Abstract
There is molecular, electrophysiological, and ultrastructural evidence that a net increase in synaptic strength occurs in many brain circuits during spontaneous wake (SW) or short sleep deprivation, reflecting ongoing learning. Sleep leads instead to a broad but selective weakening of many forebrain synapses, thus preventing synaptic saturation and decreasing the energy cost of synaptic activity. Whether synaptic potentiation can persist or further increase after long sleep deprivation is unknown. Whether synaptic renormalization can occur during chronic sleep restriction (CSR) is also unknown. Here, we addressed these questions by measuring an established ultrastructural measure of synaptic strength, the axon-spine interface (ASI), in the primary motor cortex (M1) of (1) one-month-old adolescent mice CSR using a paradigm that decreases NREM and REM sleep by two/thirds; (2) in two-week-old mouse pups sleep deprived for 15 h, or allowed afterward to recover for 16 h. Both groups were compared with mice of the same age that were asleep or awake for a few hours (both sexes). The ASI size of CSR mice (n = 3) was comparable to that measured after SW or short sleep deprivation and larger than after sleep (n = 4/group). In pups, the ASI size increased after short sleep loss (n = 3) relative to sleep (n = 4), fell below sleep levels after long sleep deprivation (n = 4), and remained low after recovery (n = 3). Long sleep deprived pups also lost some weight. These results suggest that (1) severe sleep restriction is incompatible with synaptic renormalization; (2) very young mice cannot maintain high synaptic strength during prolonged wake.
Significance Statement
The strength of many excitatory synapses increases during wake and decreases during sleep. However, because strong synapses require more energy, synaptic potentiation may be difficult to maintain when wake is enforced well beyond its physiological duration, especially in young animals whose brain is growing. Moreover, because synaptic weakening ultimately requires structural changes and the endocytosis of excitatory receptors, limited and disrupted sleep may not be compatible with synaptic renormalization. We confirmed the first prediction in two-week-old pups kept awake for 15 h, and the second prediction in one-month-old mice severely sleep restricted for 4 d.
Introduction
We recently used serial block-face scanning electron microscopy (SBEM) to obtain high-resolution measurements of the axon-spine interface (ASI), the direct area of contact between presynapse and postsynapse, in thousands of excitatory glutamatergic synapses of the mouse brain (Cirelli and Tononi, 2020). The ASI is a morphological measure of synaptic strength whose size correlates with the amplitude of the evoked unitary excitatory postsynaptic currents and with the area of the postsynaptic density (Cheetham et al., 2014). Like the latter, the ASI increases with synaptic potentiation (Desmond and Levy, 1988; Buchs and Muller, 1996; Fukazawa et al., 2003; Cheetham et al., 2014). The goal of our experiments was to use the ASI to test the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis of sleep function (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014, 2020). According to this hypothesis, ongoing learning during wake leads to a net increase in synaptic strength in many brain circuits, and sleep is needed to renormalize synaptic weights to save energy, avoid saturation, and benefit memory consolidation and integration. So far, this hypothesis has been tested in the superficial layers of primary motor and sensory cortex (de Vivo et al., 2017), as well as in the stratum radiatum of the CA1 region of the hippocampus (Spano et al., 2019). In all cases, we found that the size of the ASI was on average smaller in one-month-old adolescent mice that slept for 6–7 h during the light phase, compared to mice that during the same time of day were kept awake by exposure to novel objects. In the primary motor and sensory cortex the ASI was also larger after 6–7 h of spontaneous wake (SW) during the dark phase compared to after sleep, and the average size of the ASI was similar in mice awake at night and in mice sleep deprived during the day (de Vivo et al., 2017). We also found that, in primary motor cortex (M1), the ASI was larger in two-week-old mouse pups kept awake for 4–5 h during the day as compared to siblings allowed to sleep during the same time window (de Vivo et al., 2019). Together, these findings show that the ASI of cortical synapses is larger after wake than after sleep independent of circadian time, both in early development and during adolescence, in line with the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014, 2020).
In the experiments performed so far, mice were spontaneously awake or totally sleep deprived for only a few hours. It is not known how more severe acute sleep loss, or chronic sleep restriction (CSR), affects the ASI. To address these questions, we measured the size of the ASI in two sets of animals: (1) one-month-old adolescent mice whose sleep was restricted to a third of the normal daily amount for 4–5 d; (2) two-week-old mouse pups forced to be awake for up to 15 consecutive hours. To further assess the effects of severe sleep loss, in the same groups of mice we also analyzed several other features of the organelles inside the spines.
Materials and Methods
Animals
We used homozygous B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)16Jrs/J transgenic mice (YFP-H, The Jackson Laboratory; IMSR catalog #JAX:003709, 78RRID:IMSR_JAX:003709) that express yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) in a subset of cortical pyramidal neurons (Feng et al., 2000). The developmental changes in the sleep/wake pattern of these mice have been characterized in previous studies (Nelson et al., 2013; de Vivo et al., 2019), and this strain was used in previous SBEM experiments (de Vivo et al., 2017, 2019; Spano et al., 2019). All animal facilities were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the University of Wisconsin-Madison, and were inspected and accredited by Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care (AAALAC).
Experimental groups for SBEM studies
Postnatal day (P)13 mice
B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)16Jrs/J mice were inspected every day for new pups. P0 was defined as the day of birth. The entire litters (both sexes) and the dams were housed together in environmentally controlled recording chambers with free access to food and water (12/12 h light/dark cycle; lights on at 8 A.M.). At the beginning of each dark period a running wheel and one to two novel objects were introduced in each recording chamber, to ensure enriched housing conditions and facilitate the light/dark entrainment of the rest/activity cycle. Running wheels and novel objects were removed during the light period. The day of the experiment (P13), at the onset of the light phase, mice were left undisturbed for 4–5 h [sleep (S), four mice], or sleep deprived as much as possible for 4–5 h [extended waking (EW)4, three mice], or for 15–16 h (EW15, four mice) using a combination of different stimuli including novel objects and gentle handling, during which the cage was gently tilted, the pups were touched with a soft brush, or they were gently poked by the experimenter. These stimuli were applied any time the pups attempted to fall asleep. Another group was sleep deprived for ∼15 h and then allowed to sleep ad libitum for 15–16 h (Rec, three mice). S and EW4 pups came from the same litter, and so did EW15 and recovery (Rec) mice. The exact duration of sleep or EW varied because it took ∼1 h to perfuse the entire litter, comprising seven to eight pups. In a previous study (de Vivo et al., 2019), we performed behavioral sleep/wake scoring in 30 s epochs in pairs of P13 Thy1-YFP siblings housed with the dam and found that during the light phase these pups spend roughly 50% of each hour asleep, independent of circadian time. In that study (de Vivo et al., 2019), we also performed SBEM experiments in pups allowed to sleep or sleep deprived for 4–5 h (de Vivo et al., 2019), but the results reported here are from different sets of S and EW4 animals that were collected together with the EW15 and Rec mice. As previously reported (de Vivo et al., 2019), even after the first few hours of forced wake, pups responded to the stimuli much less than older mice, and it was impossible to keep them fully awake for extended periods of time. For this reason, the long sleep deprivation experiment, which was planned to last for 24 h, was terminated after 15–16 h. In the previous (de Vivo et al., 2019) and the current SBEM experiments, entire litters were either left undisturbed or sleep deprived. While the behavior was monitored continuously, the behavioral scoring of each pup in the litter was too difficult to perform.
Adolescent (P30) mice
B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)16Jrs/J mice were maintained on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8 A.M.) with food and water available ad libitum. CSR (n = 3 mice; 2 males) started at P26 (day 1) at the beginning of the light phase and ended on day 5 (P30) 6 h after lights on. The mice were constantly monitored by direct visual inspection and with infrared cameras during the entire duration of the experiment. During the light period animals were kept awake using a combination of stimuli, including exposure to novel objects and a running wheel, social interactions with their siblings, and by changing cages and bedding. Room lights were also turned off for brief periods in the first 6 h of the light period (∼20 min/h for a total of 2 h) to provide an additional arousal stimulus and prevent the occurrence of NREM sleep. In previous studies (Alföldi et al., 1991; Benca et al., 1998; Miller et al., 1998), short periods of darkness during the light phase had no major effects on the daily amounts and circadian patterns of sleep and wake, while acutely promoted REM sleep and wake at the expenses of NREM sleep. Note that the promoting effects on REM sleep occurred in rats that could sleep ad libitum, while our mice were forced to stay awake. Every night, our mice were housed for several hours on a conveyer over water apparatus that runs constantly at slow speed; this apparatus allows the animals to sleep for brief periods (<8 s) before being forced to move or fall into the water. This CSR method was selected because, by relying on different types of stimuli, is effective and enforces wake in a realistic manner. Specifically, in a previous study using mice implanted for chronic EEG recordings we found that this paradigm of CSR reduces total sleep time across the 4 d by ∼70% (first day 80%; last day 63%), including a decrease of 68% in NREM sleep and of 79% in REM sleep relative to baseline (de Vivo et al., 2016). In the current study, as well as in previous ultrastructural studies (de Vivo et al., 2017), the mice were not implanted with EEG electrodes to avoid possible damage to the cortex and risk of inflammation. Instead, we relied on behavior, the presence of locomotor activity, and the ability to respond to a stimulus to assess the effectiveness of the CSR protocol. While behavior was continuously monitored, a behavioral scoring was not performed, because of the difficulty of following simultaneously several CSR mice. The CSR mice were compared with mice of the same age (P30, of either sex) killed after 6–7 h of spontaneous S or sleep deprivation (EW) during the day, and 6–7 h of SW at night (four mice/group). Behavioral states in these mice were determined by continuous monitoring with infrared cameras and motor activity was quantified by custom-made video-based motion detection algorithms, as previously detailed (de Vivo et al., 2017). We previously found that video-monitoring consistently estimates total sleep time with at least 90% accuracy, although it cannot distinguish NREM sleep from REM sleep (Maret et al., 2011). SW mice were euthanized during the dark phase (∼2–3:30 A.M.) after a period of wake of at least 1 h, interrupted by periods of sleep of <5 min, and after spending at least 70% of the previous 6–7 h awake. S mice were euthanized during the light period (∼2–4:45 P.M.), after a long period of sleep of >45 min, interrupted by periods of wake of <4 min, and after spending at least 75% of the previous 6–7 h asleep. EW mice were kept awake during the first part of the light period (6–7 h) by exposure to novel objects and running wheel and by tapping on the cage whenever the animals appeared drowsy. The changes in ASI size in the M1 and primary sensory cortex of S, EW and SW mice have been published before (de Vivo et al., 2017), the data from M1 of these animals are shown here to provide a reference against which to compare the results in the CSR group. All four groups of mice (CSR, S, EW, SW) were collected at the same time and brain samples were processed and imaged using serial electron microscopy within the same period of a few months, alternating between mice of the different experimental groups. The initial segmentation of the spines for the CSR group was also done concurrently with that of the other P30 mice by the same trained annotators, who were blind to experimental condition. Because of the time-consuming nature of manual segmentation, however, and the decision to focus first on the results pertaining the normal sleep/wake cycle and short sleep deprivation conditions (de Vivo et al., 2017), the final analysis of the CSR mice was postponed and completed only for the current study. For the final analysis, as in the previous study (de Vivo et al., 2017), all segmentation data were further tested for accuracy and consistency by the same experienced tracer (L.d.V.).
Experiments to assess weight loss during sleep deprivation
B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)16Jrs/J mice were maintained on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8 A.M.) with food and water available ad libitum. Mice were inspected every day for new pups and P0 was defined as the day of birth. At P12, each mouse (of either sex) was marked on its tail and weighed. At P13, mice were assigned to two experimental groups: in the control group pups were kept with their dam and left undisturbed during the experiment. In the sleep deprivation group, pups were kept with their dam but were deprived of sleep by gentle handling and novel objects for 16 h from zeitgeber time (ZT)0 to ZT16. To monitor the body weight of the pups, we weighed them as frequently as possible while trying not to disturb their sleep/wake behavior. Specifically, at P13 the sleep deprived pups were weighted every 4 h, while the control mice were weighted only at ZT0 and ZT16. To monitor the recovery from the weight loss, we also weighed all pups at ZT10 on the day after sleep deprivation.
Tissue preparation for electron microscopy and image acquisition
Methods have been described in detail in previous studies (de Vivo et al., 2017, 2019). Blocks of tissue (1 mm2) encompassing the M1 (1.85 mm anterior to bregma, 1.5 mm lateral) were stained blind to experimental condition and serial images were obtained using a ΣIGMA VP field emission scanning electron microscope (Carl Zeiss NTS Ltd) equipped with 3View technology (Gatan Inc.) and a backscattered electron detector (aperture of 30 μm, high vacuum, acceleration voltage of 1.5–2 kV, image size of 5000 × 5000 pixels, image resolution in the xy-plane of 4 nm). Nominal thickness of the ultrathin sections was 40–50 nm. Stacks of ∼500 images each were acquired per mouse (∼10,000–21,000 μm3) in layer 2 of M1, as in the previous studies (de Vivo et al., 2017, 2019). Images were Gaussian filtered and automatically aligned using the open source software Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012). Average actual section thickness for each stack was estimated using the cylindrical diameters method as previously described (Fiala and Harris, 2001), and was comparable across conditions. Dendritic segments and all their protrusions were segmented manually in TrakEM2 (Cardona et al., 2012) by trained annotators who were blind to experimental condition. We randomly selected spiny dendritic segments whose diameter ranged between 0.44 and 2.24 μm (P13) and between 0.54 and 1.74 μm (P30). Densely spiny dendrites located in layer 2 include intermediate and terminal dendrites of layer 3 and 5 pyramidal neurons, as well as basal and oblique dendrites of layer 2 pyramidal neurons (Larkman, 1991; van Aerde and Feldmeyer, 2015). To focus on pyramidal cells, we excluded dendritic segments with few or no spines, as well as dendrites with few spines and whose number of shaft synapses exceeded the number of spine synapses.
As described before (de Vivo et al., 2017), all protrusions were defined as “spines” (in agreement with Holtmaat and Svoboda, 2009), and distinguished in spines with or without synapses. Criteria to define a synapse included the presence of a presynaptic bouton with at least one to two synaptic vesicles within a 50-nm distance from the cellular membrane facing the spine, a visible synaptic cleft and a postsynaptic density. Distribution of dendritic diameters was balanced across experimental groups (P13, p = 0.45; P30, p = 0.15). In each spine the presence/absence of the following structures was recorded: spine apparatus, one or more spinulae in the head or neck of the spine, and mitochondria in the dendritic shaft close to the spine neck and in the axonal bouton. In almost all cases a mitochondrion was present along the dendritic shaft in the vicinity of the spine neck, while mitochondria were almost never found inside the spine head. The presence of components of the non-smooth endoplasmic reticulum (non-SER), classified according to Cooney et al. (2002), was also recorded. The non-SER elements include tubules, small uncoated vesicles, large coated or uncoated vesicles, and multivesicular bodies (MVBs), and are distinct from the continuous network of SER present throughout the dendritic shaft (Cooney et al., 2002).
As described before (de Vivo et al., 2017, 2019), the ASI was used as the ultrastructural measure of synaptic strength and traced at the interface between the spine head and the presynaptic terminal or bouton. We focused on the ASI because in SBEM images its exact borders are easier to identify than those of the postsynaptic density (Holcomb et al., 2013).
Although the ASI is a less established measure than the postsynaptic density, its size correlates with the size of the postsynaptic density and of the spine head (Cheetham et al., 2014), which in turn correlate with the synaptic expression of glutamatergic AMPA receptors and with the amplitude of synaptic excitatory currents (Nusser et al., 1998; Matsuzaki et al., 2001; Katz et al., 2009).
Specifically, the region of contact between the two apposed objects was outlined in each individual section using the arealist brush suitably set at 1 pixel size. In this way, a quasi two-dimensional sheet-like object representing the interfaced region was created along the z dimension. The total surface area was calculated by computing the smoothed upper bound surface, according to the formula:
where n is the number of sections, a and b are the traced elements at the top and bottom of a section k of thickness T, Ps is the smoothed perimeter, and A is the area (Cardona et al., 2012). Finally, the areas of the traced element in the section k = 1 and in the section k = n were subtracted from the smoothed upper bound surface value and the result was divided by 2 to get an approximate value of the apposed surface.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using linear mixed effects (LMEs) models that include both random and fixed effects. The random effects are used to account for the fact that multiple spines are sampled from the same mouse and dendrite. LME models offer several advantages over repeated measures ANOVA including the ability to handle unbalanced designs, which is the case in this study.
The general form of these models is
where
and
In these models, is a vector of response variables (typically log(ASI) values), is a vector of random effects (mouse and dendrite effects), is a vector of fixed effects, and are design matrices that link the response variable to the random and fixed effects, and is a vector of residual values. Both the random effects () and the residuals () are assumed to have a Gaussian distribution, while the residuals are additionally assumed to be independent with constant variance. Model assumptions of normality and constant variance were assessed and validated graphically using residual plots for all models presented.
For response variables measured at the dendrite level (spine density, dendrite diameter), experimental condition was the only fixed effect included in the models, and mouse was the only random effect. For models at the synapse level (ASI, and interactions with the various organelles), we included dendrite as a potential random effect, and dendrite diameter as a fixed effect. Maximum likelihood estimates of the parameters in the LME models were calculated numerically using the lmer() function of the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015), and the statistical significance of effects was assessed using likelihood ratio tests. For significant effects of condition, pairwise post hoc tests with p values corrected for multiple comparisons were performed using the glht() function in the multcomp package (Bretz et al., 2011).
LME models
The parameter estimates for the LME models (P30: ASI; P13: ASI and synapse density) are shown in Table 1. A log transformation was applied to ASI values and a square-root transformation was applied to spine density values to stabilize variance and normalize residuals. The assumption of normally distributed residual values with constant variance was assessed graphically in the resulting models. Based on a scatter plot of the estimated residuals versus fitted values, and a quantile-quantile plot of the estimated residuals, we found no evidence against these assumptions.
Table 1.
P30 data: ASI model (log transformation) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Random effects | SE | ||
Dendrite (intercept) | 0.0792 | ||
Mouse (intercept) | 0.0705 | ||
Residual | 0.9470 | ||
Fixed effects | Level | Estimate | SE |
Intercept | −1.9129 | 0.0873 | |
Dendrite diameter | Continuous (linear) | 0.1818 | 0.0718 |
Condition | CSR (reference) | 0 | 0 |
EW | −0.0902 | 0.0894 | |
SW | −0.1751 | 0.0864 | |
S | −0.1166 | 0.0845 | |
Endosome | 0.1985 | 0.0644 | |
Condition × endosome | CSR (reference) | 0 | 0 |
EW | 0.0781 | 0.0874 | |
SW | 0.2596 | 0.0840 | |
S | −0.1284 | 0.0831 | |
P13 data: ASI model (log transformation) | |||
Random effects | SE | ||
Dendrite (intercept) | 0.1025 | ||
Mouse (intercept) | 0.0273 | ||
Residual | 0.8974 | ||
Fixed effects | Level | Estimate | SE |
Intercept | −2.1445 | 0.0566 | |
Dendrite diameter | Continuous (linear) | 0.1656 | 0.0522 |
Condition | EW15 (reference) | 0 | 0 |
EW4 | 0.3666 | 0.0483 | |
S | 0.2317 | 0.0460 | |
Rec | −0.1179 | 0.0503 | |
P13 data: density model (spines with synapse; square root transformation) | |||
Random effects | SE | ||
Mouse (intercept) | 0.0380 | ||
Residual | 0.1217 | ||
Fixed effects | Level | Estimate | SE |
Intercept | 0.6077 | 0.0239 | |
Condition | EW15 (reference) | 0 | 0 |
EW4 | −0.1032 | 0.0369 | |
S | −0.0559 | 0.0347 | |
Rec | −0.0344 | 0.0384 |
Scaling analysis. To assess the presence of a scaling relationship, we tested the hypothesis that H0: f1(x) = f2(x – c), i.e., that two probability distributions on a log scale differ only by a location shift. We first estimated c as the difference in the median of the two distributions, and then transformed the data so that under the null hypothesis they will have the same distribution. Finally, we applied the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for equality of distribution between two samples.
Results
Spines with endosomes have larger ASI after CSR than after sleep in one-month-old mice
We previously reported that, in layer 2 of the M1 and primary sensory cortex, the size of the ASI in axospinous excitatory synapses is larger after 6–7 h of SW or sleep deprivation (EW) as compared with 6–7 h of S (de Vivo et al., 2017). Here, we measured the ASI in mice CSR for >4 d (102 h, three CSR mice, 905 spines with synapse), and compared these results with the previously published data from the M1 of S, SW, and EW mice (Table 2; Fig. 1A,B). When the brains were collected, all mice were 30 d old.
Table 2.
P30 | P13 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S | SW | EW | CSR | S | EW4 | EW15 | Rec | |
Total N of dendrites | 38 | 32 | 31 | 31 | 57 | 51 | 54 | 36 |
Total N of measured spines with synapse | 1367 | 1347 | 1167 | 905 | 1066 | 912 | 1398 | 842 |
ASI (μm2, mean ± std) Range (μm2) |
0.26 ± 0.29 0.01–2.04 |
0.30 ± 0.34 0.01–4.02 |
0.29 ± 0.32 0.01–3.54 |
0.31 ± 0.37 0.02–4.61 |
0.25 ± 0.26 0.01–2.17 |
0.30 ± 0.34 0.01–2.60 |
0.20 ± 0.23 0.01–2.33 |
0.19 ± 0.23 0.01–2.01 |
Spine density (all protrusions) (#/μm2, mean ± std) |
0.85 ± 0.24 | 0.77 ± 0.21 | 0.82 ± 0.25 | 0.88 ± 0.23 | 0.40 ± 0.16 | 0.35 ± 0.13 | 0.49 ± 0.16 | 0.46 ± 0.24 |
Spine density (spines with synapse; #/μm2, mean ± std) |
0.70 ± 0.20 | 0.67 ± 0.19 | 0.73 ± 0.24 | 0.73 ± 0.22 | 0.31 ± 0.13 | 0.25 ± 0.10 | 0.38 ± 0.15 | 0.36 ± 0.21 |
Spine density (spines without synapse; #/μm2, mean ± std) |
0.15 ± 0.09 | 0.10 ± 0.06 | 0.10 ± 0.04 | 0.15 ± 0.08 | 0.09 ± 0.05 | 0.10 ± 0.06 | 0.10 ± 0.04 | 0.10 ± 0.06 |
Spines without synapse (%, mean ± std) |
16.0 ± 2.8 | 12.7 ± 3.3 | 11.9 ± 3.2 | 17.4 ± 3.5 | 22.0 ± 5.4 | 26.3 ± 4.4 | 21.8 ± 4.1 | 22.6 ± 5.2 |
Dendrite diameter (μm, mean ± std) |
0.87 ± 0.23 | 0.92 ± 0.24 | 0.84 ± 0.22 | 0.78 ± 0.18 | 0.92 ± 0.33 | 0.94 ± 0.35 | 0.90 ± 0.24 | 1.00 ± 0.30 |
Dendrite length (μm, mean ± std) |
21.9 ± 7.0 | 26.2 ± 4.7 | 22.8 ± 5.9 | 19.3 ± 5.1 | 23.4 ± 8.5 | 26.3 ± 7.1 | 26.7 ± 8.7 | 25.0 ± 5.7 |
Spines with spine apparatus (%, mean ± std) | 26.1 ± 4.3 | 30.5 ± 4.5 | 27.1 ± 3.1 | 30.2 ± 3.5 | 7.4 ± 2.5 | 7.2 ± 1.9 | 5.4 ± 2.3 | 9.4 ± 3.2 |
Spines with non-SER elements (tubules/ vesicles; %, mean ± std) |
49.7 ± 7.0 | 56.3 ± 13.0 | 61.2 ± 6.8 | 54.4 ± 6.2 | 70.1 ± 4.2 | 74.6 ± 7.1 | 72.6 ± 3.9 | 66.0 ± 8.3 |
All protrusions are defined as spines. In oblique spines, the ASI could not be measured because the synapse was oriented obliquely or orthogonally to the cutting plane. std, standard deviation.
We applied an LME model that uses sleep/wake condition as categorical fixed effect, dendrite diameter as linear fixed effect, and mouse and dendrite as random effects (see Materials and Methods). We found a trend main effect of condition (p = 0.0568) and post hoc tests revealed a significant increase in ASI size in the CSR mice as compared with the S mice (p = 0.0466) with no difference between CSR and the other wake groups (EW vs CSR, p = 0.9865; SW vs CSR, p = 0.9874; Fig. 1C). We previously found that the sleep/wake changes in ASI size occur specifically in spines containing vesicles, tubules, coated vesicles and/or MVBs, which together represented the majority of the spines (de Vivo et al., 2017). These organelles are believed to be mostly of endosomal origin (Cooney et al., 2002) and for brevity we refer to them here as “endosomes.” In line with the previous results (de Vivo et al., 2017), we found a significant difference between S and CSR mice in the spines containing endosomes. Specifically, using an LME model with condition, endosome (binary), dendrite diameter and condition × endosome interaction, we found a significant interaction between condition and endosome (p = 5.1e-6). Post hoc tests revealed that for synapses with endosomes, the ASI size was significantly smaller in the S group relative to all wake groups, including the CSR group (S < CSR, p = 0.0235; S < EW, p = 0.0175; S < SW, p = 0.0001; Fig. 1D). The three wake groups did not differ from each other (EW = CSR, p = 0.9999; SW = CSR, p = 0.8998; SW = EW, p = 0.7681). There were no significant differences between any conditions for the synapses without endosomes. The distribution of ASI sizes in the CSR mice was log-normal, as reported in the other groups (de Vivo et al., 2017; Fig. 1E). The cumulative distribution of ASI sizes in the S group was shifted to the left relative to all other groups, including the CSR mice, whose curve overlapped completely with those of the W and SD mice (Fig. 1F). Formal testing confirmed that, in line with the presence of scaling in S mice relative to EW and SW mice (de Vivo et al., 2017), the decrease in ASI size in S mice relative to CSR mice also obeyed a scaling relationship (no evidence against a scaling relationship between S and CSR; p = 0.9414). The percentage of spines that contained tubules and vesicles or a spine apparatus, did not change across groups (Table 2). The density of the spines with a synapse did not change across groups (p = 0.8315), but there was a significant difference in the density of spines without a synapse (p = 0.0261). Post hoc tests revealed that the density of spines without a synapse was significantly higher in CSR relative to both the EW and SW mice, and higher in S relative to SW mice (EW < CSR, p = 0.0448; SW < CSR, p = 0.0354; S = CSR, p = 0.9953; EW < S, p = 0.0564; SW = EW, p = 0.9999; SW < S, p = 0.0448; Fig. 1G). Thus, after CSR the mean ASI size is comparable to that measured after SW or short sleep deprivation, and larger than the mean ASI size after sleep.
EW in young pups leads to a decrease in ASI size
In P13 pups, we previously found in layer 2 of M1 that the ASI size of axospinous excitatory synapses is larger after 4–5 h of sleep deprivation (EW) as compared with 4–5 h of S (de Vivo et al., 2019). Here, we used four new groups of pups aged approximately two weeks (Fig. 2A). Three experimental groups were collected at P13: S pups could sleep during the first 4–5 h of the light period (n = 4); EW4 pups were kept awake as much as possible for 4–5 h at the same time of day (n = 3); EW15 pups were forced to stay awake as much as possible for 15–16 h starting at light onset on P13 (n = 4). A fourth group of pups was sleep deprived for ∼15 h starting at the beginning of the light phase at P13, then allowed to recover sleep ad libitum 15–16 h until the afternoon of P14 (recovery, Rec; n = 3). Overall, we analyzed ∼281 spines per mouse (range 260–328) for a total of 1066 spines in S, 912 in EW4, 1398 in EW15, and 842 in Rec (Fig. 2B).
We applied an LME model that uses sleep/wake condition as categorical fixed effect, dendrite diameter as linear fixed effect, and mouse and dendrite as random effects (see Materials and Methods). We found a main effect of condition (p = 8.7e-7) and post hoc tests revealed that the ASI size increased after a few hours of sleep deprivation (EW4) as compared with S (p = 0.0368), consistent with our previous findings (de Vivo et al., 2019). By contrast, the size of the ASI decreased after longer sleep deprivation (EW15) relative to both EW4 and S (EW15 < EW4, p = 7.2e-14; EW15 < S, p = 2.1e-6; Fig. 2C). The ASI size did not change significantly after recovery from long sleep deprivation, that is, it did not differ between Rec and EW15 pups and was still significantly smaller in Rec pups than in S and EW4 pups (Rec < EW4, p < 2.2e-16; Rec < S, p = 7.6e-11; Fig. 2C). By applying an LME model with condition, endosome (binary), dendrite diameter, and condition × endosome interaction, we found no significant interaction between condition and endosome (p = 0.8644), indicating that the decrease in ASI size with EW was not restricted to spines with endosomes (Fig. 2D). The distribution of ASI sizes in the pups was log-normal, as previously reported (de Vivo et al., 2019; Fig. 1E) and the cumulative plots of ASI sizes showed a clear shift to the left in the EW15 and Rec pups relative to the S and EW4 pups, as well as a shift to the left in S pups relative to EW4 pups (Fig. 2F). Formal testing confirmed the presence of scaling between S mice and EW15 mice (no evidence against a scaling relationship between S and EW15; p = 0.8446). The percentage of spines that contained tubules and vesicles, or a spine apparatus, did not change across groups (Table 2), nor did the volume of the spine apparatus (p = 0.117; LME model with condition and diameter as fixed effects, mouse and dendrite as random effects). We found a significant effect of condition on tubules/vesicles volume (p = 0.0068; LME model with condition and diameter as fixed effects, mouse and dendrite as random effects). Post hoc tests revealed that the tubules/vesicles volume was significantly lower in EW15 and Rec pups relative to EW4 pups (EW15 < EW4, p = 0.0022; Rec < EW4, p = 0.0004; Fig. 2G). A trend decrease in tubules/vesicles volume was also present in S pups relative to EW4 pups (p = 0.0803). The total density of the spines (with and without a synapse) did not change across groups (p= 0.1171; LME model with condition as a fixed effect and mouse as a random effect). The density of the spines with a synapse, however, showed a trend effect of condition (p = 0.0896) and post hoc tests showed a significant increase in EW15 relative to EW4 pups (p = 0.0261; Fig. 2H). There was no significant effect of condition on the spines without a synapse (p = 0.7458). The synapses of EW15 pups had smaller ASI but were more numerous than in EW4 pups. Since newly formed synapses are on average small, we tested whether the difference in ASI size between the two groups could be accounted for by the smallest spines. This was not the case: by fitting an LME model with condition (EW4 and EW15 only) and diameter as fixed effects, and mouse/dendrite as random effects, we found that the difference in ASI size between EW15 and EW4 pups became insignificant only after removing the 60% smallest synapses. Thus, in young pups short and long sleep deprivation have opposite effects on the ASI size, the former increasing it and the latter decreasing it relative to sleep, and this difference is unlikely to be because of the conversion of small protrusions without a synapse into spines with a synapse.
To test whether long sleep deprivation results in a significant weight loss, two additional groups of P13 pups were studied: control pups were kept with their dam and left undisturbed during the experiment, while sleep deprived pups were kept with their dam and sleep deprived for 16 h as in the experiment described above. While control pups maintained their weight during the 16 h of the experiment, the sleep deprived pups lost on average 10% of their weight (Fig. 3). Body weight increased in these pups after the experiment ended, but remained below the values in controls 5 d afterward (increase relative to the day before sleep deprivation; Control mice = 27.7 ± 1.8%, sleep-deprived mice = 17.1 ± 1.1%).
Discussion
In a first series of experiments, we studied one-month-old YFP mice. At this age both the circadian and the homeostatic regulation of sleep are present and the sleep of these adolescent animals is similar, albeit not identical, to that of adult mice (Nelson et al., 2013). CSR was enforced using a combination of exposure to novelty, social interactions, access to a running wheel, and other stimuli. This method was previously characterized in our laboratory using polygraphic recordings (de Vivo et al., 2016). Specifically we found that in the fourth week of life, with this paradigm, sleep is reduced to approximately a third of the normal daily amount and both NREM and REM sleep are decreased (68% decrease in NREM sleep and 79% decrease in REM sleep over 4 d; de Vivo et al., 2016). Sleep episodes occur throughout the 24-h cycle, either when the mice escape the attention of the experimenter or when they are allowed to sleep for 10–20 min because they appear too tired. Thus, with this paradigm short bouts of sleep occur day and night, and both sleep phases are decreased by at least two/thirds. Few other studies have examined the effects of chronic sleep loss during development. Several experiments in cats and rodents have used chronic deprivation of REM sleep (7–10 d) around the critical period and demonstrated that this manipulation disrupts the maturation of the visual pathways (Oksenberg et al., 1996; Hogan et al., 2001; Shaffery et al., 2006). In prairie voles, one week of sleep disruption during the third week of life increases the number of inhibitory cells in the primary sensory cortex and impairs social bonding behavior in adulthood (Jones et al., 2019). The cage-shaking method used in prairie voles results in fragmented sleep and a significant decrease in REM sleep, but there were no effects on NREM sleep (Jones et al., 2019). Other studies in adolescent mice have used total or selective sleep deprivation, but only for a few hours (Maret et al., 2011; Li et al., 2017). In short, it is difficult to directly compare our results with previous findings, because the extent and the pattern of sleep disruption vary significantly across studies.
We found that in one-month YFP-H mice, the majority of cortical synapses in the superficial layers of M1, those containing endosomes, have larger ASIs after >4 d of sleep restriction than after sleep, and as large as after several hours of either SW or short sleep deprivation. Thus, the severely curtailed sleep of CSR mice was not enough for synaptic renormalization to occur, at least in terms of structural changes in ASI. It remains possible that the short episodes of sleep were enough to decrease other measures of synaptic strength that can change more rapidly, for instance the phosphorylation levels of GluA1-containing AMPA receptors at Ser831 and Ser845 (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008; Hinard et al., 2012; Suzuki et al., 2020). However, synaptic weakening ultimately involves the endocytosis of the AMPA receptors from the synaptic surface. This process can happen within a few hours and the synaptic expression of GluA1-containing and GluA2-containing AMPA receptors is indeed lower after 4–8 h of sleep than after 4–8 h of wake (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008; Diering et al., 2017). Because the synaptic levels of AMPA receptors correlate with synaptic size (Nusser et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2015; El-Boustani et al., 2018), it is unlikely that the expression of these receptors in the spine could decline without a decline in ASI size. Thus, our results suggest that sleep-dependent synaptic renormalization is incompatible with the severity of the sleep loss experienced by the chronically restricted mice. We cannot be sure that other CSR methods would have produced the same outcome, nor can we rule out that the CSR method that we used, or any other method, may have confounding effects on the synapses unrelated to sleep loss per se.
The ASI size, and several other features of the organelles inside the spines, did not differ in the CSR mice relative to the other wake groups. Based on these parameters, one would have to conclude that severe CSR does not affect synapses and spines above and beyond what normal wake or a short period of sleep deprivation does. However, we previously obtained ultrastructural evidence that CSR puts a greater burden on pyramidal excitatory neurons than acute sleep deprivation. In mice of the same strain and age, and in the same region studied in the current study (layer 2 of M1), we found that the percent of cytoplasm occupied by mitochondria is larger after CSR than after sleep, and at intermediate levels after short sleep deprivation (de Vivo et al., 2016). The density of secondary lysosomes, which digest damaged cellular components, is also significantly higher after CSR relative to sleep, with no difference between sleep and short sleep deprivation (de Vivo et al., 2016). In another study, we found that in the same cortical area >80% of the spine heads, all but the smallest spines, are contacted by peripheral astrocytic processes and the extent of this astrocytic coverage increases with CSR relative to both sleep and short sleep deprivation (Bellesi et al., 2015). The percent of the ASI perimeter directly contacted by astrocytes also increases with CSR relative to both sleep and short sleep deprivation (Bellesi et al., 2015). Together, these results suggest that the pyramidal excitatory neurons require more mitochondria during CSR and rely more extensively on the peripheral astrocytic processes, presumably for the clearance of glutamate and potassium ions from the synaptic cleft. Thus, after CSR the axospinous synapses may be similar in size to those after normal wake, but the cellular burden to maintain them may be higher. Of note, in a previous study we also found that, although all mice gained weight between P21 (weaning) and P30 (end of CSR), controls did so more than CSR mice (Billeh et al., 2015), suggesting that even when food is available ad libitum, adolescent mice struggle to maintain the energy balance during severe sleep loss.
In a previous study employing a similar protocol of chronic sleep loss during early adolescence (P25–P30), we found some evidence for long-term effects on brain connectivity (Billeh et al., 2015). In that study, we focused on the secondary motor cortex, a “higher order” area with widespread projections and whose functional maturation continues throughout adolescence. In adult mice, more than a month after the end of the sleep restriction period, we measured the projection fraction, a combined measure of passing fibers and terminals originating from the secondary motor cortex. Although the results were subtle and heterogenous across animals, this measure of connectivity showed an overall decrease in adult mice previously sleep restricted relative to their siblings, and machine learning algorithms could classify significantly above chance the two groups. These findings suggest that structural connectivity may have been affected in the long term, perhaps as a consequence of the cellular burden demonstrated by the experiments mentioned above.
In a second series of experiments, we found that at P13 the ASI size was affected in opposite directions by short and long sleep loss: larger ASI size were present after 4 h of sleep deprivation compared with sleep, consistent with the results in P30 mice, while 15 h of forced wake led to a decrease in ASI size relative to both sleep and short sleep deprivation. These effects were not associated with significant changes in the number of spines, but there was a trend for the density of spines with synapses to be higher after 15 h than after 4 h of sleep deprivation. Synaptogenesis and synaptic pruning in rodents occur mainly during the second postnatal week (Cirelli and Tononi, 2015). In line with this, in the same mouse line used in the current study (YFP-H) we previously found that the cortical levels of Synapsin I, a ubiquitous presynaptic marker, increase greatly from P10 to P20 and slightly from P20 to P25, but are stable afterward (de Vivo et al., 2014). Thus, it may not be surprising that sustained wake at P13, even if “just” for 15 h, had synaptic effects that went beyond those seen after shorter periods of wake, while CSR at P30 did not.
A possible explanation for the small ASI sizes after 15 h of sleep deprivation is that despite our efforts to keep them awake, the pups were actually asleep most of the time, especially after the first few hours of sleep deprivation. Indeed, in this study we did not attempt to score sleep and wake states in 30-s epochs using behavioral criteria, as we previously did (de Vivo et al., 2019). However, after 15 h of sleep deprivation the ASI size was even smaller than after 4 h of sleep, and after 16 h of recovery it remained smaller than after normal sleep. Because the density of synapses showed a trend to increase from 4 to 15 h of sleep deprivation while it did not differ between 4 h of sleep and 4 h of EW, it is also possible that EW15 synapses were small because many of them were newly formed. However, we found no evidence to support this hypothesis, because the average ASI size remained significantly smaller in EW15 than in EW4 pups until at least 60% of the smallest synapses were removed from the comparison. Finally, the overall shrinkage of ASI size with long sleep deprivation may indicate that the immature brain can meet the high demand of sustained synaptic activity for a limited amount of time, in our case 4 h. Synaptic activity accounts for the bulk of the brain’s energy need (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001; Karbowski, 2014), and to grow and survive, synaptic connections require continuous protein synthesis (Kleim et al., 2003) and a steady supply of energy (Li et al., 2004). In flies, 36 h of sleep deprivation starting soon after eclosion impair the development of one specific olfactory glomerulus, the one that normally shows the largest growth during the same period (Kayser et al., 2014).
In a previous study, we found that P13 pups struggle to eat while sleep deprived (de Vivo et al., 2019). In the current study, despite our best attempts to promote feeding, we found that sleep deprived pups lost weight during the experiment. The increased energy need caused by enriched and prolonged wake, combined with suboptimal access to food, may have contributed to the decrease in ASI size. The inevitable stress caused by the stimulation used to enforce wake may also have contributed. While we tried to limit the stress by keeping the litter with the dam in their home cage, maternal factors (grooming, feeding and warmth) were likely affected by the stimulation. Thus, sleep deprivation may have resulted in high glucocorticoid or mineralocorticoid levels, and/or high catecholamine levels, although a reduced capacity to release corticosterone is well documented in rodent pups during the first two weeks of life (Varga et al., 2013). Future experiments should determine to which extent these signaling pathways can account for the different synaptic effects of four compared with 15 h of sleep deprivation (Popoli et al., 2011). Of note, the decrease in ASI size was still present 16 h after the end of sleep deprivation and, 5 d later, body weight was still below control levels. While we did not measure the ASI size at this later time point, these results suggest that when body and brain are growing at high speed, the recovery from prolonged sleep deprivation may take a long time, or may not be complete.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Rae Silver, Columbia University
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Radhika Basheer.
Major comments:
Most problematic is the use of prior work for controls: The dataset for long sleep deprivation in older (P30) mice is incomplete. The authors perform long sleep deprivation but do not provide matched control data. Instead they compare the results to their earlier published work. They must have matched control data to conclude anything from these studies. Basically, the P30 data cannot published without the controls. It is not possible to compare one condition collected now with 3 conditions collected and published 6 years ago by different experimenters, on mouse colonies in which there may have been considerable genetic background changes over the ensuing years. The use of controls for the present study requires a great deal of new work. Perhaps the data on young animals stands on its own.
There appear to be confounding effects of light pulses: One concern is in the method of CSR. The authors describe brief interruptions in the light by switching off the light for arousal of the mice. This method can introduce the confounding effects of light conditions on sleep physiology and thus the behavior. Light flashes during dark period increase REM and sleep time and vice versa. Thus, this aspect of stimulation is not acceptable for keeping the mice awake. If the authors think this method does not introduce any confounding effects on synaptic measures then it will be helpful to have a discussion on the appropriateness of this method. A better description of the timing, duration, and frequency of manipulations and how they control for things like circadian disruption by light manipulation is also needed.
There are issues regarding the sleep deprivation manipulations. It is intriguing as to how the mice were kept awake for 15h. It is generally difficult to keep the mice awake beyond 6h and much more difficult after 8h. As is also described by the authors. “As previously reported (de Vivo et al., 2019), even after the first few hours of forced wake pups responded to the stimuli much less than older mice, and it was impossible to keep them fully awake for extended periods.” The use of multiple methods in itself is problematic without appropriate controls. Furthermore, sleep deprivation is stressful. This aspect is accentuated in adolescent pups. It will be helpful to compare the changes with another stressful model (such as maternal separation 24h prior to sacrifice) to confirm the effects to be specific to sleep deprivation and not due to any stressful pathway activation.
A somewhat related problem is that there is no behavioral scoring of sleep deprivation in this study; instead the authors rely on earlier studies to support the efficacy of their deprivation paradigm. Especially given the difficulty of depriving young mice for long periods of time a verification that they have indeed succeeded in depriving them of sleep seems important.
Other comments:
The use of ASI as a method of evaluating the synaptic structure must be very well explained and clearly justified. It was unclear to reviewers how robust this measure might be.
-Reading the methods it is not clear how the state durations were evaluated.
-The description of pups experimental group is repeated both in the method and result section.
-In the abstract the Ns are misleading (adult 15 and pups 14). These are total number of mice used for each study but the N' of the experimental groups remain very small (for example in the pups study the Ns for each group is as follows: Sleep control (4) SD (3), 15 SD (4) and 15h SD+ 16 RS (3)). This should be clearly stated in the abstract.
References
- Alföldi P, Franken P, Tobler I, Borbély AA (1991) Short light-dark cycles influence sleep stages and EEG power spectra in the rat. Behav Brain Res 43:125–131. 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80062-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Attwell D, Laughlin SB (2001) An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 21:1133–1145. 10.1097/00004647-200110000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft 67:1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bellesi M, de Vivo L, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2015) Effects of sleep and wake on astrocytes: clues from molecular and ultrastructural studies. BMC Biol 13:66. 10.1186/s12915-015-0176-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benca RM, Gilliland MA, Obermeyer WH (1998) Effects of lighting conditions on sleep and wakefulness in albino lewis and pigmented Brown Norway rats. Sleep 21:451–460. 10.1093/sleep/21.5.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billeh YN, Rodriguez AV, Bellesi M, Bernard A, de Vivo L, Funk CM, Harris J, Honjoh S, Mihalas S, Ng L, Koch C, Cirelli C, Tononi G (2015) Effects of chronic sleep restriction during early adolescence on the adult pattern of connectivity of mouse secondary motor cortex. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0053-16.2016. 10.1523/ENEURO.0053-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bretz F, Hothorn T, Westfall P (2011) Multiple comparisons using R. Boca Raton: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
- Buchs PA, Muller D (1996) Induction of long-term potentiation is associated with major ultrastructural changes of activated synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:8040–8045. 10.1073/pnas.93.15.8040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardona A, Saalfeld S, Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Preibisch S, Longair M, Tomancak P, Hartenstein V, Douglas RJ (2012) TrakEM2 software for neural circuit reconstruction. PLoS One 7:e38011. 10.1371/journal.pone.0038011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheetham CE, Barnes SJ, Albieri G, Knott GW, Finnerty GT (2014) Pansynaptic enlargement at adult cortical connections strengthened by experience. Cereb Cortex 24:521–531. 10.1093/cercor/bhs334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cirelli C, Tononi G (2015) Cortical development, electroencephalogram rhythms, and the sleep/wake cycle. Biol Psychiatry 77:1071–1078. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cirelli C, Tononi G (2020) Effects of sleep and waking on the synaptic ultrastructure. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 375:20190235. 10.1098/rstb.2019.0235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooney JR, Hurlburt JL, Selig DK, Harris KM, Fiala JC (2002) Endosomal compartments serve multiple hippocampal dendritic spines from a widespread rather than a local store of recycling membrane. J Neurosci 22:2215–2224. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02215.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vivo L, Bellesi M, Marshall W, Bushong EA, Ellisman MH, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2017) Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science 355:507–510. 10.1126/science.aah5982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vivo L, Faraguna U, Nelson AB, Pfister-Genskow M, Klapperich ME, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2014) Developmental patterns of sleep slow wave activity and synaptic density in adolescent mice. Sleep 37:689–700. 10.5665/sleep.3570 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vivo L, Nelson AB, Bellesi M, Noguti J, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2016) Loss of sleep affects the ultrastructure of pyramidal neurons in the adolescent mouse frontal cortex. Sleep 39:861–874. 10.5665/sleep.5644 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vivo L, Nagai H, De Wispelaere N, Spano GM, Marshall W, Bellesi M, Nemec KM, Schiereck SS, Nagai M, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2019) Evidence for sleep-dependent synaptic renormalization in mouse pups. Sleep 42:zsz184. 10.1093/sleep/zsz184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmond NL, Levy WB (1988) Synaptic interface surface area increases with long-term potentiation in the hippocampal dentate gyrus. Brain Res 453:308–314. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90171-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diering GH, Nirujogi RS, Roth RH, Worley PF, Pandey A, Huganir RL (2017) Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses during sleep. Science 355:511–515. 10.1126/science.aai8355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Boustani S, Ip JPK, Breton-Provencher V, Knott GW, Okuno H, Bito H, Sur M (2018) Locally coordinated synaptic plasticity of visual cortex neurons in vivo. Science 360:1349–1354. 10.1126/science.aao0862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng G, Mellor RH, Bernstein M, Keller-Peck C, Nguyen QT, Wallace M, Nerbonne JM, Lichtman JW, Sanes JR (2000) Imaging neuronal subsets in transgenic mice expressing multiple spectral variants of GFP. Neuron 28:41–51. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00084-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiala JC, Harris KM (2001) Cylindrical diameters method for calibrating section thickness in serial electron microscopy. J Microsc 202:468–472. 10.1046/j.1365-2818.2001.00926.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukazawa Y, Saitoh Y, Ozawa F, Ohta Y, Mizuno K, Inokuchi K (2003) Hippocampal LTP is accompanied by enhanced F-actin content within the dendritic spine that is essential for late LTP maintenance in vivo. Neuron 38:447–460. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00206-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinard V, Mikhail C, Pradervand S, Curie T, Houtkooper RH, Auwerx J, Franken P, Tafti M (2012) Key electrophysiological, molecular, and metabolic signatures of sleep and wakefulness revealed in primary cortical cultures. J Neurosci 32:12506–12517. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2306-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogan D, Roffwarg HP, Shaffery JP (2001) The effects of 1 week of REM sleep deprivation on parvalbumin and calbindin immunoreactive neurons in central visual pathways of kittens. J Sleep Res 10:285–296. 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00270.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holcomb PS, Hoffpauir BK, Hoyson MC, Jackson DR, Deerinck TJ, Marrs GS, Dehoff M, Wu J, Ellisman MH, Spirou GA (2013) Synaptic inputs compete during rapid formation of the calyx of Held: a new model system for neural development. J Neurosci 33:12954–12969. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1087-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtmaat A, Svoboda K (2009) Experience-dependent structural synaptic plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:647–658. 10.1038/nrn2699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones CE, Opel RA, Kaiser ME, Chau AQ, Quintana JR, Nipper MA, Finn DA, Hammock EAD, Lim MM (2019) Early-life sleep disruption increases parvalbumin in primary somatosensory cortex and impairs social bonding in prairie voles. Sci Adv 5:eaav5188. 10.1126/sciadv.aav5188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karbowski J (2014) Constancy and trade-offs in the neuroanatomical and metabolic design of the cerebral cortex. Front Neural Circuits 8:9. 10.3389/fncir.2014.00009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz Y, Menon V, Nicholson DA, Geinisman Y, Kath WL, Spruston N (2009) Synapse distribution suggests a two-stage model of dendritic integration in CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron 63:171–177. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser MS, Yue Z, Sehgal A (2014) A critical period of sleep for development of courtship circuitry and behavior in Drosophila. Science 344:269–274. 10.1126/science.1250553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleim JA, Bruneau R, Calder K, Pocock D, VandenBerg PM, MacDonald E, Monfils MH, Sutherland RJ, Nader K (2003) Functional organization of adult motor cortex is dependent upon continued protein synthesis. Neuron 40:167–176. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00592-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larkman AU (1991) Dendritic morphology of pyramidal neurones of the visual cortex of the rat: I. Branching patterns. J Comp Neurol 306:307–319. 10.1002/cne.903060207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W, Ma L, Yang G, Gan WB (2017) REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat Neurosci 20:427–437. 10.1038/nn.4479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Z, Okamoto K, Hayashi Y, Sheng M (2004) The importance of dendritic mitochondria in the morphogenesis and plasticity of spines and synapses. Cell 119:873–887. 10.1016/j.cell.2004.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maret S, Faraguna U, Nelson AB, Cirelli C, Tononi G (2011) Sleep and waking modulate spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortex. Nat Neurosci 14:1418–1420. 10.1038/nn.2934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuzaki M, Ellis-Davies GC, Nemoto T, Miyashita Y, Iino M, Kasai H (2001) Dendritic spine geometry is critical for AMPA receptor expression in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Nat Neurosci 4:1086–1092. 10.1038/nn736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller AM, Obermeyer WH, Behan M, Benca RM (1998) The superior colliculus-pretectum mediates the direct effects of light on sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:8957–8962. 10.1073/pnas.95.15.8957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson AB, Faraguna U, Zoltan JT, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2013) Sleep patterns and homeostatic mechanisms in adolescent mice. Brain Sci 3:318–343. 10.3390/brainsci3010318 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusser Z, Lujan R, Laube G, Roberts JD, Molnar E, Somogyi P (1998) Cell type and pathway dependence of synaptic AMPA receptor number and variability in the hippocampus. Neuron 21:545–559. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80565-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oksenberg A, Shaffery JP, Marks GA, Speciale SG, Mihailoff G, Roffwarg HP (1996) Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation in kittens amplifies LGN cell-size disparity induced by monocular deprivation. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 97:51–61. 10.1016/S0165-3806(96)00131-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popoli M, Yan Z, McEwen BS, Sanacora G (2011) The stressed synapse: the impact of stress and glucocorticoids on glutamate transmission. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:22–37. 10.1038/nrn3138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A (2012) Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods 9:676–682. 10.1038/nmeth.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffery JP, Lopez J, Bissette G, Roffwarg HP (2006) Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation in post-critical period, adolescent rats alters the balance between inhibitory and excitatory mechanisms in visual cortex. Neurosci Lett 393:131–135. 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.09.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spano GM, Banningh SW, Marshall W, de Vivo L, Bellesi M, Loschky SS, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2019) Sleep deprivation by exposure to novel objects increases synapse density and axon-spine interface in the hippocampal CA1 region of adolescent mice. J Neurosci 39:6613–6625. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0380-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki A, Yanagisawa M, Greene RW (2020) Loss of Arc attenuates the behavioral and molecular responses for sleep homeostasis in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117:10547–10553. 10.1073/pnas.1906840117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Cirelli C (2014) Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81:12–34. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Cirelli C (2020) Sleep and synaptic down-selection. Eur J Neurosci 51:413–421. 10.1111/ejn.14335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Aerde KI, Feldmeyer D (2015) Morphological and physiological characterization of pyramidal neuron subtypes in rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 25:788–805. 10.1093/cercor/bht278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varga J, Ferenczi S, Kovács KJ, Garafova A, Jezova D, Zelena D (2013) Comparison of stress-induced changes in adults and pups: is aldosterone the main adrenocortical stress hormone during the perinatal period in rats? PLoS One 8:e72313. 10.1371/journal.pone.0072313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyazovskiy V, Cirelli C, Pfister-Genskow M, Faraguna U, Tononi G (2008) Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat Neurosci 11:200–208. 10.1038/nn2035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y, Cudmore RH, Lin DT, Linden DJ, Huganir RL (2015) Visualization of NMDA receptor-dependent AMPA receptor synaptic plasticity in vivo. Nat Neurosci 18:402–407. 10.1038/nn.3936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]