variable, to ensure that the moderator was employed in a prospective fashion, that is, as occurring before the timing of the key independent variable.
5.2. Child externalizing behavior and maternal spanking by income-to-poverty ratio
The use of spanking varied by child age. About 28% of mothers indicated they spanked their 1-year-old child at least once in the past month. The use of spanking increased to 53% at age 3, then decreased slightly to 47% at age 5. At age 9, slightly more than 48% of mothers indicated they had spanked the child at least once within the past year (Table 1). Rates of spanking across income groups did not differ at age 1, 5, or 9. However, at age 3, there were group differences (χ2(4) = 19.44, p = < 0.001). The proportion of mothers in the low-frequency spanking (i.e., only once or twice or a few times this past month) category appears to be driving this difference, with low-frequency spanking being more common among high-income mothers (45%) compared to low- (39%) and middle-income mothers (29%). Overall levels of child externalizing behavior were 0.65 at year 3, 0.53 at year 5, and 0.24 at year 9 (on a scale ranging from 0 to 2). As seen in Table 1, child externalizing behavior at all ages was highest in the more impoverished groups.
5.3. Structural invariance results
We began by comparing the model fit of 3 sets of constrained and unconstrained models: low-income group 0–99% compared to middle-income group 100–199%; low-income group 0–99% compared to high-income group 200+%; and middle-income group 100–199% compared to high-income group 200+%. Constrained models set the structural paths in each group to be equal, while the unconstrained models allowed structural paths in each group to be estimated freely. If the unconstrained model fits better than the constrained model, then the associations between spanking and externalizing behavior differ across groups. If the unconstrained model does not fit better than the constrained model, then the associations between spanking and externalizing behavior are statistically indistinguishable.
Across the 3 tests, the unconstrained model only fit the data better when comparing the low- and high-income groups (i.e., 0–99% and 200+%) (Satorra-Bentler χ2△(20) = 35.01, p < .05). The unconstrained model did not fit the data better when comparing the low-income and middle-income groups (i.e., 0–99% and 100–199%) (Satorra-Bentler χ2△(20) = 19.16, p > .05) or the middle-income and high-income groups (i.e., 100–199% and 200+%) (Satorra-Bentler χ2△(20) = 17.96, p > .05). This means that the associations between spanking and child externalizing behavior were statistically indistinguishable in the low- and middle-income groups; however, the associations between spanking and child externalizing behavior were different when comparing low-income and high-income groups. Therefore, we present one figure where the low- and middle-income groups’ paths were constrained to be equal, and the high-income group’s paths were unconstrained.
5.4. Path model results
Fig. 1 shows the results from the multiple-group autoregressive cross-lagged model. Model fit was acceptable (RMSEA = 0.012; CFI = 0.99). Across the 3 groups, most paths between maternal spanking and externalizing behavior were positive and statistically significant. In the low-income and middle-income groups, there were consistently positive, statistically significant associations linking maternal spanking with child externalizing behavior problems across all ages.
For the high-income group (Fig. 1c), the associations between maternal spanking and child externalizing behavior problems at earlier ages (e.g., from age 1–3, and age 3–5) were positive and showed that, on average, higher levels of maternal spanking was associated with higher levels of child externalizing behavior problems. However, there was a point estimate of 0 between age 5 maternal spanking and age 9 child externalizing behavior problems. In other words, the pathway from maternal spanking at age 5 to child externalizing behavior problems at age 9 was non-significant. This suggests that the long-term associations linking maternal spanking with child externalizing behavior problems may be most reflective of the experience of children who were born into lowand middle-income families. For children in the high-income group only, the effects of maternal spanking on child externalizing behavior problems appear to be attenuated as children age.
It is important to interpret these findings in light of the structural invariance results, which showed that the low-income group was only statistically distinguishable from the high-income group. In other words, model paths between the low-income and middle-income groups are not statistically different. Overall, the relation between maternal spanking and children’s externalizing behaviors appears to be modestly different at the extremes of the income-to-poverty ratio (i.e., 0–99% vs 200+%), with the high-income group showing no relation between age 5 maternal spanking and age 9 child externalizing behavior problems.
5.4.1. Robustness checks
The robustness check of the model that added a control variable for neighborhood poverty indicated almost no differences in the model overall. Specifically, the moderation coefficients and conclusions were the same for the constrained and unconstrained models. There was one pathway coefficient, from child externalizing behavior at age 5 to child externalizing behavior at age 9 that changed very slightly. Similarly, the robustness check for mobility of family poverty status yielded the same moderation conclusions and exhibited no differences in model coefficients, except one pathway coefficient, from child externalizing behavior at age 5 to child externalizing behavior at age 9 that changed very slightly These results suggest our model is robust to the addition of neighborhood poverty and family poverty status mobility.
6. Discussion
In the current study, we examined whether the associations of maternal spanking with child externalizing behavior problems varied by economic hardship. Research shows that maternal spanking is associated with deleterious outcomes for children, including increased risk for childhood behavioral problems (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016b; Gershoff, Sattler, & Ansari, 2018). Furthermore, theory and research suggest that economic hardship is associated with harsh parenting practices (Berger, 2004; Berlin et al., 2009; Giles-Sims et al., 1995; Linver et al., 2002; Ryan et al., 2016; Straus & Stewart, 1999; Wissow, 2001). This study examined whether the longitudinal associations linking mothers’ use of spanking with child externalizing behavior problems were moderated by economic context.
The study results were consistent with prior research showing that maternal spanking is associated with elevated levels of subsequent child externalizing behavior problems (e.g., Maguire-Jack et al., 2012). Overall, the study results indicated that for children in the low- and middle-income groups (0–99% and 100–199% of the poverty threshold), maternal spanking was associated with child externalizing behavior through age 9. However, these longitudinal pathways were attenuated for children in the high-income group (200+% of the poverty threshold). Maternal spanking was associated with child externalizing behavior problems through age 5; maternal spanking at age 5 was not associated with externalizing behavior problems at age 9 for the high income group only. This suggests that the effects of maternal spanking in low- and middle-income families may be longer-lasting (i.e., through age 9) than the effects in high-income families. Low- and middle- income parents and children may have fewer resources to act as potential moderators to maternal harsh parenting practices over time.
6.1. Conditional corporal punishment argument
The conditional corporal punishment position states that, “… the effects of spanking are not necessarily negative or positive, but may be either or both depending on many other conditions” (Benjet & Kazdin, 2003). Most frequently, this position has been applied to a number of factors related to the context or conditions in which the child is raised. Specifically, neighborhood context, mother-child warmth, mother-child attachment, and race and ethnicity have all been cited as factors that may moderate the effects of spanking on child wellbeing.
For example, as applied to race and ethnicity, researchers argued that because the use of physical punishment is more normative among certain racial or ethnic groups in the U.S., such as African Americans, the negative effects of spanking would be weaker for African American children because the practice is more normative and thus less harmful (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997). However, even though spanking is on average more commonly used by African American parents compared to White parents in the U.S., the associations of spanking to negative child outcomes were the same or similar for White and African American children (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a) and White, African American, and American Indian children (Ward, Lee, Limb, & Grogan-Kaylor, 2019).
In a review of the trends in endorsement and use of spanking since 1988, Ryan et al. (2016) found that spanking is more commonly endorsed as an effective parenting strategy and more commonly used by lower-income parents. Thus, it could be argued that spanking is more normative in low-income contexts. Even so, the current study found that the association of spanking to elevated risk for child behavioral problems between ages 1–9 is still present in economically disadvantaged contexts. Also, compared to their counterparts in high-income families, maternal spanking may exert a longer-lasting influence on externalizing behavior among children living in low- and middle-income families. Therefore, instead of low-income children being impervious to the effects of spanking, this study suggests that low-income children may be more vulnerable to long lasting effects of spanking. These findings could be interpreted to contradict the conditional corporal punishment argument.
Poverty and economic hardship are associated with other problematic factors, such as increased levels of parental depression (Reising et al., 2013), parenting stress (Cooper, McLanahan, Meadows, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009), family violence (Rennison & Planty, 2003), neighborhood crime, and alcohol and illicit drug use (Hipp, 2007). Furthermore, financial strain compromises positive parental engagement (Carlson & Berger, 2013). In a review of the research on poverty, the American Academy of Pediatrics notes that “poverty is an independent determinant of health through its adverse effects on family relationships” and that strong family relationships, including secure attachment and engaged, responsive caregivers, are an “essential protective factor” to help children overcome adversity associated with poverty (Pascoe, Wood, Duffee, Kuo, the AAP Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, & Council on Community Pediatrics, 2016). Thus, the finding of this study that low- and middle- income children appear to be the most vulnerable to the long-term effects of spanking suggests that spanking is a part of a constellation of risk factors that lead to suboptimal outcomes for children. Children from high-income families, on the other hand, may have more promotive factors present in their families and communities, which may explain why the effects of spanking did not persist through age 9 in this group. However, it’s important to recognize that spanking was still harmful prior to age 9 in this high-income group.
6.2. Implications for practice and additional considerations
One implication of this study is that parents across the income distribution are likely to benefit from parent education related to non-physical discipline of young children. Results suggested that spanking is associated with increased risk of externalizing behavior for children across income groups. Thus, an overarching practice implication is that focusing on parenting behaviors within the parent-child relationship are a critical target for prevention of physical punishment (Gershoff, Lee, & Durrant, 2017).
Indeed, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends that parents avoid use of physical punishment and replace those behaviors with nonphysical discipline (Sege & Siegel, 2018). One practice implication of the current research is that parenting resources on safe alternatives to spanking should be made available routinely by reliable sources such as pediatricians and social workers that provide services to families of all incomes. For example, there are technology-delivered parent education programs that can be delivered to parents in pediatric waiting rooms (Scholar, 2019) and other widely available intensive services that focus on the needs of low-income and at-risk parents of young children (Dubowitz, 2019).
6.3. Study limitations
It is important to note the study limitations. First, our analyses are not causal in nature. The interpretations presented herein are inherently associational. Our analyses do not model within-person change (e.g., Berry & Wiloughby, 2017), and can only be interpreted to apply to between-group differences. We note that the FFCWS oversampled nonmarital births and families were recruited from urban areas; thus, study findings are not generalizable to all families with young children or other populations. Although we controlled for a comprehensive set of covariates that included numerous confounds in the relationship between spanking and child outcomes (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016b), including neighborhood poverty, poverty category mobility, and parental race and ethnicity, it is still possible that unmeasured covariates could explain the associations. For example, one potential unmeasured covariate may be child developmental problems. Our study did not exclude children with developmental limitations that may be associated with higher rates of maternal spanking, child externalizing behavior problems, and poverty or economic hardship. Additionally, our key variables of interest were measured via self-report, which may be subject to social desirability bias or inaccurate reporting. Similarly, the FFCWS measured spanking by asking parents if they had spanked their child due to a child misbehaving or acting up. Thus, it is possible that this definition of spanking led parents to underestimate or inaccurately report the amount of times they had spanked their child, for example, if the parent had used spanking for reasons other than the child misbehaving or acting up. Finally, it is important to note the limitations of the federal poverty guidelines which do not vary geographically (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). The federal poverty guidelines are also very low, and arguably do not fully capture most households’ needs. Finally, we utilize a US-based sample for our analyses. Because there is variation in the degree to which spanking is normative across geographic region and cultural context (Grogan-Kaylor et al., 2020), future research should examine income moderation of the effects of spanking in other geographical and income contexts.
7. Conclusion
Study results are consistent with prior research in showing that spanking is deleterious for children across income groups. In addition, the results of this study may be interpreted to show that children in the lowest-income families may be especially vulnerable to the long-term effects of spanking. For higher income families, the associations of maternal spanking with child externalizing behavior problems appear to be attenuated as children age, possibly due to moderating influences that are not as readily available to children in low- and middle- income households. Because the study findings suggest that spanking is harmful for young children across income-to-needs ratio levels, universal parent education that instructs parents not to spank and that provides parents with nonviolent discipline strategies is likely to be beneficial to all parents and children.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the nearly 5000 families who participated in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study. The Fragile Families Study was funded by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD (#R01HD36916) and a consortium of private foundations. Persons interested in obtaining Fragile Families contract data should see http://www.fragilefamilies.princeton.edu for further information. The authors gratefully acknowledge use of the services and facilities of the Population Studies Center at the University of Michigan, funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P2CHD041028. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
References
- Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. [Google Scholar]
- Altschul I, Lee SJ, & Gershoff ET (2016). Hugs, not hits: Warmth and spanking as predictors of child social competence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 78(3), 695–714. 10.1111/jomf.12306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing (2019). User’s guide for the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study public data, year 5. SeptemberPrinceton, NJ: Princeton University. [Google Scholar]
- Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing (2018a). User’s guide for the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study public data, year 3. NovemberPrinceton, NJ: Princeton University. [Google Scholar]
- Bendheim-Thoman Center for Research on Child Wellbeing (2018b). User’s guide for the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study public data, year 9. NovemberPrinceton, NJ: Princeton University. [Google Scholar]
- Benjet C, & Kazdin AE (2003). Spanking children: The controversies, findings, and new directions. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 197–224. 10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00206-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger LM (2004). Income, family structure, and child maltreatment risk. Children and Youth Services Review, 26, 725–748. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2004.02.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin LJ, Ispa JM, Fine MA, Malone PS, Brooks-Gunn J, Brady-Smith C, et al. (2009). Correlates and consequences of spanking and verbal punishment for low-income White, African American, and Mexican American toddlers. Child Development, 80, 1403–1420. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01341.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry D, & Wiloughby MT (2017). On the practical interpretability of cross-lagged panel models: Rethinking a developmental workhorse. Child Development, 88, 1186–1206. 10.1111/cdev.12660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boss P (1987). Family stress. In Sussman MB, & Steinmetz SK (Eds.). Handbook of marriage and the family (pp. 695–723). New York: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carlson MJ, & Berger LM (2013). What kids get from parents: Packages of parental involvement across complex family forms. The Social Service Review, 87, 213–249. 10.1086/671015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson MJ, McLanahan SS, & Brooks-Gunn J (2008). Coparenting and nonresident fathers’ involvement with young children after a nonmarital birth. Demography, 45, 461–488. 10.1353/dem.0.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung EK, McCollum KF, Elo IT, Lee HJ, & Culhane JF (2004). Maternal depressive symptoms and infant health practices among low-income women. Pediatrics, 113(6), e523–9. 10.1542/peds.113.6.e523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger RD, Wallace LE, Sun Y, Simons RL, McLoyd VC, & Brody GH (2002). Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 179–193. 10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger KJ, Rueter MA, & Conger RD (2000). The role of economic pressure in the lives of parents and their adolescents: The family stress model. In Crockett LJ, & Silbereisen RK (Eds.). Negotiating adolescence in times of social change (pp. 201–223). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper CE, McLanahan SS, Meadows SO, & Brooks-Gunn J (2009). Family structure transitions and maternal parenting stress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 71, 558–574. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2009.00619.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dearing E, McCartney K, & Taylor BA (2001). Change in family income-to-needs matters more for children with less. Child Development, 72, 1179–1793. 10.1111/1467-8624.00378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deater-Deckard K, & Dodge KA (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161–175. 10.1207/s15327965pli0803_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dubowitz H (2019). Safe environment for every kid (SEEK). In Gershoff ET, & Lee SJ (Eds.). Ending the physical punishment of children: A guide for clinicians and practitioners (pp. 63–72). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Duncan G, Yeung W, Brooks-Gunn J, & Smith J (1998). How much does childhood poverty affect the life chances of children? American Sociological Review, 63, 406–423. 10.2307/2657556. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan G, Ziol-Guest KM, & Kalil A (2010). Early-childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior, and health. Child Development, 81, 306–325. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK, & Bandalos DL (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 430–457. 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. The American Psychologist, 59, 77–92. 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkelhor D, Turner H, Wormuth BK, Vanderminden J, & Hamby S (2019). Corporal punishment: Current rates from a national survey. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(7), 1991–1997. 10.1007/s10826-019-01426-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gard AM, McLoyd VC, Mitchell C, & Hyde LW (2020). Evaluation of a longitudinal family stress model in a population‐based cohort. Social Development, 1–21. 10.1111/sode.12446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershoff ET, & Grogan-Kaylor A (2016a). Race as a moderator of associations between spanking and child outcomes. Family Relations, 65, 490–501. 10.1111/fare.12205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershoff ET, & Grogan-Kaylor A (2016b). Spanking and child outcomes: Old controversies and new meta-analyses. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 453–469. 10.1037/fam0000191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershoff ET, Lee SJ, & Durrant JE (2017). Promising intervention strategies to reduce parents’ use of physical punishment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 71, 9–23. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershoff ET, Sattler KMP, & Ansari A (2018). Strengthening causal estimates for links between spanking and children’s externalizing behavior problems. Psychological Science, 29, 110–120. 10.1177/0956797617729816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giles-Sims J, Straus MA, & Sugarman DB (1995). Child, maternal, and family characteristics associated with spanking. Family Relations: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 44, 170–176. 10.2307/584804. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gottlieb A, Pilkauskas NV, & Garfinkel I (2014). Private financial transfers, family income, and the Great Recession. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 76, 1011–1024. 10.1111/jomf.12134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray RF, Indurkhya A, & McCormick MC (2004). Prevalence, stability, and predictors of clinically significant behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3, 5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics, 114(3), 736–743. 10.1542/peds.2003-1150-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grogan-Kaylor A, Castillo B, Pace GT, Ward KP, Ma J, Lee SJ, et al. (2020). Global perspectives on physical and nonphysical discipline: A Bayesian multilevel analysis. Manuscript under review but not yet accepted for publication.
- Gromoske AN, & Maguire-Jack K (2012). Transactional and cascading relations between early spanking and children’s social-emotional development. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74(5), 1054–1068. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01013.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hipp JR (2007). Income inequality, race, and place: Does the distribution of race and class within neighborhoods affect crime rates? Criminology, 45, 665–697. 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2007.00088.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, & Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Andrews G, Mroczek D, Ustun B, & Wittchen H-U (1998). The World Health Organization composite international diagnostic interview short-form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 7, 171–185. 10.1002/mpr.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J, Lee SJ, Taylor CA, & Guterman NB (2014). Dyadic profiles of parental disciplinary behavior and links with parenting context. Child Maltreatment, 19, 79–91. 10.1177/1077559514532009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsen R (2011). Missing data imputation versus full information maximum likelihood with second-level dependencies. Structural Equation Modeling, 18, 649–662. 10.1080/10705511.2011.607721. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SJ, Altschul I, & Gershoff ET (2015). Wait until your father gets home? Fathers’ and mothers’ spanking and development of child aggression. Children and Youth Services Review, 52, 158–166. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee DY, Harring JR, & Stapleton LM (2019). Comparing methods for addressing missingness in longitudinal modeling of panel data. Journal of Experimental Education, 87, 596–615. 10.1080/00220973.2018.1520683. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SJ, Altschul I, & Gershoff ET (2013). Does warmth moderate longitudinal associations between maternal spanking and child aggression in early childhood? Developmental Psychology, 49, 2017–2028. 10.1037/a0031630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee D, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan SS, Notterman D, & Garfinkel I (2013). The Great Recession, genetic sensitivity, and maternal harsh parenting. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 13780–13784. 10.1073/pnas.1312398110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linver MR, Brooks-Gunn J, & Kohen DE (2002). Family processes as pathways from income to young children’s development. Developmental Psychology, 38, 719–734. 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma J (2016). Neighborhood and parenting both matter: The role of neighborhood collective efficacy and maternal spanking in early behavior problems. Children and Youth Services Review, 70, 250–260. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma J, Grogan-Kaylor A, & Klein S (2018). Neighborhood collective efficacy, parental spanking, and subsequent risk of household child protective services involvement. Child Abuse & Neglect, 80, 90–98. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire-Jack K, Gromoske AN, & Berger LM (2012). Spanking and child development during the first 5 years of life. Child Development, 83(6), 1960–1977. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01820.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masarik AS, & Conger RD (2017). Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 85–90. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathiesen KS, & Tambs K (1999). The EAS Temperament Questionnaire—Factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(3), 431–439. 10.1111/1469-7610.00460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer SE, & Jenks C (1989). Poverty and the distribution of material hardship. The Journal of Human Resources, 24, 88–114. 10.2307/145934. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLeod BA, Johnson WE Jr, Cryer-Coupet QR, & Mincy RB (2019). Examining the longitudinal effects of paternal incarceration and coparenting relationships on sons’ educational outcomes: A mediation analysis. Children and Youth Services Review, 100, 362–375. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.03.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLoyd VC (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. The American Psychologist, 53, 185–204. 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meadows SO, McLanahan S, & Knab JT (2009). Economic trajectories in non-traditional families with children. RAND Working Paper. Available online at: https://www.rand.org/pubs/working_papers/WR701.html. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998). Mplus user’s guide2017(eighth edition). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Newman DA (2003). Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing data: A simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organizational Research Methods, 6, 328–362. 10.1177/1094428103254673. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pascoe JM, Wood DL, Duffee JH, Kuo A, & the AAP Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Council on Community Pediatrics (2016). Mediators and adverse effects of child poverty in the United States. Pediatrics, 137(4), e20160340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reichman NE, Teitler JO, Garfinkel I, & McLanahan SS (2001). Fragile families: Sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review, 23, 303–326. 10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reising MM, Watson KH, Hardcastle EJ, Merchant MJ, Roberts L, Forehand R, et al. (2013). Parental depression and economic disadvantage: The role of parenting in associations with internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 335–343. 10.1007/s10826-012-9582-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rennison C, & Planty M (2003). Nonlethal intimate partner violence: Examining race, gender, and income patterns. Violence and Victims, 18, 433–443. 10.1891/vivi.2003.18.4.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan RM, Kalil A, Ziol-Guest KM, & Padilla C (2016). Socioeconomic gaps in parents’ discipline strategies from 1988 to 2011. Pediatrics, 138(6), e20160720. 10.1542/peds.2016-0720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satorra A, & Bentler PM (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75, 243–248. 10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholar S (2019). Play nicely. In Gershoff ET, & Lee SJ (Eds.). Ending the physical punishment of children: A guide for clinicians and practitioners (pp. 49–61). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Sege RD, & Siegel BS (2018). Effective discipline to raise healthy children. Pediatrics, 142(6), e20183112. 10.1542/peds.2018-3112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Socolar RR, Savage E, Keyes-Elstein L, & Evans H (2005). Factors that affect parental disciplinary practices of children aged 12 to 19 months. Southern Medical Journal, 98, 1181–1191. 10.1097/01.smj.0000190177.12387.07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, & Donnelly M (2005). Theoretical approaches to corporal punishment. In Donnelly M, & Straus MA (Eds.). Corporal punishment of children in theoretical perspective (pp. 3–7). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, & Stewart JH (1999). Corporal punishment by American Parents: National data on prevalence, chronicity, severity, and duration, in relation to child and family characteristics. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 55–70. 10.1023/A:1021891529770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor CA, Manganello JA, Lee SJ, & Rice JC (2010). Mothers’ spanking of 3-year-old children and subsequent risk of children’s aggressive behavior. Pediatrics, 125(5), e1057–e1065. 10.1542/peds.2009-2678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2019). Annual update of the HHS poverty guidelines. Federal Register, 84(22), (2019, February) Accessed online at: https://www.govinfo.gov/content/pkg/FR-2019-02-01/pdf/2019-00621.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Waller R, Gard AM, Monk CS, Mitchell C, Bazzi B, Sypher I, et al. (2019). The system for coding interactions and family functioning (SCIFF) in low-income and urban adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 31, 974–984. 10.1037/pas0000719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward KP, Lee SJ, Limb GE, & Grogan-Kaylor A (2019). Physical punishment and child externalizing behavior: Comparing American Indian, White, and African American children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1–23. 10.1177/0886260519861678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward KP, Lee SJ, Pace GT, Grogan-Kaylor A, & Ma J (2020). Attachment style and the association of spanking and child externalizing behavior. Academic Pediatrics, 20(4), 501–507. 10.1016/j.acap.2019.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wissow LS (2001). Ethnicity, income, and parenting contexts of physical punishment in a national sample of families with young children. Child Maltreatment, 6, 118–129. 10.1177/1077559501006002004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]