Abstract
Contemporary models of psychosis suggest that a continuum of severity of psychotic symptoms exists, with subthreshold psychotic experiences (PEs) potentially reflecting some genetic and environmental risk factors shared with clinical psychosis. Thus, identifying abnormalities in brain activity that manifest across this continuum can shed new light on the pathophysiology of psychosis. Here, we investigated the moment-to-moment engagement of brain networks (“states”) in individuals with schizophrenia (SCZ) and PEs and identified features of these states that are associated with psychosis-spectrum symptoms. Transient brain states were defined by clustering “single snapshots” of blood oxygen level-dependent images, based on spatial similarity of the images. We found that individuals with SCZ (n = 35) demonstrated reduced recruitment of three brain states compared to demographically matched healthy controls (n = 35). Of these three illness-related states, one specific state, involving primarily the visual and salience networks, also occurred at a lower rate in individuals with persistent PEs (n = 22), compared to demographically matched healthy youth (n = 22). Moreover, the occurrence rate of this marker brain state was negatively correlated with the severity of PEs (r = −0.26, p = 0.003, n = 130). In contrast, the spatial map of this state appeared to be unaffected in the SCZ or PE groups. Thus, reduced engagement of a brain state involving the visual and salience networks was demonstrated across the psychosis continuum, suggesting that early disruptions of perceptual and affective function may underlie some of the core symptoms of the illness.
Subject terms: Schizophrenia, Neuroscience
Introduction
Studies conducted over the past decade have revealed that schizophrenia (SCZ) is associated with disruption of the coordinated functioning of distributed brain networks [1–4]. These disruptions have been commonly identified by measuring fluctuations in low-frequency blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) signal in resting-state functional magnetic imaging (rs-fMRI) data [5–8]. Rs-fMRI data are typically examined using a “static” analysis, in which “functional connectivity” is estimated by measuring correlations of magnitudes of BOLD signals in distinct brain regions. Such static connectivity investigations have detected a wide range of alterations in functional connectivity in individuals with SCZ, within fronto-parietal, default mode, ventral attention, and somatosensory networks [9–11]. Although replicable differences in static connectivity between healthy and SCZ groups have been found [12], recent work has suggested that correlations of BOLD signals over minutes do not reflect the rapid changes in brain connectivity that may be most affected in psychotic illness [13–15]. Specifically, recent methodological advances have revealed that human brain activity is characterized by continuous, rapid shifts in activity across a large number of complex, varied networks, rather than a prolonged engagement of a small number of networks [16–19].
To leverage this new understanding of brain connectivity, investigations of the rapid shifts in BOLD signal correlations occurring across the brain have been recently conducted in SCZ. These studies have reported that individuals with SCZ show lower (or higher [20]) rates of switching among functional “states,” reductions in the duration of time spent in cohesive states of regional co-activation, and decreased variation in states, compared to healthy subjects [21–25]. However, recent evidence suggests that findings of these “dynamic” studies that examine correlations of BOLD signals in short time windows are affected by unaddressed confounds related to sampling variability [26].
An alternative approach to measuring transient brain activation is to examine absolute co-activation levels of BOLD signal across the brain at each time point [27]. This method allows for a more direct investigation of the magnitude of activity of functional networks and their interactions, without relying on temporal correlations of BOLD signal in short time windows [26]. Here we used this approach to measure the configurations of functional networks, and the temporal and spatial properties of activity patterns of the brain, in individuals with SCZ and demographically matched healthy controls (HC). Critically, we used the same method in a second cohort of subjects: young adults with persistent subthreshold psychotic symptoms or what are commonly referred to as “psychotic experiences” (PEs) [28] and a group of demographically matched healthy youth (HY) without such symptoms. This second sample allowed us to determine whether any of the patterns observed in SCZ were also found in individuals with PEs, while avoiding the confounds that typically limit the interpretation of neuroimaging findings in SCZ, such as treatment with antipsychotic medication [29, 30].
Although, on average, only a small percentage of individuals with PEs will eventually develop clinical psychosis, the persistence of PEs over time substantially increases such risk (5–25 fold) [28, 31, 32]. Furthermore, recent evidence supports the model that PEs are on a biological continuum with clinical psychosis [33–37]. For example, recent studies have identified changes in brain structure and function in individuals with PEs that are similar to those observed in SCZ [38–42]. Similarities across findings in SCZ and PE groups suggest that such common findings may reflect manifestations of psychosis-related pathophysiology (and are less likely to result from effects of treatment or chronic illness in SCZ). Thus, in this study, we aimed to identify the changes in dynamic brain states that manifest across the psychosis spectrum, in both SCZ and PE groups.
Materials and methods
Participants
Three datasets were analyzed in this study. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines of the Institutional Review Boards of Partners Healthcare (Datasets I and III) and Harvard University (Dataset II). All images were collected on 3T Tim-Trio scanners (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with 12-channel phased-array head coils.
Dataset I included 35 patients with DSM-IV–diagnosed SCZ (age 41.80 ± 9.01; 8 female) and 35 age, sex, and head motion–matched HC (age 39.49 ± 10.43; 8 female, see Table 1) [43]. One to two resting-state (eyes open) scans (372 s/run) were obtained from each subject. Functional images were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 85°, 3 × 3 × 3 mm voxels, FOV = 216). Structural data included a multi-echo T1-weighted magnetization-prepared gradient-echo image (TR = 2200 ms, TI = 1100 ms, TE = 1.54 ms, flip angle = 7°, 1.2 × 1.2 × 1.2 mm). As expected, the SCZ group had a significantly lower mean IQ and years of education than the HC group.
Table 1.
A. Demographically matched healthy controls (CON) and individuals with schizophrenia (SCZ) (Dataset I) | |||
---|---|---|---|
CON (n = 35) | SCZ (n = 35) | P value | |
Age (years) | 39.49 ± 10.43 | 41.80 ± 9.01 | 0.32 |
Gender | 77.14% male | 77.14% male | >0.90 |
PANSS Total | 72.00 ± 12.72 | ||
PANSS Positive Symptoms Subscale | 17.29 ± 5.58 | ||
PANSS Negative Symptoms Subscale | 20.35 ± 4.84 | ||
PANSS General Symptoms Subscale | 34.35 ± 6.55 | ||
Head motion (FD) | 0.076 ± 0.05 | 0.089 ± 0.06 | 0.30 |
IQ | 111.76 ± 9.49 | 101.35 ± 11.40 | <0.01 |
Years of education | 17.21 ± 2.18 | 12.86 ± 2.44 | <0.01 |
B. Demographically matched HY and PE subjects (Dataset II) | |||
---|---|---|---|
HY (n = 22) | PE (n = 22) | P value | |
Age (years) | 19.27 ± 1.28 | 19.23 ± 1.07 | 0.90 |
Gender | 63.64% male | 63.64% male | >0.90 |
PDI total score | 2.09 ± 1.48 | 8.36 ± 2.52 | <0.001 |
Head motion (FD) | 0.0453 ± 0.023 | 0.0423 ± 0.016 | 0.612 |
Years of education | 13.23 ± 1.31 | 13.23 ± 1.15 | >0.90 |
CON healthy control group, SCZ schizophrenia group, PANSS Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, FD framewise displacement (in mm), HY Healthy youth group, PE psychotic experiences group, PDI Peters et al. Delusions Inventory.
Dataset II included 130 enrolled college students (age 19.5 ± 1.3; range 18–24; 91 female) with elevated scores on either a measure of depression (Beck Depression Inventory total score >5, or item #9 > 0) or PEs (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory (PDI) total score >7). Among these subjects, 22 consistently showed moderately elevated PDI scores (total PDI score >4 over two time points 1 year apart) and were included in the PE group (age 19.23 ± 1.07; 8 female). Twenty-two subjects who showed consistently low PDI scores (<4, at two time points 1 year apart) were included in the HY group (19.27 ± 1.28; 8 female). The imaging protocol was identical to that of Dataset I. The PE and HY groups were matched in gender, age, years of education, and head motion (see Table 1).
Dataset III included 20 young healthy subjects (age 29.6 ± 5.3; range 24–40; 10 female). The imaging protocol was identical to that of Datasets I and II. For this cohort only, each subject was scanned twice, once with eyes open and once with eyes closed. These data were used to examine the effects of visual input on the outcome measures of this study.
See the Supplementary methods for additional details.
Preprocessing
Rs-fMRI were preprocessed using a previously described analysis pipeline [44], which included: (1) projecting the data from the subject’s native space to FSL MNI152 space and then onto the FreeSurfer fsaverage6 surface space; (2) linear detrending and band-pass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz); (3) regressing nuisance variables including the global signal, head motion parameters, and their first temporal derivatives [45]; (4) spatial smoothing with a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian curve; (5) down-sampling the BOLD fMRI data to a mesh of 642 vertices in each hemisphere; and (6) temporal normalization to the signal extracted from each vertex.
Clustering-based single frame dynamic analysis
We applied a k-means clustering algorithm to classify BOLD images into different groups based on their spatial similarity (using a cosine similarity metric) (Fig. 1), which has been previously employed to temporally decompose resting-state networks into multiple spontaneous co-activation patterns [27]. The cluster centroids were interpreted as unique “brain states.” Each BOLD image was defined by its signal values across 1284 surface vertices. The preprocessed BOLD signals of all subjects were concatenated and temporally standardized. To improve computational efficiency, we used a principal component analysis to reduce the dimensionality of the feature space to 650, which could explain 99% of the total variance in the rs-fMRI. The optimal cluster number was selected according to the test–retest reliability of the resulted brain states in 100 young healthy subjects scanned using the same scanning protocol as for our data (see Supplementary Fig. S1). The solutions with 11, 14, 19, 31, 35 clusters were relatively reliable. In this study, we focused on 19-states estimation for further analyses.
Results
Rate of occurrence of specific brain states was reduced in individuals with SCZ
Dynamic brain states were defined by clustering the single volumes/images of fMRI data based on their spatial similarity (Fig. 1), i.e., images with similar spatial patterns were clustered together, representing a brain state. Here, the term “states” only refers to brain activity patterns rather than cognitive states. Nineteen “brain states” were derived from clustering the resting-state images from SCZ and HC groups (Supplementary Fig. S2). The probability of occurrence was not uniformly distributed across these states, i.e., some states appeared more often than others. The occurrence rate of each state was calculated as the percentage of frames assigned to the state.
Among the 19 states, we identified three states (States A, B, and C; see Supplementary Fig. S2) that demonstrated a reduced occurrence rate in SCZ, compared to HC (p < 0.001, p < 0.001, p = 0.012, respectively, FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons, Fig. 2, see also Supplementary Fig. S3 for individual data points). No states showed an increased occurrence rate in SCZ compared to HC. Maps of these states were compared to seven canonical functional networks previously described [46] (see Supplementary Fig. S4). State A was characterized by strong activation in the fronto-parietal network (FPN) and deactivation in the motor-sensory network (MN). State B had the opposite pattern, i.e., deactivation in the FPN and strong activation in the MN. State C involved strong activation in the visual network (VN) and the salience network (SN), including the insula and angular gyrus, and deactivation in the default mode network. In the SCZ group, the occurrence rate of these three states was uncorrelated with PANSS positive (p > 0.66) or negative (p > 0.32) symptom scores, or dose of antipsychotic medication (p > 0.17).
The spatial distribution of brain states did not differ between SCZ and HC
Although three states occurred less often in SCZ than in HC, the spatial maps of these states appeared to be similar between the two groups (Spearman correlations between the state maps of two groups: r = 0.94, 0.90, 0.92, respectively, see Supplementary Fig. S5). To further examine this similarity, we derived brain states from an independent dataset, the 100 healthy subjects that were used to determine the optimal cluster number (i.e., the test–retest dataset). Maps of States A, B, and C derived from the test–retest dataset resembled those derived from the HC and SCZ groups (Spearman correlations with the maps from HC: r = 0.94, 0.90, 0.84 for States A, B, and C, respectively; correlations with maps from SCZ: r = 0.91, 0.81, 0.77, respectively). We then used the states derived from the test–retest dataset as cluster centers. In the HC and SCZ groups, we calculated spatial distance (Euclidean distance) from each frame to these cluster centers. For all three states, there was no difference in Euclidean distance from the frames to the cluster centers between the SCZ and HC groups (all p > 0.05). These observations indicate that for each state, images from both groups are equally distributed around the cluster center defined in an independent dataset, confirming the spatial similarity of these images.
Occurrence rate of State C was significantly reduced in PE
To identify neural correlates of psychosis that are not confounded by medication effects or history of illness, we also derived 19 states from the rs-fMRI data of the PE and the HY groups. The 19 states were identical to those described above. We found that only one state, State C, differed between the PE and HY groups. This state occurred at a significantly lower rate in PE than in HY (p < 0.001, Fig. 3). However, the occurrence rates of State A and State B did not differ between the PE and HY groups (p = 0.16, p = 0.20, respectively).
The spatial map of State C also did not differ between the PE and HY groups (Spearman correlation: r = 0.910, comparison of distance from frames to cluster center between the two groups: p = 0.79). These observations indicated that, similar to our findings in SCZ, the spatial characteristics of these dynamic brain states are preserved in individuals with PEs.
The occurrence rate of brain State C was associated with the severity of PEs
State C occurred at a lower rate in both SCZ and PE, compared to their controls, suggesting a specific association of this state to psychosis across a spectrum of severity. Thus, we examined whether the occurrence rate of State C could predict symptom severity in the full cohort of young adults that included the PE and HY groups, plus an additional 86 subjects with a low or intermediate level of PEs. Within this cohort (n = 130), the occurrence rate of State C showed a small correlation (r = −0.26, p = 0.003) with the number of PEs, as reflected by the subjects’ total PDI score, indicating that the loss of State C is associated with the level of severity of PEs (explaining 6.7% of the variance in PEs; Fig. 4).
Static functional connectivity analyses
To investigate how the loss of dynamic brain states is related to changes in conventional “static” functional connectivity, we selected three brain regions that were most activated in States A, B, and C and then computed functional connectivity between these selected seed regions and the rest of the brain (Supplementary Fig. S6). The static connectivity maps of these seed regions resembled the maps of dynamic brain states (all ps < 0.001; see Supplementary Table S1). Moreover, compared to HC, the SCZ group showed reduced connectivity (p < 0.05, FDR corrected, Supplementary Fig. S6) between the seeds and their connected regions, indicating that reduced occurrence of these states in patients with SCZ may be associated with reduced “static” functional connectivity between brain regions that are co-activated in these states. Similarly, compared to HY, the PE group also showed a trend of reduced connectivity (but did not reach significance after FDR correction) between a seed in the visual cortex and its connected regions (Supplementary Fig. S6). This comparison of analysis approaches in the same cohort suggests that many of the prior reports of diminished functional connectivity in SCZ may be partially attributable to a reduction in the rate of recruitment of certain networks. Indeed, we further found that occurrence rates of States A, B, and C were correlated with static connectivity among the regions co-activated in these states (Supplementary Fig. S7).
Potential confounding factors
Analyses of data collected in healthy subjects scanned twice during rest, with eyes closed or open (Dataset III), indicated that our findings were not related to reductions in visual input in the SCZ or PE groups (see Supplementary results and Supplementary Fig. S8). In addition, we found that our results were not related to the selection of the number of clusters (see Supplementary results and Supplementary Fig. S9).
Discussion
In this study, measurement of the occurrence rate of “brain states,” i.e., co-activated brain areas during one acquisition of rs-fMRI (collected over 3 s), revealed three brain states that occurred at a lower rate in SCZ compared to HC. Of these three states, one also occurred at a diminished rate in individuals with persistent PEs. Moreover, the occurrence rate of this specific brain state was negatively correlated with the severity of PEs in a larger group of youth. In contrast, the spatial distribution of this state was unaffected in both the SCZ and PE groups.
Reduced engagement of FPN, MN, and VN–SN in established psychotic illness
Our results indicate that SCZ is associated with diminished recruitment of states involving the FPN, MN, and VN–SN. These results are consistent with and extend findings of prior neuroimaging studies of SCZ. Altered activation of the FPN in SCZ is a common finding of prior studies [10, 47] and has been linked to impairments in cognitive control, decision making, and the ability to initiate and persist in goal-directed behavior [48–52]. Similarly, altered sensorimotor and visual processes have been frequently reported in SCZ [53–55]. Prior studies have also revealed abnormalities in the SN in psychotic illness [56–59]. The current results suggest that a selective failure to appropriately recruit certain “brain states” that involve these networks may be present in SCZ.
Reduced engagement of the VN–SN is also evident in individuals with persistent PEs
A diminished occurrence rate of the VN–SN state was also found in the PE group. Moreover, in the larger cohort of 130 youth (of which the PE and HY groups were subsets), reduced occurrence rate of this state was related to the severity of PEs. These results are consistent with prior evidence for a role of the SN and the insula in particular, a key node of the SN, in the pathophysiology of psychotic symptoms [60] (i.e., the replicated associations found between the activity or volume of the insula and psychotic symptoms in SCZ [61–67]). Although the mechanisms underlying these associations remain unclear, the current data taken together with evidence that the insula coordinates responses of widely distributed sensory and attentional networks and mediates transitions (“switching”) between externally directed and introspective processing [56, 68–70] suggest that impairments in SN-mediated transitions may occur in psychosis [56, 57]. Also, the insula is involved in selecting or prioritizing sensory information for the purpose of generating appropriate responses to that information [70–72]. Thus, our findings suggest that reduced recruitment of VN and SN may be associated with an impairment in this selection process, potentially contributing to the development of positive symptoms [57, 73].
Model of progressive alterations in sensory, attentional, and higher-order neural systems in psychosis
Our finding of selectively impaired engagement of the SN–VN in the PE group, with the additional loss of function of the MN and FPN in SCZ, is suggestive of a progressive involvement of these networks in the pathophysiology underlying psychotic illness. We speculate that an initial disruption of sensory-salience processing at an early, subthreshold stage of illness may be followed by changes in the MN and FPN at later stages in the evolution of the illness. This model is consistent with studies showing that very early “psychosis risk” states are often characterized by the presence of mild sensory processing or cognitive changes that may involve inappropriately ascribing “salience” to incoming sensory information due to spontaneous neural signals that are independent of environmental stimuli (e.g., “basic symptoms” [74], “anomalous experiences” [75], or “aberrant salience” [76, 77]). For most, PEs are transient, often stress related and do not recur [78]. However, for a high-risk subset, these symptoms are distressing and persistent and, in some, associated with clinical progression. Progression may occur due to ongoing, persistent disruptions of sensory and salience processing, which ultimately interferes with cognitive and basic motor processes and function of the associated networks. Alternatively, the additional changes (the reduction in occurrence of States A and B) found in SCZ here, relative to those with PEs, could represent a long-term consequence of having the illness (e.g., the effects of chronic treatment with antipsychotic medication, a lack of employment or social activity). Further longitudinal work is required to test these competing models, by tracking activity of these brain states during sequential stages of illness. If such a model of progression is confirmed, low engagement of the SN–VN could represent a marker of a very early, at-risk state.
Psychotic experiences: associated risk for clinical psychosis and relationship to other prodromal states
PEs have been studied primarily in non–help-seeking, general population samples with prevalence rates conservatively estimated at ~7%. The overall risk associated with PEs for transition to clinical psychosis is estimated at ~8% [35, 36], but some studies report higher transition rates (e.g., ~25% [37]). The risk of transition depends on a number of factors such as the degree of persistence of the PEs, the presence of affective symptoms, and environmental factors [28, 79].
In contrast, the “clinical high risk” (CHR) category of the psychosis prodrome, which is also primarily defined by the presence of attenuated psychotic symptoms, includes only those who have become help-seeking and distressed, and show recent clinical deterioration [80]. Thus, the CHR state is associated with a much higher level of risk for developing psychotic illness than PEs [81]. Also, both PEs and the CHR state, while increasing risk for psychotic illness, are associated with an even greater likelihood of having or developing a non-psychotic affective or anxiety disorder [28, 82]. Given the high levels of heterogeneity within these two overlapping categories and their association with transdiagnostic risk for neuropsychiatric illness overall, objective measures that can determine who are most vulnerable of those with these risk factors are clearly needed.
Limitations
A limitation of this study is that, in order to maximize feasibility and recruitment success, the members of the PE group were not clinically assessed for the presence of psychotic disorders. However, it is unlikely that any of the subjects of this study were acutely psychotic given that none were taking antipsychotic medications and all were fully enrolled in college at the time of the study. In addition, the cross-sectional nature of the study’s design limits inferences regarding the relationship between the findings in the PE and SCZ groups; follow-up longitudinal studies can further test the model of illness progression suggested by these results.
It is not yet known whether the changes observed here in the occurrence of these transient brain states in the SCZ and PE groups reflect a relatively reversible impairment in neural responsiveness or synchrony, or a more enduring change in the integrity of the neurons within these networks or of their connections. However, the relationships between the occurrence of brain states and static connectivity suggest that changes in these brain states may be associated with enduring changes in functional coupling and network function. This hypothesis can be directly tested in future studies.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
DJH receives support from the Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) Executive Committee on Research (the MGH Research Scholar Award), the Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation, the Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, the Massachusetts Department of Mental Health Center of Excellence for Systemic and Psychosocial Research Services at MGH, and NIMH (RO1MH109562). HL receives support from the National Key Research and Development Program of China Grant No. 2016YFC1306303; the National Natural Science Foundation of China Grant No. 81790650, No. 81790652, and by NIH Grants R01NS091604, P50MH106435, P50DA046373, P20GM109040, R01DC017991, and R21MH121831. DW receives support from NIH Grant K01MH111802 and the NARSAD Young Investigator Award. This work was also supported by NIMH Grant K23MH084059 (JLR). The authors thank Dr. Randy L. Buckner for assistance with data collection.
Code availability
Codes used to generate these results are available upon request (please contact HL).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
HL is on the chief scientific advisory board for Neural Galaxy LLC and is listed as an inventor in issued patents on brain imaging that are unrelated to this work. JLR has received research support from Pamlab. MF’s lifetime financial disclosures are listed here: http://mghcme.org/faculty/faculty-detail/maurizio_fava.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Hesheng Liu, Email: hesheng.liu@mgh.harvard.edu.
Daphne J. Holt, Email: dholt@mgh.harvard.edu
Supplementary information
The online version of this article (10.1038/s41380-020-00983-1) contains Supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
References
- 1.Friston KJ, Frith CD. Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci. 1995;3:89–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McGuire PK, Frith CD. Disordered functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1996;26:663–7. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700037673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:509–27. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pettersson-Yeo W, Allen P, Benetti S, McGuire P, Mechelli A. Dysconnectivity in schizophrenia: where are we now? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1110–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM. Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:49–76. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Thermenos HW, Milanovic S, Tsuang MT, Faraone SV, McCarley RW, et al. Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:1279–84. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809141106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Anticevic A, Cole MW, Repovs G, Murray JD, Brumbaugh MS, Winkler AM. Characterizing thalamo-cortical disturbances in schizophrenia and bipolar illness. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:3116–30. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cole MW, Anticevic A, Repovs G, Barch D. Variable global dysconnectivity and individual differences in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mamah D, Barch DM, Repovs G. Resting state functional connectivity of five neural networks in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. 2013;150:601–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Baker JT, Holmes AJ, Masters GA, Yeo BT, Krienen F, Buckner RL, et al. Disruption of cortical association networks in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:109–18. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Woodward ND, Rogers B, Heckers S. Functional resting-state networks are differentially affected in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;130:86–93. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dong D, Wang Y, Chang X, Luo C, Yao D. Dysfunction of large-scale brain networks in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44:168–81. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chang C, Glover GH. Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage. 2010;50:81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cannon TD. How schizophrenia develops: cognitive and brain mechanisms underlying onset of psychosis. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19:744–56. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Keshavan MS, Giedd J, Lau JY, Lewis DA, Paus T. Changes in the adolescent brain and the pathophysiology of psychotic disorders. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1:549–58. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00081-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bressler SL, Kelso JA. Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:26–36. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01564-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Allen EA, Bandettini PA, Calhoun VD, Corbetta M, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2013;80:360–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bassett DS, Wymbs NF, Porter MA, Mucha PJ, Carlson JM, Grafton ST. Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:7641–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018985108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ma S, Calhoun VD, Phlypo R, Adali T. Dynamic changes of spatial functional network connectivity in healthy individuals and schizophrenia patients using independent vector analysis. Neuroimage. 2014;90:196–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Du Y, Pearlson GD, Yu Q, He H, Lin D, Sui J, et al. Interaction among subsystems within default mode network diminished in schizophrenia patients: a dynamic connectivity approach. Schizophr Res. 2016;170:55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Miller RL, Yaesoubi M, Turner JA, Mathalon D, Preda A, Pearlson G, et al. Higher dimensional meta-state analysis reveals reduced resting fMRI connectivity dynamism in schizophrenia patients. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0149849. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Damaraju E, Allen EA, Belger A, Ford JM, McEwen S, Mathalon DH, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity analysis reveals transient states of dysconnectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;5:298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Yu Q, Erhardt EB, Sui J, Du Y, He H, Hjelm D, et al. Assessing dynamic brain graphs of time-varying connectivity in fMRI data: application to healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2015;107:345–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Reinen JM, Chen OY, Hutchison RM, Yeo BTT, Anderson KM, Sabuncu MR, et al. The human cortex possesses a reconfigurable dynamic network architecture that is disrupted in psychosis. Nat Commun. 2018;9:1157. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03462-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Mitra A, Gordon EM, Gratton C, Adeyemo B, et al. On the stability of BOLD fMRI correlations. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:4719–32. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Liu X, Duyn JH. Time-varying functional network information extracted from brief instances of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:4392–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216856110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Med. 2009;39:179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kirsch P, Ronshausen S, Mier D, Gallhofer B. The influence of antipsychotic treatment on brain reward system reactivity in schizophrenia patients. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2007;40:196–8. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-984463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lui S, Li T, Deng W, Jiang L, Wu Q, Tang H, et al. Short-term effects of antipsychotic treatment on cerebral function in drug-naive first-episode schizophrenia revealed by “resting state” functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:783–92. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dominguez MD, Wichers M, Lieb R, Wittchen HU, van Os J. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8-year cohort study. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:84–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.van Os J, Linscott RJ. Introduction: the extended psychosis phenotype-relationship with schizophrenia and with ultrahigh risk status for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:227–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Guloksuz S, van Os J. The slow death of the concept of schizophrenia and the painful birth of the psychosis spectrum. Psychol Med. 2018;48:229–44. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kelleher I, Cannon M. Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychological medicine. 2011;41:1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hanssen M, Bak M, Bijl R, Vollebergh W, van Os J. The incidence and outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44:181–91. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. 2016;15:118–24. doi: 10.1002/wps.20310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H. Children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1053–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Drakesmith M, Caeyenberghs K, Dutt A, Zammit S, Evans CJ, Reichenberg A, et al. Schizophrenia-like topological changes in the structural connectome of individuals with subclinical psychotic experiences. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2629–43. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Orr JM, Turner JA, Mittal VA. Widespread brain dysconnectivity associated with psychotic-like experiences in the general population. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;4:343–51. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Satterthwaite TD, Vandekar SN, Wolf DH, Bassett DS, Ruparel K, Shehzad Z, et al. Connectome-wide network analysis of youth with psychosis-spectrum symptoms. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:1508–15. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Calkins ME, Vandekar SN, Erus G, Ruparel K, et al. Structural brain abnormalities in youth with psychosis spectrum symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:515–24. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.3463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Calkins ME, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Hopson RD, et al. Functional neuroimaging abnormalities in youth with psychosis spectrum symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:456–65. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Eryilmaz H, Tanner AS, Ho NF, Nitenson AZ, Silverstein NJ, Petruzzi LJ, et al. Disrupted working memory circuitry in schizophrenia: disentangling fMRI markers of core pathology vs other aspects of impaired performance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:2411–20. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang D, Buckner RL, Fox MD, Holt DJ, Holmes AJ, Stoecklein S, et al. Parcellating cortical functional networks in individuals. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1853. doi: 10.1038/nn.4164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Power JD, Plitt M, Laumann TO, Martin A. Sources and implications of whole-brain fMRI signals in humans. Neuroimage. 2017;146:609–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tu PC, Lee YC, Chen YS, Li CT, Su TP. Schizophrenia and the brain’s control network: aberrant within- and between-network connectivity of the frontoparietal network in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;147:339–47. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:3328–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Poppe AB, Barch DM, Carter CS, Gold JM, Ragland JD, Silverstein SM, et al. Reduced frontoparietal activity in schizophrenia is linked to a specific deficit in goal maintenance: a multisite functional imaging study. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:1149–57. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ray KL, Lesh TA, Howell AM, Salo TP, Ragland JD, MacDonald AW, et al. Functional network changes and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;15:161–70. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998;280:747–9. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Weinberger DR, Gallhofer B. Cognitive function in schizophrenia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;12:S29–36. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199709004-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kaufmann T, Skatun KC, Alnaes D, Doan NT, Duff EP, Tonnesen S, et al. Disintegration of sensorimotor brain networks in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41:1326–35. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Butler PD, Silverstein SM, Dakin SC. Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:40–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Javitt DC. Sensory processing in schizophrenia: neither simple nor intact. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:1059–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Palaniyappan L, Liddle PF. Does the salience network play a cardinal role in psychosis? An emerging hypothesis of insular dysfunction. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:17–27. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Palaniyappan L, Simmonite M, White TP, Liddle EB, Liddle PF. Neural primacy of the salience processing system in schizophrenia. Neuron. 2013;79:814–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Manoliu A, Riedl V, Zherdin A, Muhlau M, Schwerthoffer D, Scherr M, et al. Aberrant dependence of default mode/central executive network interactions on anterior insular salience network activity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:428–37. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wylie KP, Tregellas JR. The role of the insula in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;123:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Crespo-Facorro B, Kim J, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Bockholt HJ, Magnotta V. Insular cortex abnormalities in schizophrenia: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study of first-episode patients. Schizophr Res. 2000;46:35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00028-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Linnman C, Coombs G, III, Goff DC, Holt DJ. Lack of insula reactivity to aversive stimuli in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;143:150–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Ho BC, Andreasen NC. Insular cortex abnormalities in schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms and typical neuroleptic exposure. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:394–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Koutsouleris N, Gaser C, Jager M, Bottlender R, Frodl T, Holzinger S, et al. Structural correlates of psychopathological symptom dimensions in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1600–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Jardri R, Pouchet A, Pins D, Thomas P. Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:73–81. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Shapleske J, Rossell SL, Chitnis XA, Suckling J, Simmons A, Bullmore ET, et al. A computational morphometric MRI study of schizophrenia: effects of hallucinations. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1331–41. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.12.1331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Modinos G, Vercammen A, Mechelli A, Knegtering H, McGuire PK, Aleman A. Structural covariance in the hallucinating brain: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:465–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–15. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Amin H, Rykhlevskaia E, Nguyen DA, Greicius MD, et al. Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2636–46. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Chang LJ, Yarkoni T, Khaw MW, Sanfey AG. Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:739–49. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Powers AR, Mathys C, Corlett PR. Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science. 2017;357:596–600. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Schultze-Lutter F. Subjective symptoms of schizophrenia in research and the clinic: the basic symptom concept. Schizophrenia Bull. 2009;35:5–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Maher BA. The relationship between delusions and hallucinations. Curr psychiatry Rep. 2006;8:179–83. doi: 10.1007/s11920-006-0021-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Roiser JP, Howes OD, Chaddock CA, Joyce EM, McGuire P. Neural and behavioral correlates of aberrant salience in individuals at risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bull. 2013;39:1328–36. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Linscott R, Van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Med. 2013;43:1133–49. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Dominguez M, Wichers M, Lieb R, Wittchen H-U, van Os J. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8-year cohort study. Schizophrenia Bull. 2011;37:84–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.McGlashan TH, Walsh BC, Woods SW. Structured interview for psychosis-risk syndromes. New Haven, CT: Yale School of Medicine; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fusar-Poli P, Bonoldi I, Yung AR, Borgwardt S, Kempton MJ, Valmaggia L, et al. Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch Gen psychiatry. 2012;69:220–9. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, Addington J, Riecher-Rössler A, Schultze-Lutter F, et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:107–20. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Codes used to generate these results are available upon request (please contact HL).