Highlights
-
•
Prior studies document effects of lifestyle behaviors on the brain and academics.
-
•
Among adolescents we found that activity and eating both predict task performance.
-
•
Activity also predicts functional activation in the right lateral dlPFC, but not grades.
-
•
Substance use predicted worse grades but not brain-related mediators.
Keywords: PFC, Cortex, Academic achievement, Adolescents, fNIRS, Health behaviours
Abstract
Several adolescent health behaviours have been hypothesized to improve academic performance via their beneficial impact on cognitive control and functional aspects of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). The primary objective of this study is to examine the association between lifestyle behaviours and academic performance in a sample of adolescents, and to examine the extent to which activity within the PFC and behavioural indices of inhibition may mediate this relationship. Sixty-seven adolescents underwent two study sessions five days apart. Sleep and physical activity were measured using wrist-mounted accelerometry; eating habits, substance use and academic achievement were measured by self-report. Prefrontal function was quantified by Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT) performance, and task-related activity via functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Higher levels of physical activity predicted higher MSIT accuracy scores (β = .321, ρ = 0.019) as well as greater activation within the right dlPFC (b = .008, SE = .004, ρ = .0322). Frequency of fast-food consumption and substance use were negatively associated with MSIT accuracy scores (β = −0.307, ρ = .023) and Math grades (b = −3.702, SE = 1.563, ρ = .022), respectively. Overall, the results of this study highlight the importance of lifestyle behaviours as predictors of prefrontal function and academic achievement in youth.
1. Introduction
Strong academic performance depends on focused attention, working memory, and planfulness, all of which are partially supported by the lateral and medial subregions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Smithers et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2019a; Langner and Eickhoff, 2013; Emch et al., 2019; Nitschke et al., 2017). Longitudinal (Samuels et al., 2016; Dubuc et al., 2020) as well as cross-sectional (Colom et al., 2007; Thorell et al., 2013) studies have found reliable but modest associations between behavioural measures of these functions and academic performance (Lan et al., 2011). Other factors —including sleep, eating, substance use and physical activity— have been shown to influence functional aspects of the PFC (Lowe et al., 2017; Gustavson et al., 2017a; Ames et al., 2014; Davis et al., 2011). Therefore, it is possible that these health behaviours could also affect academic performance among adolescents.
A recent meta-analysis found that sleep restriction has a significant and moderate negative effect on cognitive performance across the lifespan (Lowe et al., 2017). These findings are also supported by imaging studies examining indicators of functional connectivity (Zhao et al., 2019b; Borragán et al., 2019). Likewise a high calorie, low nutrient diet has been shown to negatively impact performance on executive function tasks (Ames et al., 2014; Groppe and Elsner, 2014). Among adolescent girls, a higher Body Mass Index (BMI) correlates with greater impulsivity in response to inhibition tasks as well as reduced activation in the superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), medial PFC (mPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC; Batterink et al., 2010). There is also evidence that substance use and substance use disorders can be detrimental to brain regions implicated in executive functioning among adolescents. Executive functioning has been shown to be weaker among habitual users of cocaine, amphetamines, cannabis, tobacco and alcohol (Gustavson et al., 2017b). In addition, youth with a history of alcohol and cannabis use demonstrate less activation in the inferior frontal cortex, but enhanced mPFC response when completing a working memory task (Schweinsburg et al., 2005). The deleterious effects of substance use can potentially persist into early adulthood. When followed for a period of ten years, young adults with a history of alcohol use disorder or a substance use disorder demonstrated poorer performance on verbal and visual learning and memory tasks as well as reduced executive functioning compared to non-users (Hanson et al., 2011).
In terms of mechanisms, use and abuse of recreational drugs may trigger neurobehavioral cascades involving altered dopamine signalling, negative emotionality and behavioral dysregulation (Volkow et al., 2019), which may be broadly similar to the changes that are elicited by over-indulgence in calorie-dense foods (Volkow et al., 2008). Studies involving neuromodulation of cortical centres involved in executive control suggest causal connections exist for many of such appetitive substances (Jansen et al., 2013). In addition, sleep restriction, a poor diet and substance use have all been shown to detrimentally impact academic performance in adolescents (Burrows et al., 2017; Urrila et al., 2017; Bugbee et al., 2019). Because each of these lifestyle behaviours have been shown to have a negative relationship with indicators of executive function (e.g., task performance and cortical network engagement), it is plausible that the relationship between each factor and academic achievement may be mediated (in part) through the brain.
In contrast to the apparent adverse effects of the above mentioned health behaviours, both acute and regular physical activity have been shown to enhance some parameters of PFC function and improve task performance on cognitive tasks that tap executive control (Hillman et al., 2008, 2009; 2014). Physical activity training programs have improved the functional and cognitive capacity of older adults (Groot et al., 2016; Kramer and Colcombe, 2018; Ji et al., 2019), and these effects appear especially important for brain regions supporting executive control and memory (Davis et al., 2011; Hillman et al., 2008; Kramer and Colcombe, 2018; Erickson et al., 2011; Kramer and Erickson, 2007). Brain health benefits of physical activity may be present throughout the lifespan, and yet, especially important for adolescents whom may rely on such functions in the academic sphere (Åberg et al., 2009; Hillman et al., 2008, 2014; 2019; Westfall et al., 2018). While the precise pathway through which physical activity influences brain health remains unclear, it is generally thought to increase the production of growth factors critical for synaptic plasticity, angiogenesis and the development of new neuronal architecture, and changes in cerebrovascular dynamics (Hillman et al., 2008). In addition to functional improvements, there is some evidence to suggest that physical activity may contribute to structural changes in the lateral regions of the PFC (Best et al., 2017; Northey et al., 2020). In adolescents, systematic reviews and meta-analyses on the effects of physical activity on executive functions have found net positive effects (Donnelly et al., 2016; Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017a; Cerrillo‐Urbina et al., 2015; Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2015; Verburgh et al., 2013) with acute aerobic exercise and in the moderate to vigorous range producing the strongest benefits (Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017a; Cerrillo‐Urbina et al., 2015; Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2015; Verburgh et al., 2013).
There is also evidence to support a relationship between greater levels of physical activity and adaptive brain activation during cognitive task performance. A recent study investigating the effects of acute physical activity on the cognitive function of older adults found significantly greater activation in the right and left dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) post-exercise session during an interference task (Ji et al., 2019). Although there are very few studies that investigate this topic in children or adolescents, it appears that there is a reliable difference between higher- and lower-fit children. Higher-fit children have been shown to exhibit superior performance on executive performance tasks as well as increased activity in the fronto-parietal regions of the brain (Davis et al., 2011; Khan and Hillman, 2014; Chaddock et al., 2012). Given the rapid neural development in adolescence, the perceived cognitive benefits of physical activity, both in terms of performance on executive function tasks and through enhanced brain activation, during this critical period may be especially important for the progression of a healthy neurocognitive structure and function into adulthood (Meijer et al., 2020).
Furthermore, the cognitive benefits of exercise could positively impact academic achievement, through its beneficial effects on brain systems that support attentional control, memory and behavioural self-regulation (Hillman et al., 2014, 2009; Kramer and Erickson, 2007). This “brain benefit” hypothesis postulates that the cognitive enhancements within the PFC could translate into improved academic performance because achievement in school in part relies on executive functions. Currently, the bulk of evidence in support of the relationship between physical activity and academic achievement suggests a reliable but weak association between the two variables. Systematic and meta-analytic reviews of the literature have shown variable results ranging from null to small positive effects of physical activity interventions (both acute and long-term) on academic performance (Donnelly et al., 2016; Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017b; Watson et al., 2017; Vetter et al., 2020; Singh et al., 2018). However, among the studies reviewed there is a large degree of heterogeneity in intervention components assessed, a high degree of variability in the quality of the study designs, and a limited number of studies with sufficient power (Donnelly et al., 2016; Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017b; Watson et al., 2017). More problematic is the inability to achieve blinding (single or double) when assessing physical activity interventions. This also applies to studies involving exercise effects on the brain, which can lead to expectancy effects and therefore an over-estimation of a brain health benefit (in both cognitive testing and functional imaging). In addition, relatively few randomized trials exist that examine the brain health benefits of exercise in children, and the few that are present have mixed results (Davis et al., 2011; Tarp et al., 2016). It is possible that over-estimations cloud the true effect of physical activity on the brain, and that the cognitive enhancements of physical activity are not potent enough to influence academic achievement in adolescents. Therefore, further investigations into the mediating role of the brain are warranted.
In contrast to the “brain benefit” hypothesis, the de-emphasis on physical activity programs in schools across North America in favour of other core subjects (i.e., Math, Science, Languages) was often rationalised based on an assumption that physical activity programming competes for time with academic subjects (Schneider and Zhang, 2013). This perspective implies a negative effect of physical activity on academic performance based on time competition between the two (Tremblay et al., 2000). The “brain-benefit” and “time-competition” perspectives pertaining to physical activity and academic performance suggest a significant relationship, but in opposite directions. However, if both dynamics exist, the stronger of the two will determine the net effects (net benefit or net cost). Some studies have found a null (Faught et al., 2019; Hansen et al., 2014) or even slightly negative relationship between physical activity and achievement (Tremblay et al., 2000; Papasideris et al., 2021). Therefore, it could be that the variability of direction of association within the current body of literature stems from competition between the two hypotheses.
The emergence of new portable brain imaging technologies—particularly functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS)—may prove to be more logistically feasible for many types of research studies involving the brain and development and when field settings are required for data collection (Ayaz et al., 2013; Curtin and Ayaz, 2018). This technology is comparable to functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) in that it measures blood oxygenation parameters for inferring neuronal activity within brain regions (Cui et al., 2011). Although it has inferior spatial resolution to fMRI, it has better spatial resolution than Electroencephalogram (EEG; 4344). fNIRS also is less subject to motion artifacts than both EEG and fMRI (Pinti et al., 2018; Scholkmann et al., 2014) and its portability allows for it to be deployed in field settings more flexibly than any other brain imaging option currently available. Because this technology is safe for use in an age group wherein the brain is still undergoing significant maturational changes (Ayaz et al., 2013; Giedd et al., 1999), there are some advantages of fNIRS for imaging the prefrontal cortex and its subregions in adolescence. For these reasons, fNIRS is an ideal imaging modality to investigate cortical function in a sample of adolescents within a field setting.
The current study examines the relationship between adolescent health behaviours and academic performance as mediated by the brain health parameters; specifically, medial and lateral prefrontal function. Physical activity and sleep hours were assessed using accelerometers, while the other health behaviours (i.e. substance use, fast-food consumption) and academic performance were measured via self-report. The brain health parameters consist of fNIRS measurements of task-related functional activation patterns within subregions of the PFC as well as cognitive interference task performance (i.e., Multi-Source Interference Task; MSIT; Bush et al., 2003). Bush et al. identified lateral activation within the PFC as a result of completing the MSIT task (Bush et al., 2003), and so it is hypothesized that increased lateral activation in addition to enhanced task performance may mediate the relationship between higher levels of physical activity, greater sleep hours, as well as less frequent substance use and fast-food consumption and greater academic performance. Because age, sex, and BMI are known to influence structural and functional aspects of prefrontal cortex development, we examine each of these above hypotheses for moderating effects of these demographic variables (Giedd et al., 1999; Maayan et al., 2011; Cosgrove et al., 2007). In addition, English and Math grades were used as indicators of academic achievement because they have been shown in previous research to be differentially affected by physical activity and other lifestyle behaviours (Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017b; Faught et al., 2019).
2. Material and methods
2.1. Participants and setting
A sample of 67 adolescent high school students between the ages of 13–18, were recruited for this study. Participants were recruited from one public and three private high schools located in small to medium-sized urban areas. Principals, teachers and/or key members of administration disseminated information and consent materials to the student body. Those students who wished to participate returned their signed parental consent forms to the administration helping to facilitate the study, or to the student researcher upon the first study session. Participants also provided assent upon initiation of the first session. Demographic features of the sample are presented in Table 1. This study was approved by the host institutional and public school research ethics review boards.
Table 1.
Mean and SDs for the study sample characteristics.
Variable | N | % | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | |||
Female | 26 | 38.8 | |
Male | 40 | 59.7 | |
Non-response | 1 | 1.49 | |
Age | |||
13 | 2 | 2.99 | |
14 | 13 | 19.4 | |
15 | 6 | 8.96 | |
16 | 15 | 22.39 | |
17 | 26 | 38.81 | |
18 | 5 | 7.46 | |
Substance use (times in past month) | |||
0 times | 46 | 68.66 | |
1−2 times | 10 | 14.93 | |
3−5 times | 1 | 1.49 | |
6+ times | 10 | 14.93 | |
Variable | Mean | SD | |
Fast-food consumption (times in past week) | 1.91 | 1.44 | |
Grades (%) | |||
English | 80.91 | 9.57 | |
Math | 79.30 | 12.08 | |
Average sleep hours | 7.16 | 1.05 | |
Average steps counts | 8914.28 | 3451.44 | |
Average active minutes | 32.39 | 28.46 | |
MSIT % correct responses | 0.88 | 0.06 |
2.2. Procedure
The current study was a 5-day prospective observational study with two on-site data collection visits. During each visit, an open period was held during school hours where students could drop in and participate on their own time. This was necessary in order to ensure that data collection took place during free periods and did not impact class time, and that students could participate on-site with minimal inconvenience.
During the first session, students underwent an assent procedure upon presentation of their signed parental consent form. Next a questionnaire consisting of 6 questions pertaining to demographic information (age and gender), eating behaviours, substance use and academic performance was completed. To measure academic performance, participants were asked to report their past years (2018–2019) English and Math grades in percentages. Finally, students were fitted with the fNIRS headband while a cognitive interference task (Multi-Source Interference Task; MSIT; Bush and Shin, 2006) was performed. All computer tasks were presented using Inquisit software version 5.0.14.0 (Millisecond Software). In total, this first session took approximately 20 min per student. Upon completion of the cognitive task, students were given a Fitbit Inspire watch, oriented to its correct usage, and instructed to wear it consistently until the second data-collection period 5 days later.
The second period took place on the following Friday. During this session, students were asked to return their Fitbit and indicate any instances (day, time and duration) when the watch was removed during the period since the first session.
2.3. Demographics, health behaviours, and academic performance
Participants were asked to report their age and gender. In addition, students were asked “how many times have you eaten “fast-food” (e.g. McDonalds, Burger King, etc.) in past week?” as a measure of calorie dense food consumption. Participants were also be asked “how many times have you experimented in the past month with substances (e.g., alcohol, cannabis, other)?”and responded using a scale ranging from 0, 1–2, 3–5, 6+. To measure academic performance, students were asked “What was the final grade that you received last year (2018–2019) in Math class?” and “What was the final grade that you received last year (2018–2019) in English class?” Students were then able to indicate their English and Math grade in percentage.
2.4. Multi-source interference task
Participants completed the MSIT as a measure of response inhibition (Bush and Shin, 2006; Cosgrove et al., 2007). For this task, each trial consisted of three numbers (one target and two distractors) that were horizontally aligned in the centre of a black computer screen in bold white 50 pt font (See Fig. 1). A “+” sign was presented between trials and in the centre of the screen in between 1.75 s trial intervals. The numbers corresponded to the “1, 2 and 3” numbered computer keys, and participants were instructed to indicate the unique number in the triplet by pressing the corresponding key using their dominant hand.
Fig. 1.
MSIT control and interference trial examples (Bush et al., 2003).
Note. Participants required to indicate which number differed from the other two using the keyboard. Target number are presented within the correct sequence (i.e., 2 is in the second position) during the control trials, and distractors were always “0.” During interference trials, the target numbers are not in sequence (i.e., 2 not in the second position) and distractors are other possible target numbers
Control and interference trials differed by the type of distractor used as well as the position of the target numbers in relation to their location on the keyboard. In control trials, the target number always matched their location on the keyboard, and the distractors were never used as targets. During interference trials, the target number never matched its position on the keyboard and the distractors were also targets. During the initial orientation to the MSIT task, participants were asked to respond as quickly and accurately as possible in response to each number stimulus. A 1.5-minute practice trial of 48 trials initiated the task (Bush et al., 2003; Bush and Shin, 2006). Participants then completed 4 blocks of 24 control and interference trials for a total of 96 trials of each type. There was a fixed order of trials within each block, but blocks were counterbalanced between participants (i.e. either CICICICI or ICICICIC, where “C” denotes the control block and “I” denotes the interference block). A 30 s rest period with a fixation cross was included at the beginning of the first block and at the end of the last block (Bush et al., 2003; Bush and Shin, 2006).
Participants required to indicate which number differed from the other two using the keyboard. Target number are presented within the correct sequence (i.e., 2 is in the second position) during the control trials, and distractors were always “0”. During interference trials, the target numbers are not in sequence (i.e., 2 not in the second position) and distractors are other possible target numbers.
2.5. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy
fNIRS is an optical neuroimaging technique which non-invasively measures activation of the cortex using near-infrared (NIR) light (Pinti et al., 2018; Scholkmann et al., 2014). In order to measure regional activation, fNIRS relies on metabolic related increases local arteriolar vasodilation, which in turn produces an increases in oxygenated hemoglobin (OxyHb) as well as a (slightly time lagged) relative decrease in deoxygenated hemoglobin (DeoxyHb; Pinti et al., 2018). Because hemoglobin is the main chromophore that absorbs NIR light and does so differently when oxygenated (>800 nm) versus deoxygenated (<800 nm), fNIRS can utilise the spectroscopic features of hemoglobin in order to infer regional brain activation (Pinti et al., 2018).
For this study, activation within the prefrontal cortical regions was assessed using the fNIR Devices 203C imaging system (See Fig. 2) during the MSIT task. A band containing the optodes was fitted using anatomical landmarks to ensure coverage of the medial and lateral prefrontal cortex and secured in place using fabric band with Velcro fasteners. This band included 4 embedded light sources and 8 detectors, creating 16 measurement channels (25 mm apart) plus 2 short measurement channels (10 mm apart). The system collected data at 10 Hz sampling rate for both 730 nm and 850 nm as well as ambient light intensity and two additional short distance channels. The dependent variable was the change in OxyHb between MSIT control and interference trials in each target area. Binning was employed to identify a local baseline consisting of the first 2 s of each block, and an activation window of 8 s (i.e., seconds 2–10; approximately 5 trials); these were marked manually. Increase in OxyHb from local baseline (0 s–2 s) to the activation window (2 s–10 s) was the primary metric of interest. Four regions of interests (ROI) were identified, corresponding to conceptually important subregions of the prefrontal cortex. Channels 3, 4 and 6 make up the left dlPFC (L-dlPFC); 13, 14 and 15 the right dlPFC (R-dlPFC); 7, 8 the left mPFC (L-mPFC) and 9, 10 the right mPFC (R-mPFC). Raw light intensities in the 730 nm and 850 nm wavelengths as well as ambient light intensities were recorded using the COBI Studio software (Ayaz et al., 2011). Each participant’s data was checked for any potential saturation (when light intensity at the detector was higher than the analog-to-digital converter limit) and motion artifact contamination by means of a coefficient of variation based assessment, sliding window motion artifact rejection (Ayaz et al., 2010). A low-pass finite impulse response filter with an order of 100 and cut-off frequency of 0.1 Hz was designed using Hamming window and was subsequently applied to the light intensity measures to attenuate high frequency noise and physiological nuisance signals (i.e. heartbeat, respiration). Using the modified Beer-Lambert law, raw light intensities were then converted into OxyHb and DeoxyHb concentrations (Cope and Delpy, 1988). fNIRS data for eight training blocks (4 control and 4 interference) of 42 s were extracted using synchronization markers. The hemodynamic response at each optode was baseline corrected using the linear subtraction of the signal level at the beginning of each trial block, and finally averaged across time for each trial block to provide a mean hemodynamic response at each channel for each block.
Fig. 2.
Regions of the prefrontal cortex corresponding to the 16 fNIRS channels.
Note. Channels indicating lateral prefrontal region of interest outlined in red; channels indicating medial prefrontal region of interest outlined in yellow.
2.6. Accelerometry
At the conclusion of the first 20-minute session, each participant was provided a Fitbit Inspire watch to wear until the second session on the following Friday. Each Fitbit watch was embedded with an triaxial accelerometery sensor that measured linear acceleration along three orthogonal axes (X, Y and Z) and can detect movement including gravity (Collins et al., 2019). The accelerometry sensor was used to determine active minutes and sleep hours. The devices employed an estimate of metabolic equivalents (MET) and a MET over 3 was used to determine activity. A proprietary algorithm (intrinsic to the device) was then used to classify active minutes into “lightly active, fairly active and very active” (Collins et al., 2019; Hartman et al., 2018). Combination of “fairly light” and “very active” minutes per day defined the physical activity variable of interest. Sleep hours were inferred by (lack of) movement identified by the accelerometery sensor for more than one hour continuously, also identified using a proprietary scoring interpretative algorithm (Collins et al., 2019; Haghayegh et al., 2020). Participants were instructed to wear the Fitbit consistently Monday to Friday, day and night. Each Fitbit Inspire watch was linked to a Fitbit account so that physical activity and sleep data could be accessed remotely and recorded. The Fitbits were synced using the Fitbit app once during the first and last sessions. During the final session, participants were asked to drop in and return their Fitbit watch and specify whether or not the watch was removed during the week using a weekly calendar.
2.7. Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS. The Explore subcommand in SPSS was used to generate Boxplots and distributional statistics; together these were employed to assess skewness and kurtosis for each individual variable, and to any extreme outliers that may be present. One extreme outlier was removed from the fast-food consumption variable and three extreme outliers were removed from the % correct MSIT responses variable. English grades, the % correct MSIT responses, the mean MSIT RT and MSIT SD, were subjected to 90 % winsorization. Six values were winsorized for the English grade variable and for the MSIT % correct responses, RT and SD (3 above and 3 below the 95 % and 5 % percentiles, respectively).
Daily active minutes and sleep hours were average across three full days of wear. Participant data was averaged across the number of days with available data (i.e., if activity data lost for one out of three days, the counts were averaged across 2 days). The active minutes data for 6 participants as well as the sleep hours for 14 participants were lost either because the watch was not returned or due to the removal of the watch overnight.
To calculate the fNIRS indicators of oxygenation in each ROI the mean change in OxyHb was calculated for the interference and control blocks of the MSIT separately and for each channel. These mean values of oxygenation were then transformed into Z scores and the mean of the Z scores were calculated for all channels making up each ROI. All ROI aggregates were then subjected to winsorization, and 4 values for each ROI were winsorized (2 above and below the 95 % and 5 % percentiles, respectively). Following winsorization, all distributions for all variables exhibited improved normality including skewness statistics below values of 2.0 in SPSS.
Hierarchical linear regression models were employed in order to examine the relationship between each lifestyle behaviour (i.e., physical activity, sleep hours, fast-food consumption frequency and substance use frequency) and MSIT performance (% correct responses) while controlling for age. The PROCESS macro was utilised run moderated regression analyses in order to examine the moderating effect of gender and BMI on the above models (i.e., all lifestyle factors and MSIT % correct responses). Multiple mediation models were then utilised to assess the potential mediating effect of the brain health parameters (MSIT indicators, fNIRS ROI oxygenation) while controlling for the % correct responses. Finally, conditional process models assessed whether the above multiple mediation models differed based on gender, age or BMI. An estimate of BMI was calculated by the Fitbit watches upon the first day of wear, and relied on physical characteristics (year of birth, height, weight, sex) supplied by the participants.
Initial predictive models were fitted using multi-level modelling of within-person effects pertaining to trials within blocks and blocks within task. Modelling these nested effects did not significantly improve model fit, and so the primary analyses presented below utilised data averaged across trials and blocks of the same type. This enabled the use of multiple mediation models, using an ordinary least squares regression approach, which forms the primary analytic approach in the sections below. Given the focus of the study on executive control and evaluative processes, our functional imaging analyses were primarily focussed on oxygenated hemoglobin levels in interference blocks.
Initial analyses show lifestyle behaviours as predictors of interference task performance (% correct MSIT responses). These are followed by multiple mediation models testing simultaneous mediational effects of a given target behaviour (e.g., activity, sleep, etc.) on an academic performance outcome (e.g., Math grades or English grades) through all brain primary health parameters (e.g., MSIT reaction mean reaction times; MSIT reaction time variability; fNIRS parameters), while controlling for MSIT task performance. Subsequent conditional processed models explored the extent to which the multiple mediation models were moderated by age, gender and BMI. fNIRS channels were combined into neuroanatomically relevant ROIs, corresponding to the L-dlPFC, R-dlPFC, L-mPFC, and R-mPFC. All multiple mediation and conditional process models focussed on these four regions of interest.
3. Results
3.1. Lifestyle predictors of interference task performance
Means and standard deviations (SD), as well as N and % for categorical variables, for all sample characteristics and primary study variables are presented in Table 1. The majority of students were aged 16–17 (61.2 %) and Male (59.7 %).
Fig. 3 represents the average activation for both OxyHb and DeoxyHb for both interference and control tasks. To examine the extent to which MSIT performance was predicted by each target lifestyle behaviour, separate regressions were run using age as a covariate and each lifestyle behaviour as a predictor. Findings are presented in Fig. 4. Both fast-food consumption and accelerometer-assessed active minutes were significant predictors of MSIT performance. Specifically, more average daily minutes of accelerometer-assessed physical activity predicted greater % correct MSIT responses (β = .321, ρ = .019); likewise, less frequent fast-food consumption in the previous week predicted significantly greater % correct MSIT responses (β = −0.307, ρ = .023). A reduced model predicting MSIT performance was created containing only average active minutes and fast-food consumption. This reduced model accounted for 24 % of the variability in MSIT performance (ΔR2 = .244, ρ < 0.001). In the reduced model, higher average daily minutes of accelerometer-assessed physical activity predicted greater % correct MSIT responses (β = .382, ρ = .004). Likewise, less frequent fast-food consumption in the past week predicted significantly higher % correct MSIT responses (β = −0.429, ρ = .001).
Fig. 3.
Average a) OxyHb and b) DeoxyHb for interference and control blocks of the MSIT.
Note. Interference blocks are represented in orange, and control blocks are represented in blue.
Fig. 4.
Lifestyle behaviours predicting MSIT performance, controlling for age.
To determine whether the strength of association between each predictor and MSIT performance differed for male and female participants, moderated regression analyses were performed separately for each target behaviour. Gender was a significant moderator of the relationship between active minutes and MSIT performance (ΔR2 = .077, F = 4.939 (1,54), ρ = .031), such that active minutes had a significant effect on MSIT performance for females (β = 1.018, SE = .412, ρ = .017) but not for males (β = .041, SE = .155, ρ = .793). The corresponding effect sizes were 0.061 for males and 0.384 for females (Fig. 5). Gender was not a significant moderator of the relationship between fast-food consumption and MSIT performance (ΔR2 = .011, F = .699 (1,59), ρ = .407). There was also no significant moderating effect of gender on the relationship between sleep (ΔR2 = .043, F = 2.194 (1,47), ρ = .145) or substance use (ΔR2 = .000, F = .010 (1,59), ρ = .919) and MSIT performance.
Fig. 5.
Effect sizes for average active minutes predicting MSIT performance (% correct).
Additional moderated regression analyses were performed to determine whether the strength of association between each predictor and MSIT performance differed by body composition (quantified by BMI). BMI was calculated using percentile-based age and gender-specific cut-offs recommended by Centers for Disease Control and Prevention for children and adolescents (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017). Results indicated that BMI was indeed a significant moderator of the relationship between substance use and MSIT performance (ΔR2 = .127, F = 8.916 (1,59), ρ = .004). The corresponding effect sizes were 0.119 for those for those whose BMI fell within the obese range and −.095 for non-obese (Fig. 6). BMI was not a significant moderator of the relationship between active minutes (ΔR2 = .008, F = .492 (1,55), ρ = .486), fast-food consumption (ΔR2 = .019, F = 1.171 (1,58), ρ = .284), or average sleep hours (ΔR2 = .027, F = 1.384 (1,48), ρ = .245) and MSIT performance.
Fig. 6.
Effect sizes for substance use predicting MSIT performance (% correct).
Note. Age and gender-specific cut-offs were employed to define obesity categories as recommended by CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017).
3.2. Multiple mediation models
In order to examine mediational processes predicting academic achievement from target lifestyle behaviours via candidate prefrontal function mediators (MSIT parameters, fNIRS ROI), multiple mediation models were fitted using the PROCESS Macro in SPSS. This analysis was completed separately for each lifestyle behaviour (i.e., average sleep hours, average active minutes, fast-food consumption and substance use) and each academic outcome variable (i.e., English and Math grades), while controlling for % correct MSIT responses.
3.2.1. Math grades
3.2.1.1. Physical activity multiple mediation model
Fig. 7 depicts the multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from average active minutes through prefrontal function parameters. There was a significant effect of average active minutes on R-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .008, SE = .004, ρ = .032), but no effect of average active minutes on L-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .003, SE = .003, ρ = .295), L-mPFC OxyHb (b = .003, SE = .003, ρ = .401), or R-mPFC OxyHb (b = − .003, SE = .003, ρ = .266). Fig. 8a) displays a heat map showing channel specific effects of average active minutes, and Fig. 8b) shows areas of significant activation overlaid on a 3-dimentional anatomical brain. There was also no direct effect of average active minutes on Math grades (b = .106, SE = .070, ρ = .139), and no effect of average active minutes on either the MSIT mean RT (b = −.006, SE = .005, ρ = .184), or on the MSIT SD (b = .002, SE = .004, ρ = .676). None of the brain health parameters were significant predictors of Math grades.
Fig. 7.
Multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from accelerometer-assessed active minutes of physical activity through brain health parameters, controlling for MSIT % correct responses.
Fig. 8.
a) Heat map portraying F statistics for the effect of average active minutes on task-related change in OxyHb levels in each channel (yellow denotes significant effects; =0.05) and b) anatomical position of significant effects (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article).
The indirect effect of average active minutes on Math grades through R-dlPFC OxyHb was not significant; the upper and lower bound for the 95 % confidence interval for the indirect effect included zero (est. = .011 (SE = .025); CILL = −0.039, CIUL = .064), suggesting a null mediational effect. None of the other indirect effects involving prefrontal function parameters were significant (See supplementary materials).
3.2.1.2. Substance use multiple mediation model
Fig. 9 depicts the multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from substance use through brain health parameters. There was a significant direct effect of substance use on Math grades; specifically, more frequent substance use in the past month predicted significantly worse math grades (b = −3.702, SE = 1.563, ρ = .022). There was no effect of substance use on either the MSIT mean RT (b = .017, SE = .112, ρ = .883), or on the MSIT SD (b = .159, SE = .099 , ρ = .113). There was also no significant effect of substance use on L-dlPFC OxyHb (b = −.015, SE = .064, ρ = .813), R-dlPFC OxyHb (b = −.107, SE = .090, ρ = .240), L-mPFC OxyHb (b = −.106, SE = .074, ρ = .176), or R-mPFC OxyHb (b = −.036, SE = .067, ρ = .630). None of the prefrontal function parameters were significant predictors of Math grades, and none of the indirect effects involving prefrontal parameters were significant (See supplementary materials).
Fig. 9.
Multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from self-reported substance use through brain health parameters, controlling for MSIT % correct responses.
3.2.1.3. Fast-food consumption multiple mediation model
A multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from fast-food consumption through prefrontal function parameters found no direct effect of fast-food consumption on Math grades (b = −.882, SE = 1.297, ρ = .500), and no effect of fast-food consumption on either the MSIT mean RT (b = −0.037, SE = .088, ρ = .679), or on the MSIT SD (b = .128, SE = .078, ρ = .109). There was also no significant effect of fast-food consumption on L-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .008, SE = .050, ρ = .874), R-dlPFC OxyHb (b = −0.061, SE = .072, ρ = .406), L-mPFC OxyHb (b = .021, SE = .060, ρ = .731), or R-mPFC OxyHb (b = .008, SE = .053, ρ = .885). None of the prefrontal function parameters were significant predictors of Math grades, and none of the indirect effects involving prefrontal function parameters were significant (See supplementary materials).
3.2.1.4. Average sleep hours multiple mediation model
A multiple mediation model predicting Math grades from average sleep hours through prefrontal function parameters found no direct effect of average sleep hours on Math grades (b = −.742, SE = 1.807, ρ = .684), and no effect of average sleep hours on either the MSIT mean RT (b = .090, SE = .129, ρ = .488), or on the MSIT SD (b = .025, SE = .110, ρ = .823). There was also no significant effect of average sleep hours on L-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .065, SE = .074, ρ = .385), R-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .167, SE = .104, ρ = .114), L-mPFC OxyHb (b = −.046, SE = .081, ρ = .577), or R-mPFC OxyHb (b = −.077, SE = .081, ρ = .346). None of the prefrontal function parameters were significant predictors of Math grades, and none of the indirect effects involving prefrontal function were significant (See supplementary materials).
3.2.2. English grades
3.2.2.1. Physical activity multiple mediation model
The above multiple mediation models were repeated with English grades as the outcome variable. Fig. 10 depicts the multiple mediation model predicting English grades from average active minutes through the prefrontal function parameters. There was a significant effect of average active minutes on R-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .008, SE = .004, ρ = .032), but no effect of average active minutes on L-dlPFC OxyHb (b = .003, SE = .003, ρ = .295), L-mPFC OxyHb (b = .003, SE = .003, ρ = .401), or R-mPFC OxyHb (b = − .003, SE = .003, ρ = .266). There was also no direct effect of average active minutes on English grades (b = −.070, SE = .055, ρ = .659), and no effect of average active minutes on either the MSIT mean RT (b = −.006, SE = .005, ρ = .184), or on the MSIT SD (b = .002, SE = .004, ρ = .676). None of the prefrontal function parameters were significant predictors of English grades.
Fig. 10.
Multiple mediation model predicting English grades from accelerometer-assessed active minutes of physical activity through brain health parameters, controlling for MSIT % correct responses.
The indirect effect of average active minutes on English grades through R-dlPFC OxyHb was not significant; the upper and lower bound for the 95 % confidence interval for the indirect effect included zero (est. = .020 (SE = .027); CILL = −0.023, CIUL = .081), suggesting a null mediational effect. None of the other indirect effects involving prefrontal function parameters were significant (See supplementary materials).
3.3. Conditional process models
A set of conditional process models were tested to examine the extent to which lifestyle behaviour associations with academic performance mediated through prefrontal function variables might be conditional upon age, gender and BMI. A significant moderation effect emerged regarding BMI on the indirect effect of average active minutes on English and Math grades through MSIT SD (ΔR2 = .086, F = 6.318 (1,45), ρ = .016). Specifically, average active minutes had a stronger effect on the MSIT SD for those in the highest BMI category (β = .431, SE = .192, ρ = .030). The corresponding effect sizes were 0.112 for those in the obesity range (≥95th percentile) and −0.095 for all others (<95th percentile; Fig. 11). All additional models produced null moderated mediational effects (See supplemental material).
Fig. 11.
Effect sizes for active minutes predicting MSIT SD.
Note. Age and gender-specific cut-offs were employed to define obesity categories as recommended by CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017).
4. Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships between health behaviors and academic performance, and mediational pathways through medial and lateral prefrontal function. A prospective observational design was employed, utilizing a combination of accelerometery and self-reported measures of health behaviours as well as fNIRS and task-related quantification of prefrontal function. Findings revealed that higher levels of accelerometer-assessed physical activity, as well as less frequent fast-food consumption both independently predicted significantly better interference task performance. Higher levels of physical activity were associated with larger increases in task-related oxygenation in the right dlPFC during interference blocks, and relative to baseline. There were, however, no significant links between physical activity or fast-food consumption and academic achievement, either directly or mediated by brain health parameters. Finally, higher levels of self-reported substance use were associated with poorer performance in Math.
Using a multiple mediation approach, approach was used to investigate the relationship between each lifestyle predictor and academic outcomes, as mediated through all prefrontal function mediators (task performance and functional imaging parameters). Fast-food consumption was associated with MSIT performance, but eating habits were not significantly associated with any other MSIT indicator (i.e., MSIT mean RT and SD) or oxygenation in any prefrontal sub-region. Finally, average sleep hours were not significantly associated with either of the academic outcomes, or indirectly associated with the PFC functional parameters. However, the mean number of sleep hours was close to the recommended 8 h per night (Tremblay et al., 2016), and so it is possible that the sample was not experiencing sufficient sleep deprivation in order to observe effects. It is also possible that the effects of sleep on achievement, as mediated through the brain, do exist but are too subtle to be measured with the portable fNIRS system used here, or the relatively coarse measures of academic achievement. Future studies with larger samples should explore this possibility.
Conditional process (i.e., moderated mediation) models found a significant moderating effect for BMI on the indirect effect of average active minutes on English and Math grades through reaction time variability on the interference task. Initial models also suggested that BMI was a significant moderator in the relationship between substance use and task performance, where performance on the interference task was better for those whose BMI fell within the obese range. However, there was no moderating effect of BMI in the relationship between substance use and task mean reaction time, or reaction time variability when utilizing the conditional process models. Future research will be required to examine the reliability and meaning of this finding.
Although there was no direct effect of physical activity on either English or Math achievement, greater levels of physical activity were associated with better performance on the cognitive interference task and higher levels of neuronal activation in the right dlPFC during the interference task. This is consistent with a number of other empirical findings, which have found cognitive benefits of physical activity across age ranges (Hillman et al., 2008; 2019; Kramer and Colcombe, 2018), when investigating the effects of activity on task-related performance using various interference tasks (e.g., Stroop, Flanker; Byun et al., 2014; Hillman et al., 2014; Tarp et al., 2016), and when utilizing various neuroimaging techniques (Davis et al., 2011; Hillman et al., 2014; Ji et al., 2019; Khan and Hillman, 2014). Although stronger activation in the lateral PFC is commonly associated with interference tasks, the association between active minutes and oxygenation in the R-dlPFC still supports the notion that physical activity does promote greater activation within the lateral PFC. In conjunction with the behavioural indices of performance on the interference task, greater activity in this region points to improved executive functioning as a result of physical activity.
Contrary to hypotheses, there was no direct association between physical activity and either indicator of academic achievement. It is possible that such activity-induced brain benefits were simply not strong enough to induce changes in academic achievement. Meta-analyses on the topic have shown a small effect of physical activity on academic performance in children (Álvarez-Bueno et al., 2017b; de Greeff et al., 2018), and so a high statistical power may be required to detect a translatable effect. Alternatively, academic performance is an outcome that is highly multi-determined, such that it relies on the cooperation of many cognitive processes (Colom et al., 2007; Rabiner et al., 2016) and is a construct that can be influenced by many external factors, including socioeconomic status, race and gender (von Stumm, 2017; Sutton et al., 2018). Consequently, activity may only marginally impact academic performance because the total universe of influences is so expansive. Likewise, the time-competition hypothesis—which implies a negative association between physical activity and academic performance—or an interaction between the brain benefit and time-competition hypotheses, could produce this pattern of findings such that any brain health benefits of activity could be offset by the time detracted from academic pursuits. The pattern of data in the current study is most consistent with this latter interpretation.
Interestingly, performance on the cognitive interference task was moderated by sex, such that the effect of physical activity on task performance was stronger for females than for males. Sex has previously been shown to moderate the effects of physical activity effects on the brain, but primarily in older adults (Barha et al., 2017). It has been hypothesized that differences between females and males sex steroid hormones could impact cognition and that the hormonal environment during pubertal development may have enduring effects on both the structure and function of the brain from the adolescent developmental period and onwards (Barha et al., 2017; de Vries and Forger, 2015). This may explain why the current findings suggest that such sex differences may extend to lower age ranges.
In addition to the effects of physical activity on task performance, more frequent fast-food consumption was associated with poorer performance on the cognitive interference task. Previous studies have identified a negative relationship between poor dietary habits and reduced executive functioning (Ames et al., 2014; Nyaradi et al., 2014), and experimental studies involving transient up- or down-regulation of the lateral PFC have produced causal changes in food consumption, which supports the alternative temporal relationship (Lowe et al., 2019; Hall, 2019). However, the correlational nature and short time interval of this study does not allow for interpretations of the temporal relationship between these two variables with confidence. Moreover, it is not possible to extrapolate true dietary quality or overall calorie dense food consumption of this sample through fast-food intake alone. Notwithstanding, the results of this study do emphasise importance of calorie-dense food consumption on prefrontal function in this age group.
Furthermore, greater substance use was found to predict poorer Math grades. This is consistent with hypotheses, and prior findings that substance use among adolescents negatively impacts academic achievement (Bugbee et al., 2019; Townsend et al., 2007). Substance use experimentation and substance use disorders often emerge in adolescents (Government of Canada, 2018) and this variable was the only significant predictor of academic achievement, high schools should be especially aware of the impact of substance use when tailoring preventative initiatives.
Limitations of this study include the observational research design, which limits the extent to which causal effects can be identified. Likewise, some variables were studied with temporal lags but not others, making directionality inferences challenging in the latter. In addition, the small sample size may have resulted in reduced statistical power to detect effects, which could have impacted the ability to detect significant associations. Furthermore, the field setting introduced noise into the fNIRS measurements, and the use of self-reported academic achievement, substance use and eating variables may have been subjected to social desirability bias, which in turn may have contributed to lower quality data. More males were included in the sample of adolescents and so it is possible that type 1 error could have contributed to the observed gender differences. In addition, cardiorespiratory fitness was not accounted for in the analysis; future studies may benefit from measuring and examining fitness as a mediating factor.
Key strengths of this study included the use of objective measures of prefrontal function, physical activity and sleep; the latter two in particular are thought to be more reliable and valid than self-report measures (Cui et al., 2011; Sushames et al., 2016). In addition, the use of both fNIRS imaging and a standardised cognitive task allowed for the derivation of several indicators of brain health, both in terms of behavioural markers of executive function and task related activation in the PFC. Furthermore, the field setting allowed for direct recruitment of participants in a naturalistic environment. Finally, there are relatively few studies that employ a brain imaging protocol in order to investigate how lifestyle behaviours impact academic performance through brain health parameters in adolescents and so this study fills a gap in the current literature on the subject.
4.1. Conclusion
Utilizing a sample of adolescents, this study aimed to identify to what extent the relationship between lifestyle behaviours and academic performance was mediated by brain health. Although there was no direct association present between accelerometer-assessed physical activity and either English or Math grades, greater active minutes did have a positive effect on interference task performance and on lateral PFC recruitment. The effect of active minutes on cognitive task performance was also moderated by gender, such that females experienced a greater cognitive benefit of physical activity compared to males. Investigation into the other lifestyle behaviours found that more frequent fast-food consumption and substance use were negatively associated with performance on the cognitive interference task and Math grades respectively. This study provides support for the cognitive enhancement potential of physical activity, but not for the hypothesized mediating role of brain health on academic achievement. Overall, the results speak to the complexity of the relationship between lifestyle behaviours, brain health and academic performance in adolescent populations.
Data statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Author contributions
Mia Papasideris: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Writing-Original draft, Project administration, Investigation Adrian Safati: Investigation Hasan Ayaz: Formal Analysis, Data Curation Plinio Morita: Investigation, Data curation Peter Hall: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Supervision, Writing- Original draft/Review and Editing
Funding
This funding was supported by operating grants to the senior author (PH) by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) and the Social Sciences Research Council of Canada (SSRHC).
The authors MP, AS, PL and PH declare no competing interest.
fNIR Devices, LLC manufactures the optical brain imaging instrument and licensed IP and know-how from Drexel University. HA was involved in the technology development and thus offered a minor share in the startup firm fNIR Devices, LLC. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All authors have approved this manuscript.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests:
The authors MP, AS, PM, and PH declare no competing interest.
fNIR Devices, LLC manufactures the optical brain imaging instrument and licensed IP and know-how from Drexel University. HA was involved in the technology development and thus offered a minor share in the startup firm fNIR Devices, LLC. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All authors have approved this manuscript.
Acknowledgements
Thank you to Laura Xavier Fadrique who accessed and consolidated the raw Fitbit data. Thank you as well to the schools and participants who participated and to the teachers and administration who helped to facilitate this study. Thank you to Bronwyn Papasideris for her guidance and expertise within the school system.
Footnotes
Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100983.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following is Supplementary data to this article:
References
- Åberg M.A.I., Pedersen N.L., Cooper-Kuhn C.M., Åberg N.D., Nilsson M., Kuhn H.G. Cardiovascular fitness is associated with cognition in young adulthood. PNAS. 2009;106(49):20906–20911. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905307106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Álvarez-Bueno C., Pesce C., Cavero-Redondo I., Sánchez-López M., Martínez-Hortelano J.A., Martínez-Vizcaíno V. The effect of physical activity interventions on children’s cognition and metacognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2017;56(9):729–738. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Álvarez-Bueno C., Pesce C., Cavero-Redondo I., Sánchez-López M., Garrido-Miguel M., Martínez-Vizcaíno V. Academic achievement and physical activity: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2017;140(6) doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-1498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ames S.L., Kisbu-Sakarya Y., Reynolds K.D., Boyle S., Cappelli C., Cox M.G. Inhibitory control effects in adolescent binge eating and consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages and snacks. Appetite. 2014;81:180–192. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayaz H., Izzetoglu M., Shewokis P.A., Onaral B. Sliding-window motion artifact rejection for functional near-infrared spectroscopy. 2010 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology. 2010:6567–6570. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2010.5627113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayaz H., Shewokis P.A., Curtin A., Izzetoglu M., Izzetoglu K., Onaral B. Using MazeSuite and functional near infrared spectroscopy to study learning in spatial navigation. J. Vis Exp. JoVE. 2011;8(October (56)) doi: 10.3791/3443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayaz H., Onaral B., Izzetoglu K., Shewokis P.A., McKendrick R., Parasuraman R. Continuous monitoring of brain dynamics with functional near infrared spectroscopy as a tool for neuroergonomic research: empirical examples and a technological development. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barha C.K., Davis J.C., Falck R.S., Nagamatsu L.S., Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2017;1(July 46):71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batterink L., Yokum S., Stice E. Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2010;52(4):1696–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best J.R., Chiu B.K., Hall P.A., Liu-Ambrose T. Larger lateral prefrontal cortex volume predicts better exercise adherence among older women: evidence from two exercise training studies. J Gerontol. Ser. A. 2017;72(6):804–810. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borragán G., Guerrero-Mosquera C., Guillaume C., Slama H., Peigneux P. Decreased prefrontal connectivity parallels cognitive fatigue-related performance decline after sleep deprivation. An optical imaging study. Biol. Psychol. 2019;144:115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bugbee B.A., Beck K.H., Fryer C.S., Arria A.M. Substance use, academic performance, and academic engagement among high school seniors. J. Sch. Health. 2019;89(2):145–156. doi: 10.1111/josh.12723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burrows T., Goldman S., Pursey K., Lim R. Is there an association between dietary intake and academic achievement: a systematic review. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet Off. J. Br. Diet Assoc. 2017;30(2):117–140. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G., Shin L. The multi-source interference task: an fMRI task that reliably activates the cingulo-frontal-parietal cognitive/attention network. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1(1):308–313. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G., Shin L., Holmes J., Rosen B.R., Vogt B.A. The Multi-Source Interference Task: validation study with fMRI in individual subjects. Mol. Psychiatry. 2003;8(1):60–70. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byun K., Hyodo K., Suwabe K., Ochi G., Sakairi Y., Kato M. Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. NeuroImage. 2014;98:336–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Clinical Growth Charts [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2020 Jul 8]. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/growthcharts/clinical_charts.htm.
- Cerrillo‐Urbina A.J., García‐Hermoso A., Sánchez‐López M., Pardo‐Guijarro M.J., Gómez J.L.S., Martínez‐Vizcaíno V. The effects of physical exercise in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized control trials. Child Care Health Dev. 2015;41(6):779–788. doi: 10.1111/cch.12255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaddock L., Erickson K.I., Prakash R.S., Voss M.W., VanPatter M., Pontifex M.B. A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control. Biol. Psychol. 2012;89(1):260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins J.E., Yang H.Y., Trentadue T.P., Gong Y., Losina E. Validation of the Fitbit Charge 2 compared to the ActiGraph GT3X+ in older adults with knee osteoarthritis in free-living conditions. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colom R., Escorial S., Shih P.C., Privado J. Fluid intelligence, memory span, and temperament difficulties predict academic performance of young adolescents. Personal Individ Differ. 2007;42(8):1503–1514. [Google Scholar]
- Cope M., Delpy D.T. System for long-term measurement of cerebral blood and tissue oxygenation on newborn infants by near infra-red transillumination. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 1988;26(3):289–294. doi: 10.1007/BF02447083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosgrove K.P., Mazure C.M., Staley J.K. Evolving knowledge of sex differences in brain structure, function, and chemistry. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62(October 15 (8)):847–855. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cui X., Bray S., Bryant D.M., Glover G.H., Reiss A.L. A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. NeuroImage. 2011;54(4):2808–2821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtin A., Ayaz H. The age of neuroergonomics: towards ubiquitous and continuous measurement of brain function with fNIRS. Psychol. Res. 2018;60(4):374–386. [Google Scholar]
- Davis C.L., Tomporowski P.D., McDowell J.E., Austin B.P., Miller P.H., Yanasak N.E. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2011;30(1):91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Greeff J.W., Bosker R.J., Oosterlaan J., Visscher C., Hartman E. Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2018;21(5):501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vries G.J., Forger N.G. Sex differences in the brain: a whole body perspective. Biol. Sex Differ. 2015;6(1):15. doi: 10.1186/s13293-015-0032-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnelly J.E., Hillman C.H., Castelli D., Etnier J.L., Lee S., Tomporowski P. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2016;48(6):1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubuc M.-M., Aubertin-Leheudre M., Karelis A.D. Relationship between interference control and working memory with academic performance in high school students: the Adolescent Student Academic Performance longitudinal study (ASAP) J. Adolesc. 2020;80:204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emch M., von Bastian C.C., Koch K. Neural correlates of verbal working memory: an fMRI meta-analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019;13 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson K.I., Voss M.W., Prakash R.S., Basak C., Szabo A., Chaddock L. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. PNAS. 2011;108(7):6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esteban-Cornejo I., Tejero-Gonzalez C.M., Sallis J.F., Veiga O.L. Physical activity and cognition in adolescents: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2015;18(5):534–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faught E.L., Qian W., Carson V.L., Storey K.E., Faulkner G., Veugelers P.J. The longitudinal impact of diet, physical activity, sleep, and screen time on Canadian adolescents’ academic achievement: an analysis from the COMPASS study. Prev. Med. 2019;1(August 125):24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd J.N., Blumenthal J., Jeffries N.O., Castellanos F.X., Liu H., Zijdenbos A. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Government of Canada . Public Health Agency of Canada; Ottawa: 2018. Preventing Problematic Substance Use in Youth: CPHO Report on the Health Status of Canadians 2018; pp. 18–28. [Google Scholar]
- Groot C., Hooghiemstra A.M., Raijmakers P.G.H.M., van Berckel B.N.M., Scheltens P., Scherder E.J.A. The effect of physical activity on cognitive function in patients with dementia: a meta-analysis of randomized control trials. Ageing Res. Rev. 2016;25:13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groppe K., Elsner B. Executive function and food approach behavior in middle childhood. Front. Psychol. 2014;5 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustavson D.E., Stallings M.C., Corley R.P., Miyake A., Hewitt J.K., Friedman N.P. Executive functions and substance use: relations in late adolescence and early adulthood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2017;126(2):257. doi: 10.1037/abn0000250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustavson D.E., Stallings M.C., Corley R.P., Miyake A., Hewitt J.K., Friedman N.P. Executive functions and substance use: relations in late adolescence and early adulthood. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2017;126(2):257. doi: 10.1037/abn0000250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haghayegh S., Khoshnevis S., Smolensky M.H., Diller K.R., Castriotta R.J. Performance assessment of new-generation Fitbit technology in deriving sleep parameters and stages. Chronobiol. Int. 2020;37(1):47–59. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2019.1682006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall P.A. Brain stimulation as a method for understanding, treating, and preventing disorders of indulgent food consumption. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2019;6(3):266–272. [Google Scholar]
- Hansen D.M., Herrmann S.D., Lambourne K., Lee J., Donnelly J.E. Linear/nonlinear relations of activity and fitness with children’s academic achievement. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2014;46(12):2279–2285. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson K.L., Medina K.L., Padula C.B., Tapert S.F., Brown S.A. Impact of adolescent alcohol and drug use on neuropsychological functioning in young adulthood: 10-year outcomes. J. Child Adolesc. Subst. Abuse. 2011;20(2):135–154. doi: 10.1080/1067828X.2011.555272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman S.J., Nelson S.H., Weiner L.S. Patterns of fitbit use and activity levels throughout a physical activity intervention: exploratory analysis from a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mhealth. Uhealth. 2018;6(2):e29. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.8503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillman C.H., Erickson K.I., Kramer A.F. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9(1):58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillman C.H., Pontifex M.B., Raine L.B., Castelli D.M., Hall E.E., Kramer A.F. The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience. 2009;159(3):1044–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillman C.H., Pontifex M.B., Castelli D.M., Khan N.A., Raine L.B., Scudder M.R. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1063–71. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillman C.H., Logan N.E., Shigeta T.T. A review of acute physical activity effects on brain and cognition in children. Transl. J. Am. Coll. Sports Med. 2019;4(17):132–136. [Google Scholar]
- Jansen J.M., Daams J.G., Koeter M.W.J., Veltman D.J., van den Brink W., Goudriaan A.E. Effects of non-invasive neurostimulation on craving: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37(10 Pt 2):2472–2480. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ji Z., Feng T., Mei L., Li A., Zhang C. Influence of acute combined physical and cognitive exercise on cognitive function: an NIRS study. PeerJ. 2019;7:e7418. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan N.A., Hillman C.H. The relation of childhood physical activity and aerobic fitness to brain function and cognition: a review. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 2014;26(2):138–146. doi: 10.1123/pes.2013-0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer A.F., Colcombe S. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study—revisited. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2018;13(2):213–217. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer A.F., Erickson K.I. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11(8):342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lan X., Legare C.H., Ponitz C.C., Li S., Morrison F.J. Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011;108(3):677–692. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langner R., Eickhoff S.B. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol. Bull. 2013;139(4):870–900. doi: 10.1037/a0030694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe C.J., Safati A., Hall P.A. The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;80:586–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe C.J., Reichelt A.C., Hall P.A. The prefrontal cortex and obesity: a health neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23(4):349–361. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maayan L., Hoogendoorn C., Sweat V., Convit A. Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity. 2011;19(7):1382–1387. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer A., Königs M., Vermeulen G.T., Visscher C., Bosker R.J., Hartman E. The effects of physical activity on brain structure and neurophysiological functioning in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020;45 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitschke K., Köstering L., Finkel L., Weiller C., Kaller C.P. A Meta-analysis on the neural basis of planning: activation likelihood estimation of functional brain imaging results in the Tower of London task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38(1):396–413. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Northey J.M., Rattray B., Pumpa K.L., Pryor D.J., Fraser M.A., Shaw M.E. Objectively measured physical activity is associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume in older adults. NeuroImage. 2020;221 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyaradi A., Foster J.K., Hickling S., Li J., Ambrosini G.L., Jacques A. Prospective associations between dietary patterns and cognitive performance during adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014;55(9):1017–1024. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papasideris M., Leatherdale S.T., Battista K., Hall P.A. An examination of the prospective association between physical activity and academic achievement in youth at the population level. PLOS ONE. 2021;16(Jun 11(6)) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0253142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinti P., Tachtsidis I., Hamilton A., Hirsch J., Aichelburg C., Gilbert S. The present and future use of functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for cognitive neuroscience. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018:1–15. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabiner D.L., Carrig M.M., Dodge K.A. Attention problems and academic achievement: do persistent and earlier-emerging problems have more adverse long-term effects? J. Atten. Disord. 2016;20(11):946–957. doi: 10.1177/1087054713507974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samuels W.E., Tournaki N., Blackman S., Zilinski C. Executive functioning predicts academic achievement in middle school: a four-year longitudinal study. J. Educ. Res. 2016;109(5):478–490. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider H., Zhang N. School accountability and youth obesity: can physical education mandates make a difference? Educ. Res. Eval. 2013;2013 [Google Scholar]
- Scholkmann F., Kleiser S., Metz A.J., Zimmermann R., Mata Pavia J., Wolf U. A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. NeuroImage. 2014;85:6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinsburg A.D., Schweinsburg B.C., Cheung E.H., Brown G.G., Brown S.A., Tapert S.F. fMRI response to spatial working memory in adolescents with comorbid marijuana and alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79(2):201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh A.S., Saliasi E., van den Berg V., Uijtdewilligen L., de Groot R.H.M., Jolles J. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: a novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. Br. J. Sports Med. 2018;0:1–10. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2017-098136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smithers L.G., Sawyer A.C.P., Chittleborough C.R., Davies N.M., Davey Smith G., Lynch J.W. A systematic review and meta-analysis of effects of early life non-cognitive skills on academic, psychosocial, cognitive and health outcomes. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2(11):867–880. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0461-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sushames A., Edwards A., Thompson F., McDermott R., Gebel K. Validity and reliability of fitbit flex for step count, moderate to vigorous physical activity and activity energy expenditure. PLoS One. 2016;11(9) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton A., Langenkamp A.G., Muller C., Schiller K.S. Who gets ahead and who falls behind during the transition to high school? Academic performance at the intersection of race/ethnicity and gender. Soc. Probl. 2018;65(2):154–173. doi: 10.1093/socpro/spx044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarp J., Domazet S.L., Froberg K., Hillman C.H., Andersen L.B., Bugge A. Effectiveness of a school-based physical activity intervention on cognitive performance in danish adolescents: LCoMotion—learning, cognition and motion – a cluster randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2016;11(6) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorell L.B., Veleiro A., Siu A.F.Y., Mohammadi H. Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychol. 2013;19(6):630–638. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.727792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Townsend L., Flisher A.J., King G. A systematic review of the relationship between high school dropout and substance use. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2007;10(4):295–317. doi: 10.1007/s10567-007-0023-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay M.S., Inman J.W., Willms J.D. The relationship between physical activity, self-esteem, and academic achievement in 12-year-old children. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 2000;12(3):312–323. [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay M.S., Carson V., Chaput J.-P., Connor Gorber S., Dinh T., Duggan M. Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for children and youth: an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. Physiol. Appl. Nutr. Metab. 2016;41(6 Suppl 3):S311–327. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2016-0151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urrila A.S., Artiges E., Massicotte J., Miranda R., Vulser H., Bézivin-Frere P. Sleep habits, academic performance, and the adolescent brain structure. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):41678. doi: 10.1038/srep41678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verburgh L., Königs M., Scherder E.J.A., Oosterlaan J. Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2013;48(12):973–979. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter M., Orr R., O’Dwyer N., O’Connor H. Effectiveness of active learning that combines physical activity and math in schoolchildren: a systematic review. J. Sch. Health. 2020 doi: 10.1111/josh.12878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N.D., Wang G.-J., Fowler J.S., Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philos. Trans. R Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008;363(1507):3191–3200. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N.D., Michaelides M., Baler R. The neuroscience of drug reward and addiction. Physiol. Rev. 2019;99(4):2115–2140. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Stumm S. Socioeconomic status amplifies the achievement gap throughout compulsory education independent of intelligence. Intelligence. 2017;60:57–62. [Google Scholar]
- Watson A., Timperio A., Brown H., Best K., Hesketh K.D. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017;14(1):114. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westfall D.R., Gejl A.K., Tarp J., Wedderkopp N., Kramer A.F., Hillman C.H. Associations between aerobic fitness and cognitive control in adolescents. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:1298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J., Li R., Ma J., Zhang W. Longitudinal relations between future planning and adolescents’ academic achievement in China. J. Adolesc. 2019;75:73–84. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao R., Zhang X., Fei N., Zhu Y., Sun J., Liu P. Decreased cortical and subcortical response to inhibition control after sleep deprivation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13(3):638–650. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9868-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.