Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2021 Jul 15;19(7):e3001172. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001172

Perception of biological motion by jumping spiders

Massimo De Agrò 1,2,3,*, Daniela C Rößler 1,2, Kris Kim 1,2, Paul S Shamble 1,2
Editor: Lars Chittka4
PMCID: PMC8282030  PMID: 34264925

Abstract

The body of most creatures is composed of interconnected joints. During motion, the spatial location of these joints changes, but they must maintain their distances to one another, effectively moving semirigidly. This pattern, termed “biological motion” in the literature, can be used as a visual cue, enabling many animals (including humans) to distinguish animate from inanimate objects. Crucially, even artificially created scrambled stimuli, with no recognizable structure but that maintains semirigid movement patterns, are perceived as animated. However, to date, biological motion perception has only been reported in vertebrates. Due to their highly developed visual system and complex visual behaviors, we investigated the capability of jumping spiders to discriminate biological from nonbiological motion using point-light display stimuli. These kinds of stimuli maintain motion information while being devoid of structure. By constraining spiders on a spherical treadmill, we simultaneously presented 2 point-light displays with specific dynamic traits and registered their preference by observing which pattern they turned toward. Spiders clearly demonstrated the ability to discriminate between biological motion and random stimuli, but curiously turned preferentially toward the latter. However, they showed no preference between biological and scrambled displays, results that match responses produced by vertebrates. Crucially, spiders turned toward the stimuli when these were only visible by the lateral eyes, evidence that this task may be eye specific. This represents the first demonstration of biological motion recognition in an invertebrate, posing crucial questions about the evolutionary history of this ability and complex visual processing in nonvertebrate systems.


The body of most creatures is composed of interconnected joints, constraining their motion to a pattern known as “biological motion.” This study shows that jumping spiders can distinguish animate from inanimate objects based solely on visual motion characteristics — an ability previously known only in vertebrates.


For the vast majority of animals, determining if an object in the environment is another animal is crucial for survival. However, recognizing every single object individually quickly becomes computationally impractical. We thus expect evolution to favor simple but robust systems with general rules exploiting common characteristics that are able to cover most situations [1]. In most vertebrates, there are characteristic spatiotemporal relationships between different body parts while in motion—relationships imposed by the overall body plans of these animals. Because the vertebrate body consists of linked rigid segments, movements between joints are accordingly semirigid—while some joints may seem independent from one another (e.g., wrist to knee) with their relative distances varying during locomotion, other joint pairs are not (e.g., wrist to elbow), with relative distances that are fixed. Thus, when observed visually, the movements of these animals result in a statistically identifiable idiosyncratic pattern. This is known as biological motion.

Indeed, even when presented with just 11 dots moving in correspondence with the position of the main joints of the human body, observers can correctly identify the presence of an agent [26]. Such dot-only stimuli (called point-light displays) are completely devoid of structural information, but retain motion-based information. The detection of these patterns in humans is not confined to stimuli depicting conspecifics, but enables the detection of many other animals [7]. Even “scrambled” displays, where dots maintain a semirigid relationship, are perceived as unknown, yet alive, entities [8]. Moreover, the discrimination of these patterns seems to be present at a very young age [9]. Studies on animal models also allowed to demonstrate the innateness of this ability [10,11]. Overall, studies in many other vertebrates [1114], demonstrated that this visuo-cognitive strategy is widespread across vertebrates and is thus evolutionarily ancient. As such, biological motion detection is widely considered to be a key mechanism in enabling the detection of living animals within a visual scene [6,8,15]. That this system relies on motion [4,16] is important, as it has been proposed that motion-based visual cues can be extracted more quickly than many static cues, are less vulnerable to disruption, and are potentially governed by smaller networks of neurons [17].

In most arthropods, body plans and movements follow the same rules of semirigidity described above for vertebrates, since their exoskeleton is composed of connected, rigid components. Furthermore, like for many vertebrate species, the ability to visually differentiate moving animals from other visual stimuli is likely to provide a strong selective advantage. Because of these similarities in both form and function, we set out to explore whether biological motion cues might also be used by invertebrates, suggesting an even more widespread ability across animals.

We used jumping spiders (Salticidae) to address this question, as these animals are among the most visually adept of all arthropods, with vision playing a central role in a wide range of behaviors [18]. The visual system of these animals is also unique, with a total of 8 eyes and with different pairs thought to be specialized for different visual tasks [18]. The large anterior medial eyes (AME or primary eyes) are forward facing and characterized by a narrow visual field (<5°) and high visual acuity [18] and are believed to play a central role in figure recognition [19,20]. The other 3 pairs are termed “secondary eyes.” The anterior lateral eyes (ALE) have lower visual acuity but a much wider visual field (≈±50°) and are believed to be specialized for motion detection [21,22]. The posterior medial eyes (PME) are smaller and believed to be vestigial in many jumping spiders species [23]. The posterior lateral eyes (PLE) are characterized by a very wide visual field, and, together with the ALE, grant the spider a near 360° field of view. The function of the PLE seems to be similar to the ALE. Behavioral observations support the functional division of labor hypothesis: When a moving target is detected by the secondary eyes, the spider will pivot to face it directly and track it with the AMEs [21,24,25]. These rotational pivots are so rapid, precise, and robust that they are referred to as saccades [26], analogous to the eye movements of other animals, including humans. However, salticids do not turn toward all moving targets in the visual field, suggesting a selective attention–like process and/or that secondary eyes can inform target differentiation, such as the detection/discrimination of biological motion cues, rather than simply acting solely as motion detectors [27]. This idea is also supported by the fact that, neuroanatomically, the 2 sets of eyes (primary and secondary) possess completely separate early visual processing pathways [28], suggesting that there may be specialized computational tasks for which each network is responsible.

We tested Menemerus semilimbatus spiders in a forced-choice paradigm using point-light display stimuli. For a detailed description of the procedure, see Materials and methods section. Individuals were suspended above a polystyrene sphere so that their legs could contact it, while the sphere was supported and allowed to spin freely by a constant stream of compressed air from below. In spiders, the legs attach to the cephalothorax (the head)—thus, in this setup, the spider remained in a fixed position and maintained a fixed head orientation, but was able to move its legs freely, transferring its intended locomotor actions to the sphere. Stimuli were then presented on a computer monitor placed 14 cm in front of the spider (Fig 1A). Two stimuli were presented simultaneously, entering the monitor space from opposite sides and moved toward the center, where they disappeared. Using a video-based sphere tracking system (FicTrac) [29], we then measured how spiders moved the sphere in response to the stimuli presentation. As stated above, jumping spiders produce rotations or “saccades” with their body upon detection of a stimulus with the secondary eyes, turning rapidly about a single central axis (like a top or a military tank). Thus, the measured output was the rotational locomotor behavior—which of the 2 targets the spider chose to rotate toward.

Fig 1. Stimuli movement across the screen and of each point.

Fig 1

(A) Schematic representation of the setup, left half. The black horizontal line represents the computer screen. The colored line above represents the position of the stimulus across time (color scale). Note the sudden color changes where the stimulus paused for 1.5 seconds before starting to move again. Once at the center, the stimulus disappeared behind a white box. To turn toward any stimulus on the screen, the spider can produce saccades, by pivoting around its vertical axis (z-axis of the sphere). Cones drawn toward the screen: visual field of the different eyes. In black, the visual field of the AME. The striped area represents the extended visual field of the AME when moved. In gray, the visual field of the ALE. Note that the visual field of the remaining eyes covers all the rest of the screen. Large overlay schematic on left depicts a jumping spider cephalothorax, seen frontally with the AME in black, and the ALE in gray. (B) Point-light displays. On the left, the full set of dots for the first frame. For the biological, the silhouette has been superimposed on the dots, to show how they correlate with the spider’s joints. For each stimulus, the paths for 3 points (the same 3 in all displays) is highlighted. Note how for the scrambled display, while the position of the points is different from the biological, the path that they follow is identical. ALE, anterior lateral eyes; AME, anterior medial eyes.

We designed 5 different stimuli, paired in a total of 4 different conditions (Fig 1B; see Materials and methods section for details; videos of the stimuli are available in S1S11 Videos). Stimuli were designed as spider versions of standard point-light display stimuli. The “biological motion” stimulus consisted of a point-light display following the motion of a walking spiders from a side-view perspective. In the “scrambled motion” stimulus, the starting point of each dot was randomized, but it then followed the same path as in the biological motion stimulus—a pattern perceived as biological in other systems [8]. The “random motion” stimulus consisted of the same number of dots constrained to the same overall area as the biological motion stimulus, moving with the same velocities, but in randomized directions. A spider silhouette, based on the same video used to create the biological motion stimulus and thus following the same movements and an ellipse translating at the same speed of the other stimuli but without the finer-scale joints movement was also used. In conditions 1 and 2 (Fig 2C), the random motion stimulus was paired against the biological and the scrambled stimuli, respectively, testing responses to biological versus nonbiological motion. In condition 3, the biological stimulus was paired against the scrambled stimulus, thus presenting 2 semirigid stimuli. In condition 4, the spider silhouette was paired against the ellipse, a control condition to observe responses toward structurally detailed stimuli compared to moving stimuli of similar size but with minimal detail.

Fig 2. Results.

Fig 2

(A) Recorded peak rotation speeds for each trial (every set of 10 stimuli pair presentations; ntrials = 188; nspiders = 60). The x-axis represents time (in seconds) from the start of the experiment, and the y-axis represents rotational speed (in degrees per second). Individual dots represent peaks, while the dark line shows the running average. In the top-right corner, a schematic representation of the spider on top of the sphere, with the 3 possible axis of rotation. The data presented in this and the following graphs refer to rotation observed on the sphere’s z-axis, which correspond to the spider’s saccades. Below, colors show when the stimuli were onscreen (purple) or not present (gray). When stimuli are present, the average rotational speed increases, while when stimuli are not present, peak rotational speed remains low. This suggests that rotational speed is representative of saccades and that spiders are turning toward stimuli (see Materials and methods section). (B) Time-aligned responses across all stimuli. Upper portion: same as panel A, with addition of right y-axis indicating position of the stimuli on the screen (in degrees from the center, dashed purple line). Below portion: histogram of the peaks frequency. Peak frequency increases when the stimuli are moving and decreases when they are stationary (flat sections of the stimulus position line). (C) Stimulus preference for each condition. The x-axis represents the rotational speed. Positive or negative values correspond to a preference for the stimulus indicated. Purple boxes refer to sections when the stimuli were visible (during stimuli period), and gray boxes refer to sections when stimuli were not visible (between stimuli period). Dashed lines in the box represent the average, and shaded areas represent SE. For this analysis, every peak was either positive, when turning toward the stimulus depicted on the right of the graph (the one with biological characteristics), or negative, when turning toward the stimulus shown on the left. Thus, average speed is a proxy of relative preference, with 0 corresponding to no preference. For the biological versus random, scrambled versus random, and silhouette versus ellipse, the spiders preferred the less realistic stimulus. We observed no preference in the biological versus scrambled condition. Across all conditions, there was no preference during the 30-second between-stimuli period. Data underlying panels A and B are available in S1 Data. Panel C depicts result of the GLMM based on the same data. The analysis script is available in S2 Analysis. GLMM, generalized linear mixed model; SE, standard error.

Results

Only the main results are reported here. For the full analysis, see S1 Scripts. Firstly, we checked whether the observed rotations of the sphere did truly describe the reaction of the spider to the presence of any stimulus on screen. We found a strong correlation between absolute rotational speed and the stimuli angular location (both occupied the same angle, being symmetric to the center) (generalized linear mixed model [GLMM] analysis of deviance, chi-squared = 3330.8, p-value < 0.0001) across all experimental conditions (difference between conditions: chi-squared = 2.52, p-value = 0.47) (Fig 2A and 2B). We also observed a significant interaction between stimulus position and condition (chi-squared = 46.77, p-value < 0.0001): Specifically, we found this correlation to be stronger in the silhouette versus ellipse condition than in the other conditions (post hoc analysis, Tukey correction. Silhouette ellipse versus scrambled random: est. = 0.004, SE = 0.001, t = 4.73, p < 0.0001 versus biological scrambled: est. = 0.005, SE = 0.001, t = 6.31, p < 0.0001 versus biological random: est. = 0.004, SE = 0.001, t = 5.12, p < 0.0001). This was likely due to the fact that the silhouette and the ellipse stimuli are composed of more black pixels, resulting in higher contrast between the stimuli and the white background than the point-light displays. This would have allowed those stimuli to be detected earlier, strengthening the correlation between rotational speed and stimulus position. All together, these observations suggest that the observed Z rotation of the sphere is indicative of saccades produced by the spiders toward the stimuli on screen.

Having established that, we proceeded by analyzing the average direction of the saccades. To do so, we multiplied by 1 Z rotation values in a direction congruent with the position of the more biological stimulus, while we multiplied them by −1 when congruent with the other. We then used this as the dependent variable to compute the spider’s preference (Fig 2C). During stimulus presentation, the spiders turned more toward the ellipse over the silhouette (est. = −23.73, SE = 3.41, t = −6.96, p < 0.0001), the random over the scrambled (est. = −15.1, SE = 3.41, t = −4.435, p = 0.0001), the random over the biological (est. = −13.37, SE = 3.38, t = −3.96, p < 0.0006), and showed no difference in the biological versus scrambled condition (est. = −1.04, SE = 3.3, t = −0.32, p = 1). When considering the 30-second between-stimulus period instead, we observed no difference for all conditions (post hoc analysis, Tukey correction. Biological random: est. = 0.52, SE = 1.59, t = 0.32, p = 1; biological scrambled: est. = 1.68, SE = 1.53, t = 1.1, p = 0.922; scrambled random: est. = 2.46, SE = 1.68, t = 1.47, p = 0.706; silhouette-ellipse: est. = 3.4, SE = 1.72, t = 1.97, p = 0.328). Note that most saccades happens as soon as the stimuli start moving (Fig 2B). Among those sections, the higher frequency of rotation can be observed around 4.5 seconds from stimuli appearance (Fig 2B), when both are positioned at an angle of ≈±50° (Fig 1A). As stated in the introduction, this angular position likely aligns with the start of the ALE field of view.

Discussion

These results clearly demonstrate the ability of jumping spiders to discriminate between biological motion cues. The results also show strong internal consistency, with preferences across all stimulus pairs for the less realistic stimuli, with the exception of the paired semirigid stimuli where no preference was observed. We initially found this “reverse choice” surprising, as we expected spiders to turn toward the stimulus with the highest probability of being a living organism, as has been previously found in other systems [9,11]. Even though the stimuli may have been perceived as predators or competitors, given their size, it seems unlikely that our result can be explained as an avoidance effect. Rather than turning toward the “less dangerous” stimulus under this perspective, we would have expected spiders to have maintained their attention on the “more dangerous” stimulus and possibly attempted to run away. However, orientation toward the less-biological stimulus appears consistent with the functional organization of the visual system of the jumping spiders. As described above, these animals produce saccades upon detection of a target with the secondary eyes, allowing further inspection with the AME. As per our initial hypothesis, the secondary eyes may be immediately able to decode motion-based information, enabling them to determine which of the stimuli requires more detailed investigation. In a forced-choice paradigm, it may be advantageous to focus the AME on the stimulus that cannot be decoded with the secondary eyes alone, particularly since the other target will still remain in the visual field of the secondary eyes following rotation. Moreover, it is crucial to consider that many of the species in which a preference for biologically moving stimuli has been observed are highly social. Indeed, it has been amply argued that an innate “animacy detection” system may exist because of the need of these animals to quickly aggregate with social companions [30]. In such species, it would be advantageous to show an early attentional preference to movements that are more likely to be generated from an agent. However, jumping spiders are not typically social; thus, we should not necessarily have the same expectation for stimulus preference. It is crucial to point out that motion cues in point-light displays can be used for much more than just “animacy detection.” It has been shown that humans cannot only detect the presence or absence of a living being in the stimulus, but also much finer details, like, for example, the sex of the actor [31]. It has been suggested that, also, jumping spiders could detect and use more subtle motion cues to discriminate between different species to approach or avoid, like conspecifics or ants [32]. The lack of a preference between the scrambled and the biological displays in our experiment seems to point against the presence of such finer discrimination, but more direct inquiries are needed to provide a definitive answer.

In our setup, spiders could only observe the target with both AME and ALE eyes when the stimuli reached the center of the screen (see Fig 1A). Specifically, this opportunity for primary–secondary overlap occurs after ≈7s from the stimuli appearance, yet the majority of during-stimulus turns, and thus choices, occurred before this phased (see Fig 2B). That spiders demonstrate this preference even when targets can only be viewed by the secondary eyes is striking. Indeed, multiple studies have been carried out about the visual discrimination abilities of jumping spiders [33,34], and the secondary eyes have been found to possess a reasonably high spatial acuity [22,24,35]. However, even though it has been suggested that these eyes are capable of feats beyond mere motion detection [27], no study before had directly demonstrated that these eyes can solve a discrimination task without aid from the primary eyes. As point-light displays are designed to contain minimal visual detail, the discrimination operated by the secondary eyes must be based on motion.

Our findings provide evidence for the discrimination of point-light display biological motion stimuli [3] in an invertebrate. If this is evidence of convergent evolution or of deep homology (e.g., presence in the shared common ancestor) still remains unclear. However, it is widely believed that the complexity of high-acuity vision in the vertebrates and invertebrates (e.g., vertebrates, insects, spiders, and mollusks) have evolved independently [36], suggesting that this is more likely a result of convergent evolution. Indeed, the detection of biological motion represents a solution or strategy to the problem of “animacy detection” [6,8], which could have been adopted by spiders and vertebrates alike. We suspect that, given this, future work in other lineages (e.g., insects and mollusks) may reveal similar results. Indeed, the use of motion perception by insects to extract other forms of information has been amply studied: For example, flies, bees, and wasps use optic flow information to calculate ground speed and thus navigate the environment [37], while locusts use of motion parallax to estimate distance [38]. These systems remain different from the detection of biological motion, as they rely on changes in the visual scene caused by one’s own movement, while responses to dynamic targets still function when the observer is stationary.

The presence of a biological motion-based detection system in jumping spiders deepens questions regarding the evolutionary origins of this visual processing strategy and opens the possibility that such mechanisms might be widespread across the animal kingdom and not necessarily related to sociality.

Materials and methods

Subjects

We used 60 M. semilimbatus in the experiment, of which 31 were females, 10 were males, and 19 were juveniles. The spiders were collected in the wild, in the garden of Esapolis’ living insect museum, Padua, Italy between June and August 2020. Only animals with a body length bigger than 7 mm were collected to guarantee the proper functioning of the methodology. Once caught, the spiders were maintained in clear plastic boxes measuring 80 × 65 × 155 mm and immediately fed a small Tenebrio molitor to ensure a shared level of satiation before the test. The day after capture, a magnet was fixed to the head of each subject to allow us to constrain the animal on top of the treadmill for the duration of the experiment (see next paragraph). Subjects were first constrained between a sponge and a latex film. The latter presented a hole in correspondence with the location of the spider’s head. Here, a 1 × 1 × 1 mm neodymium magnet was applied using a UV activated resin. Each spider underwent its first test between 1 and 3 days after receiving the magnet. At the end of the full experiment, each spider was again constrained under the latex film to remove the magnet. Spiders were then freed in the same place they were captured. Magnets did not appear to negatively affect the animals during the short period in which they were housed in the lab, and spiders that had the magnet removed appeared to move and behave normally.

Experimental apparatus

In order to ensure that spiders would always see the stimuli from the same position, orientation, distance, and, most importantly, both stimuli at the same time, we built a sphere-based treadmill, similar to that described in Moore and colleagues [29]. Similar procedures have been used in jumping spiders, and these animals appear to react to digital stimuli as if they were freely moving in the environment [22,25,3941].

To construct the treadmill, a polystyrene sphere (diameter = 38 mm) was placed on top of a shallow concave holder. A second piece of plastic with a 24-mm hole in the center was placed on top of the sphere, leaving 1 mm of clearance where this plate met the sphere. In this setup, the sphere was free to rotate, but its overall position was fixed in place. The plastic holder was designed so that the side of the sphere was still mostly visible, with the plastic pieces covering only around 30% of its surface, and the top portion of the sphere was left exposed so that the spider could contact/hold it. This plastic holder also contained 16, 1-mm diameter air inlets directed toward the sphere. When a compressed air source set to a constant pressure of 50 mbar was connected to the plastic holder, it produced an air cushion that the sphere floated on, providing almost frictionless rotational movement. The pressure was low enough not to cause the sphere to rotate by itself. The rotational inertia of the ball was also such that the spider could rotate it freely.

To initiate each trial, we used a stick with a magnet glued at one of its ends to pick up a spider. After adjusting the position/orientation of this stick, we positioned the spider on top of the sphere so that the center of the spider’s cephalothorax was at the top of the sphere, its body aligned toward the screen, and at a height above the sphere that enabled a normal walking gait. In this setup, the subject could not move nor rotate, as its cephalothorax was fixed in place, but it would still behave as if it was able to move, acting directly on the sphere. By measuring sphere rotations, we could infer the spider’s intended action.

To enable tracking of the sphere, we used acrylic paint to draw uneven shapes across its surface. From the side view, we recorded video of each experiment at 120 fps. By running the FicTrac software by Moore and colleagues [29] on these videos, we were able to extract the exact orientation of the sphere in each frame, expressed by its X (left-right), Y (fore-aft), and Z (rotational) rotational components, and, in turn, the rotational speed.

After every 4 trials, the system was disassembled to clean the sphere and the plastic pieces from any silk residue, which might interfere with sphere rotation.

Stimuli of the experiment

All stimuli were based on a video recording of the side view of a Salticus scenicus walking from the right to the left of the frame. The video was captured with a Nikon D7200 DLSR camera (Nikon, USA), focused on a 5-cm wide runway recorded at 60 fps and 1080p. From the video, we extracted 72 frames, which contained a total of 8 full steps of the subject, with step defined as a movement of all 8 legs starting and terminating with each limb occupying the same position relative to each other. In each frame, we manually registered the position of the eyes, the pedicel, the spinneret, and for every leg, the end of the tarsus, the joint between the metatarsus and the tibia, and the joint between the patella and the femur. If any of the points were not visible in a specific frame (which did often happen especially for the right side legs, being covered by the body), the point position was registered as not available.

All stimuli were presented to the spider on a 1080p screen, with a pixel size of 0.248 mm, positioned 14 cm away from the center of the treadmill sphere. In each condition (except for the single-stimulus control trials), 2 stimuli were presented simultaneously, one entering from the left side and the other entering from the right, both moving toward the center, disappearing behind an ideal white screen in the exact center of the monitor. The presentation of the stimuli was controlled by a script written in python 3.8 [42], using the package PsychoPy [43]. The same script also controlled the camera recording of the treadmill, with the package OpenCV [44]. This way, stimulus presentation and camera recording were matched, in order to know the position of the sphere for every frame of the display. All the stimuli were presented on the screen at 30 fps. Videos of the stimuli are available in S1S11 Videos.

Biological motion

For this point-light display stimuli, a black dot was presented at each anatomical location registered in the original video, rescaled in order for the stimulus to occupy a size of 170 × 60 px on the screen (the total width and height changed slightly following the extension and retraction of the spider legs). The starting position of the stimulus was set outside the screen, so that the picture would not be shown suddenly on the screen but would instead appear to move in from outside. After every 72 frames (all the frames extracted from the original video), the stimulus would remain static for 45 frames, then loop over the original 72 frames, but, this time, translated in the X direction to center the first frame over the current position. This 45 frames pause was repeated another time and was followed by a third loop iteration, which terminated with the stimulus disappearing progressively behind a white box at the center of the screen. A full stimulus presentation, from start to end position, was a total of 270 frames, or 9 seconds at the display rate of 30 frames per second. The total time the stimulus was visible to the spider, thus excluding the time it took to appear from outside of the screen, was 8 seconds.

Scrambled motion

In the scrambled motion stimulus, the path of every dot was maintained, but their relative positions to one another in the first frame was randomized. In other species tested, scrambled motion stimuli are still perceived as biological, giving the impression to the observer of seeing an unknown living creature [8]. To generate this stimulus, we calculated for every point in each frame its distance to the position it occupied in the previous one, but while maintaining the center of the stimulus in a fixed position. Then, the position of every dot was randomized for the first frame. The position of the dots in the following frames would be determined by reapplying the distance and direction extracted from the original stimulus to the random position of the dot. We then reapplied the translation, moving all the dots by placing the center of the picture in correspondence with the center of the original stimulus. This way, the scrambled motion stimulus retained the same speed as the original, by globally translating the same amount, and each dot would follow the same path, but the relative position of the dots in relation to one another was eliminated. We created 4 different versions of this stimulus, with 4 different randomizations. For each trial, one of these 4 was chosen, so that each spider would never see the same randomization in different trials (for example, 2 different conditions present the scramble motion—biological versus scrambled and scrambled versus random—any one spider will see 2 different versions of the scrambled stimuli, 1 for each condition), and different spiders would see different versions for the same condition.

Random motion

In the random motion stimulus, the position of every dot in every frame was randomized, and according to the literature, this should not be perceived as biological [12]. To build this stimulus, we began with the initial, full stimulus used in the biological motion stimuli. We then followed the same procedure as for the scrambled motion but defined the position of the dots in frames 2 to 72 using the same distance from the previous frame as the biological motion stimulus, but in a random direction. Thus, the magnitude, or amount of motion, is maintained, but each dot takes a random path. To prevent dots from shifting too far from the center, we recalculated a new random direction if the resulting position of the dots exceeded the perimeter of an ideal rectangle of 170 × 60 px, exactly as the one enclosing all the dots in the biological motion stimulus. We created 4 different versions of this stimulus, with 4 different randomizations, following the same presentation as per scrambled motion.

Silhouette

This stimulus was intended as a control, to test whether spiders would show a preference in this setup at all. Previous experiments have shown that jumping spiders respond differently to different silhouettes [45], but we wanted to evaluate responses to stimulus presentation on our spherical treadmill. For this stimulus, we connected the dots from the biological motion stimulus belonging to the same leg, resulting in a thick black line representing the full leg in its movements. We also added black shapes to represent the spider cephalothorax and opisthosoma, based on the original walking spider video. One edge of each body segment shape was then centered on the picture and each shape tilted according to the eyes and spinneret dots. Thus, this stimulus moved in the same way as the biological motion stimuli, but provided a full silhouette of the target in terms of static appearance.

Ellipse

This shape matched exactly the silhouette in terms of the number of black pixels. For every frame, the center of the ellipse was placed to the coordinates of the center of the biological motion, and, in turn, all the other stimuli.

Detailed procedure

Each spider underwent a total of 4 trials, 2 of which were administered on a given day, and the other 2 were administered after a 3-day break. Every trial started with 5 minutes of habituation, in which no stimulus was shown, just a white screen. This phase was included in the experiment to give the animals enough time to decrease their level of arousal from the manipulation before the experiment started and revert to normal behavior. After habituation, the first presentation started, where 2 stimuli would appear, one from the left and one from the right, move toward the center of the screen, and then disappear. As described, the stimuli stop twice, for around 1.5 seconds, before reaching the center. After the stimulus disappearance, a 30-second pause, with no presentation, followed. This pattern of presentation pause was repeated another 9 times, for a total of 10 stimuli presentation. Thus, a full trial lasted 11 minutes and 30 seconds. In a given trial, the same stimulus pair was shown for all the 10 stimuli presentation, and the starting position of each stimulus (left or right) would follow a semi-random pattern (left, right, left, right, left, left, right, right, left, right or vice versa for the other stimulus). This way, each trial was part of a single condition. Each spider was subjected to a different condition (namely, a specific stimuli pair) in every trial and was therefore presented with all experimental conditions. The order of the trials was randomized for each spider.

After the end of the experiment, we added a fifth condition: ellipse versus nothing. Only 1 subject was used to test it, as it was only intended to retrieve a baseline measure of preference, to inform the interpretation of the data of the other conditions.

Data analysis

The sphere tracking software FicTrac [29] provided, among other measures, the radians per frame that the sphere rotated around its x-, y-, and z-axis, for each video frame (Fig 2A). A rotation around each of these axes represented a different kind of motion of the spider:

  • x-axis: Parallel to the screen, the rotation of the sphere around this axis would occur when the subject would move forward or backwards. From the perspective of the side-view camera that was used, a counterclockwise rotation would happen when the spider moved forward and vice versa.

  • y-axis: Perpendicular to the screen, such that rotation around this axis would happen if the spider moved directly sideways (left or right).

  • z-axis: Vertically, through the spider, such that rotation would be registered when the spider rotated around its center. A turn to the left of the spider would correspond to a clockwise rotation and vice versa.

Other types of movements (e.g., the spider moving diagonally) would result in a combination of the 3 rotations. Before further analysis, the values of each axis were smoothed with a Butterworth filter (order = 3 and critical frequency = 0.5) from the python package SciPy [46]. What we were interested in were the turns toward the left and toward the right, which would correspond with z-axis rotation peaks in the absence of X and Y components. Due to noise in the system and to individual differences of the spiders and videos, movements typically contained rotational velocities across all 3 axes. To reduce the impact of these cross-axes effects, for every frame, we subtracted from the absolute value of Z the absolute value of X and Y, with 0 as the minimum allowable resulting value. Then, the original sign of Z component (+ or −) was reapplied. This way, Z peaks with low concurrent X and Y components remained high, while highly coupled rotations would disappear. Thus, saccadic movements (large Z rotations and low X and Y components) could be clearly separated from non-saccadic movements such as walking forwards (large X and low Y and Z) or curved walking (large X, large Y, and medium Z).

To analyze the impact of stimuli on saccadic turns, we focused on the stimuli presentation part of the trials, including the 30-second pauses between stimuli pair presentations, and changed the sign of each rotation according to the position of the expected preferred stimulus: Instead of positive numbers meaning left and negative meaning right, positive numbers represented rotations toward the more biologically realistic stimulus and vice versa. For example, in condition biological motion versus scrambled motion, biological motion was coded as positive. Note that because of this conversion, the scrambled motion stimulus was negative in one condition (biological versus scrambled) but positive in another (scrambled versus random). The same coding was maintained for the pause section following each stimulus, with rotation toward the side in which the correct stimulus was previously present being positive. We then isolated peaks higher than 0.001 rad/sec for both the positive and the negative space and used these peak heights and times in our analysis.

With this coding, a saccadic preference for the more realistic stimulus (i.e., more turns toward it) would result in a positive peak average, saccadic preference for the less realistic stimulus will result in a negative average, and an average of 0 would signify no saccadic preference. Indeed, we expected to observe no preference for the between-stimulus sections, regardless of condition, given the a priori expectation that rotational movements in the absence of stimuli should be directed randomly.

Statistical analysis

All data and graphs were prepared using python 3.8 [42] with the libraries pandas [47,48], NumPy [49,50], SciPy [46], and Matplotlib [51]. Analyses were carried out with R 4.0.2 [52], using the packages glmmTMB [53], car [54], emmeans [55], DHARMa [56], and readODS [57]. For the full script, see Supporting information text. The full database is available as a Supporting information. As suggested by Forstmeier and Schielzeth [58], we included in the models only factors that we had an a priori reason for including. First, we calculated a model with the absolute (without the sign coding the direction) peak height as a dependent variable and the angular position of the stimulus on the screen as a predictor. We employed a generalized linear mixed-effect model, with subject as a random effect. This analysis was performed to check whether extracted peak height represent rotational speed of the spider. Indeed, we know from previous literature [22,25] that when moving on similar spherical treadmills, the rotation caused by the detection of a target (saccadic turns) performed by spiders match very closely the actual angular position of the target. Moreover, these saccadic rotations tend to take similar amounts of time, resulting in faster rotation when the target angle is higher. Subsequently, we calculated a second model, using peak height (with positive values when its direction was congruent with the more realistic target and vice versa) and the dependent variable, condition, and stimulus presence (visible or not visible) as predictors. Here, we expected the average peak height to be indistinguishable from 0 when the stimulus is not visible (as saccadic rotations should be less frequent, and even if they did appear, they should be toward a random direction) and to be higher than 0 for conditions biological versus random, scrambled versus random, and silhouette versus ellipse, signifying a preference for the biologically structured stimulus.

Methods validation results

To check whether our scoring based on the average Z rotation was indeed an effective proxy for preference, in condition 5, we presented spiders with a choice between a moving ellipse and nothing on the other side. As expected, we observed a clear preference for the ellipse position when the stimulus was on screen (post hoc analysis, Tukey correction est. = 107.0, SE = 12, t = 8.89, p < 0.0001) and no preference in the 30-second between-stimulus time (est. = −24.5, SE = 13.9, t = 1.76, p = 0.155), supporting the utilized score as representative of spiders saccadic preference.

Supporting information

S1 Data. Data used in the analysis.

The first sheet contains metadata. The second sheet contains all the data for the experiment with the X, Y filtering on Z applied. The third sheet contains the control with the ellipse put in comparison with no stimulus. The fourth sheet is the same as the second, but without the filtering applied.

(XLSX)

S1 Script. R and python scripts.

This compressed folder contains 2 file. The first file (Analysis) contains the full R script of the data analysis. The second one (DataFiltering) contains the python script used to subtract X and Y value for the Z, as described in the Materials and methods section, and to find peaks.

(ZIP)

S1 Video. Biological motion point-light display.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S2 Video. Spider silhouette.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. In the experiment, this stimulus was always paired with the ellipse, which followed the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S3 Video. Ellipse.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. In the experiment, this stimulus was always paired with the silhouette, which followed the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S4 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, first randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S5 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, second randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S6 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, third randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S7 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, fourth randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S8 Video. Random motion point-light display, first randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S9 Video. Random motion point-light display, second randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S10 Video. Random motion point-light display, third randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S11 Video. Random motion point-light display, fourth randomization.

The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

(MP4)

S12 Video. Example of a single-stimulus pair presentation for the condition “silhouette versus ellipse”.

Initially, the spider is in a normal locomotion phase. Around second 18 of the video, the first saccade can be appreciated, specifically toward the right side of the screen where the ellipse stimulus appeared (which will enter the visual field of the camera shortly). Then, some more saccades follow.

(MP4)

Acknowledgments

We thank Esapolis’ Living Insect Museum of the Padua Province, Padua, Italy and its director Enzo Moretto for providing the extra facilities unexpectedly needed due to the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic. We also thank Maria Loconsole and Lucia Regolin for suggestions regarding the manuscript.

Abbreviations

ALE

anterior lateral eyes

AME

anterior medial eyes

GLMM

generalized linear mixed model

PLE

posterior lateral eyes

PME

posterior medial eyes

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

PSS was supported by the John Harvard Distinguished Science Fellows Program within the FAS Division of Science, Harvard University. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Chittka L, Niven J. Are Bigger Brains Better? Curr Biol. 2009;19:R995–R1008. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Johansson G. Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception. Psychol Res. 1976;38:379–93. doi: 10.1007/BF00309043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Johansson G. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept Psychophys. 1973;14:201–11. doi: 10.3758/BF03212378 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Neri P, Morrone MC, Burr DC. Seeing biological motion. Nature. 1998;395:894–6. doi: 10.1038/27661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Troje NF. 2013. What Is Biological Motion? Definition, Stimuli, and Paradigms In: Rutherford MD, Kuhlmeier VA, editors. Social Perception. The MIT Press. p. 13–36. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262019279.003.0002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Troje NF, Westhoff C. The inversion effect in biological motion perception: evidence for a “life detector”? Curr Biol. 2006;16:821–4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mather G, West S. Recognition of Animal Locomotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays. Perception. 1993;22:759–66. doi: 10.1068/p220759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Troje NF, Westhoff C. Detection of direction in scrambled motion: a simple “life detector”? J Vis. 2005;5:1058–8. doi: 10.1167/5.8.1058 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Simion F, Regolin L, Bulf H. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:809–13. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707021105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vallortigara G, Regolin L. Gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion by inexperienced chicks. Curr Biol. 2006;16:R279–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Vallortigara G, Regolin L, Marconato F. Visually Inexperienced Chicks Exhibit Spontaneous Preference for Biological Motion Patterns. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brown J, Kaplan G, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Perception of biological motion in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): by females only. Anim Cogn. 2010;13:555–64. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0306-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Nakayasu T, Watanabe E. Biological motion stimuli are attractive to medaka fish. Anim Cogn. 2014;17:559–75. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0687-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Regolin L, Tommasi L, Vallortigara G. Visual perception of biological motion in newly hatched chicks as revealed by an imprinting procedure. Anim Cogn. 2000;3:53–60. doi: 10.1007/s100710050050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vallortigara G. 2012. Aristotle and the Chicken: Animacy and the Origins of Beliefs In: Fasolo A, editor. The Theory of Evolution and Its Impact. Milano: Springer Milan. p. 189–199. doi: 10.1007/978-88-470-1974-4_12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Poom L, Olsson H. Are mechanisms for perception of biological motion different from mechanisms for perception of nonbiological motion? Percept Mot Skills. 2002;95:1301–10. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.95.3f.1301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Borst A, Egelhaaf M. Principles of visual motion detection. Trends Neurosci. 1989;12:297–306. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90010-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Harland DP, Li D, Jackson RR. 2012. How Jumping Spiders See the WorldHow Animals See the WorldComparative Behavior, Biology, and Evolution of Vision. Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195334654.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Land MF. Structure of the Retinae of the Principal Eyes of Jumping Spiders (Salticidae: Dendryphantinae) in Relation to Visual Optics. J Exp Biol. 1969. a;51:443–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Land MF. Movements of the Retinae of Jumping Spiders (Salticidae: Dendryphantinae) in Response to Visual Stimuli. J Exp Biol. 1969. b;51:471–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Land MF. Stepping Movements Made by Jumping Spiders During Turns Mediated by the Lateral Eyes. J Exp Biol. 1972. a;57:15–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zurek DB, Nelson XJ. Hyperacute motion detection by the lateral eyes of jumping spiders. Vis Res. 2012. a;66:26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Land MF. Short Communication: Fields of View of the Eyes of Primitive Jumping Spiders. J Exp Biol. 1985;119:381–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Jakob EM, Long SM, Harland DP, Jackson RR, Carey A, Searles ME, et al. Lateral eyes direct principal eyes as jumping spiders track objects. Curr Biol. 2018;28:R1092–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Zurek DB, Nelson XJ. Saccadic tracking of targets mediated by the anterior-lateral eyes of jumping spiders. J Comp Physiol A. 2012. b;198:411–7. doi: 10.1007/s00359-012-0719-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Land MF. 1972. b. Mechanisms of Orientation and Pattern Recognition by Jumping Spiders (Salticidae) In: Wehner R, editor. Information Processing in the Visual Systems of Anthropods: Symposium Held at the Department of Zoology, University of Zurich, March 6–9, 1972. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. p. 231–247. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-65477-0_34 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Spano L, Long SM, Jakob EM. Secondary eyes mediate the response to looming objects in jumping spiders (Phidippus audax, Salticidae). Biol Lett. 2012;8:949–51. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Steinhoff POM, Uhl G, Harzsch S, Sombke A. Visual pathways in the brain of the jumping spider Marpissa muscosa. J Comp Neurol. 2020;528:1883–902. doi: 10.1002/cne.24861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Moore RJD, Taylor GJ, Paulk AC, Pearson T, van Swinderen B, Srinivasan MV. 2014. FicTrac: A visual method for tracking spherical motion and generating fictive animal paths. J Neurosci Methods. 225:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rosa-Salva O, Mayer U, Versace E, Hébert M, Lemaire BS, Vallortigara G. Sensitive periods for social development: Interactions between predisposed and learned mechanisms. Cognition. 2021:104552. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kozlowski LT, Cutting JE. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Percept Psychophys. 1977;21:575–80. doi: 10.3758/BF03198740 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cross FR, Jackson RR. When it looks and walks like an ant. Learn Behav. 2018;46:103–4. doi: 10.3758/s13420-017-0311-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.De Agrò M. SPiDbox: design and validation of an open-source “Skinner-box” system for the study of jumping spiders. J Neurosci Methods. 2020;346:108925. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Dolev Y, Nelson XJ. Innate pattern recognition and categorization in a jumping spider. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097819 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zurek DB, Taylor AJ, Evans CS, Nelson XJ. The role of the anterior lateral eyes in the vision-based behaviour of jumping spiders. J Exp Biol. 2010;213:2372–8. doi: 10.1242/jeb.042382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gehring WJ. The evolution of vision. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Dev Biol. 2014;3:1–40. doi: 10.1002/wdev.96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Egelhaaf M, Borst A. A look into the cockpit of the fly: visual orientation, algorithms, and identified neurons. J Neurosci. 1993;13:4563–74. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-11-04563.1993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kral K, Poteser M. Motion parallax as a source of distance information in locusts and mantids. J Insect Behav. 1997;10:145–63. doi: 10.1007/BF02765480 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Humphrey B, Helton WS, Bedoya C, Dolev Y, Nelson XJ. Psychophysical investigation of vigilance decrement in jumping spiders: overstimulation or understimulation? Anim Cogn. 2018;21:787–94. doi: 10.1007/s10071-018-1210-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Melrose A, Nelson XJ, Dolev Y, Helton WS. Vigilance all the way down: Vigilance decrement in jumping spiders resembles that of humans. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2018;1747021818798743. doi: 10.1177/1747021818798743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Peckmezian T, Taylor PW. A virtual reality paradigm for the study of visually mediated behaviour and cognition in spiders. Anim Behav. 2015;107:87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.06.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Van Rossum G, Drake FL. 2009. Python 3 Reference Manual. Scotts Valley, CA: CreateSpace. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Peirce J, Gray JR, Simpson S, MacAskill M, Höchenberger R, Sogo H, et al. PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behav Res Ther. 2019;51:195–203. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bradski G. The OpenCV Library. Dr Dobb’s Journal of Software Tools. 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Shamble PS, Hoy RR, Cohen I, Beatus T. Walking like an ant: a quantitative and experimental approach to understanding locomotor mimicry in the jumping spider Myrmarachne formicaria. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2017;284:20170308. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.0308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Virtanen P, Gommers R, Oliphant TE, Haberland M, Reddy T, Cournapeau D, et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat Methods. 2020;17:261–72. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Reback J, McKinney W, Mendel B, Van den Bossche J, Augspurger T, Cloud P, et al. pandas-dev/pandas: Pandas 1.0.3. Zenodo. 2020. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.3715232 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.McKinney W. Data Structures for Statistical Computing in Python. In: Proceedings of the 9th Python in Science Conference. 2010:56–61. doi: 10.25080/Majora-92bf1922-00a [DOI]
  • 49.Oliphant TE. 2006. A guide to NumPy. Trelgol Publishing; USA. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.van der S, Colbert SC, Varoquaux G. The NumPy Array: A Structure for Efficient Numerical Computation. Comput Sci Eng. 2011;13:22–30. doi: 10.1109/MCSE.2011.37 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hunter JD. Matplotlib: A 2D graphics environment. Comput Sci Eng. 2007;9:90–5. doi: 10.1109/MCSE.2007.55 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brooks ME, Kristensen K, Benthem KJ, van Magnusson A, Berg CW, Nielsen A, et al. glmmTMB Balances Speed and Flexibility Among Packages for Zero-inflated Generalized Linear Mixed Modeling. R J. 2017;9:378–400. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fox J, Weisberg S. 2011. An R Companion to Applied Regression. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lenth R. 2018. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means.
  • 56.Hartig F. 2018. DHARMa: Residual Diagnostics for Hierarchical (Multi-Level / Mixed) Regression Models.
  • 57.Schutten G-J, Chan C, Leeper TJ, Foster J. contributors and other. readODS: Read and Write ODS Files. 2020.
  • 58.Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H. Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:47–55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Roland G Roberts

22 Mar 2021

Dear Dr De Agrò,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Perception of biological motion in a jumping spider" for consideration as a Short Report by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff, as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise, and I'm writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Mar 24 2021 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. Once your manuscript has passed all checks it will be sent out for review.

Given the disruptions resulting from the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic, please expect delays in the editorial process. We apologise in advance for any inconvenience caused and will do our best to minimize impact as far as possible.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Roli Roberts

Roland G Roberts, PhD,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Decision Letter 1

Roland G Roberts

7 May 2021

Dear Dr De Agrò,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Perception of biological motion in a jumping spider" for consideration as a Short Report at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by three independent reviewers.

You’ll see that all three reviewers are broadly positive about your manuscript. Reviewer #2 has a request for an additional analysis, and all three reviewers raise textual and presentational issues, but our impression is that there is not much work to do. We do note that, contrary to what rev #3 says, you already provide some supplementary videos.

In light of the reviews (below), we are pleased to offer you the opportunity to address the comments from the reviewers in a revised version that we anticipate should not take you very long. We will then assess your revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments and we may consult the reviewers again.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 1 month.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point by point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Resubmission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this resubmission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Roli Roberts

Roland Roberts

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

*****************************************************

REVIEWERS' COMMENTS:

Reviewer #1:

The paper reports the first evidence of recognition of biological motion in an invertebrate species, a finding that I believe deserves to be published. I have only minor requests for changes.

p. 2 second para. line 10: This is incorrect of course. Human newborns cannot have been completely prevented the possibility to see motion patterns. Direct evidence is possible only in controlled conditions in animal models (e.g. the Plos Biology 2005 paper quoted below on newly-hatched chicks does provide such an evidence). This part should be better formulated. Please also consider on the same topic Gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion by inexperienced chicks. Current Biology (2006), 16: 279-280.

p. 2 second para. line 15: On the 'animacy detector' see also Vallortigara, G. (2012). Aristotle and the chicken: Animacy and the origins of beliefs. In "The Theory of Evolution and its Impact" (A. Fasolo, ed.), pp. 189-200, Springer, New York.

p. 3 second para. the part starting from "Neuroanatomically..." This seems to be of little importance with respect to the rest of the paper and should be reduced or better omitted altogether.

p. 6 line 5 and line 11: change "agent detection" with "animacy detection" (recognition of agency is different than recognition of 'animacy')

p. 6 second para., the last three lines "How this might be implemented..." should be omitted.

p. 7 line 2: "animacy detection" not agent

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript details the responses of salticid spiders toward point light stimuli with different degrees of biological relevance (or 'realism') with respect to the movement depicted by the dots. This is a novel and interesting method and investigative tool to use on this group of animals. The results were somewhat surprising, and the authors have done a reasonable job of interpreting these results. I have mostly fairly minor comments, although there are some fairly glaring mistakes with the use of terminology of the eyes, and with respect to the descriptions of the fields of view (fov) of the different pairs of eyes. These can be easily rectified.

My principal comment is that, as you tested male and female spiders, and the sexes are known to be somewhat different in their contrast (and other) visual thresholds (Zurek work), it would have been interesting to see a comparison between males and females in this piece of work. I know this would require additional analysis, but I do think that this would make the current manuscript considerably stronger, both in content, and in its ability to make predictions about the visual system of this group of animals. Given that you have the rotational paths of each individual, and you know M/F, it should not be too hard?

All other comments and notation is made with respect to pages on downloaded pdf, as there are no line numbers.

On page 9, I am unclear how in joints "relative distances varying across time,". Do you mean phylogeny? I think you don't mean that - perhaps delete "across time", or of not clarify what you mean.

P10. Eye structure- this is incorrect. Note that the PME have a very narrow field of view, if any at all, in most species (where this pair of eyes is typically vestigial). The ALE and PLE have wide fields of view. See Land, M. F. (1985a). "Fields of view of the eyes of primitive jumping spiders." J. exp. Biol. 119: 381-384

Fig 2C. I am having some trouble working out the dark lines (mean) versus shaded area (SEM) - partly because there is coloured shading representing stimulus ON/OFF screen. Can a dashed line be used for the mean and shading for the SEM?

End of results. For people who understand salticid eyes and the set-up, it is not especially surprising that stimuli elicited a response at about 4.5 s, when they were within the fov of the ALE. However, for others, this sentence might appear a bit 'random'. I would make this clear.

P 12. "detection of a target with the lateral eyes" - you should be consistent. This is not technically correct. You should be referring here to the secondary eyes unless you are discussing solely your results in the narrower context, in which case this should be made clear and it should be stated that this is the ALE you are referring to and specifically with respect to this experiment.

P. 13, beginning. "…AME on the stimulus which cannot be decoded with the lateral eyes alone, particularly since the other target will still remain in the visual field of the secondary eyes following rotation." The work in Zurek, D. B., et al. (2010). "The role of the anterior lateral eyes in the vision-based behaviour of jumping spiders." Journal of Experimental Biology 213(14): 2372-2378, seems especially pertinent here, as it clearly demonstrated the spatial ability of the ALE, which is something that the authors do not seem to be aware of. This reference is also pertinent to your later statement: "That spiders demonstrate this preference even when targets can only be viewed by the secondary eyes is striking". The point made later, about discriminations being made on motion is very valid, and an interesting one.

P. 13. Some salticid species are, in fact, somewhat social. I would add the caveat "jumping spiders are not TYPICALLY social"

Very minor

Sometimes "cm" is written directly after the number (no space), and other times there is a space.

Reviewer #3:

In this interesting research paper, De Agrò and colleagues investigate the responses of jumping spiders to visual displays presented on a computer screen. The Authors focus on response to biological motion vs other manipulations of visual stimuli implemented using point light displays and silhouettes. Similarly to other studies conducted in vertebrate species, the Authors used pairs of stimuli including point displays of biological motion (that move semi-rigidly following the movement of a spider) vs scrambled motion vs random motion. They also tested a silhouette of a spider vs an ellipse. All stimuli translated horizontally, while joints/silhouettes were manipulated in different ways.

The results solidly support the idea that in jumping spiders biological motion produces different behavioural responses (eg saccades) compared to random motion and scrambled motion. Although the visual representation of results is less than straightforward, the results appear robust and novel.

It would be interesting to know whether all spiders had similar responses or whether the effect is driven by few individuals.

Can the Authors clarify how many saccades were present in each condition?

The Authors commented on the "unexpected" result of a preference of spiders for orienting towards the less naturalistic stimulus. Some potential interpretations have not been mentioned. For instance: Can this be due to the fact that the more naturalistic stimuli are considered as potential competitors for catching a potential prey? Is it possible that more realistic stimuli are considered potential predators to flee away from? These two options are basically prey capture and predator avoidance or competitor avoidance. Other solitary species appear to actively move away from conspecifics (e.g. tortoise hatchlings), so there are several possible reasons behind this outcome.

This study leads to further questions on whether the size of objects and type of biological motion can influence the direction of preference.

I would like to ask the Authors to consider to use a more straightforward visual representation to help the reader. For instance, can they use legends for the different coloured lines? Is it possible make a connection between the unit of measurement to the variable of interest? (e.g. rotation toward the object?).

In the Data analyses part I was initially confused by the X vs Y axis of the sphere. Would it be worth to add a visual representation of the sphere and its axis?

This paper would also benefit from a video of the subjects performing the task.

Decision Letter 2

Roland G Roberts

29 May 2021

Dear Dr De Agrò,

Thank you for submitting your revised Short Reports entitled "Perception of biological motion in a jumping spider" for publication in PLOS Biology. The Academic Editor and I have now assessed your responses and revisions. 

Based on his assessment, we will probably accept this manuscript for publication, provided you satisfactorily address the following points:

a) Please could you make a slight change to the title to make it most explicit for our broader readership? We suggest "Perception of biological motion by jumping spiders"

b) Please supply a blurb, according to the instructions in the submission form.

c) Many thanks for supplying the data underlying the Figures. Please could you rename the data file from “supp-2.xlsx” to “S1_Data.xlsx” and cite it in the legends to Figs 1 and 2 (e.g. "The data underlying this Figure can be found in S1 Data")?

As you address these items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

-  a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

-  a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable)

-  a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript. 

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information  

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Early Version*

Please note that an uncorrected proof of your manuscript will be published online ahead of the final version, unless you opted out when submitting your manuscript. If, for any reason, you do not want an earlier version of your manuscript published online, uncheck the box. Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Roli Roberts

Roland G Roberts, PhD,

Senior Editor,

rroberts@plos.org,

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA NOT SHOWN?

- Please note that per journal policy, we do not allow the mention of "data not shown", "personal communication", "manuscript in preparation" or other references to data that is not publicly available or contained within this manuscript. Please either remove mention of these data or provide figures presenting the results and the data underlying the figure(s).

Decision Letter 3

Roland G Roberts

11 Jun 2021

Dear Dr De Agrò,

On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Lars Chittka, I'm pleased to say that we can in principle offer to publish your Short Reports "Perception of biological motion by jumping spiders" in PLOS Biology, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email that will follow this letter and that you will usually receive within 2-3 business days, during which time no action is required from you. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have made the required changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS: We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have not yet opted out of the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may do so on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely,

Roli Roberts

Roland G Roberts, PhD 

Senior Editor 

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data. Data used in the analysis.

    The first sheet contains metadata. The second sheet contains all the data for the experiment with the X, Y filtering on Z applied. The third sheet contains the control with the ellipse put in comparison with no stimulus. The fourth sheet is the same as the second, but without the filtering applied.

    (XLSX)

    S1 Script. R and python scripts.

    This compressed folder contains 2 file. The first file (Analysis) contains the full R script of the data analysis. The second one (DataFiltering) contains the python script used to subtract X and Y value for the Z, as described in the Materials and methods section, and to find peaks.

    (ZIP)

    S1 Video. Biological motion point-light display.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S2 Video. Spider silhouette.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. In the experiment, this stimulus was always paired with the ellipse, which followed the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S3 Video. Ellipse.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. In the experiment, this stimulus was always paired with the silhouette, which followed the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S4 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, first randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S5 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, second randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S6 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, third randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S7 Video. Scrambled motion point-light display, fourth randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S8 Video. Random motion point-light display, first randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S9 Video. Random motion point-light display, second randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S10 Video. Random motion point-light display, third randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S11 Video. Random motion point-light display, fourth randomization.

    The size, speed, and positions are true to the experiment. This example moves left to right, for right to left version the video was mirrored. A second stimulus, depending on the condition, would follow the same path, just mirrored.

    (MP4)

    S12 Video. Example of a single-stimulus pair presentation for the condition “silhouette versus ellipse”.

    Initially, the spider is in a normal locomotion phase. Around second 18 of the video, the first saccade can be appreciated, specifically toward the right side of the screen where the ellipse stimulus appeared (which will enter the visual field of the camera shortly). Then, some more saccades follow.

    (MP4)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Answer_to_Reviewers.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Answer_to_Editor.odt

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES