Abstract
An increasingly influential hypothesis posits that many of the diverse symptoms of psychosis can be viewed as reflecting dysfunctional predictive mechanisms. Indeed, to perceive something is to take a sensory input and make a prediction of the external source of that signal; thus, prediction is perhaps the most fundamental neural computation. Given the ubiquity of prediction, a more challenging problem is to specify the unique predictive role or capability of a particular brain structure. This question is relevant when considering recent claims that one aspect of the predictive deficits observed in psychotic disorders might be related to cerebellar dysfunction, a subcortical structure known to play a critical role in predictive sensorimotor control and perhaps higher-level cognitive function. Here, we review evidence bearing on this question. We first focus on clinical, behavioral, and neuroimaging findings suggesting cerebellar involvement in psychosis and, specifically, schizophrenia. We then review a relatively novel line of research exploring whether computational models of cerebellar motor function can also account for cerebellar involvement in higher-order human cognition, and in particular, language function. We end the review by highlighting some key gaps in these literatures, limitations that currently preclude strong conclusions regarding cerebellar involvement in psychosis.
Keywords: Cerebellum, Corollary discharge, Internal model, Prediction, Psychosis, Schizophrenia
PREDICTION, PSYCHOSIS, AND THE CEREBELLUM
Prediction
When walking down the stairs in the dark, we anticipate, both in space and time, that our foot is about to strike a tread. Deviations from this expected sensory input will trigger fast corrective actions, and should we encounter a missing tread repeatedly, we will in the future generate compensatory responses to anticipate this gap. This example makes clear how prediction is crucial for adaptive behavior, an idea that has a long history in the study of sensorimotor function (1). For example, in the early 19th century, Bell and Purkinje proposed that our percept of the world remains stable during saccades because the brain anticipates the sensory consequences resulting from an eye movement (1). Predictive mechanisms of this sort have since been verified in many model systems (2), with the term efference copy (3) or corollary discharge (4) used to capture the idea that in addition to generating the signals that produce a movement, a copy of the motor commands is used to generate the predicted sensory consequences of that movement. A related concept, originating from control theory in engineering, is that of an internal model, “a system that mimics its next state given the current state and a motor command” (5). An impressive body of empirical studies has provided compelling demonstrations of the neural signatures of predictive mechanisms, or rather, of how these predictions are presumably subtracted from the actual sensory input. For example, neural activity in the auditory cortex is markedly attenuated in response to hearing oneself speak, relative to hearing the same sounds played back in a passive listening condition (6-9). Importantly, online perturbations of feedback from speech reduces this suppression (10,11), indicating that the suppression is due to a detailed prediction of the auditory input, rather than to nonspecific attentional mechanisms.
Psychosis
As evidenced by this special issue of Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, an increasingly influential hypothesis posits that many of the diverse symptoms of psychosis can be viewed as reflecting dysfunctional predictive mechanisms (12-15). To build on the speech example described above, the suboptimal operation of predictive mechanisms involved in suppressing reafferent auditory feedback could make it difficult to distinguish between self- and externally generated stimuli, leading to delusions of control (16). Likewise, if internal speech engages similar processes as external speech, a failure of anticipatory processes might lead to auditory hallucinations (14,16); i.e., the internal thoughts could be attributed to an external source. In line with these ideas, an increasing body of research has documented decreased suppression of self-generated somatosensory (17,18), visual (19-24), and auditory (25-27) stimuli in patients with schizophrenia, the most debilitating of psychotic disorders.
Moving from such relatively low-level sensory phenomena to higher-level cognition, one of the most characteristic clinical features of psychosis is a loss of coherence in speech output, assumed to reflect an underlying thought disorder (28). One hypothesis is that thought disorder arises from an impairment in predictive processes, with the affected individual unable to use predictive mechanisms to generate lucid ideas. When coupled with a second problem associated with an increase in automatic spreading activation across semantic networks (29), one can understand phenomena such as context-inappropriate associative leaps, exemplified in the following passage: “If you think you are being wise to send me a bill for money I have already paid, I am in nowise going to do so unless I get the whys and wherefores from you to me. But where the fours have been, then fives will be, and other numbers and calculations and accounts to your no-account” (30). This sentence shows several signs of hyperassociation on the level of single-word identity (“wise” – “no-wise”, “accounts” – “no-account”), phonology (“whys and wherefores”), and category membership (“four” – “five”), but simultaneously reveals a near-complete context blindness. The initial parts of the message are minimally predictive of what follows, and the lack of connection from one phrase to the next renders the final output unintelligible. Empirically, many behavioral studies have highlighted a deficient use of predictive context in both language production and comprehension in schizophrenia (28,30-32), and reduced semantic coherence even shows promise as a predictive marker of conversion to psychosis in high-risk groups (33).
Cerebellum
Theories such as Friston’s free-energy principle (27) make clear that prediction is perhaps the most fundamental neural computation; to perceive something is to take a sensory input and make a prediction of the external source of that signal. For this reason, it is not surprising that prediction has been associated with many parts of the brain. Given the ubiquity of prediction, a more challenging problem is to specify the unique predictive role or capability of a particular brain structure. Here, we focus on the cerebellum, highlighting how the concept of prediction has been central to theories of cerebellar function (34,35).
One influential hypothesis is that the cerebellum is a critical node in a system required for the construction and adaptation of internal models for sensorimotor control (34-36). These internal models take as input an efference copy of the motor commands and, via simulation through an internal model, generate the expected sensory consequences of that movement. The mismatch between the predicted and actual feedback constitutes a sensory prediction error, the signal that can be used to retune the internal model so that in future iterations, the prediction is better matched to the actual feedback. This model provides an elegant account of the critical role of the cerebellum as a feedforward system in sensorimotor control and for sensorimotor learning. Indeed, prediction is essential for skillful motor control, given that the inherent delays in feedback control would produce instabilities in control (5). The internal model hypothesis has spawned a rich empirical test bed, showing how people learn to move in different environmental contexts or interact with objects and tools (5,37), as well as accounting for the sensorimotor adaptation impairments observed in individuals with cerebellar pathology (38-44). The prediction deficits cerebellar patients display in motor (41,43), proprioceptive (45), and auditory (46,47) processing are also in line with the internal model hypothesis.
These computational accounts draw their inspiration from detailed studies of the unique anatomy and physiology of the cerebellum [for more detailed accounts, see Ito (48)].The massive cortical and subcortical input onto cerebellar granule cells from the pontine nuclei allows for the encoding of very complex multimodal contexts (49-51), including what might be thought of as an efference copy. The output of the cerebellum, refined via processing in the cerebellar cortex, can be viewed as the sensory prediction (52). At some place or places, this prediction is compared with the actual feedback, with mismatches resulting in the generation of complex spikes, relayed to the cerebellar cortex via the other major input, the inferior olivary nuclei (35). The complex spike has historically been viewed as an error signal, hypothesized to serve as the teaching signal that modifies Purkinje cell synapses and thus improves the internal model (35). Neurophysiological cerebellar recordings in behaving animals are remarkably consistent with the internal model hypothesis (52,53).
IS THE CEREBELLUM A RELEVANT BRAIN REGION FOR PSYCHOSIS?
As noted above, prediction is a general feature of brain function. Nonetheless, a number of researchers have asked if the predictive deficits observed in psychotic disorders might, at least in part, be related to cerebellar dysfunction. One motivation for this hypothesis comes from work seeking to generalize the relatively well-understood cerebellar mechanisms for predictive sensorimotor control to higher-level cognition; by this reasoning, these more abstract forms of prediction might be disrupted in psychosis.
Here, we review some recent evidence bearing on these two questions. We first focus on clinical, behavioral, and neuroimaging findings suggesting cerebellar involvement in psychosis and, specifically, schizophrenia. Then we review a relatively novel line of research exploring whether computational models of cerebellar motor function can also account for cerebellar involvement in higher-order human cognition (see Figure 1), and in particular, language function. We also highlight some key gaps in these literatures, limitations that currently preclude strong conclusions regarding cerebellar involvement in psychosis.
Figure 1.
A flat-map functional representation of the cerebellar cortex, based on functional magnetic resonance imaging data while participants performed a large battery of motor and cognitive tasks (107). The labels refer to the cognitive processes most closely related to the tasks that engaged each region. The study highlights that nonmotor features provide the best descriptors of most of the cerebellar cortex. 1, Autobiographical recall; 2, Semantic knowledge; 3, Active maintenance; 4, Narrative; 5, Divided attention; 6, Visual working memory; 7, Saccades; 8, Action observation; 9, Left hand presses; 10, Right hand presses.
Is Cerebellar Pathology Associated With Psychosis?
If cerebellar dysfunction is central to the pathophysiology of psychosis, one might expect individuals with cerebellar disturbances to exhibit psychotic symptoms. At present, the evidence in favor of this hypothesis is modest at best. There are a number of case studies reporting the co-occurrence of cerebellar pathology and psychosis (54-56); however, few systematic studies have been conducted. One notable exception involved the assessment of a wide range of psychiatric symptoms in a cohort of 31 patients with cerebellar degeneration (57). Within this group, 3 of the 31 patients met the criteria for a diagnosis of psychosis, a prevalence estimate of ~10%, compared with the ~1% to 2% prevalence in the general population. Although the sample size is small, this estimate is similar to that reported in a retrospective chart review of a larger group of patients with cerebellar degeneration (n = 133). Here, too, the prevalence of psychotic disorders was ~10%, although the presence of psychosis appeared to primarily be related to concurrent basal ganglia involvement; that is, patients with multisystem atrophy (58). A more recent study also found evidence of basal ganglia impairment in patients with cerebellar degeneration who displayed features of psychosis (59). Thus, while the literature suggests an increased (and possibly underdiagnosed) prevalence of psychotic disorders in patients with cerebellar degeneration, it also highlights that concurrent pathology in the basal ganglia may be more central to psychosis than pathology in the cerebellum.
Given that brain pathology in patients with cerebellar degeneration may extend to extracerebellar structures, it is also important to consider patients with more focal pathology of the cerebellum. Unfortunately, the current literature is limited to case reports: psychosis has been associated with cerebellar congenital malformations (54), cerebellar tumors (60), and cerebellar strokes (61,62). Of course one would expect a small number of these individuals to have psychoses, given the prevalence rate in the general population. However, there are a few reports of rapid-onset psychosis following cerebellar infarcts, including in individuals with no history of psychopathology (61,62). Cases such as these potentially offer the most compelling evidence for an association between the cerebellum and psychosis, although the database here is quite small.
In summary, while the clinical literature offers some intriguing cases of psychosis linked to cerebellar pathology, more extensive studies are clearly needed to assess the magnitude of this association and examine whether the association also involves abnormalities in noncerebellar structures.
Do Motor Disturbances in Psychotic Disorders Indicate Cerebellar Dysfunction?
Despite the accumulating evidence that the functional territory of the cerebellum extends beyond motor control (see Figure 1), the most unequivocal behavioral signs of cerebellar dysfunction are impairments in motor coordination, or ataxia. To what extent are such symptoms seen in psychotic disorders? While not included among the diagnostic criteria, marked disruptions in bodily movements were noted in the earliest clinical descriptions of schizophrenia (63). More systematic studies have now established a high prevalence of motor abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia (59%–80%) (64-66), even before the onset of antipsychotic medication (65,67). In addition, meta-analyses consistently find that individuals at increased familiar risk for psychosis exhibit delayed motor development (68-74) and marked motor abnormalities as early as 7 years of age (75-81). Thus, motor disturbances appear to be a core and relatively consistent feature of schizophrenia (64,68,82,83). While some of these motor symptoms (e.g., involuntary movements) are typically associated with a basal ganglia etiology, some of the most frequently observed motor deficits are associated with cerebellar dysfunction (65,78,84-86); these include disturbances of gait (e.g., tandem walk), balance (e.g., enhanced postural sway), and manual coordination (e.g., finger-nose test) (65). Indeed, when a relatively large cohort of patients (n = 155) with schizophrenia were specifically assessed for cerebellar neurological signs, 21% presented signs of cerebellar dysfunction (87). Even more compelling behavioral evidence comes from studies using experimental tasks known to critically rely on cerebellar circuitry, such as classical conditioning of the defensive eyeblink reflex (88). Impaired eyeblink conditioning has been observed in both chronic (89) and first-episode (90,91) schizophrenia patients, as well as in their first-degree relatives (92).
In summary, cerebellar motor deficits appear to be a relatively frequent, but largely understudied and underemphasized, feature of schizophrenia (93). The recent addition of a motor domain (93) to the Research Domain Criteria framework (94) advocated by the National Institute of Mental Health should result in a stronger focus on these features of the disorder.
Is Cerebellar Structure and Function Affected in Psychotic Disorders?
Reports of cerebellar structural alterations in schizophrenia date back to the late 1970s and early 1980s (95,96); see Table 1 for selected examples.
Table 1.
Key Findings From Studies Examining Cerebellar Structural Alterations in Psychotic Disorders
Year, Reference | n | Main Cerebellar Finding(s) |
---|---|---|
1979 (96) | 70 patients with SZ | Pronounced atrophy of the cerebellar vermis in ~17% of patients with SZ |
1994 (166) | 52 patients with SZ, 90 HC subjects | No group differences in total cerebellar volume between patients with SZ and HC subjects |
1994 (167) | 36 patients with SZ, 52 HC subjects | No group differences in vermal volumes between patients with SZ and HC subjects |
2010 (168) | 54 patients with SZ, 100 HC subjects | Reduced gray matter volume in bilateral anterior cerebellum |
2012 (103) | 29 patients with SZ, 45 HC subjects | Reduced volume of left cerebellar crus I and II in patients with SZ |
2015 (169) | 32 patients with SZ, 52 HC subjects | Reduced total cerebellar volume in patients with SZ |
2015 (170) | 784 patients with SZ, 936 HC subjects | Patterns of both reduced and increased cerebellar gray matter concentration in patients with SZ |
2017 (171) | 37 patients with SZ, 62 HC subjects | Reduced volume of bilateral cerebellar crus I and II in patients with SZ |
2018 (104) | 158 patients with SZ, 88 HC subjects | Reduced volume of bilateral cerebellar lobules VIII and VI/crus I in patients with SZ |
2018 (101) | 983 patients with SZ, 1349 HC subjects | Reduced total and regional cerebellar volumes in patients with SZ, with most prominent effects in “cognitive” cerebellar regions (e.g., crus I/II) |
2018 (105) | 218 patients with SZ, 190 patients with BD, 256 HC subjects | Pronounced cerebellar volume reductions in a small subset of patients with SZ and patients with BD, but large within-group variability |
2019 (106) | 417 patients with SZ, 389 HC subjects | Reduced cerebellar gray matter volume (vermal lobules IV–V and left crus I) in first-episode, drug-naïve patients with SZ (meta-analysis) |
2019 (102) | 1401 HC subjects | Level of (primarily subclinical) psychotic symptoms were associated with cerebellar volume (VI/crus I) in a community adolescent sample |
BD, bipolar disorder; HC, healthy control; SZ, schizophrenia.
For example, Weinberger et al. (96) reported pronounced atrophy of the cerebellar vermis in 10 of 60 patients with chronic schizophrenia examined using computerized tomography. With the emergence of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in the 1990s, results proved more equivocal, and review articles and meta-analyses thus indicate that structural abnormalities are less consistently observed in the cerebellum than in other brain regions such as the hippocampus, frontal lobe, and temporal lobe (97-99). However, it is also important to keep in mind that there is a corticocentric bias in cognitive neuroscience (100); both scanning protocols and structural analysis tools are generally optimized for the cerebral cortex.
Aiming to avoid such methodological biases, we recently employed an analysis pipeline that was optimized for both the cerebellum and cerebral cortex, looking at volumetric measures in a large sample of participants tested over multiple sites. In sum, we were able to obtain data from 983 individuals with schizophrenia, comparing their brain measures with those obtained from age- and sex-matched healthy control subjects (n = 1349) (101). Overall, the schizophrenia group showed small (Cohen’s d = 0.35), but highly significant, reductions in cerebellar volume (Figure 2A). Interestingly, the volumetric reductions in the cerebellum were as pronounced in younger as in older patients, suggesting a neurodevelopmental rather than a neurodegenerative etiology.
Figure 2.
(A) Significant cerebellar gray matter reductions in a large sample of patients with schizophrenia (n = 983) relative to healthy control subjects (n = 1349) (101). (B) Significant negative associations between cerebellar gray matter volume and level of psychotic symptoms in a large community sample (n = 1401) centered on adolescence (102). corr., corrected.
We also quantified other subcortical and cortical regions, with the final database containing 49 brain features (e.g., volumetric measures of areas such as the cerebellum and hippocampus, and gray matter thickness for regions of the cerebral cortex). Comparing all brain features, cerebellar volume reductions were among the most pronounced, with stronger effects only observed for reduced hippocampal and increased pallidal volume. Moreover, the cerebellar reduction was the most consistent finding across scanning sites. Strongly supporting our findings in adult patients, later we also found gray matter volume in the posterior cerebellum (lobule VI/crus I) to be the most robust brain predictor of (primarily subclinical) psychotic symptom severity in a large community sample of children and young adults (n = 1401; age range = 8–23 years; mean age = 15.1 years) (102) (Figure 2B). Across the majority of studies (101-106), the most prominent changes are seen in areas considered part of the “cognitive” cerebellum (e.g., crus I/II), given their consistent activation during the performance of tasks such as active maintenance of information in working memory, language processing, and autobiographical memory (107-109) and functional connectivity with cognitive networks of the cerebral cortex (110-112). However, it should be noted that reduced volumes of the anterior vermis [lobules IV–V, primarily associated with motor control (113)], have also emerged as a consistent finding across studies (106).
Functional neuroimaging methods have also been brought to bear on the question of cerebellar abnormalities in schizophrenia (114). A frequently reported finding is that this group exhibits altered cerebello-thalamo-cortical functional connectivity (115-126), and similar patterns have been reported in subjects at increased risk for psychosis (85,127-130) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Key Findings From Studies Examining Cerebellar Functional Connectivity Alterations in Psychotic Disorders
Year, Reference | n | Main Finding(s) in Patients (SZ/PSY/CHR) Relative to HC Subjects |
---|---|---|
2014 (119) | 90 patients with SZ, 90 HC subjects | Reduced functional connectivity between the thalamus and the cerebellum |
2014 (119) | 90 patients with SZ, 146 HC subjects | Reduced functional connectivity between the mediodorsal (prefrontal-projecting) thalamus and cerebellar regions |
2015 (127) | 243 individuals at CHR, 154 HC subjects | Reduced cerebellothalamic functional connectivity in youths at CHR for psychosis, with the most pronounced reductions seen in participants who later converted to frank psychosis |
2015 (123) | 44 patients with SZ, 28 HC subjects | Reduced functional connectivity between associative regions of the cerebral cortex and posterior cerebellum (crus I/II), increased functional connectivity between sensorimotor regions of the cerebral cortex and cerebellar regions |
2016 (117) | 148 patients with PSY, 105 HC subjects | Reduced functional connectivity between prefrontal-projecting thalamus and posterior cerebellar regions (crus I/II) in patients with PSY |
2018 (116) | 183 patients with SZ, 178 HC subjects | Reduced functional connectivity between the thalamus and posterior cerebellar regions (crus I/II), associated with delusions and bizarre behavior in SZ |
2018 (115) | Sample 1: 182 individuals at CHR, 120 HC subjects Sample 2: 50 patients with SZ, 49 patients with BD, 40 patients with ADHD, 123 HC subjects |
Increased cerebello-thalamo-cortical network connectivity in a CHR group, with most pronounced increases seen in those who later converted to psychosis; this hyperconnectivity pattern was also found in patients with SZ, but not in patients with BD or ADHD or in HC subjects |
2019 (131) | 3434 HC subjects | Both increased and decreased connectivity between the cerebellum (treated as a single region of interest in this study) and various cortical networks were found to be associated with the level of psychotic-like experiences in 9- to 10-year-old children |
2019 (132) | 1582 patients with PSY, 1272 HC | Failure to observe any consistent changes in thalamocerebellar connectivity in psychotic patients in a meta-analysis of 17 studies |
ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; BD, bipolar disorder; CHR, clinical high risk; HC, healthy control; PSY, psychotic disorder; SZ, schizophrenia.
Of particular note, a recent well-powered study (n = 3434) found an association between altered cerebrocerebellar connectivity (patterns of both hyper- and hypoconnectivity) and psychotic-like phenomena in 9- to 11-year-olds, suggesting that these brain phenotypes may precede the onset of more serious pathology (131). Consistent with this hypothesis, a few studies have shown longitudinal associations between altered cerebello-thalamo-cortical connectivity and symptom progression and/or conversion to psychosis in clinical high-risk groups (115,127,128). However, a recent meta-analysis failed to find consistent thalamocerebellar connectivity changes associated with psychoses, although the author acknowledged that this may be related to methodological limitations (132). Moreover, as can be seen in Table 2, the directionality of these effects (i.e., hypo- or hyperconnectivity in patients relative to control subjects) varies across studies, complicating the interpretation of these findings. Intriguingly, the connectivity abnormalities may be region specific (123). Thus, hypoconnectivity appears to be more prominent in cerebellar areas involved in cognitive functions (e.g., crus I/II), while reports of hyperconnectivity may be associated with cerebral sensorimotor regions (123).
In summary, structural neuroimaging findings provide compelling evidence for cerebellar involvement in schizophrenia, while the evidence from functional MRI (fMRI) studies is more ambiguous. Moreover, the functional implications of these findings remain unclear.
CAN THE INTERNAL MODEL HYPOTHESIS OF CEREBELLAR FUNCTION BE EXTENDED FROM MOTOR CONTROL TO HIGHER-LEVEL COGNITION?
Over the last 3 decades, there has been widespread recognition that the functional domain of the cerebellum encompasses much of cognition (110). For instance, cerebellar activation in fMRI studies is consistently observed during tasks requiring a broad range of cognitive and affective processes, and these activity patterns cannot be accounted for by the motor demands of the tasks (109) (see Figure 1). Moreover, the distribution of activity, observed during either task performance (109) or rest (111,112), indicates that a larger proportion of the human cerebellum is better classified as cognitive rather than motor. However, exactly how the cerebellum contributes to cognition remains an enigma, despite considerable effort on this problem.
Many of the hypotheses concerning the cognitive functions of the cerebellum are extensions of mechanistic ideas developed for understanding how this structure contributes to motor control (34,133,134). This approach has largely been motivated by the relatively homogenous cerebellar microanatomy and physiology (135), features that suggest a corresponding uniformity of function (34,136-139). Of particular relevance to this special issue are efforts to apply the notions of internal models to cognition: might this idea, which has been fruitful in explaining the cerebellar role in predictive motor control (5,37-44), be extended to account for the cerebellar contribution to cognition? We next review some recent studies addressing this question in the language domain [for more extensive reviews, see Argyropoulos (140) and Moberget and Ivry (141)].
During conversation, the interval between turn taking is close to 0 ms (142). This simple observation underscores the importance of predictive mechanisms in language—assuming we are listening to our conversational partner, we must be anticipating the end of their sentence as we initiate our response. Indeed, the concept of internal models has played a prominent role in theories of language processing. For instance, Pickering and Garrod (143) argued that people use internal models in both language production and language comprehension to predict “what they are about to perceive or to do, in a way that allows them to ‘get ahead of the game.’” In speech production, an internal model can support a comparison between the predicted and actual speech, allowing the output to be adjusted when discrepancies are detected (144); in speech perception, an internal model could facilitate language comprehension through the active prediction of the speaker’s next utterance. Supporting a role for the cerebellum in predictive language production, patients with cerebellar degeneration show impairments in adjusting their speech output to predictable perturbations (145), similar to that observed in studies of arm movements (38,39).
But the more intriguing question concerns the role of the cerebellum on the perceptual side of language. Evidence of a cerebellar role in language comprehension was provided by an experiment in which participants listened to spoken sentences and were required to look, as quickly as possible, at 1 of 4 pictures that corresponded to the last word (146,147). The sentences either provided a context that strongly predicted the immediately upcoming final word or created a context in which all of the pictures were equally plausible. Crucially, transient disruption of the right cerebellar hemisphere with either repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (146) or cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (147) selectively slowed saccade reaction times in the predictive conditions, indicating a causal role for the cerebellum in anticipating semantic content.
Following up on the extensive neuroimaging literature implicating the cerebellum in semantic processing (123,124), we designed an fMRI study to focus on the relationship between linguistic predictions and the hemodynamic response in the cerebellum (148). Participants in each trial read sequentially presented words that were of 1 of 3 types: confirmed predictions, in which a coherent sentence ended with a highly predictable last word (e.g., “two plus two is four”); violated predictions, in which the last word violated the context established by the preceding words; and nonpredictive sentences, in which the stimulus consisted of a random sequence of words (e.g., “fast in clock plane”).
The analyses focused on the blood oxygen level–dependent response, time-locked to the final word in the sentence. When comparing the violated predictions, either to the confirmed predictions or to the nonpredictive sentences, a broad pattern of activation was observed across the cerebellum, including bilateral posterior regions (Figure 3A). This pattern is consistent with that observed in studies of motor control both in humans (149,150) and animals (151,152), with the cerebellum sensitive to the presence of an error, arising here in the linguistic domain. Perhaps more surprising was the finding that cerebellar activation was greater in the confirmed prediction sentences compared with the nonpredictive sentences. The activation here was much more focal, limited to a small cluster in crus I/II in the right cerebellar hemisphere (Figure 3B). We hypothesize that this signal is reflecting the operation of a linguistic internal model, one involved in generating the semantic expectancy (148). This interpretation is further supported by another fMRI study (153) in which the stimuli involved the parametric manipulation of the semantic likelihood of the final target word. Activity in the right posterior cerebellum was positively correlated with contextual probability, again suggesting that the cerebellum is engaged when a linguistic context can be exploited to generate a semantic prediction [see also (154,155)] (see Figure 3C).
Figure 3.
Cerebellar functional magnetic resonance imaging activations related to (A) the violation of semantic expectancies and (B) the predictability of an upcoming word (148). Panel (C) shows peak cerebellar activations related to linguistic predictability across 4 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies: red (144); blue (153); green (154); yellow (155).
The noninvasive brain stimulation and fMRI work presented above provide examples of how a hypothesis established in studies of sensorimotor control may be extended to consider an expanded view of cerebellar function. However, it is important to keep in mind three caveats when considering whether the internal model idea provides a useful characterization of cerebellar function in a broad context. First, patients with cerebellar pathology, from either focal lesions or degenerative processes, do not show marked impairments on tasks such as those used in the imaging studies (156). It may be that the cerebellum is not essential for generating semantic predictions, but rather, it helps makes these operations more fluid; as such, behavioral studies may require sensitive measures to detect impairments. Second, to repeat a point raised previously, prediction is a general property of brain function. Surely, the generation of semantic predictions is not the sole province of the cerebellum. As with all work on the cerebellum and cognition, future work is needed to understand how this subcortical structure interacts with other regions of the brain in enabling complex cognition. Third, it remains to be seen if cerebellar dysfunction within the linguistic domain is relevant to our understanding of the language and thought impairments observed in psychosis (28,157).
THE CEREBELLUM, PREDICTION, AND PSYCHOSIS: PLEASE MIND THE GAPS
More generally, might the predictive deficits associated with psychotic disorders reflect an impairment in cerebellar-dependent processes required for generating and implementing internal models? As reviewed above, various lines of evidence point to cerebellar involvement in psychosis and a generalized role in internal model mechanisms across motor and cognitive domains. However, to date, these two literatures have not been integrated. Missing from this picture are experiments that more directly evaluate the hypothesized links among disorder, structure, and function [see Bernard et al. (158) for a notable exception].
First, larger studies are needed to map the prevalence and nature of psychotic symptoms in cerebellar disease and cerebellar impairments in psychotic disorders. The emerging picture is that these associations have been underestimated or underappreciated. However, it is also clear that there is considerable heterogeneity in both patient groups. This heterogeneity mandates the need that future studies be sufficiently powered to be able to detect and characterize subgroups of patients (e.g., identify areas within cerebellum that if lesioned, increase likelihood of psychotic symptoms).
In terms of structural imaging, multisite studies have been able to achieve impressive sample sizes, revealing abnormalities in cerebellar structure in both patients with schizophrenia (101) and youths reporting elevated levels of psychotic symptoms (102). However, it is also important to keep in mind that these structural changes tend to be small to modest, with effect sizes very rarely exceeding Cohen’s d values of 0.5. These modest effect sizes also suggest considerable heterogeneity in individuals who receive the same diagnostic label (104). And, of course, structural abnormalities in schizophrenia are not limited to the cerebellum, but rather are broadly observed in cortical (159) and subcortical (160) structures.
The inconsistencies in the functional connectivity literature—for example, reports of both decreased (116-119,161) and increased (115,162) cerebello-thalamo-cortical connectivity in psychotic disorders—may likewise reflect the heterogeneity of this population. Moreover, as suggested by Table 2, the direction of these connectivity changes may vary for different cerebellar regions. Future meta-analyses evaluating the robustness and regional specificity of cerebellar connectivity abnormalities would be very useful.
Both structural MRI and fMRI methods have clear limitations when it comes to making inferences about the underlying neurobiology (e.g., microcircuits, cell types). For instance, alterations in gray matter volume may reflect a wide range of micro-structural changes (e.g., in neuron density, dendritic arborization, glia), and current MRI methods lack the resolution to differentiate between them. As for fMRI, the blood oxygen level–dependent signal measured in fMRI studies is likely dominated by inputs to the cerebellum from the pontine nuclei, signals carried along the mossy fiber pathway (163). This measure appears to be much less sensitive (or even blind) to the other major input, the climbing fibers originating in the inferior olive, as well as to activity in the Purkinje cells themselves, the output of the cerebellar cortex (163). These limitations point to the need for animal models of cerebellar function [see Person (164) in this special issue], perhaps with an eye on developing tasks that can ask about more generalized applications of internal models.
Finally, while there are emerging literatures examining predictive mechanisms in psychosis as well as cerebellar involvement in (motor and nonmotor) predictive mechanisms, the links between these two literatures are scarce. Figure 4 presents a schematic, highly subjective picture of the current state of knowledge, with the main intent to feature missing and weak links. The boxes and arrows with solid outlines represent well-established phenomena and those with the strongest evidence of an association. In terms of symptoms, delusions of control, auditory hallucinations, and thought disorder are commonly accepted as core clinical features of psychosis; in contrast, motor symptoms have received far less attention (82,93). At the computational level, there is compelling evidence of predictive mechanisms at play in the coordination of movements (5,37), suppression of sensory signals arising from self-generated actions [such as speech (6-11)], and even higher level aspects of language comprehension (143,165). Whether impairment in these mechanisms plays a core causal role in their associated symptoms is an open question, one in which predictions based on an internal model account need to be pitted against alternative mechanistic hypotheses. Similarly, while there is reasonably strong evidence of a cerebellar role in the putative computations depicted in Figure 4, research on how the core symptoms of psychosis relate to cerebellar dysfunction remains in its infancy.
Figure 4.
Schematic summary of existing evidence and knowledge gaps. Symptoms associated with psychosis are represented at the top level, while the middle and bottom levels represent putative computational mechanisms and neural substrates. Arrows denote the links between these levels, solid lines represent relatively well-established phenomena and associations, and dotted and gray lines denote phenomena and associations with a weaker evidence base.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND DISCLOSURES
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant Nos. NS105839 and NS092079 (to RBI) and South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority Grant No. 2016-083 (to TM).
Footnotes
The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
Torgeir Moberget, Norwegian Centre for Mental Disorders Research, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California..
Richard B. Ivry, KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; and the Department of Psychology and Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California.
REFERENCES
- 1.Grüsser OJ (1995): On the history of the ideas of efference copy and reafference. Clio Med 33:35–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Crapse TB, Sommer MA (2008): Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:587–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950): Das Reafferenzprinzip. Natur-wissenschaften 37:464–476. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sperry RW (1950): Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43:482–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995): An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Flinker A, Chang EF, Kirsch HE, Barbaro NM, Crone NE, Knight RT (2010): Single-trial speech suppression of auditory cortex activity in humans. J Neurosci 30:16643–16650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Greenlee JD, Behroozmand R, Larson CR, Jackson AW, Chen F, Hansen DR, et al. (2013): Sensory-motor interactions for vocal pitch monitoring in non-primary human auditory cortex. PLoS One 8:e60783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Creutzfeldt O, Ojemann G, Lettich E (1989): Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. II. Responses to the subjects own voice. Exp Brain Res 77:476–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chen C-MA, Mathalon DH, Roach BJ, Cavus I, Spencer DD, Ford JM (2011): The corollary discharge in humans is related to synchronous neural oscillations. J Cogn Neurosci 23:2892–2904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Heinks-Maldonado TH, Mathalon DH, Houde JF, Gray M, Faustman WO, Ford JM (2007): Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64:286–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Behroozmand R, Larson CR (2011): Error-dependent modulation of speech-induced auditory suppression for pitch-shifted voice feedback. BMC Neurosci 12:54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Feinberg I (1978): Efference copy and corollary discharge: Implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophr Bull 4:636–640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Frith CD, Blakemore S, Wolpert DM (2000): Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 31:357–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ford JM, Mathalon DH (2005): Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: Can it explain auditory hallucinations? Int J Psychophysiol 58:179–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sterzer P, Adams RA, Fletcher P, Frith C, Lawrie SM, Muckli L, et al. (2018): The Predictive coding account of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 84:634–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Frith C (2005): The neural basis of hallucinations and delusions. C R Biol 328:169–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shergill SS, White TP, Joyce DW, Bays PM, Wolpert DM, Frith CD (2014): Functional magnetic resonance imaging of impaired sensory prediction in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71:28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shergill SS, Samson G, Bays PM, Frith CD, Wolpert DM (2005): Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 162:2384–2386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Synofzik M, Thier P, Leube DT, Schlotterbeck P, Lindner A (2010): Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one’s actions. Brain 133:262–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lindner A, Thier P, Kircher TT, Haarmeier T, Leube DT (2005): Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions. Curr Biol 15:1119–1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rosler L, Rolfs M, van der Stigchel S, Neggers SF, Cahn W, Kahn RS, et al. (2015): Failure to use corollary discharge to remap visual target locations is associated with psychotic symptom severity in schizophrenia. J Neurophysiol 114:1129–1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Thakkar KN, Schall JD, Heckers S, Park S (2015): Disrupted saccadic corollary discharge in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 35:9935–9945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Spering M, Dias EC, Sanchez JL, Schutz AC, Javitt DC (2013): Efference copy failure during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 33:11779–11787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bansal S, Bray LCJ, Schwartz BL, Joiner WM (2018): Transsaccadic perception deficits in schizophrenia reflect the improper internal monitoring of eye movement rather than abnormal sensory processing. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 3:168–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ford JM, Palzes VA, Roach BJ, Mathalon DH (2013): Did I do that? Abnormal predictive processes in schizophrenia when button pressing to deliver a tone. Schizophr Bull 40:804–812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ford JM, Mathalon DH, Roach BJ, Keedy SK, Reilly JL, Gershon ES, et al. (2013): Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge function during vocalization in psychotic patients and their nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Schizophr Bull 39:1272–1280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ford JM, Mathalon DH (2012): Anticipating the future: Automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol 83:232–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kuperberg GR (2010): Language in schizophrenia part 1: An introduction. Lang Linguist Compass 4:576–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kuperberg GR, Kreher DA, Ditman T (2010): What can event-related potentials tell us about language, and perhaps even thought, in schizophrenia? Int J Psychophysiol 75:66–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Maher BA (1983): A tentative theory of schizophrenic utterance. Prog Exp Pers Res 12:1–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kuperberg GR, Kreher DA, Goff D, McGuire PK, David AS (2006): Building up linguistic context in schizophrenia: Evidence from self-paced reading. Neuropsychology 20:442–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kuperberg GR, McGuire PK, David AS (1998): Reduced sensitivity to linguistic context in schizophrenic thought disorder: Evidence from on-line monitoring for words in linguistically anomalous sentences. J Abnorm Psychol 107:423–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Corcoran CM, Carrillo F, Fernandez-Slezak D, Bedi G, Klim C, Javitt DC, et al. (2018): Prediction of psychosis across protocols and risk cohorts using automated language analysis. World Psychiatry 17:67–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ramnani N (2006): The primate cortico-cerebellar system: Anatomy and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:511–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ito M (2006): Cerebellar circuitry as a neuronal machine. Prog Neurobiol 78:272–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Streng ML, Popa LS, Ebner TJ (2018): Modulation of sensory prediction error in Purkinje cells during visual feedback manipulations. Nat Commun 9:1099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011): Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Butcher PA, Ivry RB, Kuo SH, Rydz D, Krakauer JW, Taylor JA (2017): The cerebellum does more than sensory prediction error-based learning in sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurophysiol 118:1622–1636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Taylor JA, Ivry RB (2014): Cerebellar and prefrontal cortex contributions to adaptation, strategies, and reinforcement learning. Prog Brain Res 210:217–253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bastian AJ (2011): Moving, sensing and learning with cerebellar damage. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:596–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bastian AJ (2006): Learning to predict the future: The cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:645–649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bhanpuri NH, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ (2014): Predicting and correcting ataxia using a model of cerebellar function. Brain 137:1931–1944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bo J, Block HJ, Clark JE, Bastian AJ (2008): A cerebellar deficit in sensorimotor prediction explains movement timing variability. J Neurophysiol 100:2825–2832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Therrien AS, Bastian AJ (2019): The cerebellum as a movement sensor. Neurosci Lett 688:37–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bhanpuri NH, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ (2013): Predictive modeling by the cerebellum improves proprioception. J Neurosci 33:14301–14306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Knolle F, Schröger E, Baess P, Kotz SA (2012): The cerebellum generates motor-to-auditory predictions: ERP lesion evidence. J Cogn Neurosci 24:698–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Knolle F, Schröger E, Kotz SA (2013): Cerebellar contribution to the prediction of self-initiated sounds. Cortex 49:2449–2461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ito M (2012): The Cerebellum: Brain for an Implicit Self. Upper Saddle River, NJ: FT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sawtell NB (2010): Multimodal integration in granule cells as a basis for associative plasticity and sensory prediction in a cerebellum-like circuit. Neuron 66:573–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Huang C-C, Sugino K, Shima Y, Guo C, Bai S, Mensh BD, et al. (2013): Convergence of pontine and proprioceptive streams onto multimodal cerebellar granule cells. Elife 2:e00400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Knogler LD, Kist AM, Portugues R (2019): Motor context dominates output from Purkinje cell functional regions during reflexive visuo-motor behaviours. Elife 8:e42138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rasmussen A, Hesslow G (2014): Feedback control of learning by the cerebello-olivary pathway. Prog Brain Res 210:103–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Laurens J, Meng H, Angelaki DE (2013): Computation of linear acceleration through an internal model in the macaque cerebellum. Nat Neurosci 16:1701–1708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Trehout M, Zhang N, Blouet M, Borha A, Dollfus S (2018): Dandy-Walker malformation-like condition revealed by refractory schizophrenia: A case report and literature review. Neuropsychobiology 77:59–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Trikamji B, Singh P, Mishra S (2015): Spinocerebellar ataxia-10 with paranoid schizophrenia. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 18:93–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mignarri A, Tessa A, Carluccio MA, Rufa A, Storti E, Bonelli G, et al. (2014): Cerebellum and neuropsychiatric disorders: Insights from ARSACS. Neurol Sci 35:95–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Leroi I, O’Hearn E, Marsh L, Lyketsos CG, Rosenblatt A, Ross CA, et al. (2002): Psychopathology in patients with degenerative cerebellar diseases: A comparison to Huntington’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 159:1306–1314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Liszewski CM, O’Hearn E, Leroi I, Gourley L, Ross CA, Margolis RL (2004): Cognitive impairment and psychiatric symptoms in 133 patients with diseases associated with cerebellar degeneration. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 16:109–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Turk KW, Flanagan ME, Josephson S, Keene CD, Jayadev S, Bird TD (2018): Psychosis in spinocerebellar ataxias: A case series and study of tyrosine hydroxylase in substantia nigra. Cerebellum 17:143–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Madhusoodanan S, Ting MB, Farah T, Ugur U (2015): Psychiatric aspects of brain tumors: A review. World J Psychiatry 5:273–285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bielawski M, Bondurant H (2015): Psychosis following a stroke to the cerebellum and midbrain: A case report. Cerebellum Ataxias 2:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Neufeld N, Gallagher D, Aviv R, Feinstein A (2016): Remote cerebellar stroke associated with delusions and disorganization. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 28:335–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kendler KS (2016): Phenomenology of schizophrenia and the representativeness of modern diagnostic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry 73:1082–1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Walther S, Strik W (2012): Motor symptoms and schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 66:77–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Peralta V, de Jalon EG, Campos MS, Basterra V, Sanchez-Torres A, Cuesta MJ (2011): Risk factors, pre-morbid functioning and episode correlates of neurological soft signs in drug-naive patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Psychol Med 41:1279–1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Peralta V, Cuesta MJ (2001): Motor features in psychotic disorders. I. Factor structure and clinical correlates. Schizophr Res 47:107–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Browne S, Clarke M, Gervin M, Lane A, Waddington JL, Larkin C, et al. (2000): Determinants of neurological dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med 30:1433–1441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Burton BK, Hjorthoj C, Jepsen JR, Thorup A, Nordentoft M, Plessen KJ (2016): Research Review: Do motor deficits during development represent an endophenotype for schizophrenia? A meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:446–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Keskinen E, Marttila A, Marttila R, Jones PB, Murray GK, Moilanen K, et al. (2015): Interaction between parental psychosis and early motor development and the risk of schizophrenia in a general population birth cohort. Eur Psychiatry 30:719–727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Clarke MC, Tanskanen A, Huttunen M, Leon DA, Murray RM, Jones PB, et al. (2011): Increased risk of schizophrenia from additive interaction between infant motor developmental delay and obstetric complications: Evidence from a population-based longitudinal study. Am J Psychiatry 168:1295–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Isohanni M, Jones PB, Moilanen K, Rantakallio P, Veijola J, Oja H, et al. (2001): Early developmental milestones in adult schizophrenia and other psychoses. A 31-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Schizophr Res 52:1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Sorensen HJ, Mortensen EL, Schiffman J, Reinisch JM, Maeda J, Mednick SA (2010): Early developmental milestones and risk of schizophrenia: A 45-year follow-up of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort. Schizophr Res 118:41–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M (1994): Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344:1398–1402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ising HK, Ruhrmann S, Burger NA, Rietdijk J, Dragt S, Klaassen RM, et al. (2016): Development of a stage-dependent prognostic model to predict psychosis in ultra-high-risk patients seeking treatment for co-morbid psychiatric disorders. Psychol Med 46:1839–1851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Osborne KJ, Bernard JA, Gupta T, Dean DJ, Millman Z, Vargas T, et al. (2017): Beat gestures and postural control in youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 185:197–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Dean DJ, Samson AT, Newberry R, Mittal VA (2017): Motion energy analysis reveals altered body movement in youth at risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 200:35–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Bernard JA, B Millman Z, Mittal VA (2015): Beat and metaphoric gestures are differentially associated with regional cerebellar and cortical volumes. Hum Brain Mapp 36:4016–4030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Dean DJ, Kent JS, Bernard JA, Orr JM, Gupta T, Pelletier-Baldelli A, et al. (2015): Increased postural sway predicts negative symptom progression in youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 162:86–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Dean DJ, Mittal VA (2015): Spontaneous parkinsonisms and striatal impairment in neuroleptic free youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosis. NPJ Schizophr 1:14006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Burton BK, Thorup AAE, Jepsen JR, Poulsen G, Ellersgaard D, Spang KS, et al. (2017): Impairments of motor function among children with a familial risk of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder at 7 years old in Denmark: An observational cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 4:400–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Crow TJ, Done DJ, Sacker A (1995): Childhood precursors of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 245:61–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hirjak D, Thomann PA, Kubera KM, Wolf ND, Sambataro F, Wolf RC (2015): Motor dysfunction within the schizophrenia-spectrum: A dimensional step towards an underappreciated domain. Schizophr Res 169:217–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Mittal VA, Walker EF (2007): Movement abnormalities predict conversion to Axis I psychosis among prodromal adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol 116:796–803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Gowen E, Miall RC (2007): The cerebellum and motor dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders. Cerebellum 6:268–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Bernard JA, Dean DJ, Kent JS, Orr JM, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Lunsford-Avery JR, et al. (2014): Cerebellar networks in individuals at ultra high-risk of psychosis: Impact on postural sway and symptom severity. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4064–4078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kinney DK, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Woods BT (1999): Neurologic signs of cerebellar and cortical sensory dysfunction in schizophrenics and their relatives. Schizophr Res 35:99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Ho BC, Mola C, Andreasen NC (2004): Cerebellar dysfunction in neuroleptic naive schizophrenia patients: Clinical, cognitive, and neuroanatomic correlates of cerebellar neurologic signs. Biol Psychiatry 55:1146–1153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Jirenhed D-A, Bengtsson F, Hesslow G (2007): Acquisition, extinction, and reacquisition of a cerebellar cortical memory trace. J Neurosci 27:2493–2502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Forsyth JK, Bolbecker AR, Mehta CS, Klaunig MJ, Steinmetz JE, O’Donnell BF, et al. (2012): Cerebellar-dependent eyeblink conditioning deficits in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull 38:751–759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Parker KL, Andreasen NC, Liu D, Freeman JH, O’Leary DS (2013): Eyeblink conditioning in unmedicated schizophrenia patients: A positron emission tomography study. Psychiatry Res 214:402–409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Coesmans M, Röder CH, Smit AE, Koekkoek SKE, De Zeeuw CI, Frens MA, et al. (2014): Cerebellar motor learning deficits in medicated and medication-free men with recent-onset schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci 39:E3–E11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Bolbecker AR, Kent JS, Petersen IT, Klaunig MJ, Forsyth JK, Howell JM, et al. (2013): Impaired cerebellar-dependent eyeblink conditioning in first-degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 40:1001–1010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2015): Updating the research domain criteria: The utility of a motor dimension. Psychol Med 45:2685–2689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Cuthbert BN, Insel TR (2013): Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 11:126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Heath RG, Franklin DE, Shraberg D (1979): Gross pathology of the cerebellum in patients diagnosed and treated as functional psychiatric disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 167:585–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Weinberger DR, Torrey EF, Wyatt RJ (1979): Cerebellar atrophy in chronic schizophrenia. Lancet 1:718–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frumin M, McCarley RW (2001): A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 49:1–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Honea R, Crow TJ, Passingham D, Mackay CE (2005): Regional deficits in brain volume in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Am J Psychiatry 162:2233–2245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Shepherd AM, Laurens KR, Matheson SL, Carr VJ, Green MJ (2012): Systematic meta-review and quality assessment of the structural brain alterations in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:1342–1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Parvizi J (2009): Corticocentric myopia: Old bias in new cognitive sciences. Trends Cogn Sci 13:354–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Moberget T, Doan NT, Alnæs D, Kaufmann T, Córdova-Palomera A, Lagerberg TV, et al. (2018): Cerebellar volume and cerebellocerebral structural covariance in schizophrenia: A multisite mega-analysis of 983 patients and 1349 healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry 23:1512–1520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Moberget T, Alnæs D, Kaufmann T, Doan NT, Córdova-Palomera A, Norbom LB, et al. (2019): Cerebellar gray matter volume is associated with cognitive function and psychopathology in adolescence. Biol Psychiatry 86:65–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Kühn S, Romanowski A, Schubert F, Gallinat J (2012): Reduction of cerebellar grey matter in Crus I and II in schizophrenia. Brain Struct Funct 217:523–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Quinn M, McHugo M, Armstrong K, Woodward N, Blackford J, Heckers S (2018): Impact of substance use disorder on gray matter volume in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 280:9–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Wolfers T, Doan NT, Kaufmann T, Alnaes D, Moberget T, Agartz I, et al. (2018): Mapping the heterogeneous phenotype of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder using normative models. JAMA Psychiatry 75:1146–1155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Ding Y, Ou Y, Pan P, Shan X, Chen J, Liu F, et al. (2019): Cerebellar structural and functional abnormalities in first-episode and drug-naive patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 283:24–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.King M, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Poldrack RA, Ivry R, Diedrichsen J (2019): Functional boundaries in the human cerebellum revealed by a multi-domain task battery. Nat Neurosci 22:1371–1378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Keren-Happuch E, Chen S-HA, Ho M-HR, Desmond JE (2014): A meta-analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp 35:593–615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD (2009): Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage 44:489–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Buckner RL (2013): The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80:807–815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT (2011): The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106:2322–2345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Marek S, Siegel JS, Gordon EM, Raut RV, Gratton C, Newbold DJ, et al. (2018): Spatial and temporal organization of the individual human cerebellum. Neuron 100:977–993.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Schoch B, Dimitrova A, Gizewski ER, Timmann D (2006): Functional localization in the human cerebellum based on voxelwise statistical analysis: A study of 90 patients. NeuroImage 30:36–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2015): Dysfunctional activation of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci 3:545–566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Cao H, Chen OY, Chung Y, Forsyth JK, McEwen SC, Gee DG, et al. (2018): Cerebello-thalamo-cortical hyperconnectivity as a state-independent functional neural signature for psychosis prediction and characterization. Nat Commun 9:3836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Ferri J, Ford JM, Roach BJ, Turner JA, van Erp TG, Voyvodic J, et al. (2018): Resting-state thalamic dysconnectivity in schizophrenia and relationships with symptoms. Psychol Med 48:2492–2499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Woodward ND, Heckers S (2016): Mapping thalamocortical functional connectivity in chronic and early stages of psychotic disorders. Biol Psychiatry 79:1016–1025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Anticevic A, Cole MW, Repovs G, Murray JD, Brumbaugh MS, Winkler AM, et al. (2013): Characterizing thalamo-cortical disturbances in schizophrenia and bipolar illness. Cereb Cortex 24:3116–3130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Anticevic A, Yang G, Savic A, Murray JD, Cole MW, Repovs G, et al. (2014): Mediodorsal and visual thalamic connectivity differ in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with and without psychosis history. Schizophr Bull 40:1227–1243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Duan H-F, Gan J-L, Yang J-M, Cheng Z-X, Gao C-Y, Shi Z-J, et al. (2015): A longitudinal study on intrinsic connectivity of hippocampus associated with positive symptom in first-episode schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res 283:78–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Liu H, Fan G, Xu K, Wang F (2011): Changes in cerebellar functional connectivity and anatomical connectivity in schizophrenia: A combined resting-state functional MRI and diffusion tensor imaging study. J Magn Reson Imaging 34:1430–1438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Shen H, Wang L, Liu Y, Hu D (2010): Discriminative analysis of resting-state functional connectivity patterns of schizophrenia using low dimensional embedding of fMRI. NeuroImage 49:3110–3121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Shinn AK, Baker JT, Lewandowski KE, Ongur D, Cohen BM (2015): Aberrant cerebellar connectivity in motor and association networks in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci 9:134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Wang H-LS, Rau C-L, Li Y-M, Chen Y-P, Yu R (2015): Disrupted thalamic resting-state functional networks in schizophrenia. Front Behav Neurosci 9:45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Wang L, Zou F, Shao Y, Ye E, Jin X, Tan S, et al. (2014): Disruptive changes of cerebellar functional connectivity with the default mode network in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 160:67–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Zhuo C, Wang C, Wang L, Guo X, Xu Q, Liu Y, et al. (2017): Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Brain Imaging Behav 12:383–389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Anticevic A, Haut K, Murray JD, Repovs G, Yang GJ, Diehl C, et al. (2015): Association of thalamic dysconnectivity and conversion to psychosis in youth and young adults at elevated clinical risk. JAMA Psychiatry 72:882–891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Bernard JA, Orr JM, Mittal VA (2017): Cerebello-thalamo-cortical networks predict positive symptom progression in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. NeuroImage Clin 14:622–628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Pelletier-Baldelli A, Bernard JA, Mittal VA (2015): Intrinsic functional connectivity in salience and default mode networks and aberrant social processes in youth at ultra-high risk for psychosis. PLoS One 10:e0134936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Whalley HC, Simonotto E, Marshall I, Owens DG, Goddard NH, Johnstone EC, et al. (2005): Functional disconnectivity in subjects at high genetic risk of schizophrenia. Brain 128:2097–2108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Karcher NR, O’Brien KJ, Kandala S, Barch DM (2019): Resting-state functional connectivity and psychotic-like experiences in childhood: Results from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study. Biol Psychiatry 86:7–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Ramsay IS (2019): An activation likelihood estimate meta-analysis of thalamocortical dysconnectivity in psychosis [published online ahead of print Apr 24]. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Balsters JH, Whelan CD, Robertson IH, Ramnani N (2013): Cerebellum and cognition: Evidence for the encoding of higher order rules. Cereb Cortex 23:1433–1443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Ramnani N (2014): Automatic and controlled processing in the corticocerebellar system. Prog Brain Res 210:255–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Bloedel JR (1992): Functional-heterogeneity with structural homogeneity: How does the cerebellum operate? Behav Brain Sci 15:666–678. [Google Scholar]
- 136.Ito M (2008): Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:304–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Ito M (1993): Movement and thought: Identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci 16:448–450; discussion 453–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS (1991): The human cerebro-cerebellar system: Its computing, cognitive, and language skills. Behav Brain Res 44:113–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Leiner HC, Leiner AL, Dow RS (1993): Cognitive and language functions of the human cerebellum. Trends Neurosci 16:444–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Argyropoulos GPD (2015): The cerebellum, internal models and prediction in ‘non-motor’ aspects of language: A critical review. Brain Lang 161:4–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Moberget T, Ivry RB (2016): Cerebellar contributions to motor control and language comprehension: Searching for common computational principles. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1369:154–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Wilson M, Wilson TP (2005): An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychon Bull Rev 12:957–968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Pickering MJ, Garrod S (2013): An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behav Brain Sci 36:329–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Tourville JA, Reilly KJ, Guenther FH (2008): Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech. NeuroImage 39:1429–1443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Parrell B, Agnew Z, Nagarajan S, Houde J, Ivry RB (2017): Impaired feedforward control and enhanced feedback control of speech in patients with cerebellar degeneration. J Neurosci 37:9249–9258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Lesage E, Morgan BE, Olson AC, Meyer AS, Miall RC (2012): Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Curr Biol 22:R794–R795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Miall RC, Antony J, Goldsmith-Sumner A, Harding SR, McGovern C, Winter JL (2016): Modulation of linguistic prediction by TDCS of the right lateral cerebellum. Neuropsychologia 86:103–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Moberget T, Gullesen EH, Andersson S, Ivry RB, Endestad T (2014): Generalized role for the cerebellum in encoding internal models: Evidence from semantic processing. J Neurosci 34:2871–2878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R (2005): Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci 25:9919–9931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Schlerf J, Ivry RB, Diedrichsen J (2012): Encoding of sensory prediction errors in the human cerebellum. J Neurosci 32:4913–4922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Gilbert PF, Thach WT (1977): Purkinje cell activity during motor learning. Brain Res 128:309–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Horn KM, Pong M, Gibson AR (2004): Discharge of inferior olive cells during reaching errors and perturbations. Brain Res 996:148–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Lesage E, Hansen PC, Miall RC (2017): Right lateral cerebellum represents linguistic predictability. J Neurosci 37:6231–6241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.D’Mello AM, Turkeltaub PE, Stoodley CJ (2017): Cerebellar tDCS modulates neural circuits during semantic prediction: A combined tDCS-fMRI study. J Neurosci 37:1604–1613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Bonhage CE, Mueller JL, Friederici AD, Fiebach CJ (2015): Combined eye tracking and fMRI reveals neural basis of linguistic predictions during sentence comprehension. Cortex 68:33–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Alexander MP, Gillingham S, Schweizer T, Stuss DT (2012): Cognitive impairments due to focal cerebellar injuries in adults. Cortex 48:980–990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Kuperberg GR (2010): Language in schizophrenia part 2: What can psycholinguistics bring to the study of schizophrenia…and vice versa? Lang Linguist Compass 4:590–604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Bernard JA, Orr JM, Dean DJ, Mittal VA (2018): The cerebellum and learning of non-motor associations in individuals at clinical-high risk for psychosis. Neuroimage Clin 19:137–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.van Erp TGM, Walton E, Hibar DP, Schmaal L, Jiang W, Glahn DC, et al. (2018): Cortical brain abnormalities in 4474 individuals with schizophrenia and 5098 control subjects via the Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics Through Meta Analysis (ENIGMA) Consortium. Biol Psychiatry 84:644–654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.van Erp TG, Hibar DP, Rasmussen JM, Glahn DC, Pearlson GD, Andreassen OA, et al. (2016): Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Mol Psychiatry 21:547–553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Brady RO Jr, Gonsalvez I, Lee I, Öngür D, Seidman LJ, Schmahmann JD, et al. (2019): Cerebellar-prefrontal network connectivity and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 176:512–520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Walther S, Stegmayer K, Federspiel A, Bohlhalter S, Wiest R, Viher PV (2017): Aberrant hyperconnectivity in the motor system at rest is linked to motor abnormalities in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull 43:982–992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Diedrichsen J, Verstynen T, Schlerf J, Wiestler T (2010): Advances in functional imaging of the human cerebellum. Curr Opin Neurol 23:382–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Person AL (2019): Corollary discharge signals in the cerebellum. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 4:813–819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Pickering MJ, Clark A (2014): Getting ahead: Forward models and their place in cognitive architecture. Trends Cogn Sci 18:451–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Aylward EH, Reiss A, Barta PE, Tien A, Han W, Lee J, et al. (1994): Magnetic resonance imaging measurement of posterior fossa structures in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 151:1448–1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Andreasen NC, Flashman L, Flaum M, Arndt S, Swayze V 2nd, O’Leary DS, et al. (1994): Regional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia measured with magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA 272:1763–1769. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Tanskanen P, Ridler K, Murray GK, Haapea M, Veijola JM, Jaaskelainen E, et al. (2010): Morphometric brain abnormalities in schizophrenia in a population-based sample: Relationship to duration of illness. Schizophr Bull 36:766–777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Laidi C, d’Albis M-A, Wessa M, Linke J, Phillips ML, Delavest M, et al. (2015): Cerebellar volume in schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder with and without psychotic features. Acta Psychiatr Scand 131:223–233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Gupta CN, Calhoun VD, Rachakonda S, Chen J, Patel V, Liu J, et al. (2015): Patterns of gray matter abnormalities in schizophrenia based on an international mega-analysis. Schizophr Bull 41:1133–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Ota M, Matsuo J, Sato N, Teraishi T, Hori H, Hattori K, et al. (2017): Correlation of reduced social communicational and interactional skills with regional grey matter volumes in schizophrenia patients. Acta Neuropsychiatr 29:374–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]