Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Jul 14;16(7):e0254610. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254610

Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey

Kazumasa Ebisawa 1,2,*,#, Shunya Nakayama 1,, Chungyu Pai 1,, Rie Kinoshita 1,, Hiroshi Koie 1,#
Editor: I Anna S Olsson3
PMCID: PMC8279392  PMID: 34260621

Abstract

A case control study was conducted to estimate the prevalence of feather-damaging behavior and evaluate the correlation with risk factors among pet psittacine birds in Japan. Although feather-damaging behavior among pet parrots is frequently observed in Japan, its prevalence and potential risk factors have not been investigated. Therefore, we conducted an online questionnaire survey on parrot owners throughout Japan to examine regional differences in feather-damaging behavior and associated risk factors. In total, 2,331 valid responses were obtained. The prevalence of feather-damaging behavior was 11.7%, in general agreement with prior studies. The highest prevalence was among Cockatoos (Cacatua spp., etc.; 30.6%), followed by Lovebirds (Agapornis spp.; 24.5%) and African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus; 23.7%). Multivariate logistic regression was carried out to calculate the adjusted odds ratio (ORadj) for potential risk factors and adjust the confounding of the variables. The odds of feather-damaging behavior were significantly higher for Conures (Aratinga spp., Pyrrhura spp., Thectocercus acuticaudatus, Cyanoliseus patagonus) (ORadj = 2.55, P = 0.005), Pacific parrotlets (Forpus coelestis) (ORadj = 3.96, P < 0.001), African grey parrots (ORadj = 6.74, P < 0.001), Lovebirds (ORadj = 6.79, P < 0.001) and Cockatoos (ORadj = 9.46, P < 0.001) than Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus), and for young adults (ORadj = 1.81, P = 0.038) and adults (ORadj = 3.17, P < 0.001) than young birds, and for signs of separation anxiety (ORadj = 1.81, P < 0.001). Species, bird age and signs of separation anxiety were significantly higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than any other potential risk factors. Our findings, which include broad species diversity, are a good source of data for predicting risk factors for feather-damaging behavior and could be useful in preventing declines in welfare.

Introduction

Feather-damaging behavior (FDB) is a behavioral disorder generally seen in parrots kept captive as pets [1, 2]. FDB is a troublesome problem for pet owners, caregivers, and clinicians, and generally indicates poor welfare [1, 3, 4]. FDB includes picking, plucking, chewing, fraying, and biting [57], and may also include self-mutilation of skin or muscles, which can inhibit the normal regrowth of feathers [8]. The prevalence of FDB appears to vary among the more than 200 bird species that are commonly kept in captivity [9, 10]. The prevalence of FDB among parrots has been estimated to be 10–17.5% [1114].

FDB may be an intentional way of coping with stress due to an unsuitable environment and poor management [8, 15]. The causes of FDB have been reported to have origins in boredom (e.g., deprivation of environmental enrichment or foraging opportunities, unsuitable cage size or design) [9, 1618], environmental stress (e.g., always caged, living with other parrots) [1, 14, 19], loneliness (e.g., social isolation, absence of the preferred owner) [7, 20, 21], separation anxiety [7, 9, 22] and sexual frustration (e.g., delayed reproductive behavior) [18, 23]. In addition, sex [14, 19], age (adult) [13], acquisition source (rescued/rehomed and pet store) [19, 22], hand-rearing [14, 21], being out of the cage for more than 8 hours [22] and sleeping time (more than 8 hours) [19] have been suggested as risk factors for FDB. Conversely, it has been suggested that interacting with people for more than 4 hours a day may help prevent FDB [22].

Problems regarding the understanding of the mechanisms underlying FDB are related to the relative lack of controlled studies on FDB in pet birds and limited veterinary medical knowledge of feather loss and FDB [24]. More accurate information on FDB could not only facilitate better treatment of affected birds, but also lead to the prevention of FDB onset. The purpose of the study reported here was to estimate the prevalence of FDB and evaluate its correlation with risk factors among pet psittacine birds in Japan. We focused on how the presence of humans, conspecific cage mates, and other birds or animals affects FDB because parrots are highly social animals [9, 25]. We investigated whether separation anxiety can cause FDB and whether the presence of humans, conspecific birds, other birds or animals can prevent FDB. Although FDB among pet parrots is frequently observed in Japan, to our knowledge, its prevalence and potential risk factors have not been investigated.

Materials and methods

Study population and data collection

Since the approval of human subjects research is required only for medical research in Japan, the Ethics Review Committee of the College of Bioresource Sciences of Nihon University determined that approval is not required for conducting this study. An online questionnaire was compiled using Google Forms (https://www.google.com/forms/about/). All participants were recruited from across Japan through an advertisement on the authors’ website (https://www.yokohamabirdclinic.jp), Internet forums, and social networks. Participants were able to respond to up to five birds if they had more than one bird. The survey was carried out online for 16 weeks, from October 2018 to January 2019. Participants were asked to consent to inform before responding the questionnaire. Consent was obtained by click the Yes button and type the participant’s name in the box (S1 File). This study was conducted with great care to ensure the privacy and confidentiality of the participants, and to guarantee that the data would be used only for the purposes of scientific research. To avoid any bias in the numbers of FDB or non-FDB responses, the participants were told that the purpose of the survey was research into problem behavior.

Questionnaire

An original questionnaire form was developed on the basis of FDB risk factors listed in prior studies [1214, 19, 22] and review papers [7, 26]. There was a total of 26 questions (Table 1). The bird species, not limited to parrots, was selected from a pull-down list. If the species were not on the list or the exact species was not known, the owner could input the information into the text box manually. The wing clipping was asked to confirm if the bird could fly. Wild-caught birds were not listed as an acquisition source because they are not available in pet stores in Japan. A question item on the frequency of bathing/spraying was included to assess the relationship between the frequency of feather wetting and FDB, with “Rarely” defined as once a month or less, “Weekly” as about once a week, and “Daily” as almost every day. Fresh foods were designated as vegetables and/or fruits. Human foods were designated as foods excluding fresh foods, with “Sometimes” defined as about once a week and “Always” as almost every day. FDB was defined as feather picking, plucking, chewing, or biting. Stereotyped behavior was defined as excessive self-grooming, incessant screaming, wire chewing, sham chewing, beak rubbing, food manipulation, wing flapping, pacing, perch circles, corner flips, or route tracing [7, 27, 28]. Reproductive behavior was defined as courtship behavior, copulation behavior, or nesting [29]. The terms used for each behavior were supplemented by a description (S2 File). Signs of separation anxiety were defined as vocalization and/or locomotor activity (e.g., pacing, wing flapping) when the owner leaves home or decreased appetite and/or destructiveness while the owner is absent [3032]. The presence or absence of signs of separation anxiety was determined by the owner based on the behavior of the bird when the owner leaves home and the condition inside the cage when the owner returns home.

Table 1. Overview of 26 questions of the questionnaire and predictors for pet parrots in Japan.

Questions Available options Predictors
What is your gender? Male Gender
Female
What is your age? Pull-down list Owner age
Are you married? Yes Married
No
Do you have any children? Yes Children
No
How many people are there in your family? Pull-down list Family size
What is the species? Pull-down list or free text box Species
What sex is the bird? Male Sex
Female
Don’t know
How old is the bird? Pull-down list (includes Don’t know) Bird age
Are you clipping wing feathers? Yes Wing clipping
No
How did you acquire the bird? Pet store/breeder Acquisition source
Bred at home
Adopted
Stray bird
Don’t know
How was the bird reared? Hand-rearing by owner Rearing methods
Hand-rearing by pet store or breeder
Parent-rearing
Parent-rearing with neonatal handling
Don’t know
Do you have any other birds and/or animals? Yes Other birds and/or animals
No
Is there any conspecific cage mate? Yes Conspecific cage mate
No
Not caged
How many hours a day do you let the bird outside of the cage and interact with it? Pull-down list or Does not let outside Time let outside of the cage and interaction
How many hours do you let the bird sleep at night? Pull-down list Sleeping time
Do you cover the entire cage at night? Yes Cage covered at night
No
Not caged
How many hours are there with no human presence in a typical day? Pull-down list Time with no human presence
Do you use ultraviolet light? Yes Ultraviolet light
No
How often do you bathe and/or spray the bird? Never Bathing/spraying
Rarely
Weekly
Daily
What are the staple foods? Only seeds Staple foods
Only pellets
Seeds & pellets
Do you feed the bird any fresh foods? Yes Fresh foods
No
How often do you feed the bird human foods? Always Human foods
Sometimes
Never
Is feather-damaging behavior observed? Yes Feather-damaging behavior
No
Is stereotyped behavior observed? Yes Stereotyped behavior
No
Has reproductive behavior been observed within the past 6 months? Yes Reproductive behavior
No
Are signs of separation anxiety observed? Yes Separation anxiety
No

Data analyses

In total, 3,392 sets of responses were obtained. Responses corresponding to any of the following were excluded from the analysis: duplicated responses, responses that were defective in any way (e.g., unknown species, sex, age, acquisition source and rearing method), responses for hybrid species, and responses for non-psittacine species (e.g., Sparrows, Pheasants, Pigeons, Owls). After removing responses from owners of unknown age, all stray birds were excluded. In addition, responses showing FDB from the time of the acquisition were excluded because the environment at the time of the onset of FDB was unknown. Some of the congeners or closely related species were grouped together because of their similar prevalence of FDB. Species with a proportion of less than 2% were excluded to clarify the trends in FDB according to species or groups. The overall prevalence of FDB was calculated after this exclusion. Finally, 2,331 sets of responses were included in the analysis, which is provided as a S3 File. The true species before grouping is listed in the S4 File.

The genus Agapornis was grouped as Lovebirds. The genera Aratinga, Cyanoliseus, and Pyrrhura were grouped as Conures. The genera Cacatua, Calyptorhynchus, Eolophus, and Lophochroa were grouped as Cockatoos. Although Cockatiels are Cacatuidae, they were not grouped into Cockatoos because of their small size and large sample size. As the biological significance of chronological age differs between species, age was classified by species-specific life stages as follows: juvenile/adolescent (period after weaning and fledging, until sexual maturation), young adult (early period of sexual maturation), and adult (period after full sexual maturation) [13]. Some criteria were excluded, and similar levels were pooled to increase the sample sizes. “Not caged” was removed from “Conspecific cage mate” and “Cage covered at night”. “Parent-rearing” and “Parent-rearing with neonatal handling” were pooled into “Parent-rearing” for the rearing method. “Never” and “Rarely” were pooled into “Rarely” for Bathing/spraying. “Sometimes” and “Always” were pooled into “Yes”, and “Never” was changed to “No” for Human foods.

Univariate logistic regression was first used to compare potential risk factors between FDB and non-FDB parrots, estimate odds ratios (ORs), and calculate 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The phi coefficient or Cramer’s V was used to measure correlations between categorical variables where P < 0.05. A predictor was selected based on biological plausibility, the significance of associations with FDB and model fit, when the correlation between variables was very strong (phi or Cramer’s V > 0.25) [33]. Multivariate logistic regression was used to calculate the adjusted ORs (ORadj) and 95% CIs for potential risk factors for FDB and to adjust the confounding of the variables. The forward selection method based on the likelihood ratio was used to select the variables. Predicted probabilities, Predicted group membership, Standardised residuals and Cook’s were checked on save option. Classification plots, Hosmer–Lemeshow goodness of fit and CI for exp(B) were checked on options. Variables were left in the final model when P < 0.05 [34]. All nonsignificant variables were tested in the final model to check for residual confounding [35]. Regression diagnostics were performed on the full adjusted analyses using the Hosmer–Lemeshow test for goodness of fit [36]. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows (version 20.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

The overall prevalence of FDB was 11.7% (272/2,331 responses) and varied cross-species and groups. The highest FDB prevalence was 30.6% for Cockatoos, followed by 24.5% for African grey parrots and 23.7% for Lovebirds. The prevalence of FDB for Budgerigars, which received the highest number of responses, was 4.9%, followed by Cockatiels, at 7.6% (Table 2).

Table 2. Sample sizes included in the studied population (n = 2,331) and prevalence of feather-damaging behavior (FDB) by parrot species and group.

Species Sample size Prevalence of FDB
n % n % 95% CI
Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) 853 36.6 42 4.9 3.6–6.6
Cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) 608 26.1 46 7.6 5.6–10.0
Lovebirds (Agapornis spp.)a 470 20.2 115 24.5 20.6–28.6
Conures (various species)b 126 5.4 14 11.1 6.2–17.9
Pacific parrotlets (Forpus coelestis) 79 3.4 14 17.7 10.0–27.9
Cockatoos (various species)c 72 3.1 22 30.6 20.2–42.5
Barred parakeets (Bolborhynchus lineola) 64 2.7 5 7.8 2.6–17.3
African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus)d 59 2.5 14 23.7 13.6–36.6

aAgapornis roseicollis, A. personata, A. fischeri.

bAratinga spp., Cyanoliseus sp., Pyrrhura spp.

cCacatua spp., Calyptorhynchus sp., Eolophus sp., Lophochroa sp.

d Include Psittacus erithacus timneh.

CI = confidence interval.

Ten of the 26 predictors in univariate analysis were significantly associated with increased or decreased ORs for FDB (Tables 3 and 4). Some correlations were found between variables (Table 5). The P values of the Chi-square test derived for each correlation were also shown in Table 5. In the correlation between the presence of children and family size (Cramer’s V = 0.591, P < 0.001), family size was selected based on model fit. In the correlation between species and bathing/spraying (Cramer’s V = 0.316, P < 0.001), species was selected based on the significance of the association with FDB. In the correlation between bird age and acquisition source (Cramer’s V = 0.515, P < 0.001), bird age was selected based on biological plausibility.

Table 3. Results of univariate analysis of owner characteristics associated with feather-damaging behavior (FDB) in 2,331 parrots.

Variable FDB Non-FDB Prevalence of FDB OR 95% CI P-value
n n %
Gender
 Male 19 173 9.9
 Female 253 1,886 11.8 1.22 0.75–2.00 0.425
Owner age
 18–39 years 50 408 10.9
 40–49 years 133 902 12.9 1.20 0.85–1.70 0.294
 ≥ 50 years 89 749 10.6 0.97 0.67–1.40 0.970
Married
 No 103 802 11.4
 Yes 169 1,257 11.9 1.05 0.81–1.36 0.730
Children
 No 197 1,32173 13.0
 Yes 75 8 9.2 0.68 0.52–0.90 0.007*
Family size
 One 42 331 11.3
 Two 128 773 14.2 1.30 0.90–1.89 0.160
 Three 66 487 11.9 1.07 0.71–1.61 0.754
 Four or more 36 468 7.1 0.61 0.38–0.97 0.036*

*P < 0.05.

OR = odds ratio.

CI = confidence interval.

Table 4. Results of univariate analysis of bird characteristics associated with feather-damaging behavior (FDB) in 2,331 parrots.

Variable FDB Non-FDB Prevalence of FDB OR 95% CI P-value
n n %
Species
 Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) 42 811 4.9
 Cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) 46 562 7.6 1.58 1.03–2.43 0.038*
 Barred parakeets (Bolborhynchus lineola) 5 59 7.8 1.64 0.62–4.29 0.317
 Conures (various species)a 14 112 11.1 2.41 1.28–4.56 0.007*
 Pacific parrotlets (Forpus coelestis) 14 65 17.7 4.16 2.16–8.01 < 0.001*
 African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) 14 45 23.7 6.01 3.06–11.80 < 0.001*
 Lovebirds (Agapornis spp.)b 115 355 24.5 6.26 4.30–9.10 < 0.001*
 Cockatoos (various species)c 22 50 30.6 8.50 4.71–15.30 < 0.001*
Bird sex
 Male 143 1,240 10.3
 Female 129 819 13.6 1.37 1.06–1.76 0.016*
Bird age
 Juvenile/adolescentd 18 227 7.3
 Young adulte 90 866 9.4 1.31 0.77–2.22 0.314
 Adultf 164 966 14.5 2.14 1.29–3.56 0.003*
Wing clipping
 No 248 1,915 11.5
 Yes 24 144 14.3 1.29 0.82–2.02 0.274
Acquisition source
 Pet store/breeder 217 1,754 11.0
 Bred at home 34 177 16.1 1.55 1.05–2.30 0.028*
 Adopted 21 128 14.1 1.33 0.82–2.15 0.252
Rearing methods
 Hand-rearing by owners 159 1,166 12.0
 Hand-rearing by pet store or breeder 100 799 11.1 0.92 0.70–1.20 0.527
 Parent-rearing (including neonatal handling) 13 94 12.1 1.01 0.56–1.85 0.963
Lives with other birds and/or animals
 No 81 600 8.3
 Yes 191 1,459 11.6 0.97 0.74–1.28 0.828
Conspecific cage mate
 No 247 1,840 11.8
 Yes 25 219 9.8 0.85 0.55–1.31 0.465
Time let outside of the cage and interaction (h)
 < 2 175 1,271 12.1
 ≥ 2 86 693 11.0 0.90 0.69–1.19 0.458
 Not let outside 11 95 10.4 0.84 0.44–1.60 0.598
Sleeping time (h)
 < 8 52 468 10.0
 8–12 158 1,172 11.9 1.21 0.87–1.69 0.252
 > 12 62 419 12.9 1.33 0.90–1.97 0.151
Cage covered at night
 No 48 358 11.8
 Yes 224 1,701 11.6 0.98 0.71–1.37 0.915
Time with no human presence (h)
 < 3 76 508 13.0
 3–7 74 665 10.0 0.74 0.53–1.05 0.088
 7–11 79 651 10.8 0.81 0.58–1.13 0.222
 > 11 43 235 15.5 1.22 0.82–1.83 0.329
Ultraviolet light
 No 231 1,756 11.6
 Yes 41 303 11.9 1.03 0.72–1.47 0.876
Bathing/spraying
 Rarely 71 864 7.6
 Weekly 173 1,043 14.2 2.02 1.51–2.70 < 0.001*
 Daily 28 152 15.6 2.24 1.40–3.59 < 0.001*
Staple foods
 Only seeds 53 563 8.7
 Only pellets 56 322 14.8 1.85 1.24–2.76 0.003*
 Seeds and pellets 163 1,174 12.2 1.47 1.06–2.04 0.019*
Fresh foods
 No 52 401 11.5
 Yes 220 1,658 11.7 1.02 0.74–1.41 0.889
Human foods
 No 206 1,702 10.8
 Yes 66 357 16.6 1.53 1.13–2.06 0.006*
Reproductive behavior
 No 85 647 11.6
 Yes 187 1,412 11.7 1.01 0.77–1.32 0.954
Stereotyped behavior
 No 224 1,717 11.5
 Yes 48 342 12.3 1.06 0.77–1.50 0.667
Separation anxiety
 No 160 1,411 10.2
 Yes 112 648 14.7 1.52 1.18–1.97 < 0.001*

aAratinga spp., Cyanoliseus sp., Pyrrhura spp.

bAgapornis roseicollis, A. personata, A. fischeri.

cCacatua spp., Calyptorhynchus sp., Eolophus sp., Lophochroa sp.

dSmall parrot: < 5 months, medium parrot: < 1 year 11 months, large parrot: < 4 years 11 months.

eSmall parrot: 5 months to 3 years 11 months, medium parrot: 2 years to 5 years 11 months, large parrot: 5 years to 10 years 11 months.

fSmall parrot: > 4 years, medium parrot: > 6 years, large parrot: > 11 years.

*P < 0.05.

OR = odds ratio.

CI = confidence interval.

Table 5. Phi and Cramer’s V coefficient for correlations between variables where P < 0.05 in the univariate analysis.

Presence of children Family size Species Bird sex Bird age Acquisition Source Bathing/spraying Staple foods Human foods
Family size 0.591a
0.000**
Species 0.105 0.107
0.001* 0.000**
Bird sex –0.140 0.037 0.090
0.449 0.354 0.009*
Bird age 0.090 0.070 0.209 0.014
0.000** 0.001* 0.000** 0.788
Acquisition source 0.067 0.043 0.123 0.019 0.515a
0.005* 0.184 0.000** 0.654 0.000**
Bathing/spraying 0.070 0.062 0.316a 0.098 0.075 0.034
0.004* 0.006* 0.000** 0.000** 0.000** 0.254
Staple foods 0.037 0.066 0.249 0.022 0.077 0.015 0.089
0.210* 0.002* 0.000** 0.564 0.000** 0.910 0.000**
Human foods 0.001 0.039 0.230 0.050 0.029 0.076 0.040 0.066
0.953 .316 0.000** 0.016 0.383 0.001* 0.157 0.006*
Separation anxiety 0.029 0.032 0.212 0.000 0.076 0.097 0.036 0.069 0.081
0.166 0.488 0.000** 0.994 0.001* 0.000** 0.220 0.004* 0.000**

Above the cell shows the Phi and Cramer’s V coefficient,

Phi,

aCramer’s V > 0.25.

Below the cell shows the P values for the Chi-square test,

*P < 0.01,

**P < 0.001.

Seven predictors—family size, species, bird age, bird sex, staple food, human foods, and separation anxiety—were included in the multivariable model. The final model included 3 predictors (species, bird age, and separation anxiety) in the multivariate logistic regression. Compared with Budgerigars, the odds of FDB were 2.5 times higher in Conures (ORadj = 2.55, P = 0.005), almost four times higher in Pacific parrotlets (ORadj = 3.96, P < 0.001), almost seven times higher in African grey parrots (ORadj = 6.74, P < 0.001), almost seven times higher in Lovebirds (ORadj = 6.79, P < 0.001), and 9.5 times higher in Cockatoos (ORadj = 9.46, P < 0.001). Compared with juveniles/adolescents, the odds of FDB were almost two times higher in young adults (ORadj = 1.81, P = 0.038) and almost three times higher in adults (ORadj = 3.17, P < 0.001). Signs of separation anxiety (ORadj = 1.81, P < 0.001) were significantly correlated with FDB (Table 6).

Table 6. Final model of the multivariate analysis of risk factors significantly associated with feather-damaging behavior in 2,331 parrots.

Variable B SE ORadj 95% CI P value
Species
 Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus)
 Cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) 0.402 0.225 1.49 0.96–2.32 0.074
 Barred parakeets (Bolborhynchus lineola) 0.457 0.498 1.58 0.60–4.19 0.358
 Conures (various species)a 0.936 0.332 2.55 1.33–4.89 0.005*
 Pacific parrotlets (Forpus coelestis) 1.377 0.339 3.96 2.04–7.71 < 0.001*
 African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) 1.908 0.360 6.74 3.33–13.65 < 0.001*
 Lovebirds (Agapornis spp.)b 1.916 0.195 6.79 4.64–9.95 < 0.001*
 Cockatoos (various species)c 2.247 0.321 9.46 5.05–17.73 < 0.001*
Bird age
 Juvenile/adolescent
 Young adult 0.591 0.286 1.81 1.03–3.16 0.038*
 Adult 1.154 0.275 3.17 1.85–5.44 < 0.001*
Separation anxiety
 No
 Yes 0.595 0.144 1.81 1.37–2.40 < 0.001*

aAratinga spp., Cyanoliseus sp., Pyrrhura spp.

bAgapornis roseicollis, A. personata, A. fischeri.

cCacatua spp., Calyptorhynchus sp., Eolophus sp., Lophochroa sp.

*P < 0.05.

B = partial regression coefficient.

SE = standard error.

ORajd = adjusted odds ratio.

CI = confidence interval.

Discussion

Owner characteristics

The greatest response to the present survey was from older women, which is in line with prior studies [12, 22, 37]. There is a possibility of a potential response bias because older women respond more cooperatively to Internet surveys, or are just very skilled at keeping animals and enjoy it as primary caregivers. We hypothesized that if birds perceived a human family as a flock, then the characteristics of the owner would be related to FDB. The presence of children and family size were likely to decrease the odds of FDB; however, this trend did not remain in the final model. Further studies of owner characteristics such as the personality of the owner, type of human-bird interaction and the purpose of keeping a bird may further clarify the impact on FDB.

Regional differences in FDB prevalence and trends in species

The prevalence of FDB among parrots in Japan was estimated at 11.7%, and this largely agrees with prior studies on captive parrots [1214]. The majority of responses were for small-size parrots (e.g., Budgerigars, Cockatiels, Lovebirds), and only a small proportion of responses was seen for medium-size and large parrots. Previous studies conducted in the US, UK, and Italy have shown a high proportion of medium- (e.g., Senegal parrots [Poicephalus senegalus], Quaker parrots [Myiopsitta monachus]) and large-size parrots (e.g., African grey parrots, Cockatoos, Amazon parrots [Amazona spp.], Macaws [Ara spp.]) [1214]. It is of considerable interest that even though the species and numbers of individuals included in the survey population of the present study in Japan were largely different from those of prior studies, despite different captive environments and management in different countries, no large difference was seen in the prevalence of FDB. In addition, species-specific and group FDB prevalence also generally agreed with those in previous studies (Table 2) [1214].

Species was shown to be a significant risk factor in the final model. There were significant differences among species due to interactions among several variables. Species in diverse other taxa are known to differ in their relative vulnerabilities to abnormal repetitive behaviors, including FDB [38], and our findings were consistent with this assumption. Species were also a significant risk factor in other studies, although there were differences in the variables analyzed simultaneously [13]. Our findings also suggest that a certain number of FDB may be observed in each species, regardless of differences in environment and management among countries.

Bird age

Age was found to be a significant risk factor for FDB. The findings suggest that increasing age may be a risk factor for FDB, which is in agreement with the results of prior studies [13]. It has been suggested that most captive parrots show behavioral problems as soon as they reach sexual maturity, and it has been proposed that sexual maturity is the key phase of the onset of FDB in parrots [39]. It has also been proposed that FDB becomes increasingly prevalent in young adulthood, with the incidence plateauing as birds enter adulthood [13]. The higher odds seen in this study for FDB among adults (ORadj = 3.17, P < 0.001) compared with young adults (ORadj = 1.81, P = 0.038) is consistent with these proposals. However, young adults were not a significant variable in univariate analysis (OR = 1.31, P = 0.314), though were a significant variable in the final model. The finding suggests that age is not the only risk factor for FDB, but other factors may play a role. A follow-up cohort study may be needed to clarify further the interrelationships between risk factors of FDB and age-related changes, as the current environment may have changed since the onset of FDB.

Sleeping and cage covered at night times

Sleeping time is potentially very important for FDB. The perception that insufficient sleeping time may contribute to FDB is widely accepted [40]. Conversely, sleeping more than 8 hours has been reported to increase the odds for FDB significantly in African grey parrots [19]. In this study, neither sleeping nor cage covered at night time affected the odds for FDB. However, sleeping time was not clearly defined in this study; thus, the owners may have answered the question for sleeping time based on time the cage was covered, assuming that the bird was resting despite still being in a noisy environment. To investigate the association between sleep time and FDB more accurately, it is recommended to define sleeping time as starting from the time the bird is placed in a dark and uninterrupted quiet area for sleep, and to inquire about the environmental conditions when the cage is covered.

Reproductive behavior

Although sexual frustration has been suggested as a cause of FDB [18, 23], no correlation with FDB was observed in this study. However, the owners may have overlooked reproductive behavior or been unable to identify reproductive behaviors accurately in their birds. Some reproductive behaviors may be simply displacement behaviors that have no correlation with reproductive activity. For example, tearing up paper and substrate has been shown to be related to a lack of foraging opportunities [16]. Therefore, to investigate the relationship between reproductive behavior and FDB more accurately, comparing sex hormone levels in the droppings and/or blood of birds with and without FDB may be necessary.

Rearing method

No difference was seen in the prevalence of FDB by rearing method. It has been reported that FDB is more common in hand- than in parent-reared birds [14, 21]. Alternatively, the number of parrots that were parent-reared may not have been numerous enough to detect any significant differences compared with hand-reared parrots. The proportion of parent-reared parrots with FDB in this study, including those that were handled during rearing, was very small (n = 13). Rearing method is a vital question involving the welfare of birds, and is therefore essential for investigating differences in FDB onset between rearing methods and determining the optimal rearing method for each species.

Signs of separation anxiety

Signs of separation anxiety were a significantly higher risk factor for FDB. Gaskins and Hungerford [22] suggested that adoption (i.e., rescued or rehomed) may be a risk factor for separation anxiety, which could be the underlying cause of FDB. However, in this study, adoption showed no significant difference in the odds of FDB between acquisition sources. We defined signs of separation anxiety by observing behavior when the owner left home or was absent because we predicted that daily rather than permanent separation was the cause of FDB. It has been suggested that loneliness is a cause of FDB [7, 20, 21] because parrots are highly social and depend on flocks in the wild [9, 25]. In this study, neither family size, time with no human presence, presence of other birds and/or animals, nor presence of conspecific cage mates affected the odds of FDB. If birds prefer humans, separation anxiety may occur even if conspecifics are present. In addition, we did not collect information about relationships with other birds or animals. If the birds were afraid of other birds or potential predators such as dogs or cats, this could be a potential risk factor, and the presence of other birds or animals would not prevent separation anxiety. This result also involves interaction with other variables, therefore, to investigate further the environment that causes separation anxiety, it will be necessary to ask whether birds prefer humans or conspecifics and to clarify the relationships with other birds or animals.

Limitations of the study

A survey carried out over the Internet is likely to involve a response bias, so the reliability of the survey results are potentially limited [41]. A limitation of the present study is that rather than a definitive diagnosis of FDB being given by a veterinarian, FDB was diagnosed by the owner. FDB status may have included medical issues (e.g., infection, parasites, skin diseases, neoplasms), reproductive behavior (e.g., brood patch), and molting. It is also possible that cases of latent feather picking or slight feather fraying not clear to the owners were overlooked, or that the owner did not know the correct species. Similar-looking species, such as the genera Amazona and Pyrrhura, or hybrids, may have led to erroneous responses by the owners. Furthermore, it may be more difficult even for an experienced bird owner to recognize different kinds of behavior such as stereotyped behavior, reproductive behavior, or signs of separation anxiety and provide reliable data, than for them to provide reliable data for the other parameters. These issues would weaken the power to detect risk factors or lead to the detection of erroneous risk factors.

Although explanations were given for the definitions of the options for question items regarding bathing/spraying and human foods, the owner might have responded to these items subjectively, which would not preserve the reliability of the data. Future surveys should take care to design questions that can be responded to objectively.

It was not clear whether the parrot’s current environment and management were the same as that before the onset of FDB. When owners attempted to improve the environment and management for FDB treatment, the survey could not be taken as a risk factor for the onset of FDB. Removing this uncertainty by carrying out a chronological study would provide better data on the relationship between risk factors and FDB. In addition, FDB in parrots is generally regarded as a multifactorial disease that may be influenced by a number of medical, genetic, neurobiological and/or socio-environmental factors [7]. The etiological routes of this study approach are mainly environmental and partly neurobiological; thus, a wide range of other etiological routes needs to investigate as potential causes of FDB.

A cross-species approach provides a good source of information for research into general risk factors for FDB in parrots; however, various species or parameters and the limited number of species limit the reliability of the data. In addition, multivariate testing or biases between predictors may omit other predictors, so species-specific potential risk factors may not be accurately represented. A species-specific approach is required to reveal more specific information. Clarifying the environment and management at the time of onset of FDB in a chronological study would be of immense clinical value and would likely reveal which species are better suited to captivity, how they should be reared, and what sort of environment and management should be provided. This would likely be valuable information for the improved welfare of captive birds.

Supporting information

S1 File. Informed consent form.

(PDF)

S2 File. Descriptions of the terms used for each behavior.

(PDF)

S3 File. 2,331 sets of responses included in this study.

(XLSX)

S4 File. 2,331 sets of responses listed true species.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Akitsugu Konno and Yuko Ikkatai for their support in carrying out this study.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its S1S4 Files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Garner JP, Meehan CL, Famula TR, Mench JA. Genetic, environmental, and neighbor effects on the severity of stereotypies and feather picking in Orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica): An epidemiological study. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2006;96: 153–168. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.09.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Clubb S, Cray C, Arheart KL, Goodman M. Comparison of selected diagnostic parameters in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) with normal plumage and those exhibiting Feather-damaging behavior. J Avian Med Surg. 2007;21: 259–264. doi: 10.1647/2006-039R.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mason G. Stereotypic Behaviour in Captive Animals: Fundamentals and Implications for Welfare and Beyond. In: Mason G, Rushen J, editors. Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare. 2nd ed. Wallingford: CAB International; 2006. pp. 325–356. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Meehan C, Mench J. Captive parrot welfare. In: Luescher AU, editor. Manual of Parrot Behavior. Ames, IA: Blackwell Publishing; 2006. pp. 301–318. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Harisson GJ. Disorders of the integument. In: Harrison GJ, Harrison LR, Ritchie BW, editors. Clinical Avian Medicine and Surgery. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders; 1985. pp. 509–524. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jenkins JR. Feather picking and self-mutilation in psittacine birds. Vet Clin North Am Exot Anim Pract. 2001;4: 651–667. doi: 10.1016/s1094-9194(17)30029-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.van Zeeland YRA, Spruit BM, Rodenburg TB, Riedstra B, van Hierden YM, Buitenhuis B, et al. Feather-damaging behaviour in parrots: a review with consideration of comparative aspects. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;121: 75–95. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.09.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rosskopf WJ. Woerpel RW. Feather picking and therapy of skin and feather disorders. In: Rosskopf WJ, Woerpel RW, editors. Diseases of Cage and Aviary birds. 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1996. pp. 397–405. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Seibert LM. Feather-picking disorder in pet birds. In: Luescher AU, editor. Manual of Parrot Behavior. Ames, IA: Blackwell Publishing; 2006. pp. 255–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Garner MM, Clubb SL, Mitchell MA, Brown L. Feather-picking psittacines: histopathology and species trends. Vet Pathol. 2008;45: 401–408. doi: 10.1354/vp.45-3-401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Grindlinger HM. Compulsive feather picking in birds. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48: 857. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810330081012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gaskins LA, Bergman L. Surveys of avian practitioners and pet owners regarding common behavior problems in psittacine birds. J Avian Med Surg. 2011;25: 111–118. doi: 10.1647/2010-027.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McDonald Kinkaid HY, Mills DS, Nichols SG, Rebecca K, Meagher RK, Mason GJ. Feather-damaging behavior in companion parrots: an initial analysis of potential demographic risk factors. Avian Biol Res. 2013;6: 289–296. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Costa P, Macchi E, Tomassone L, Ricceri F, Bollo E, Scaglione FE, et al. Feather picking in pet parrots: sensitive species, risk factor and ethological evidence. Ital J Anim Sci. 2016;15: 473–480. doi: 10.1080/1828051X.2016.1195711 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Levine BS, Practice CA. Common disorders of Amazons, Australian parakeets, and African grey parrots. Semin Avian Exot Pet Med. 2003;12: 125–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Meehan CL, Millam JR, Mench JA. Foraging opportunity and increased physical complexity both prevent and reduce psychogenic feather picking by young Amazon parrots. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;80: 71–85. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00192-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lumeij JT, Hommers CJ. Foraging ‘enrichment’ as treatment for pterotillomania. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;111: 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.05.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lightfoot T, Nacewicz CL. Psittacine behavior. In: Bays TB, Lightfoot T, Mayer J, editors. Exotic Pet Behavior: Birds, Reptiles, and Small Mammals. St Louis, MO: Saunders Elsevier; 2006. pp. 51–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jayson SL, Williams DL, Wood JLN. Prevalence and risk factors of feather plucking in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus and Psittacus erithacus timneh) and cockatoo (Cacatua spp.). J Exot Pet Med. 2014;23: 250–257. doi: 10.1053/j.jepm.2014.06.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Koski MA. Dermatologic diseases in psittacine birds: an investigational approach. Semin Avian Exot Pet Med. 2002;11: 105–124. doi: 10.1053/saep.2002.123981 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schmid R, Doherr MG, Steiger A. The influence of the breeding method on the behaviour of adult African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2006;98: 293–307. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.09.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gaskins LA, Hungerford L. Nonmedical factors associated with feather picking in pet psittacine birds. J Avian Med Surg. 2014;28: 109–117. doi: 10.1647/2012-073R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Davis CS. Parrot psychology and behaviour problems. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. 1991;21: 1281–1289. doi: 10.1016/s0195-5616(91)50138-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rubinstein J, Lightfoot T. Feather loss and feather destructive behaviour in pet birds. J Exot Pet Med. 2012;21: 219–234. doi: 10.1053/j.jepm.2012.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nicol CJ, Pope SJ. A comparison of the behaviour of solitary and group-housed budgerigars. Anim Welf. 1993;2: 268–277. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Greenwell P, Montrose V. The gray matter: Prevention and reduction of abnormal behavior in companion gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus). J Vet Behav. 2017;20: 44–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2017.06.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Meehan CL, Garner JP, Mench JA. Environmental enrichment and development of cage stereotypy in Orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica) Dev Psychobiol. 2004;44: 209–218. doi: 10.1002/dev.20007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Polverino G, Manciocco A, Vitale A, Alleva E. Stereotypic behaviours in Melopsittacus undulatus: behavioural consequences of social and spatial limitations. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;165: 143–55. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.02.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Spoon TR. Parrot Reproductive Behavior, or Who Associates, Who Mates, and Who Cares?. In: Luescher AU, editor. Manual of Parrot Behavior. Ames, IA: Blackwell Publishing; 2006. pp. 63–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.McCrave EA. Diagnostic criteria for separation anxiety in the dog. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. 1991;21: 247–255. doi: 10.1016/s0195-5616(91)50030-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Overall KL, Dunham AE, Frank D. Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety, thunderstorm phobia, and noise phobia, alone or in combination. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2001;219: 467–473. doi: 10.2460/javma.2001.219.467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bergman L, Reinisch US. Parrot Vocalization. In: Luescher AU, editor. Manual of Parrot Behavior. Ames, IA: Blackwell Publishing; 2006. pp. 219–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Akoglu H. User’s guide to correlation coefficients. Turk J Emerg Med. 2018;18: 91–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tjem.2018.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Heck RH, Thomas SL, Tabata LN. Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling with IBM SPSS. New York: Routledge; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cox DR, Wermuth N. Multivariate Dependencies: Models, Analysis and Interpretation. London: Chapman and Hall; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Barton B, Peat J. Medical statistics: a guide to SPSS, data analysis, and critical appraisal. 2nd ed. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Anderson P. A bird in the house: an anthropological perspective on companion parrots. Soc Anim. 2003;11: 393–418. doi: 10.1163/156853003322796109 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mason GJ. Species differences in responses to captivity: stress, welfare and the comparative method. Trends Ecol Evol. 2010;25: 713–721. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wedel A. Verhaltensstörungen bei papageien. In: Wedel A editor. Ziervögel–Erkrankungen, Haltung, Fütterung. Wien: Parey-Verlag; 1999. pp. 284–286. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chitty J. Feather and skin disorders. In: Harcourt-Brown N, Chitty J editors. BSAVA Manual of Psittacine Birds. 2nd ed. Quedgeley: BSAVA Publishing; 2005. pp. 200–203. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dohoo I, Martin W, Stryhn H. Veterinary Epidemiologic Research. Charlottetown: Atlantic Veterinary College; 2003. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

I Anna S Olsson

19 Mar 2021

PONE-D-21-02618

Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Ebisawa,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please see the reviewer comments below for specific changes to be made during the revision process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 03 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

I Anna S Olsson, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

'NO - The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.'

At this time, please address the following queries:

  1. Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution.

  2. State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

  3. If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

  4. If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper is well-written, and based on a very nice dataset that is also well-analysed.

Please see attached document for our complete comments on this manuscript. At this stage I recomend 'Major Revision'

Reviewer #2: As the authors highlighted, feather-damaging behaviour is a serious welfare concern in pet psittacine birds, with still a poor understanding of the risk factors to this behavioural problem; therefore the study has merit to help further our understanding of the potential causes of this problem, and the authors acknowledge the limitations of the study and approach used.

The manuscript is generally well written, clear and to the point. There are a few sentences that need to be checked for English grammar.

Specific comments:

L36: I think you should add the basis for comparison, at least for “young adults” and “adults”, so add “compared to young birds”, as right now we don’t know what those 2 categories compare to.

L37-38: “significantly highly” does not make sense, maybe “significantly higher” but the authors still need to state the basis for comparison, so higher than which other factors, or state that those factors were significant risk factors.

L68-69: not all parrots species live in stable flocks in the wild, some live in pairs, other associates with others at various times of the year depending on, for instance, food availability. Hence, this statement could be nuanced in that parrots are social animals.

L107-108: it could be useful to add the definition provided for those terms, in the text or as a supplementary data file.

Tables 3 and 4: I would find it useful to add a column to show the prevalence of FDB within each species or variable presented; right now you show % across the FDB and non-FDB parrots but this still makes it difficult to compare the prevalence within each species/variable (so comparing the FDB to non-FDB as % within each species) . Adding a column to show this per variable reported would help.

Table 5: I think you have a typo with twice “Bird sex” in the first row, and the second one should probably be corrected for “Bird age”.

L201-202 and elsewhere: I think you should replace “Separation anxiety” by “Signs of separation anxiety”, since separation anxiety per se can probably not be confidently assessed by the owners, but signs of it could be recognized. It is also unclear how separation anxiety was assessed: you mention L295 that it was “by observing behaviour when the owner left home or was absent”, but on which basis were these behavioural observations then conducted? Video-camera recording?

L222-223: I think you could also add the type of human-bird interaction also as an important owner characteristic, given that the human-animal relationship has been shown to have an important impact on the welfare of other animal species, and may have more direct influences on the bird than the owner’s personality for instance.

L.274-275 and elsewhere: “In addition, regurgitation may not be unique to courtship, as it also occurs in anxious or sick birds”: a few statements in the discussion appear to be unsubstantiated such as this one. Please add a reference to support this statement, or some other facts, or alternatively remove those.

L335: should it be “cross-species”?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Georgia Mason

Reviewer #2: Yes: Jean-Loup Rault

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Ref report for Japanese FDB paper.docx

PLoS One. 2021 Jul 14;16(7):e0254610. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254610.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


12 Apr 2021

I Anna S Olsson, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

April 09, 2021

Dear Dr. I Anna S Olsson:

Thank you for inviting us to submit a revised draft of our manuscript entitled, “Prevalence and Risk Factors for Feather-damaging Behavior in Psittacine Birds: Analysis of a Japanese Nationwide Survey” to Journal of PLOS ONE. We also appreciate the time and effort you and each of the reviewers and editor have dedicated to providing insightful feedback on ways to improve our paper. Thus, it is with great pleasure that we resubmit our article for further consideration. We have incorporated changes that reflect the detailed suggestions that you graciously provided. We also hope that our changes and responses below satisfactorily address the issues and concerns that you raised.

To facilitate your review of our revisions, the following is a point-by-point response to the comments delivered in your letter dated March 20, 2021.

Response to reviewer #1

Thank you for your review of our paper. We have answered each of your points below.

METHODS

1. Did you need human ethics approval, for surveying owners? (In my country one would need that)

Response: Approval of human subjects research is required only for medical research in Japan. We confirmed with the Ethics Review Committee of the College of Bioresource Sciences of Nihon University whether approval was necessary for this study, they replied that it was not necessary. We mentioned this in lines 80-83.

2. Re identifying species, any idea how accurate owners were? Was doubt here a reason why “Some of the congeners or closely related species were grouped together because of their similar prevalence of FDB”.? (Lines 121 – 123).

Response: You have raised an important point; We had no way of ascertaining the accuracy of the species response. However, In Japan, the owners usually know the exact species of their birds because pet stores tell owners the exact species. Therefore, we did not doubt the response.

3. Also then in Tables 4 - 6, the term “species” does not really mean “species” then? If not it should be changed to some else (e.g. “Species/genus”??)

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We revised “species” to “Species/genus” on Tables 4 and 6.

4. Lines 122- 124: “Species with a proportion of less than 2% were excluded to clarify the trends in FDB according to species or groups”. I understand this is because you want to ID the risk factors for FDB, but can you clarify whether your overall prevalence value of 11.7% was calculated before or after this exclusion?

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We mentioned in line 130 that the overall prevalence was calculated after this exclusion.

5. Lines 146-147: “The forward selection method based on the likelihood ratio was used to select the variables”.: can you give more details so that someone else could replicate what you did?

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We added SPSS option settings in lines 153-155.

RESULTS

6. Table 5: The interactions need explaining: it’s great that you ran them, but it's not enough to just present the P values – please let the reader know what the pattern was that lead to each significant interaction.

Response: We agree with you and added “The P values of the Chi-square test derived for each potential interaction were also shown in Table 5.” to lines 176-177.

7. Also is one of the two “bird sex” column really “bird age”?

Response: We agree with you and added “type of human-bird interaction” to lines 238-239.

8. Furthermore, where an interaction between terms A and B is significant, that means it’s not OK to talk about any significant main effects of terms A or B, because they probably are just driven by the interaction. This has considerable interactions for the Discussion.

Response: We agree with you and removed “and acquisition source showed only a weak interaction with signs of separation anxiety (Cramer’s V = 0.097, P < 0.001)” and “We cautiously propose that signs of separation anxiety may be affected by species. In fact, signs of separation anxiety showed a strong interaction with species (Cramer’s V = 0.212, P < 0.001)”.

DISCUSSION

9. This is interesting and well written, but needs work because it presents effects of “species” (really “species and genus”), “age” and other effects are presented as though they are simple main effects, when really they have interactive effects so such an interpretation is not valid. For example, species interacts with 8 other terms (see Table 5), which means that effect of species varies in magnitude with these 8 factors (it may hold for some levels of these but not for other). Likwise bird age interacts with three or more other factors (hard to tell because of a possible typo in the columns names of Table 5), such that there is no general pattern of bird age – it varies according to all these other factors.

Response: We agree with you and revised those as follows.

Regional differences in FDB prevalence and trends in species

We added “There were significant differences among species due to interactions among several variables” to lines 254-255 and “Species were also a significant risk factor in other studies, although there were differences in the variables analyzed simultaneously [13]” to lines 257-259.

Bird age

We removed “However, in the present study, there was insufficient information to interpret age as a risk factor”.

We added “However, young adults were not a significant variable in univariate analysis (OR = 1.31, P = 0.314), though were a significant variable in the final model. The finding suggests that age is not the only risk factor for FDB, but other factors may play a role. A follow-up cohort study may be needed to clarify further the interrelationships between risk factors of FDB and age-related changes, as the current environment may have changed since the onset of FDB” to lines 270-275.

Signs of separation anxiety

We removed “and acquisition source showed only a weak interaction with signs of separation anxiety (Cramer’s V = 0.097, P < 0.001)” and “We cautiously propose that signs of separation anxiety may be affected by species. In fact, signs of separation anxiety showed a strong interaction with species (Cramer’s V = 0.212, P < 0.001)”.

We added “This result also involves interaction with other variables, therefore,” to lines 322-323.

10. Line 217 – “for companionship as a substitute for children": isn’t this rather sexist? Maybe older women are just very skilled at keeping animals an enjoy it?

Response: We agree with you and removed “It has been suggested that older women may be overrepresented because they tend to acquire birds for companionship as a substitute for children [38]”. We revised lines 233-235 to “There is a possibility of a potential response bias because older women respond more cooperatively to Internet surveys, or are just very skilled at keeping animals and enjoy it as primary caregivers.”

11. Line 294 “ acquisition source showed only a weak interaction with separation anxiety (Cramer’s V = 0.097, P < 0.001).” Isn’t a P that small indicative of a rather strong interaction? Also what does this interaction mean i.e. what causes it? (Again more information needs to be provided in the Results).

Response: We agree with you revised those as above (Signs of separation anxiety).

Response to reviewer #2

Thank you for your comments. Our answers to your points are as follows.

1. L36: I think you should add the basis for comparison, at least for “young adults” and “adults”, so add “compared to young birds”, as right now we don’t know what those 2 categories compare to.

L37-38: “significantly highly” does not make sense, maybe “significantly higher” but the authors still need to state the basis for comparison, so higher than which other factors, or state that those factors were significant risk factors.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We revised “significantly highly” to “significantly higher”. And we added a comparison not only to bird age, but also to species.

2. L68-69: not all parrots species live in stable flocks in the wild, some live in pairs, other associates with others at various times of the year depending on, for instance, food availability. Hence, this statement could be nuanced in that parrots are social animals.

Response: We agree with you and removed [that live in stable flocks in the wild] from line 70.

3. L107-108: it could be useful to add the definition provided for those terms, in the text or as a supplementary data file.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We added a supplementary data file with a descriptions of the terms used for each behavior (S2_File).

4. Tables 3 and 4: I would find it useful to add a column to show the prevalence of FDB within each species or variable presented; right now you show % across the FDB and non-FDB parrots but this still makes it difficult to compare the prevalence within each species/variable (so comparing the FDB to non-FDB as % within each species). Adding a column to show this per variable reported would help.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We added a column to show the prevalence of FDB within each species or variable presented and removed the columns of % across the FDB and non-FDB parrots in Tables 3 and 4.

5. Table 5: I think you have a typo with twice “Bird sex” in the first row, and the second one should probably be corrected for “Bird age”.

Response: Thank you for finding the typo. We revised “Bird sex” to “Bird age” in a row heading of Table 5.

6. L201-202 and elsewhere: I think you should replace “Separation anxiety” by “Signs of separation anxiety”, since separation anxiety per se can probably not be confidently assessed by the owners, but signs of it could be recognized. It is also unclear how separation anxiety was assessed: you mention L295 that it was “by observing behaviour when the owner left home or was absent”, but on which basis were these behavioural observations then conducted? Video-camera recording?

Response: We agree with you and have incorporated this suggestion throughout our paper. We mentioned in lines 113-116 when the owner observed the behavior of the bird and conditions inside the cage and the owner did not use the video camera recording.

7. L222-223: I think you could also add the type of human-bird interaction also as an important owner characteristic, given that the human-animal relationship has been shown to have an important impact on the welfare of other animal species, and may have more direct influences on the bird than the owner’s personality for instance.

Response: We agree with you and added “type of human-bird interaction” to lines 238-239.

8. L.274-275 and elsewhere: “In addition, regurgitation may not be unique to courtship, as it also occurs in anxious or sick birds”: a few statements in the discussion appear to be unsubstantiated such as this one. Please add a reference to support this statement, or some other facts, or alternatively remove those.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We remove this sentence from line 295.

9. L335: should it be “cross-species”?

Response: We agree with you and revised “across-species” to “cross-species” in lines 162 and 360.

Again, thank you for giving us the opportunity to strengthen our manuscript based on your valuable comments and queries. We have worked hard to incorporate your feedback and hope that you find these revisions sufficient for accepting our submission.

Sincerely,

Kazumasa Ebisawa

Nihon University, College of Bioresource Sciences, Laboratory of Veterinary Physiology

1866 Kameino, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0880, Japan

TEL: +81-466-84-3800

E-mail: yokohamabirdclinic1997@gmail.com

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

Decision Letter 1

I Anna S Olsson

9 May 2021

PONE-D-21-02618R1

Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Ebisawa,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The remaining issues are minor, although important. Reviewer 1 has an important comment about the statistical analysis, which you need to address. As the editor, I find that the text is sometimes a little misleading, which I ask you to address. You will find our detailed comments below.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 23 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

I Anna S Olsson, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

Line 41 "higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than which other potential risk factors" is not grammatically correct - do you mean "higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than any other potential risk factors"?

Line 54 This sentence begins with "Although the cause of FDB is believed to be psychological stress" and then you present a number of potential origins, of which many are indeed situations of psychological stress. "Although" is misleading in this context, as it suggests that the examples are of situations other than psychological stress. You can simply start the sentence with "The cause of FDB".

Lines 73-75  This paragraph is overall about the aim of your study and what you collected data on. It is misleading to say that you focused on regional differences, as the comparison between countries is not a part of your study, but instead refers to a discussion of your results with those of other studies. Please remove this sentence.

Lines 106-116 and 313-4: The way you refer to separation anxiety gives the impression that you asked the owner to observe the behaviour of the bird specifically in the situation of leaving home and returning home. Did you do this? Or did you simply ask the owner how the bird behaves when they leave home and when they return? If you did ask them specifically to observe behaviour to collect data, then your description is correct. If you asked them how the bird behaves but did not ask them to observe this in a specific way, you need to revise the wording.

Section Limitations of the study

Please reflect on the limitations of owner-reported data for behaviour. How reliably do you expect that owners can distinguish the different kinds of behaviour? I would argue that it is more difficult even for an experienced bird owner to recognise different kinds of behaviour and hence provide reliable data, than for them to provide reliable data for the other parameters you collected information on, so I would expect that there is more noise in the behaviour data.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The paper is much improved. The one outstanding issue is that interactions are still handled incorrectly. When a model reveals a significant interaction, ADDITIONAL ANALYSES need to be run to investigate what drives it. An interaction means one of three things: an apparent effect is stronger in some sub-groups than others; an apparent effect is only present in some sub-groups; or an apparent effect flips direction in some sub-groups compared to others. The only way to find out what's happening is id to drill down into the data and run additional analyses using subsets (sorry).

Also could I ask that when you supply the survey results as an SOM, you upload your values as they were BEFORE you pooled them into your (somewhat arbitrary) taxon groups? That way interested researchers could then see values broken up by true species (not genus or higher), which would be very useful for people interested in running meta-analyses.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for this revised version of the manuscript. You have addressed all my comments and suggestions, and I think that it made the manuscript clearer.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Georgia Mason

Reviewer #2: Yes: Jean-Loup Rault

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jul 14;16(7):e0254610. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254610.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


13 May 2021

I Anna S Olsson, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

May 13, 2021

Dear Dr. I Anna S Olsson:

Thank you for inviting us to submit a revised draft of our manuscript entitled, “Prevalence and Risk Factors for Feather-damaging Behavior in Psittacine Birds: Analysis of a Japanese Nationwide Survey” to Journal of PLOS ONE. We also appreciate the time and effort you and each of the reviewers and editor have dedicated to providing insightful feedback on ways to improve our paper. Thus, it is with great pleasure that we resubmit our article for further consideration. We have incorporated changes that reflect the detailed suggestions that you graciously provided. We also hope that our changes and responses below satisfactorily address the issues and concerns that you raised.

To facilitate your review of our revisions, the following is a point-by-point response to the comments delivered in your letter dated May 9, 2021.

Response to Additional Editor

Thank you for your review of our paper. We have answered each of your points below.

1. Line 41 "higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than which other potential risk factors" is not grammatically correct - do you mean "higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than any other potential risk factors"?

Response: Thank you for correcting the grammar. We revised "higher risk factors for feather-damaging behavior than any other potential risk factors".

2. Line 54 This sentence begins with "Although the cause of FDB is believed to be psychological stress" and then you present a number of potential origins, of which many are indeed situations of psychological stress. "Although" is misleading in this context, as it suggests that the examples are of situations other than psychological stress. You can simply start the sentence with "The cause of FDB".

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We revised to start the sentence with "The cause of FDB". We remove reference No.18. We renumbered the reference list.

3. Lines 73-75 This paragraph is overall about the aim of your study and what you collected data on. It is misleading to say that you focused on regional differences, as the comparison between countries is not a part of your study, but instead refers to a discussion of your results with those of other studies. Please remove this sentence.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We remove this sentence. Reference No.12–14 are cited elsewhere.

4. Lines 106-116 and 313-4: The way you refer to separation anxiety gives the impression that you asked the owner to observe the behaviour of the bird specifically in the situation of leaving home and returning home. Did you do this? Or did you simply ask the owner how the bird behaves when they leave home and when they return? If you did ask them specifically to observe behaviour to collect data, then your description is correct. If you asked them how the bird behaves but did not ask them to observe this in a specific way, you need to revise the wording.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We asked the owner how the bird behaves when they leave home and when they return. We revised this sentence to “The presence or absence of signs of separation anxiety was determined by the owner based on the behavior of the bird when the owner leaves home and the condition inside the cage when the owner returns home”.

Section Limitations of the study

5. Please reflect on the limitations of owner-reported data for behaviour. How reliably do you expect that owners can distinguish the different kinds of behaviour? I would argue that it is more difficult even for an experienced bird owner to recognise different kinds of behaviour and hence provide reliable data, than for them to provide reliable data for the other parameters you collected information on, so I would expect that there is more noise in the behaviour data.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We added the sentence “it may be more difficult even for an experienced bird owner to recognize different kinds of behavior such as stereotyped behavior, reproductive behavior, or signs of separation anxiety and provide reliable data, than for them to provide reliable data for the other parameters”.

Response to reviewer #1

Thank you for your review of our paper. We have answered each of your points below.

1. The paper is much improved. The one outstanding issue is that interactions are still handled incorrectly. When a model reveals a significant interaction, ADDITIONAL ANALYSES need to be run to investigate what drives it. An interaction means one of three things: an apparent effect is stronger in some sub-groups than others; an apparent effect is only present in some sub-groups; or an apparent effect flips direction in some sub-groups compared to others. The only way to find out what's happening is id to drill down into the data and run additional analyses using subsets (sorry).

Response: I apologize for using the inappropriate term "interaction". We used the phi coefficient or Cramer’s V to measure correlations between categorical variables where P < 0.05. Therefore, we revised the term "interaction" to "correlation". Since multivariate logistic regression was used to adjust the confounding of the variables, we think that sub-group analysis is not necessary. We apologize again for the confusion.

2. Also could I ask that when you supply the survey results as an SOM, you upload your values as they were BEFORE you pooled them into your (somewhat arbitrary) taxon groups? That way interested researchers could then see values broken up by true species (not genus or higher), which would be very useful for people interested in running meta-analyses.

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We added a supplementary file with the true species and scientific names. We added the sentence “The true species before grouping is described in the S4 file” to Lines 127-128.

Response to reviewer #2

Thank you for your review of our paper.

Reference list

The reference number 18 has been retracted because line 54 has been deleted. Therefore, the reference list has changed. We reviewed reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct.

Again, thank you for giving us the opportunity to strengthen our manuscript based on your valuable comments and queries. We have worked hard to incorporate your feedback and hope that you find these revisions sufficient for accepting our submission.

Sincerely,

Kazumasa Ebisawa

Nihon University, College of Bioresource Sciences, Laboratory of Veterinary Physiology

1866 Kameino, Fujisawa, Kanagawa 252-0880, Japan

TEL: +81-466-84-3800

E-mail: yokohamabirdclinic1997@gmail.com

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

Decision Letter 2

I Anna S Olsson

30 Jun 2021

Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey

PONE-D-21-02618R2

Dear Dr. Ebisawa,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

I Anna S Olsson, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

I Anna S Olsson

2 Jul 2021

PONE-D-21-02618R2

Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey

Dear Dr. Ebisawa:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. I Anna S Olsson

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Informed consent form.

    (PDF)

    S2 File. Descriptions of the terms used for each behavior.

    (PDF)

    S3 File. 2,331 sets of responses included in this study.

    (XLSX)

    S4 File. 2,331 sets of responses listed true species.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Ref report for Japanese FDB paper.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its S1S4 Files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES