Skip to main content
Cells logoLink to Cells
. 2021 Jul 20;10(7):1829. doi: 10.3390/cells10071829

Examining the Role of the Noradrenergic Locus Coeruleus for Predicting Attention and Brain Maintenance in Healthy Old Age and Disease: An MRI Structural Study for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative

Emanuele R G Plini 1,*, Erik O’Hanlon 1,2,3, Rory Boyle 1, Francesca Sibilia 3, Gaia Rikhye 1, Joanne Kenney 1, Robert Whelan 4, Michael C Melnychuk 1, Ian H Robertson 1,4, Paul M Dockree 1
Editors: Otto Witte, Kathrin Finke, Anja Urbach
PMCID: PMC8306442  PMID: 34359997

Abstract

The noradrenergic theory of Cognitive Reserve (Robertson, 2013–2014) postulates that the upregulation of the locus coeruleus—noradrenergic system (LC–NA) originating in the brainstem might facilitate cortical networks involved in attention, and protracted activation of this system throughout the lifespan may enhance cognitive stimulation contributing to reserve. To test the above-mentioned theory, a study was conducted on a sample of 686 participants (395 controls, 156 mild cognitive impairment, 135 Alzheimer’s disease) investigating the relationship between LC volume, attentional performance and a biological index of brain maintenance (BrainPAD—an objective measure, which compares an individual’s structural brain health, reflected by their voxel-wise grey matter density, to the state typically expected at that individual’s age). Further analyses were carried out on reserve indices including education and occupational attainment. Volumetric variation across groups was also explored along with gender differences. Control analyses on the serotoninergic (5-HT), dopaminergic (DA) and cholinergic (Ach) systems were contrasted with the noradrenergic (NA) hypothesis. The antithetic relationships were also tested across the neuromodulatory subcortical systems. Results supported by Bayesian modelling showed that LC volume disproportionately predicted higher attentional performance as well as biological brain maintenance across the three groups. These findings lend support to the role of the noradrenergic system as a key mediator underpinning the neuropsychology of reserve, and they suggest that early prevention strategies focused on the noradrenergic system (e.g., cognitive-attentive training, physical exercise, pharmacological and dietary interventions) may yield important clinical benefits to mitigate cognitive impairment with age and disease.

Keywords: locus coeruleus, reserve, brain age, visual attention, Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, normal aging, neuroimaging, voxel based morphometry

1. Introduction

Neurodegeneration does not always affect cognitive impairment and daily life functioning to the same extent [1,2,3]. In fact, some individuals even with marked brain deterioration present relatively preserved cognitive function compared to healthy individuals. The observation that the extent of brain deterioration does not reliably or linearly predict the severity of cognitive dysfunction and symptomatology can be viewed as a “cognition-pathology gap” [4]. This phenomenon has been partly explained by the role of compensatory neural processes, underscored by genetic and/or environmental factors that help mitigate the effects of advancing pathology. This resilience to brain damage has been conceptualised by Yaakov Stern (2002) [5] as a model of reserve that moderates between the degree of neural pathology and clinical outcome. Reserve is conceived as a protective factor shaped by cumulative improvement of neural resources due to genetic and environmental factors over a lifetime. In the earlier stages of the development of this construct, it has been differentiated as cognitive reserve and brain reserve [6]. Cognitive reserve refers broadly to a person’s adaptability (i.e., efficiency, capacity, flexibility of cognitive processes) that helps to explain differential susceptibility of cognitive abilities or day-to-day function to brain aging, pathology or insult [7,8]. Cognitive reserve is often represented by intelligence quotient (I.Q.), educational level, occupational complexity and cognitive performance on tests of attention, memory and executive functioning. By contrast, brain reserve is quantified at the neurobiological level in terms of, for example, brain volume, and the degree of functional and structural connectivity between diverse brain regions. However, the current consensus is moving towards a broader and more comprehensive concept of reserve, since neural and cognitive components are deeply integrated with each other. Accordingly, the current terminology, referring to reserve as the cumulative improvement due to genetic and environmental factors pertains to both neurobiological and cognitive levels of reserve [8]. Two important sub-components of reserve are maintenance and compensation [9]. Maintenance is considered to reflect how well a brain is preserved in structural and functional terms, and compensation is the capacity to recruit existing resources with greater efficiency or to employ alternative neural networks in response to cognitive demand. These two sub-components are therefore measured in different ways [8]. First, an index of brain maintenance has been developed to assess the degree of brain deterioration relative to chronological age (Brain Predicted Age Difference—BrainPAD or Brain Gap Estimation—BrainAGE) in order to address a more precise value of maintenance than just brain volume alone [10,11,12]. Based on the brain deterioration in optimal normal ageing, BrainPAD is a cross-sectional measure, which compares an individual’s structural brain health, reflected by their voxel-wise grey matter density, to the state typically expected at that individual’s age. Therefore, higher discrepancies between the biological brain age and chronological age are indices of abnormal ageing and lower reserve with respect to the brain maintenance sub-component. Second, compensation can be indexed by upregulation of a particular neural network or by alternative patterns of functional connectivity during a task, yielding a beneficial behavioural outcome [8,9]. Thus, it can be inferred that such patterns of activation or re-organisation serve compensatory processes.

The evidence of the last decades has shown that the most common and well addressed predictors of reserve—higher education, I.Q., cognitive stimulation, social interaction and physical activity—are related to reduced risk of dementia, better cognitive functioning and overall greater brain mass [13]. Moreover, these factors mediate the gap between brain pathology and cognitive functioning. In the extensive review by Livingstone and colleagues [13], it was estimated that among the 35% of the modifiable risk factors of dementia, high education has a mitigating impact of 8% on disease severity, which increases resilience. Although progress has been made in identifying the contribution of these genetic and/or life experience variables to the accumulation of reserve, the neurobiological substrate underpinning reserve and the extent to which it can mitigate neurodegeneration are not yet understood.

In this regard, Robertson [1,2] proposed the noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve, arguing that the continuous upregulation of the noradrenergic system throughout the life-time can be a key component of cognitive reserve. The noradrenergic (NA) system originates in a small bilateral nucleus of the pons of the brain stem named the locus coeruleus (LC). LC synthesizes approximately 70% of noradrenaline (NA) in the brain and is responsible of the 90% of the total brain efflux of NA [14]. NA is one of the most diffusely distributed neurotransmitters in the brain and underpins arousal, alertness and attention [14,15,16,17]. Furthermore, several studies of neurodegeneration have found that NA supply and LC integrity are significantly reduced in Alzheimer’s disease [18,19,20,21]. Tau pathology in the LC has been found earlier than in other brain regions during the preclinical stages of the disease [20,22,23,24,25,26], and reduced volume of the LC has been associated with the extent of beta-amyloid and tau pathologies [27,28,29], suggesting it as possible biomarker of neurodegenerative diseases [30]. In a post-mortem study on 165 individuals, Wilson et al. [31] found that higher neural density in the LC was significantly associated with better baseline cognitive functioning and with slower cognitive decline. There is also evidence that the LC exhibits neural loss up to 80% greater in Alzheimer’s patients than in controls [32]. Comparatively, both neuroimaging and histopathological human and animal studies also report that lower LC volume was associated with increased beta-amyloid and tau pathologies across the brain and with increased overall brain deterioration and inflammation [25,28,29,33,34,35,36,37]. Furthermore, murine models demonstrate that NA promotes phagocytosis of beta-amyloid plaques, suggesting that the LC–NA system may offer protection against the development of these plaques in the human brain [38].

Robertson’s theory links the recent evidence concerning the LC–NA involvement in neurodegeneration with its primary role in attention, learning and memory consolidation. The key cognitive reserve factors (higher IQ and education, life-long social and cognitive stimulation and physical exercise) are components, which require activation of the LC in producing and releasing NA [1,2,13,39]. These reserve proxies require arousal modulation and attentional processing in response to problem solving and novelty. In Robertson’s theoretical framework, the continuous protraction of these activities, on a regular basis throughout life-time, can lead to an overall higher noradrenergic tone, facilitating more active cognition and building more resilient neurobiological networks in the face of age- and disease-related decline. The ability to sustain attention and the arousal in response to cognitive engagement and novelty may increase general awareness and mental stimulation, both of which positively contribute to the construct of reserve. Therefore, the proposed neurobiological mechanism behind the protective action of reserve indices may in part be explained by the properties of a more active noradrenergic system, which optimises structural and functional brain connectivity, providing greater resistance to neurodegeneration. NA has been known for its neuroprotective effects [40,41,42], which reduce brain inflammation and promote neurogenesis and synaptogenesis increasing brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which could increase the number of brain cells [43,44,45,46,47,48]. This noradrenergic system may therefore be critical in to reduce the brain’s vulnerability to both the normal ageing process and pathological neurodegeneration via a more active attentional system [1,2,49]. Therefore, in this theoretical framework, greater LC volume is proposed to reflect a greater noradrenergic tone; conversely, lower LC volume is associated with lower NA levels compromising cognition and brain heath, according to previous studies [17,18,20,21,22,25,26,28,30,33,36,47,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61].

The main aim of this study was to test the hypothesized relationship between the structural integrity of the LC and indices of reserve and attention in healthy older controls (HC) and patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). To this end, we adopted a neuroimaging voxel-based morphometry (VBM) approach utilizing 3T T1-weighted structural MRI scans from 686 subjects [n = 395 (HC), n = 156 (MCI), and 135 (AD)] provided by Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative—ADNI (ADNI2 and ADNI3 phases) [62,63]. Structural volumetric analyses on 3T T1-weighted MRIs with this methodology have been already carried out by several studies showing accurate reliability investigating the integrity of the Brainstem [64,65,66,67], the LC also in the ADNI [68,69,70,71] and the other neuromodulators’ seeds such as the Raphe Nuclei [72,73,74,75], the ventral tegmental area (VTA) [76,77,78,79] and the nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM) [80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87]. The main objectives of this study were threefold. First (1st branch of VBM analyses), to examine the relationship between LC integrity and cognitive function measured by the Trail Making Test A (TMT-A), which is a clinical tool sensitive to basic attentional efficiency, visual search and motor processing speed [88,89,90]. TMT has been chosen over other attentional tests available in the ADNI since visuospatial engagement of attention more is markedly underpinned by LC–NA system [1,2,91] compared to tasks involving auditory or verbal processing. Furthermore, the LC–NA system expressly innervates the medial prefrontal regions of cortex [14,91,92], which are commonly activated during the spatial attention and visuo-motor requirements of TMT performance in fMRI studies [93,94,95,96]. More specifically, TMT part A has been chosen over the part B since the latter was not completed by all the selected participants included in the ADNI (specifically in the Alzheimer’s group), and also because it is considered to evaluate more structured cognitive processes such as cognitive flexibility and divided attention [97,98]. We anticipated that greater LC volume would be negatively associated with TMT-A completion time in seconds, according to previous findings [31,49,68] and consistent with Robertson’s theory [1,2].

Second (2nd branch of VBM analyses), to examine the extent to which LC integrity mediates the relationship between BrainPAD and attentional efficiency. We tested the hypothesis that higher LC volume would be associated with both higher brain maintenance and better cognitive performance as postulated by Robertson [1,2]. It was expected that higher LC volume would predict more negative values of BrainPAD reflecting ‘younger’ brain age. These analyses investigated the potential compensatory role of the LC–NA system in mediating the relationship between brain maintenance and cognitive performance. Third (3rd and 4th branches of VBM analyses), to analyse the potential relationships between LC integrity and putative indices of reserve, namely, educational level and occupational status held by people throughout their lifetime. It was anticipated that greater LC volume would be associated with both higher levels of education and occupational complexity demands. Indeed, in Robertson’s theory, jobs with higher cognitive demands require greater stimulation of the noradrenergic system contributing to the reserve component [1,2].

As a control procedure, each of the above analyses were repeated testing the opposite relationships as hypothesized, namely, lower LC volume related to better cognition and brain health [88,99,100]. Furthermore, the analyses were repeated for brainstem nuclei of the serotoninergic, dopaminergic and cholinergic systems in order to compare the Noradrenergic hypothesis to the other main neuromodulators involved in cognition. Finally, the analyses were also performed on an ROI drawn in the ventro-rostral portion of the Pons, which to the best of our knowledge is without anatomical nuclei projecting neuromodulators to the cortex, and can therefore be considered a “neuromodulator-free” control region.

Secondary objectives of the study were to investigate which of the above-mentioned neuromodulators showed the greatest covariance with cognitive decline across the three groups (5th branch of VBM analyses) and to explore the differential volumetric variation of the neuromodulators due to gender at different stages of cognitive decline (6th branch of VBM analyses).

2. Materials and Methods

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu accessed on 15 January 2021). The ADNI was launched in 2003 as a public–private partnership, led by Principal Investigator Michael W. Weiner, MD. The primary goal of ADNI has been to test whether serial magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), positron emission tomography (PET), other biological markers and clinical and neuropsychological assessment can be combined to measure the progression of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and early Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

2.1. Neuroimaging

3Tesla T1-weighted images in Nifti format of ADNI 2 and ADNI 3 phases [62,63] were downloaded from IDA (image data archive powered by LONI—https://ida.loni.usc.edu/ accessed on 15 December 2018). Baseline T1-images of all subjects were selected and organised according to diagnosis: cognitive normal/healthy controls (CN/HC), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Diagnostic criteria of the ADNI are described by Petersen et al. 2010 [101]. A rigorous manual quality control of the images was carried out according to the rating scale guidelines (1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, 4 = excellent) of the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (https://www.humanconnectome.org/ accessed on 15 December 2018). Subjects with low definition (excessive blurriness) and/or marked ringing, inhomogeneities and motion artefacts were removed from the dataset. The selected scans were then segmented using CAT12 (Computational Anatomy Toolbox—http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/ accessed on 15 December 2018) implemented in SPM12. The segmentation was performed using the default CAT12 settings, with a 1 mm isotropic voxel size. The segmentation was run separately for the three groups with the same pipeline and parameters. Subjects with an image quality score below 70% (no more than 0.6% of the sample) were then discarded according to CAT12 reports of quality assurance rating scale (100–90 = A [Excellent]; 90–80 = B [Good]; 80–70 = C [Satisfactory]; 70–60 = D [Sufficient]; 60–50 = E [Critical]; 50–40 = F [unacceptable/failed]. Only the 6.2% of the selected images were below 80% (B). Subsequently, the processed normalized images Grey Matter (GM) + White Matter (WM) were smoothed with a 2 mm3 FWHM kernel. Lastly, in order to better account for individual volumetric variability, the Total Intracranial Volume (TIV) was calculated for each subject using CAT12 interface. More details are provided in the Supplementary Materials.

2.1.1. Region of Interest (ROI) Masks

All binary ROIs had a 1 mm3 isotropic voxel size and were oriented in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space as the processed T1 (GM + WM) images. The six ROI masks were obtained by previously published atlases. The technical details and theoretical justifications for the specific ROI definitions are described in the following section for each neuromodulator seed. Further details are provided in the Supplementary Materials.

Accurate MRI localization of the LC in the human brain is still lacking wide-spread agreement [34]. In the last few years, several probabilistic maps of the LC have been released; however, these probabilistic maps are inconsistent in both localization and volume extent within the MNI space. Indeed, different sample sizes have been recruited and this exacerbates the limitations due to different methodologies involved. These differences reflect a large anatomical variability of the samples scanned, suggesting that the LC varies across the general population.

In order to perform volumetric analyses appropriate to the present research and to attempt to resolve these differences, it was necessary to define a common space that included all the previous maps as to increase the likelihood of inclusion of the entirety of the LC, given the probable increase in between-subject anatomical variability in the present ADNI populations (n = 395 HC, n = 156 MCI, n = 135 AD).

Therefore, a new symmetrical “omni-comprehensive” LC mask in the MNI space was created in order to include the whole LC rostro-caudal extent (see Figure 1). Indeed, it was observed that with increasing age, the LC signal intensity tends to shift from the rostral to the caudal portion [34,102]. This process might be influenced by manifold variables, such as ageing, the degree of biological brain maintenance and even dementia progression, which is likely to exacerbate this “caudal-shifting” process. Moreover, it is acknowledged how the noradrenergic system is susceptible to compensatory changes across the brain involving the caudal portion of the LC and peri-coeruleus/LC-peri-dendritic regions (Epi-coeruleus and Sub-coeruleus) [14,54,55,61,103,104,105,106]. Therefore, a larger area rather than a very specific and concise region would be more informative and appropriate while investigating the LC–NA system on different groups, particularly known the heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s disease. The new “omni-comprehensive” LC mask included the six LC MNI atlases previously published: (1) Keren et al. (2009) [102,107], (2) Tona et al. (2017) [108], (3) Betts et al. (2017) [109], (4) Dahl et al. (2019) [33], (5) Liu et al. (2019) [110] and (6) Rong Ye et al. (2020) [111] without encroaching the median raphe (MR) and the dorsal raphe (DR) defined by Beliveau et al. (2015) [112] and the cerebellar white matter. Additionally, the new created LC “omni-comprehensive” mask included the LC meta-mask developed by Dahl et al. (2021) [70], but with a larger and symmetrical rostro-caudal extent to avoid induced lateralization biases in the analyses. Indeed, as pointed out by Betts et al. [109], the LC asymmetries reported in certain MRI studies could be caused by MRI biases of how radiofrequencies are transmitted and received in the scanner. In fact, post-mortem histological studies consistently revealed symmetrical distribution of LC cells [14,113,114,115,116,117].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The spatial resolution of the new created symmetrical “omni-comprehensive” LC mask in comparison with the previously published LC MRI atlases and masks.

MR and DR 1 mm3 MNI masks were generously provided by Beliveau et al. (2015) [112]. The probabilistic maps and masks were obtained by analysing 232 PET scans matched with high-res 3T structural MRI of healthy subjects between 18 and 45 years old.

The VTA mask was obtained by downloading the VTA MNI probabilistic map from the atlas made by Pauli et al. 2018 [118] from the NeuroVault website (https://neurovault.org/ accessed on 15 December 2018). The atlas was made using the MRI data from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) and was derived from a selected sample size of 168 healthy subjects between 22 and 35 years old. The localisation of the substantia innominata (SI)/NBM was more controversial than the previous nuclei, as there are no specific maps available in MNI space. Albeit, probabilistic MNI maps of the acetylcholine cells of the Forebrain are provided by SPM Anatomy Toolbox 2.2c (https://www.fzjuelich.de/inm/inm1/EN/Forschung/_docs/SPMAnatomyToolbox/SPMAnatomyToolbox_node.html accessed on 15 December 2018) [119]. However, the probabilistic map referring to the SI/NBM (4ch.nii) defined by Zaborszky et al. (2008) [119] overlaps several subcortical nuclei delineated in other atlases [118,120,121,122,123,124,125,126]. Therefore, the “4ch.nii” was used as main reference to delineate the SI/NBM, but was adjusted by excluding the subcortical nuclei identified by other atlases while accounting for the probabilistic localisation of the SI/NBM delineated in previous works [81,82,127,128,129]. In order to control for a “neuromodulator-free” brainstem’s region, a squared binary ROI not referring to any anatomical nuclei was drawn in the ventro-rostral portion of the pons. A greater number of voxels were used in order to obtain a control region similar in voxel-size well-suited for the detection of false positives. The six ROIs are displayed in Figure 2 below.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The neuromodulators’ seeds and the neuromodulator-free control region investigated in the VBM analyses. Blue: Locus Coeruleus (LC—“omni-comprehensive”)—Noradrenaline (NA)—(714 voxels). Red: Dorsal Raphe Nuclei (DR)—Serotonin (5HT)—(174 voxels). Orange: Median Raphe Nuclei (MR)—Serotonin (5HT)—(108 voxels). Green: Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA)—Dopamine (DA)—(252 voxels). Purple: Nucleus Basalis of Meynert (NBM)—Acethylcholine (Ach)—(492 voxels). White: Control Pontine ROI (“neuromodulator-free” region)—(906 voxels). All the ROI binary masks were symmetrical with a 1 mm3 isotropic voxel size, and there were no overlapping boundaries between the masks. Image (a) displays the anatomical localization of the six ROIs in sagittal view of the Cerebellum, Brainstem and Diencephalon. Image (b) displays the coronal view. Image (c) shows a fronto-lateral view of the whole brain and the anatomical localization of the six ROIs.

2.1.2. BrainPAD Measure Calculation

Brain Predicted Age Difference (BrainPAD) is a measure of how the brain is ageing, and it is obtained by calculating the discrepancy between the chronological age and the biological age of the brain defined on healthy brain ageing of typical people. Subjects with a younger brain than their chronological age have negative values, whereas if a subject is ageing faster than their chronological age the index is a positive value. BrainPAD is thought to reflect how well Grey Matter (GM) is maintained, hence it is proposed to be an index of brain maintenance. BrainPAD measure by Boyle et al. (2020) [12] was developed using several datasets. In the first instance they defined the normal GM ageing in healthy subjects. They then trained an algorithm to predict successfully the degree of GM deterioration in relation to the chronological age in further three populations of healthy subjects. The algorithm used in this study is described in detail in Boyle et al. (2020) [12].

2.1.3. Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) Analyses

VBM analyses were performed using T1 whole brain images (WM+GM), after being pre-processed and smoothed with a 2 mm3 FWHM kernel. Each set of analysis aimed to investigate five main research interests, based on Robertson’s theoretical framework [1,2]. Each analysis first considered the LC and was then repeated separately for the other brainstem nuclei and the CP ROI as a control procedure testing both positive and negative relations. The statistical thresholds were settled at p < 0.01, and later increased progressively until the results disappeared (namely: p < 0.001, p < 0.01 FWE, p < 0.001 FWE).

2.1.4. Relationship between LC Volume and Attention

The first question addressed by this study was to investigate whether the LC volume can be a predictor of attentional performances measured with TMT-A (visuo-motor speed processing). Three multiple regression models were run for each group. In the model, TMT-A was included as a continuous variable and TIV, education and age were entered as covariates. Then, based on previous literature and the main hypothesis, a negative relation between the LC volume and the TMT-A was also investigated, namely, a larger LC volume was expected to be related to faster attentional performances (fewer seconds spent in completing the task). The following contrast was used for negative relation: 0 0 0 0 −1. The contrast: 0 0 0 0 1 was used as well as control analyses testing the positive relation. A further step in the analyses was to indicate the LC mask as “inclusive mask” in order to isolate the LC involvement in the model. Similarly, all the other ROIs were tested in the same way.

2.1.5. Relationship between LC Volume and Biological Brain Maintenance

The negative relation between LC volume and BrainPAD was then tested across the three groups, with the hypothesis that greater LC volume would be associated with lower and negative BrainPAD scores, reflecting reduced brain aging relative to chronological age. As in the previous analyses, BrainPAD was treated as dependent continuous variable and the same covariates and contrasts were used, including chronological age as suggested by Le TT et al. (2018) [130]. The following contrast was used for negative relation: 0 0 0 0 −1. The contrast: 0 0 0 0 1 was used as well as control analyses testing the positive relation. A further step in the analyses was to indicate the LC mask as inclusive in order to isolate the LC involvement in the model. Similarly, all the other ROIs were tested in the same way.

2.2. Mediation Analyses

Mediation analyses with multiple parallel mediators were carried out in order to better clarify the possible role of the LC as key component in mediating reserve indices and attentional performances. The analyses considered the three groups separately and were performed using the toolbox PROCESS v3.4 and SPSS macro developed by Andrew F. Hayes (http://www.processmacro.org/ accessed on 15 December 2018) implemented in SPSS 25 (https://www.ibm.com/products/spss-statistics accessed on 15 December 2018). The toolbox PROCESS enables one to perform manifold types of mediation and moderation analyses. In SPSS, from the PROCESS interface it was selected the model number four with 95% confidence intervals and 5000 bootstrap samples. This model number can be used to perform mediation analyses with parallel multiple mediators and covariates. TMT- A time in seconds was used as the Y variable and BrainPAD scores as the X variable. The extracted average volume values of the six ROIs were considered as six parallel mediators. TIV and age were treated as covariates.

2.3. Bayes Factors Calculation

In JASP (https://jasp-stats.org/ accessed on 15 January 2019), Bayes Factors (BF) were calculated in order to better discriminate the differential involvement of the six ROIs in the two main domains investigated, visuo-motor speed processing (TMT-A) and biological brain maintenance (BrainPAD). The extracted average volumes of the six ROIs were tested in a Bayesian correlation model in order to establish the strength of the relationships between TMT-A and BrainPAD across the three groups. Further details are provided in the Supplementary Materials in Section S2.2.

2.4. Rationale for the Neuromodulatory Subcortical System ROI Selection

From dopamine, noradrenaline is synthetized subcortically in the brainstem, specifically in the LC, in the dorsal pontine tegmentum and in the lateral tegmental neurons [14]. However, LC is the main structure responsible for the NA production and also accounts for 90% of its cortical NA innervation. LC’s projections are vastly spread throughout the cortex and the cerebellum [15,91]. For these reasons, it has been defined as the main core structure of investigation of the noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve. Given that the analyses were designed to be performed also on clinical samples, control analyses were made to account for the possibility to detect false positive concerning the LC involvement in attention and reserve due the ongoing diffuse neurodegeneration of the samples. Therefore, other main neuromodulators and their main core nuclei were considered control regions to better assess the implication the LC–NA system in attention and reserve. The first control neuromodulator defined was the serotoninergic system because it is believed to broadly modulate markedly different processes than NA. Additionally, Serotonin is synthesised in the pons very closely to the LC but from a different precursor (tryptophan) [14]; thus, this anatomical area is particularly well suited for controlling purposes. The median and the dorsal raphe nuclei are the largest serotoninergic seeds producing and projecting serotonin to the cortex and the cerebellum via the basal forebrain [14,112]. The dopaminergic system was also examined, and so the ventral tegmental area was taken as a core area for the analyses. VTA is the main brain nucleus together with the substantia nigra (SN), where dopamine is synthesised from the amino acid tyrosine [14]. The VTA is responsible for the cortical irroration of dopamine while the SN projects subcortically. For this reason, VTA was defined as control region to control for the dopaminergic system. Regarding acetylcholine, the substantia innominata/nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM) was chosen over the tegmental cholinergic neurons because it has the largest number of cholinergic neurons and it projects diffusely to the entire cortex. More than 90% of the NBM includes cholinergic magnocellular neurons [14,127]. Finally, in order to control for a “neuromodulator-free” brainstem region, an ROI not referring to any anatomical nuclei was designed in the ventro-rostral portion of the pons.

3. Results

Descriptive statistics for key demographic, neural and neuropsychological variables are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic, neural indices and neuropsychological characteristics of the HC, MCI and AD groups. Key: TIV, total intracranial volume; BrainPAD, Brain predicted age discrepancy (biological maintenance index); TMT-A. trail making test part A time in seconds. (* p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001 Bonferroni correction).

Sociodemographic Neural Indices Neuropsychological
Gender Age *** Education *** Occupation * TIV BrainPAD *** TMT-A ***
Groups M F Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Ranges min max min max min max min max min max min max
HC (n = 395) 168 227 73.45 7.21 16.96 2.21 6.21 1.72 1408.16 146.28 1.59 7.69 30.64 8.85
56 95 11 20 2 8 1082 1844 −22.01 37.46 13 63
MCI (n = 156) 90 66 75.51 8.10 15.98 2.76 5.78 1.85 1426.12 142.78 5.45 9.00 39.07 17.86
55 97 8 20 2 8 1060 1884 −21.23 26.21 18 150
AD (n = 135) 74 61 76.61 8.43 15.64 2.54 5.93 1.84 1385.79 167.47 13.32 9.13 61.61 36.59
55 95 11 20 2 8 1046 1785 −6.46 42.33 19 150

3.1. Sociodemographic Characteristics and Indices of Reserve and Attention

Age significantly differed across groups, F (2.683) = 10.13, p < 0.001. Post hoc comparisons showed that the HC group was significantly younger than the MCI group (p = 0.014) and the AD group (p < 0.001). The MCI and AD groups did not significantly differ from each other in age (p = 0.68). Groups also differed in education, F (2.683) = 19.41, p < 0.001. The average years of education was higher for the HC group compared to the MCIs (p < 0.001) and the AD group (p < 0.001). The MCI and AD groups did not differ in mean years of education (p = 0.68). There was an effect of occupation, F (2.683) = 11.71, p = 0.025. The HC showed a higher occupational rank compared to MCI (p = 0.031). No other group comparisons for occupation were significant (all p > 0.3). A Chi-squared test showed that gender was not evenly distributed across the three groups (Χ2 = 12.35; df: 2; p = 0.002). As reported in Table 1, the HC contained significantly more females (57%) while in MCI and AD males were more highly represented (57.1% and 54.8%, respectively). There was no significant difference in TIV by group, F(2.683) = 2.62, p = 0.07. However, there was a significant difference in BrainPAD scores, F(2.683) = 101.2, p < 0.001. There was a systematic pattern in which AD patients showed greater BrainPAD scores (indicating an older brain relative to chronological age) compared to MCI patients (p < 0.0001), who in turn showed a greater mean BrainPAD score than the HC group (p < 0.0001). All groups significantly differed in time taken to complete the TMT- A, F (2.683) = 127, p < 0.001. AD patients exhibited the longest duration (mean seconds) compared to the MCI (p < 0.001), which showed a longer mean time-to-completion than the HC group (p < 0.001). More details are provided in the Supplementary Materials including the average volumetric and gender differences across the three groups for the six ROIs (reported in Tables S5 and S6).

3.2. 1st Branch of VBM Analyses: Multiple Regressions—TMT-A (Attention—Visuo-Motor Speed Processing)

Does the LC predict attention performance relative to other neuromodulator seed regions?

As can be observed in Table 2, for the statistical threshold of p < 0.001 only the three voxels of the NBM were significant in the HC. For the MCI group, the reduced volume of the LC (142 voxels p < 0.01 and 21 voxels for p < 0.001) was associated with longer TMT-A completion time. Similarly, in the AD, the strongest predictor of attention performance was the LC with nine (122 voxels for p < 0.01) voxels negatively associated with the attentional performance. As shown in Figure 3 the average LC results are localised within a region overlapping the LC core defined by previous atlases. Further neuromodulators’ seeds also contributing to the variance in TMT-A performance included 14 VTA voxels and 19 NBM voxels negatively associated with performance. For the three groups, these results did not hold when FWE correction was applied. As a control measure, the opposite relationships (positive associations with TMT-A) across the three groups for all six ROIs were assessed and revealed no significant results. (More details are provided in the Supplementary Materials Section S3.5).

Table 2.

VBM multivariate linear regression analyses for the six ROIs across the three groups (HC n = 395; MCI n = 156; AD n = 135). The results were adjusted for total intracranial volume (TIV), and age and education were entered as continuous variables. TMT-A time in seconds was treated as a continuous dependent variable and the structural scans as the independent variable. For a statistical threshold of p < 0.001, the table reports the number of significant voxels negatively related to TMT-A time in seconds.

Brain Regions Side MNI Coordinates Peak
T Value a
Peak
Z-Score b
Peak Cluster Ke c p-Value Uncorr d FEW e FDR f Total Number of Voxels
p < 0.001 Threshold g
x y Z
HC (n. 395)
Locus Coeruleus / / / / / / / / / / /
Dorsal Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Median Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Ventral Tegmental Area / / / / / / / / / / /
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert right 14 −4 −12 3.35 3.32 3 0.000 1.000 0.571 3
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /
MCI (n. 156)
Locus Coeruleus left −4 −40 −28 3.57 3.49 7 0.000 1.000 0.639 21
Dorsal Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Median Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Ventral Tegmental Area / / / / / / / / / / /
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert / / / / / / / / / / /
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /
AD (n. 135)
Locus Coeruleus right 6 −36 −20 4.41 4.25 8 0.000 0.992 0.463 9
Dorsal Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Median Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Ventral Tegmental Area left −4 −22 −16 3.78 3.68 13 0.000 1.000 0.618 14
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert right 14 −4 −12 3.83 3.72 19 0.000 1.000 0.583 19
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /

a Peak T value: T value of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; b Peak Z-score: Z-score of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; c Peak cluster Ke: number of voxels of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; d p Value Uncorrected; e FWE = family wise error correction value; f FDR = false discovery rate correction value (q); g Total number of voxels outcoming in the ROI including all clusters of contiguous voxels.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Results from the VBM multivariate linear regression analyses performed in CAT12 for the three groups (HC, MCI, AD). The results are covaried for total intracranial volume (TIV), age and education. The scatterplot displays the relationship between Locus Coeruleus (LC) volume (y axis) and TMT-A time in seconds (x axis) for the three groups: green (HC, n.395), orange (MCI, n.156), red (AD, n.135). On the x axis (TMT-A) are the seconds required to complete TMT-A. More seconds spent in completing TMT-A mirror a slower visuo-spatial cognitive processing related to the LC decline. The systematic decline of the LC volume across the three groups is related to a slower visuo-motor attentional performance. On the left portion of the figure, blue shows the average LC results for the three groups (n = 686, p < 0.001) on a 3D fronto-lateral view of the Brainstem and the Diencephalon. On the right portion of the figure are the 3D reconstructions (displayed in the MNI152 space) of the results in comparison with previously published LC atlases and masks. (a) average LC result; (b) average LC result is shown in the LC “omni-comprehensive” mask; (c) Keren et al. (2009) [102,107]; (d) Tona et al. (2017) [108]; (e) Betts et al. (2017) [109]; (f) Dahl et al. (2019) [33]; (g) Liu et al. (2019) [110]; (h) Rong Ye et al. (2020) [111]; (i) LC meta-mask by Dahl et al. (2021) [70]. The last column on the right shows the regions of the LC mask negatively related to TMT-A performances for the three groups considered separately (p < 0.01): HC n = 395 (no results), MCI n = 156 and AD n = 135.

3.3. 2nd Branch of VBM Analyses: Multiple Regressions—BrainPAD (Reserve—Brain Maintenance)

Does the LC predict brain maintenance relative to the other neuromodulator seed regions?

Table 3 shows a significant (p < 0.001 threshold FWE corrected) cluster of 153 LC voxels predicting BrainPAD score in the HC group, demonstrating that greater LC volume is associated with a lower or negative (i.e., younger) BrainPAD score. Similarly, 71 voxels of the DR were significant as well. MCI showed a similar trend, with statistical threshold of p < 0.001, 59 LC voxels and 109 DR voxels predicting BrainPAD score.

Table 3.

VBM multivariate linear regression analyses for the six ROIs across the three groups (HC n = 395; MCI n = 156; AD n = 135). The results were adjusted for total intracranial volume (TIV), while age and education were entered as continuous variables. BrainPAD values were treated as the continuous dependent variable and the structural scans as the independent variable. For a statistical threshold of p < 0.001 FWE corrected (HC) and p < 0.001 (MCI and AD), the table reports the significant voxels negatively related to BrainPAD.

Brain Regions Side MNI Coordinates Peak
T Value a
Peak
Zscore b
Peak Cluster Ke c p-Value Uncorr d FEW e FDR f Total Number of Voxels
p < 0.001 FWE Threshold g
x y Z
HC (n. 395)
Locus Coeruleus left −2 −38 −22 9.10 inf 106 0.000 0.000 0.000 153
Dorsal Raphe right 2 −32 −12 8.14 7.81 71 0.000 0.000 0.000 71
Median Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Ventral Tegmental Area / / / / / / / / / / /
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert / / / / / / / / / / /
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /
MCI (n. 156) total number of voxels
for p < 0.001 threshold g
Locus Coeruleus left −2 −34 −14 4.16 4.04 16 0.000 1.000 0.125 59
Dorsal Raphe right 2 −30 −8 5.78 5.48 109 0.000 0.021 0.563 109
Median Raphe / / / / / / / / / / /
Ventral Tegmental Area left −2 −16 −14 3.22 3.16 1 0.001 1.000 0.369 2
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert right 14 −8 −10 4.15 4.03 12 0.000 0.999 0.093 12
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /
AD (n. 135)
Locus Coeruleus left −4 −36 −16 5.56 5.25 94 0.000 0.065 0.005 192
Dorsal Raphe left −2 −32 −12 4.74 4.54 90 0.000 0.786 0.037 90
Median Raphe / 0 −34 −22 4.19 4.05 20 0.000 1.000 0.135 20
Ventral Tegmental Area left −4 −22 −16 4.49 4.32 9 0.000 0.976 0.067 9
Nucleus Basalis of Meynert right 14 −6 −12 3.41 3.33 5 0.000 1.000 0.655 5
Control Pontine ROI / / / / / / / / / / /

a Peak T value: T value of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; b Peak Z-score: Z-score of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; c Peak cluster Ke: number of voxels of the most significant cluster of contiguous voxels; d p Value Uncorrected; e FWE = family wise error correction value; f FDR = false discovery rate correction value (q); g Total number of voxels outcoming in the ROI including all clusters of contiguous voxels.

As shown in Figure 4, the average LC results are localised within a region overlapping the LC core defined by previous atlases. A lesser contribution of 2 VTA voxels and 12 NBM voxels also predicted BrainPAD score. In the AD, the most widespread effects were observed. All the ROIs, except the control pontine region, were found to negatively predict BrainPAD scores. At the p < 0.001 threshold, the most significant cluster was found in the LC (192 voxels). Two clusters of 90 and 20 voxels were observed in the DR and MR, respectively.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Results from the VBM multivariate linear regression analyses performed in CAT12 for the three groups (HC, MCI, AD). The results are covaried for total intracranial volume (TIV), age and education. The scatterplot displays the relationship between Locus Coeruleus (LC) volume (y axis) and BrainPAD (x axis) for the three groups: green (HC, n.395), orange (MCI, n.156), red (AD, n.135). On the x axis (BrainPAD) are the years of discrepancy between chronological age and the actual biological brain age based on the degree of Grey Matter deterioration. Negative values of BrainPAD mirror a better GM brain maintenance corresponding to a “younger brain age” than the chronological age. Positive values of BrainPAD mirror the accelerated brain’s GM deterioration, namely, positive values correspond to a worse biological maintenance, and to an older brain than the chronological age. The systematic decline of the LC volume across the three groups is related to a progressive accelerated GM deterioration. Indeed, HC values are shifted more towards the upper-left portion of the graph (negative values of BrainPAD and greater LC volume), whereas MCI and AD are more shifted towards the lower-right portion of the graph (positive values of BrainPAD and lower LC volume). On the left portion of the figure, blue shows the average LC results for the three groups (n = 686, p < 0.00001 FWE) on a 3D fronto-lateral view of the Brainstem and the Diencephalon. On the right portion of the figure are the 3D reconstructions (displayed in the MNI152 space) of the results in comparison with previously published LC atlases and masks. (a) average LC result; (b) average LC result is shown in the LC “omni-comprehensive” mask; (c) Keren et al. (2009) [102,107]; (d) Tona et al. (2017) [108]; (e) Betts et al. (2017) [109]; (f) Dahl et al. (2019) [33]; (g) Liu et al. (2019) [110]; (h) Rong Ye et al. (2020) [111]; (i) LC meta-mask by Dahl et al. (2021) [70]. The last column on the right shows the regions of the LC mask negatively related to BrainPAD values for the three groups considered separately: HC n = 395 (p < 0.001 FWE), MCI n = 156 (p < 0.001) and AD n = 135 (p < 0.001).

There were also two bilateral clusters in the VTA (nine voxels) and a cluster in the NBM (five voxels). When FWE correction was applied, no results survived for MCI and AD. When tested, the opposite relationships (positive associations with BrainPAD) across the three groups for all six ROIs showed no significant results. (more details are provided in the Supplementary Materials Section S3.6).

3.4. Mediation Analyses

Does the LC mediate the relationship between BrainPAD (X) and attention performance (Y)?

A multiple parallel mediation analysis was conducted for each of the three groups. Bootstrap confidence intervals were used to examine the role of the six subcortical nuclei in mediating the relationship between the BrainPAD score and attention performance, while controlling for age and TIV (see Figure 5—schematic mediation pathways for MCI group). In HC and AD, the total effect of X on Y was significant. The direct effects of X on Y were found not significant for the HC and significant for AD groups, indicating that better brain maintenance relative to chronological age was predictive of attention performance (HC total effect of X on Y: 0.1586; se: 0.0552; T:2.874, p = 0.0043; LLCI: 0.0501; ULCI: 0.2672; c_ps: 0.0179; c_cs: 0.1378) (HC direct effect of X on Y: 0.1225; se: 0.0877; T:1.3979, p = 1.630; LLCI: −0.0498; ULCI: 0.2949; c_ps: 0.0138; c_cs: 0.1065). (AD total effect of X on Y: 1.7088; se: 0.3918; T:4.3608, p < 0.0000; LLCI: 0.9336; ULCI: 2.4840; c_ps: 0.0467; c_cs: 0.4262) (AD direct effect of X on Y: 1.2410; se: 0.4658; T:2.6644, p = 0.0088; LLCI: 0.3189; ULCI: 2.1630; c_ps: 0.0339; c_cs: 0.3095). However, no indirect effects of the six mediators were apparent. On the other hand, in MCI, the LC alone was found to significantly mediate the relationship between BrainPAD (Y) and TMT-A (X) (indirect effect of X on Y: 0.0927; BootSE: 0.0499; BootLLCI: 0.0111; BootULCI: 0.2043). The total effect of X on Y was also significant (effect: 0.4224; se: 0.1583; t: 2.6678; p: 0.0085; LLCI: 0.1096; ULCI: 0.7352; c_ps: 0.0237; c_cs: 0.2130). Controlling for the mediation effect, the direct effect of X on Y was not significant (direct effect: 0.0383; se: 0.2496; t: 0.1535; p: 0.8782, LLCI: -0.4552; ULCI: 0.5318, c’_ps: 0.0021, c’_cs:0.0193), implying that the effect of BrainPAD on attention performance in MCI is mediated indirectly through the LC volume. This finding suggests that the way brain maintenance affects attention performance in MCI patients is disproportionately influenced by the noradrenergic system compared to other neuromodulatory systems.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Statistical diagram of the parallel mediation model (model n.4) testing the relation between BrainPAD (biological measure of brain maintenance) and TMT-A (attentional visuo-motor speed processing) in MCI group (n = 156). The average volumes of the six ROIs were treated as parallel mediators in order to test the noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve versus the other main neuromodulator’s seeds: serotoninergic (Dorsal and Median Raphe), dopaminergic (Ventral Tegmental Area), cholinergic (Nucleus Basalis of Meynert) and additionally with a brainstem control region neuromodulator-free (control pontine ROI). In the MCI group, the only significant mediator of the relation between brain maintenance and cognitive performance was the volume of the LC (Noradrenaline); all the other five ROIs were not significant in mediating the relationship. The model was covaried for age and total intracranial volume (TIV).

3.5. Bayes Factors: Parameters of Evidence Strength for the Six ROIs Involved in Attention and Brain Maintenance across the Three Groups

BF10 confirmed the disproportional predictive involvement of the LC–NA system observed in the VBM analyses. Bayesian modelling demonstrated that LC volume exhibited the strongest relationship with BrainPAD and TMT-A (with the one exception of the LC—TMT-A correlation in the HC group).

Overall, across the three groups, as indicated by BF10, the LC likelihood to predict brain maintenance was 19321.07 times more than the null hypothesis, whereas it was 779.29 for the other five ROIs when summed together. Similarly, the LC likelihood to predict attention was 4158.30 times more than the null hypothesis, while it was 241.35 for the sum of the other five ROIs. However, there were notable differences between the groups. Concerning BrainPAD, the MCI group compared to HC and AD showed the strongest evidence for LC (BF10 19303.214), followed by DR (BF10 399.634) and NBM (BF10 339.646). Furthermore, it is noteworthy that there is evidence of absent relationships (support for the null hypothesis) for all other ROIs in the MCI group (BF10 < 1 indicating more evidence for the null hypothesis (i.e., no relationship). Similarly, for TMT-A, the LC showed the strongest evidence (BF10 517.357), followed by the DR (BF10 4.219), and for all other ROIs, there were no relationships. In the AD group, the evidence for the LC was also the most substantial (BF10 46.538), but there was also evidence for more distributed involvement of other nuclei as the DR (BF10 7.031), the MR (BF10 2.541) and the VTA (BF10 4.657) supporting BrainPAD. In the same vein, for TMT-A, the strongest evidence was in support of the LC (BF10 3640.710), but strong evidence was also found for the VTA (BF10 124.076) and the NBM (BF10 102.490) but no evidence for the other ROIs. In the HC group, the magnitude of the results was less pronounced but followed the same pattern. The LC showed the strongest evidence supporting BrainPAD (BF10 142.324) followed by the DR (BF10 22.466), and no evidence was found for all the other ROIs. In contrast to the clinical groups, no evidence for the ROIs and TMT-A was found in the HC, with the exception of the DR (BF10 9.117). BF10 values are reported specifically for the three groups in Table 4 along with Pearson’s correlation coefficients. BF10 values were covaried for age, education and total intracranial volume.

Table 4.

The Bayes factors (BF01) for the three groups showing the strength of relationships between the six ROIs, BrainPAD and TMT-A. ROIs average values were corrected for age, education and total intracranial volume.

Locus Coeruleus Dorsal Raphe Median Raphe Ventral Tegmental Area Nucleus Basalis of Meynert Control Pontine ROI
HC (n = 395)
BrainPAD Pearson’s r −0.197 *** −0.292 *** −0.107 −0.074 −0.107 −0.109
BF10 142.324 22.466 0.608 0.185 0.599 0.643
TMT-A Pearson’s r −0.082 −0.158 −0.025 −0.045 0.085 −0.042
BF10 0.236 9.117 0.071 0.094 0.257 0.089
MCI (n = 156)
BrainPAD Pearson’s r −0.385 *** −0.321 *** −0.039 0.147 −0.318 *** 0.007
BF10 19303.214 399.634 0.113 0.523 339.646 0.101
TMT-A Pearson’s r −0.326 *** −0.219 0.001 0.019 0.029 0.007
BF10 517.357 4.219 0.100 0.103 0.107 0.101
AD (n = 135)
BrainPAD Pearson’s r −0.297 ** −0.249 −0.217 −0.305 −0.132 0.100
BF10 46.538 7.031 2.541 4.657 0.340 0.209
TMT-A Pearson’s r −0.384 *** −0.119 −0.041 −0.319 *** −0.315 *** −0.5224
BF10 3640.710 0.272 0.120 124.076 102.490 0.108

** BF10 > 30, *** BF10 > 100.

3.6. Brief Summary of the 3rd, 4th, 5th and 6th VBM Analyses

The other VBM branches of analyses are reported in Supplementary Materials (extended methods and results sections). A brief summary is provided here.

The third and the fourth branches of VBM analyses examined the extent to which education and the degree of occupational cognitive demand were predicted by the six ROIs. In summary, absent or negligible findings were observed for education (3rd VBM branch) and occupational cognitive demand (4th VBM branch). For further details, see Sections S2.3.12 and S2.3.13 and Section S3.9 with Tables S1 and S2 in Supplementary Materials.

The fifth branch of VBM analyses investigated how the factor group (HC, MCI, AD) affected the volumetric variations within the neuromodulatory subcortical systems. The main areas where the MCI showed decreased volume compared to HC were the DR and VTA. By contrast, there was no LC volume difference in MCI vs. HC group. AD patients showed decreased volume compared to MCI in the LC and in the VTA areas. Consistent with the previous literature [25,27,28,29,32,81,83,131,132,133], the main effect of group on volume reduction was observed in change in the NBM, the DR, the VTA and the LC (listed in order of statistical power). These areas showed the greater deterioration across the different stages of cognitive decline. For further details, see Sections S2.3.14 (5th VBM branch) and S3.10 with Tables S3 and S4 in Supplementary Materials along with Figure 6 for spatial resolution.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

The main effect of factor condition (HC n = 395, MCI n = 156, AD n = 135) on the six ROIs. The VBM results were thresholded for p < 0.05 FWE (corrected) and adjusted for total intracranial volume, age and education. The significant clusters of voxels are shown on a 3D reconstruction of the Cerebellum, the Brainstem and the Diencephalon (sagittal to coronal view from left to right). The color bar represents F value ranges and reflects the significance of volume reduction across the three groups. The main volume reduction occurred in the Nucleus Basalis of Meynert (NBM) 35 voxels F 33.89, in the Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA) 65 voxels F 23.79, in the Dorsal Raphe (DR) 132 voxels F 28.66 and in the Locus Coeruleus (LC) 64 voxels F 21,46. See Tables S3 and S4 in Supplementary Materials with Figure 2 for further details.

Since the literature concerning LC gender differences is controversial [25,34,56,117,134], the sixth branch of VBM analyses explored the differential volumetric variation of the neuromodulators due to gender at different stages of cognitive decline. Negligible differences between gender were observed. These differences became weaker or disappeared when covariates (age, TIV and education) were removed from the models. For more details, see Sections S2.3.15 (6th VBM branch) and S3.11 with Table S7 in Supplementary Materials.

4. Discussion

The present study conducted a volumetric analysis of subcortical nuclei in healthy older controls (HC, n = 395), patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI, n = 156) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD, n = 135). We hypothesized that structural integrity of the locus loeruleus (LC) would be particularly important for brain maintenance and cognitive reserve in the context of age and disease, due to the known neuroprotective effects of the LC–NA system [1,2]. As anticipated, we observed a systematic reduction in attention performance and biological brain maintenance (BrainPAD) across the HC, MCI and AD groups. We also observed a general reduction in the volume of subcortical nuclei at different stages of the cognitive decline. Compared to the other subcortical nuclei, LC volume was most extensively associated with the degree of brain maintenance across the three groups. However, only in MCI patients was the relationship between brain maintenance and attention performance mediated by LC volume, suggesting a unique compensatory role of noradrenergic neuromodulation for MCI patients. Although these findings suggest a significant role for the LC–NA system in brain maintenance, we found negligible or inconsistent associations between the subcortical ROI volumes, BrainPAD scores and indices of cognitive reserve (i.e., education level and occupational status). However, education level did show a positive association with overall brain volume (in the HC and MCI groups), suggesting it might be more closely associated with brain development and expansion rather than later life brain maintenance. Few studies have investigated the relationship between LC volume and attentional performance in older adults [34,49,68]. The present study utilised the Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) as a putative measure of visual attention speed and found that greater LC volume in MCI and AD patients was associated with faster attention performance. A ceiling effect of TMT-A performance in the HC group gave limited variability for detecting an association with LC volume. However, a pattern of reduced and more heterogeneous TMT-A performance in both MCI and AD group revealed relationships with brainstem nuclei that may counteract progressive cortical decline seen in these patients. Histopathological evidence shows that in the early stages of neurodegeneration, the metencephalic areas appear to be less affected than cortical areas [25,26,50,51,52,53,135,136]. It is possible that the degree of structural integrity within these metencephalic regions provides an important neuromodulatory and compensatory role in the face of declining cortical function [1,2,17,21,54,55,61]. Indeed, although the LC shows markers of neurodegeneration years before cognitive decline [26,50], neuronal cell loss in the LC is most appreciable in advanced stages of neurodegeneration rather than in the earlier stages (evidence reported from Braak stages III-IV) [25,26,50,56]. Therefore, throughout the pathological course, the volumetric and functional resilience of the LC (including the maintenance of the surrounding LC-peri dendritic and the epi and sub-coeruleus areas [14,104,105]) may provide a supporting and compensating role in the face of the beta-amyloid pathology occurring in the cortex [16,17,21,26,50,104,136]. Interestingly in this regard, a recent work by Bachman et al. [137] found positive relationships between the volume of the LC and surface measures in cortical regions including the rostral medial frontal cortex, which has a high density of noradrenergic varicosities [92].

In MCI patients, we found that the structural variation of the LC was unique among the six subcortical ROIs in predicting visual attention performance. Specifically, Bayesian analyses demonstrated that there was substantial evidence in support of LC involvement relative to all other ROIs. These analyses strengthened the specificity of the LC–NA system over the other neuromodulators’ seeds and outlines its remarkable role in brain health. Widespread transmission of NA from the LC via diverse cortical efferent projections has been shown to significantly affect attention performance [54,99,138,139,140,141,142]. In the context of MCI, these findings suggest that neuromodulation of the LC–NA system, in particular, may help support the diminishing efficiency of cortical attention networks in this group. In AD patients, several relationships were observed in which greater volumes of the LC, VTA and NBM were associated with faster visual attention performance. Due to the more severe cortical neurodegeneration in AD, it is conceivable that further compensatory support from both ascending catecholaminergic nuclei, including dopaminergic (VTA) and noradrenergic (LC) regions as well as activation of cholinergic basal forebrain nuclei (NBM), would be necessary to support declining cortical function [143]. Indeed, the basic requirements of the TMT-A are exploratory behaviour and selective attention [89,90,98], which are modulated by the catecholaminergic and cholinergic systems, respectively [1,2,14,59,143,144].

A seminal study by Wilson and colleagues [31] demonstrated that greater LC neural density was associated with reduced cognitive decline and increased reserve from an older adult cohort. In the present study, we found LC volume to be a reliable predictor of brain maintenance in each of the three groups. This pattern of results is consistent with Robertson’s noradrenergic theory of reserve [1,2], hypothesizing that protracted activation of the LC–NA system across the lifespan can enhance brain reserve through neuroplastic change, as well as through the neuroprotective effects of reduced neuroinflammation [23,40,41,42,145] and increased BDNF production [43,44,45,46,47,48].

Given the high sensitivity of predicted age discrepancy measures to brain deterioration [10,146,147,148], the universal relationship between BrainPAD and LC volume could indicate that the LC–NA system is a key driver of brain reserve, which is shaped through a more active and efficient attentional system [1,2]. Our findings are consistent with the evidence of post-mortem and neuroimaging studies relating greater LC volume to indices of reserve and to reduced neurodegeneration [18,19,20,21,31,145]. In addition to the LC volume, the observed relationship between DR volume and BrainPAD may also suggest a marked serotoninergic involvement in brain maintenance, particularly in MCI. Serotoninergic deterioration in neurodegenerative diseases has been reported [26,50,131,149,150,151], and degeneration of the DR is well documented [25,52,53,150,152]. However, DR is not only a serotoninergic seed, but a multifunctional nucleus with significant non-serotoninergic pathways [153,154]. Although 70% of the DR neurons contain serotonin, the remaining 30% include other neurotransmitters (such as catecholamine [155,156]) and NA transporters (NET) [157]. Early research [158] has reported that 38% of the amount of the noradrenaline found in the LC is found in the DR (LC: 1.22 mg/g tissue mean; DR: 0.47 mg/g tissue mean). Therefore, the relationship between BrainPAD and the DR volume must also be considered in the context of the degeneration of the LC–NA system, particularly given the absence of evidence for the MR and BrainPAD. The LC has been shown to exhibit early signs of neurodegeneration before the DR nucleus [25], and patients with dementia exhibit significantly reduced noradrenaline transporters (NET) in the LC but not in the DR [55]. Together these findings suggest that noradrenergic neurons within the DR might provide compensatory support for the broader LC–NA system [54,55,103,104]. Accordingly, the resilience of the DR as a compensatory nucleus to the LC–NA system decay may, in part, explain the observed relationships between DR volume and BrainPAD in the present study.

In the present study, we also compared the influence of each of the five neuromodulator seed ROIs in mediating the relationship between brain maintenance and attentional efficiency. In keeping with Robertson’s noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve, we hypothesized that greater LC volume would be a key mediator in this relationship. However, only in MCI patients did LC volume account for the association between BrainPAD and attentional performance, suggesting that at prodromal stages of neurodegeneration greater LC integrity in the context of declining cortical systems may provide critical support for attention processes. By contrast, healthy older adults may have sufficient brain maintenance without additional neuromodulatory support from the LC and, in AD patients, pronounced LC degeneration compared to MCI and HC [31,32,55,56,69] may undermine its compensatory role in later stages of neurodegeneration. Although the DR nucleus has significant noradrenergic expression, we did not find a direct relationship between DR volume and attention performance, nor did DR volume mediate the association between brain maintenance and attention performance. In the current study, putative indices of cognitive reserve, education and occupational status were strongly related to each other but not associated with BrainPAD scores, subcortical ROI volumes or attention performance. Although previous findings [159] have reported an association between higher education and younger brain age, more recent research has not yielded such a relationship [160]. Alternatively, it is plausible that factors such as education and occupation are more directly related to actual reserve built through developmental plasticity in the early stages of life, and indexed by total intracranial volume (TIV), and are therefore more indirectly related to brain maintenance in the later stages of life [8].

5. Limitations

TMT-A is a valuable clinical tool to assess higher cognitive functions and it is widely used for its sensitivity to basic attentive efficiency, but it is a limited measure for assessing overall noradrenergic contributions to cognition. The cortical influence of NA is global and complex, and a single measure lacks the necessary richness to capture this entirely. Indeed, it is acknowledged how the LC–NA system plays a crucial role in memory [33,68,69,70,131,145] and in other higher cognitive functions distinct from visuo-spatial attention [1,2,17,19,59]. Nevertheless, our findings do suggest a markedly predominant role for the LC in attention and brain maintenance, particularly given the absence of a relevant effect for other neuromodulatory control nuclei. The sample size of the HC group (n = 395) is considerably larger than both MCI (n = 156) and AD (n = 135); therefore, uncorrected results for BrainPAD in MCI and AD might be driven by the reduced size of these groups.

The retrospective broad nature of the measures of sociodemographic indices of reserve, particularly job complexity, may not offer information precise enough to capture the effects of this variable. The job classification in ranks according to the supposed cognitive demand can only reflect hypothetical contingencies related to the occupation.

In addition, despite that neuropsychological and volumetric measures showed clear and systematic differences across the groups (as expected based on the standard diagnostic criteria used by the ADNI), it should be considered that subsequent modifications to the diagnostic criteria may yield some miss-classification when compared to the initial baseline rollover of the ADNI3 phase [161,162,163], since MCI criteria for diagnosis are constantly being reviewed and scrutinized [164]. However, TMT-A performances of HC group are within the normative values for their age and education, suggesting that there is no evidence of behavioural impairment, at least based on normative scores for the TMT-A, suggesting no evidence of impairment in this group [165].

Lastly, the methodological limitations of VBM analyses in cross-sectional studies should be taken into account while considering these findings. However, within these limitations, the utmost rigour was employed in defining the subcortical regions, while acknowledging the comprehensive literature on the subject. The processing pipeline was also strict in terms of quality assurance: an attempt was made to account for all possible confounding covariates. Additionally, opposite hypotheses have been tested as well. Despite these limitations, the findings of the current study are consistent both with histopathological and neuroimaging studies on noradrenergic, serotoninergic, cholinergic and dopaminergic systems in the context of neurodegenerative diseases [25,27,28,29,32,81,83,131,132,133].

6. Conclusions and Clinical Implications

The findings of this study are consistent with the vast literature on the decay of the LC–NA system in neurodegenerative diseases, and with a growing number of studies showing how the LC–NA system is a crucial mediator of reserve both in healthy and pathological ageing, as postulated by Robertson. This work is among the largest MRI studies carried out on the noradrenergic system, counting more than 250 VBM analyses on 686 subjects. Additionally, it is the first work investigating the comparative relationships between an objective measure of biological brain health and the integrity of neuromodulatory subcortical systems. This work extends the knowledge of the role of the LC–NA system in the neurobiology of cognitive decline and also as potential in-vivo biomarker of neurodegenerative diseases.

The relationships identified in this study highlight the need to target therapeutic approaches, which focus on enhancing the function and the structural integrity of the LC–NA system. As proposed by Robertson, early prevention strategies which focus on upregulation of the noradrenergic system in ageing and dementia may yield important clinical benefits. This is possible through cognitive stimulation via attentional training (involving sustained attention and working memory capacity) [1,2,21]. Indeed, visual attention and working memory are domains underpinned by the noradrenergic system [1,2,21,59,91]. Cognitive interventions involving these domains performing exercises of mental flexibility, problem solving and visual search might stimulate the LC–NA system enhancing cognition [1,2]. Short cognitive interventions based on this approach showed cognitive improvements related to frontal areas with predominant presence of noradrenergic receptors [92,166].

In addition, noradrenergic drugs which increase LC activity and cognitive performances might be beneficial [57,167,168,169]. Studies using noradrenergic drugs showed that interventions ameliorated AD-like pathology and partially re-stored noradrenergic tone in humans [104,167,168]. However, the benefit of noradrenergic drugs is controversial, and side effects should be considered while evaluating this approach (see Holland et al. (2021) for a review) [59].

Another potential intervention might involve physical exercise, which is among the main preventing factors of dementia [13]. Physical exercise is known to help maintain proper upregulation of the LC–NA system. It has been shown that physical exercise boosts NA release [39] and activates LC–NA system, which is linked to improved cognition in healthy and MCI populations [21,58,170].

Diet may also play a supportive role within the noradrenergic system as a preventing factor of neurodegenerative diseases. Indeed, poor and wrong diet-styles and their related neuroinflammatory consequences [171,172,173] are associated with worse prognosis and higher neurodegeneration risks [13,171,172,174,175]. Interventions on diet may be beneficial to restore potential harmful micro and macro nutrient insufficiencies according to current guidelines for prevention [13,176]. In particular, as observed in regards of the cholinergic system decay with dementia, a higher level of choline intake and supplementation has been shown to improve cognitive performances in both healthy people and patients [177,178,179]. Likewise, tyrosine intake and supplementation in healthy elderly and in MCI patients offer another possible conceivable way of ameliorating the LC–NA system degeneration and cognitive decline [51,55,180,181,182,183,184]. It is worth mentioning that several studies in the past linked tyrosine supplementation to increased cognitive performances in healthy subjects [185,186,187,188,189]. Furthermore, a recent study by Kühn et al. (2019) [180] found in 1724 healthy individuals that the average dietary tyrosine intake in grams related to better cognitive performances in working memory, episodic memory and fluid intelligence.

Finally, other potential interventions may involve breathing practices such as meditation, pranayama and breath-control, which are thought to involve the LC–NA system [190,191]. Some studies on long-term meditators have indeed reported increased brain volume in critical areas affected in dementia, including the brainstem, along with better cognitive efficiency [165,192,193,194,195], after as a little as 5–10 h of practice [196,197].

As an ultimate conclusion, it should be considered that the current study did not directly investigate the upregulation of the LC–NA system but explored cross-sectionally the volumetric integrity of the LC–NA system in comparison with the other main neuromodulator seeds. These investigations highlight the relevance of the LC–NA system in attentional domain and in the biological component of reserve, linking brain health to greater integrity of the LC–NA system. Although greater LC volume may ensure greater availability of NA reported in multiple studies [1,2,17,18,20,21,22,25,26,28,30,33,36,47,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61], it is important to acknowledge that further integration of the relationship between structure and function of the LC is warranted.

Acknowledgments

Thanks are extended to Francesca Fabbricatore for proofreading the manuscript and for the crucial suggestions. Data collection and sharing for this project were funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/cells10071829/s1, Figure S1. Comparisons between the previously published Locus Coeruleus probabilistic atlases and the Locus Coeruleus mask created for the current study. The new LC mask was manually drawn merging the regions of MNI space defined by the atlases. The new mask was corrected in order to not encroach other pontine structures (Raphe Nuclei and cerebellar WM). Table S1. Reports the VBM results predicting the Education score for each of the 6 ROI across the 3 groups (HC, MCI, AD). The results from the multiple regression models are covaried for age, education and total intracranial volume. The table shows the significant voxels for a statistical threshold of p < 0.01; Table S2. Reports the VBM results predicting the Occupation score for each of the 6 ROI across the 3 groups (HC, MCI, AD). The results from the multiple regression models are covaried for age, education and total intracranial volume. The table shows the significant voxels for a statistical threshold of p < 0.01; Table S3. VBM full-factorial statistical model investigating the volume reduction of the 6 ROIs across the factor “condition” (HC, MCI, AD). In the table are shown the ROIs of reduced volume due the main effect of condition for a statistical threshold of p < 0.001. The results are adjusted for total intracranial volume (TIV), age and education. In bold are highlighted the clusters survived when p < 0.05 FWE was applied; Table S4. VBM full-factorial statistical model investigating the volume reduction of the six ROIs across the factor “condition” (HC, MCI, AD). The table shows the regions of significant reduced volume in MCI compared with HC for a statistical threshold of p < 0.001. The lower portion shows the regions of significant reduced volume in AD compared with MCI. The results are adjusted for total intracranial volume (TIV), age and education; Table S5. Shows the average volumes of the six ROIs for the three groups (HC, MCI, AD). The average values were extracted from the whole brain images (GM+WM) smoothed 2 mm3 FWHM kernel. More precise analyses are provided by the VBM analyses; Table S6. Shows the average volumes of the six ROIs for the three groups (HC, MCI, AD) divided by biological gender (1 males, 2 females). The average values were extracted from the whole brain images (GM+WM) smoothed 2 mm3 FWHM kernel. More precise analyses are provided by the VBM analyses on biological gender; Table S7. Results from VBM T-Test comparisons within the three groups (HC, MCI, AD). The table shows the number of significant voxels of difference between the two sub-groups (male vs. female) across the three groups. The left column (M more than F) shows the number of voxels males have more than the females. The right column shows the number of voxels females have more than males. The results are covaried for Age, Education and Total Intracranial Volume for 2 different statistical thresholds p < 0.01 and p < 0.001. All the results did not survive multiple comparison correction. When the same analyses were repeated without covariates the results became weaker or disappeared. This suggests that using the above-mentioned covariates is sufficient to account for gender differences.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.R.G.P.; methodology, E.R.G.P., P.M.D., I.H.R.; formal analysis, E.R.G.P.; data curation, E.R.G.P., R.B., G.R., F.S.; writing—original draft preparation, E.R.G.P.; writing—review and editing, E.R.G.P., P.M.D., M.C.M., F.S., R.B.; visualization, E.R.G.P.; supervision, P.M.D., I.H.R., E.O., M.C.M., R.W., J.K.; project administration, E.R.G.P.; funding acquisition, P.M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Project founded by the Irish Research Council—Irish Research Council Laureate Consolidator Award (2018-23) IRCLA/2017/306 to Paul Dockree.

Data Availability Statement

* Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu accessed on 15 January 2021). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf (accessed on 15 January 2021).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Robertson I.H. A noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve: Implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2013;34:298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Robertson I.H. A right hemisphere role in cognitive reserve. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35:1375–1385. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tucker A.M. Cognitive Reserve in Aging. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2011;8:354–360. doi: 10.2174/156720511795745320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Valenzuela M.J., Sachdev P. Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic review. Psychol. Med. 2005;36:441–454. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11:1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Stern Y., Urquiljo E.M.A., Bartrés-Faz D., Belleville S., Cantillon M., Chetelat G., Ewers M., Franzmeier N., Kempermann G., Kremen W.S., et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2020;16:1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cabeza R., Albert M., Belleville S., Craik F.I.M., Duarte A., Grady C.L., Lindenberger U., Nyberg L., Park D.C., Reuter-Lorenz P.A., et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018;19:772. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Barulli D., Stern Y. Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gaser C., Franke K., Klöppel S., Koutsouleris N., Sauer H., Initiative A.D.N. BrainAGE in Mild Cognitive Impaired Patients: Predicting the Conversion to Alzheimer’s Disease. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e67346. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Habeck C., Razlighi Q., Gazes Y., Barulli D., Steffener J., Stern Y. Cognitive Reserve and Brain Maintenance: Orthogonal Concepts in Theory and Practice. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;27:3962–3969. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Boyle R., Jollans L., Rueda-Delgado L.M., Rizzo R., Yener G.G., McMorrow J.P., Knight S.P., Carey D., Robertson I.H., Emek-Savaş D.D., et al. Brain-predicted age difference score is related to specific cognitive functions: A multi-site replication analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021;15:327–345. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00260-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Livingston G., Sommerlad A., Orgeta V., Costafreda S.G., Huntley J., Ames D., Ballard C., Banerjee S., Burns A., Cohen-Mansfield J., et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390:2673–2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mai J.K., Paxinos G. The Human Nervous System. 3rd ed. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chandler D.J., Jensen P., McCall J.G., Pickering A.E., Schwarz L.A., Totah N.K. Redefining Noradrenergic Neuromodulation of Behavior: Impacts of a Modular Locus Coeruleus Architecture. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:8239–8249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1164-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Borodovitsyna O., Flamini M., Chandler D. Noradrenergic Modulation of Cognition in Health and Disease. Neural Plast. 2017;2017:6031478. doi: 10.1155/2017/6031478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sara S.J. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:211–223. doi: 10.1038/nrn2573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brunnström H., Friberg N., Lindberg E., Englund E. Differential degeneration of the locus coeruleus in dementia subtypes. Clin. Neuropathol. 2011;30:104–110. doi: 10.5414/NPP30104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Benarroch E.E. The locus ceruleus norepinephrine system: Functional organization and potential clinical significance. Neurologhy. 2009;73:1699–1704. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c2937c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kelly S., He B., Perez S.E., Ginsberg S.D., Mufson E.J., Counts S.E. Locus coeruleus cellular and molecular pathology during the progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. Commun. 2017;5:1–14. doi: 10.1186/s40478-017-0411-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mather M., Harley C.W. The Locus Coeruleus: Essential for Maintaining Cognitive Function and the Aging Brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:214–226. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Olivieri P., Lagarde J., Lehericy S., Valabrègue R., Michel A., Macé P., Caillé F., Gervais P., Bottlaender M., Sarazin M. Early alteration of the locus coeruleus in phenotypic variants of Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 2019;6:1345–1351. doi: 10.1002/acn3.50818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mravec B., Lejavova K., Cubinkova V. Locus (coeruleus) minoris resistentiae in pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2014;11:992–1001. doi: 10.2174/1567205011666141107130505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grudzien A., Shaw P., Weintraub S., Bigio E., Mash D.C., Mesulam M.M. Locus coeruleus neurofibrillary degeneration in aging, mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2007;28:327–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ehrenberg A.J., Nguy A.K., Theofilas P., Dunlop S., Suemoto C.K., Alho A.T.D.L., Leite R.P., Rodriguez R.D., Mejia M.B., Rüb U., et al. Quantifying the accretion of hyperphosphorylated tau in the locus coeruleus and dorsal raphe nucleus: The pathological building blocks of early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2017;43:393–408. doi: 10.1111/nan.12387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Braak H., Thal D.R., Ghebremedhin E., Del Tredici K. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: Age categories from 1 to 100 years. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2011;70:960–969. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Giorgi F.S., Ryskalin L., Ruffoli R., Biagioni F., Limanaqi F., Ferrucci M., Busceti C.L., Bonuccelli U., Fornai F. The Neuroanatomy of the Reticular Nucleus Locus Coeruleus in Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Neuroanat. 2017;11:80. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Satoh A., Iijima K.M. Roles of tau pathology in the locus coeruleus (LC) in age-associated pathophysiology and Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis: Potential strategies to protect the LC against aging. Brain Res. 2019;1702:17–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pamphlett R., Jew S.K. Different Populations of Human Locus Ceruleus Neurons Contain Heavy Metals or Hyperphosphorylated Tau: Implications for Amyloid-β and Tau Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015;45:437–447. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Betts M.J., Kirilina E., Otaduy M.C.G., Ivanov D., Acosta-Cabronero J., Callaghan M.F., Lambert C., Cardenas-Blanco A., Pine K., Passamonti L., et al. Locus coeruleus imaging as a biomarker for noradrenergic dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain. 2019;142:2558–2571. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wilson R.S., Nag S., Boyle P.A., Hizel L., Yu L., Buchman A.S., Schneider J.A., Bennett D.A. Neural reserve, neuronal density in the locus ceruleus, and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2013;80:1202–1208. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182897103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bondareff W., Mountjoy C.Q., Roth M. Loss of neurons of origin of the adrenergic projection to cerebral cortex (nucleus locus ceruleus) in senile dementia. Neurology. 1982;32:164. doi: 10.1212/WNL.32.2.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dahl M.J., Mather M., Düzel S., Bodammer N.C., Lindenberger U., Kühn S., Werkle-Bergner M. Rostral locus coeruleus integrity is associated with better memory performance in older adults. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3:1203–1214. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0715-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Liu K.Y., Marijatta F., Hämmerer D., Acosta-Cabronero J., Düzel E., Howard R.J. Magnetic resonance imaging of the human locus coeruleus: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;83:325–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Heneka M.T., Nadrigny F., Regen T., Martinez-Hernandez A., Dumitrescu-Ozimek L., Terwel D., Jardanhazi-Kurutz D., Walter J., Kirchhoff F., Hanisch U.-K., et al. Locus ceruleus controls Alzheimer’s disease pathology by modulating microglial functions through norepinephrine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:6058–6063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909586107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chalermpalanupap T., Schroeder J.P., Rorabaugh J.M., Liles L.C., Lah J.J., Levey A.I., Weinshenker D. Locus Coeruleus Ablation Exacerbates Cognitive Deficits, Neuropathology, and Lethality in P301S Tau Transgenic Mice. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:74–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1483-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Duffy K.B., Ray B., Lahiri D.K., Tilmont E.M., Tinkler G.P., Herbert R.L., Greig N.H., Ingram D.K., Ottinger M.A., Mattison J.A. Effects of Reducing Norepinephrine Levels via DSP4 Treatment on Amyloid-β Pathology in Female Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2019;68:115–126. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Heneka M.T., Ramanathan M., Jacobs A.H., Dumitrescu-Ozimek L., Bilkei-Gorzo A., Debeir T., Sastre M., Galldiks N., Zimmer A., Hoehn M., et al. Locus Ceruleus Degeneration Promotes Alzheimer Pathogenesis in Amyloid Precursor Protein 23 Transgenic Mice. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:1343–1354. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4236-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kjær M., Secher N., Galbo H. Physical stress and catecholamine release. Baillieres Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1987;1:279–298. doi: 10.1016/S0950-351X(87)80064-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Heneka M.T., Carson M.J., El Khoury J., Landreth G.E., Brosseron F., Feinstein D.L., Jacobs A.H., Wyss-Coray T., Vitorica J., Ransohoff R.M., et al. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:388–405. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)70016-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Troadec J.D., Marien M., Darios F., Hartmann A., Ruberg M., Colpaert F., Michel P.P. Noradrenaline provides long-term protec-tion to dopaminergic neurons by reducing oxidative stress. J. Neurochem. 2001;79:200–210. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Feinstein D.L., Heneka M.T., Gavrilyuk V., Russo C.D., Weinberg G., Galea E. Noradrenergic regulation of inflammatory gene expression in brain. Neurochem. Int. 2002;41:357–365. doi: 10.1016/S0197-0186(02)00049-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Giorgi F.S., Saccaro L.F., Galgani A., Busceti C.L., Biagioni F., Frati A., Fornai F. The role of Locus Coeruleus in neuroinflammation occurring in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. Bull. 2019;153:47–58. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2019.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Traver S., Salthun-Lassalle B., Marien M., Hirsch E., Colpaert F., Michel P.P. The Neurotransmitter Noradrenaline Rescues Septal Cholinergic Neurons in Culture from Degeneration Caused by Low-Level Oxidative Stress. Mol. Pharmacol. 2005;67:1882–1891. doi: 10.1124/mol.104.007864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Counts S.E., Mufson E.J. Noradrenaline activation of neurotrophic pathways protects against neuronal amyloid toxicity. J. Neurochem. 2010;113:649–660. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06622.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mannari C., Origlia N., Scatena A., Del Debbio A., Catena M., Dell’Agnello G., Barraco A., Giovannini L., Dell’Osso L., Domenici L., et al. BDNF Level in the Rat Prefrontal Cortex Increases Following Chronic but Not Acute Treatment with Duloxetine, a Dual Acting Inhibitor of Noradrenaline and Serotonin Re-uptake. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2008;28:457–468. doi: 10.1007/s10571-007-9254-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Aghajanov M., Chavushyan V., Matinyan S., Danielyan M., Yenkoyan K. Alzheimer’s disease-like pathology-triggered oxidative stress, alterations in monoamines levels, and structural damage of locus coeruleus neurons are partially recovered by a mix of proteoglycans of embryonic genesis. Neurochem. Int. 2019;131:104531. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2019.104531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hassani O.K., Rymar V.V., Nguyen K.Q., Huo L., Cloutier J.-F., Miller F.D., Sadikot A.F. The noradrenergic system is necessary for survival of vulnerable midbrain dopaminergic neurons: Implications for development and Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2020;85:22–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Clewett D.V., Lee T.-H., Greening S., Ponzio A., Margalit E., Mather M. Neuromelanin marks the spot: Identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;37:117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Del Tredici K., Braak H. To stage, or not to stage. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2020;61:10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Andrés-Benito P., Fernández-Dueñas V., Carmona M., Escobar L.A., Torrejón-Escribano B., Aso E., Ciruela F., Ferrer I. Locus coeruleus at asymptomatic early and middle Braak stages of neurofibrillary tangle pathology. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2017;43:373–392. doi: 10.1111/nan.12386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Simic G., Stanic G., Mladinov M., Jovanov-Milosevic N., Kostovic I., Hof P.R. Does Alzheimer’s disease begin in the brainstem? Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2009;35:532–554. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2990.2009.01038.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Simić G., Babić Leko M., Wray S., Harrington C.R., Delalle I., Jovanov-Milošević N., Bažadona D., Buée L., De Silva R., Di Giovanni G., et al. Monoaminergic neuropathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 2017;151:101–138. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2016.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Szot P., White S., Greenup J., Leverenz J., Peskind E., Raskind M. Changes in adrenoreceptors in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with dementia: Evidence of compensatory changes. Neuroscience. 2007;146:471–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Szot P., White S.S., Greenup J.L., Leverenz J., Peskind E.R., Raskind M.A. Compensatory Changes in the Noradrenergic Nervous System in the Locus Ceruleus and Hippocampus of Postmortem Subjects with Alzheimer’s Disease and Dementia with Lewy Bodies. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:467–478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4265-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Theofilas P., Ehrenberg A., Dunlop S., Alho A.T.D.L., Nguy A., Leite R.E.P., Rodriguez R.D., Mejia M.B., Suemoto C.K., Ferretti-Rebustini R.E.D.L., et al. Locus coeruleus volume and cell population changes during Alzheimer’s disease progression: A stereological study in human postmortem brains with potential implication for early-stage biomarker discovery. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2017;13:236–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.06.2362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Minzenberg M.J., Watrous A.J., Yoon J.H., Ursu S., Carter C.S. Modafinil Shifts Human Locus Coeruleus to Low-Tonic, High-Phasic Activity During Functional MRI. Science. 2008;322:1700–1702. doi: 10.1126/science.1164908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mather M., Huang R., Clewett D., Nielsen S.E., Velasco R., Tu K.P., Han S., Kennedy B. Isometric exercise facilitates attention to salient events in women via the noradrenergic system. NeuroImage. 2020;210:116560. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Holland N., Robbins T.W., Rowe J.B. The role of noradrenaline in cognition and cognitive disorders. Brain. 2021 doi: 10.1093/brain/awab111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Klimek V., Stockmeier C., Overholser J., Meltzer H.Y., Kalka S., Dilley G., Ordway G.A. Reduced Levels of Norepinephrine Transporters in the Locus Coeruleus in Major Depression. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:8451–8458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08451.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Hoogendijk W.J., Feenstra M.G., Botterblom M.H., Gilhuis J., Sommer I.E., Kamphorst W., Eikelenboom P., Swaab D.F. Increased activity of surviving locus ceruleus neurons in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1999;45:82–91. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199901)45:1&#x0003c;82::AID-ART14&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mueller S.G., Weiner M.W., Thal L.J., Petersen R.C., Jack C., Jagust W., Trojanowski J.Q., Toga A.W., Beckett L. The Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative. Neuroimaging Clin. N. Am. 2005;15:869–877. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2005.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mueller S.G., Weiner M.W., Thal L.J., Petersen R.C., Jack C.R., Jagust W., Trojanowski J.Q., Toga A.W., Beckett L. Ways toward an early diagnosis in Alzheimer’s disease: The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) Alzheimer’s Dement. 2005;1:55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ji X., Wang H., Zhu M., He Y., Zhang H., Chen X., Gao W., Fu Y., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Brainstem atrophy in the early stage of Alzheimer’s disease: A voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021;15:49–59. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00231-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lee J.H., Ryan J., Andreescu C., Aizenstein H., Lim H.K. Brainstem morphological changes in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport. 2015;26:411–415. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chen Z., Chen X., Liu M., Liu S., Ma L., Yu S. Volume gain of periaqueductal gray in medication-overuse headache. J. Headache Pain. 2017;18:12. doi: 10.1186/s10194-016-0715-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Protopopescu X., Pan H., Tuescher O., Cloitre M., Goldstein M., Engelien A., Yang Y., Gorman J., LeDoux J., Stern E., et al. Increased brainstem volume in panic disorder: A voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroreport. 2006;17:361–363. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000203354.80438.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Dutt S., Li Y., Mather M., Nation D.A. Brainstem substructures and cognition in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s11682-021-00459-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dutt S., Li Y., Mather M., Nation D.A., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Brainstem Volumetric Integrity in Preclin-ical and Prodromal Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2020;77:1579–1594. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dahl M.J., Mather M., Werkle-Bergner M., Kennedy B.L., Guzman S., Hurth K., Miller C.A., Qiao Y., Shi Y., Chui H.C., et al. Locus coeruleus integrity is related to tau burden and memory loss in autosomal-dominant Alzheimer’s disease. medRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.11.16.20232561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Takahashi J., Shibata T., Sasaki M., Kudo M., Yanezawa H., Obara S., Kudo K., Ito K., Yamashita F., Terayama Y. Detection of changes in the locus coeruleus in patients with mild cognitive impairment and A lzheimer’s disease: High-resolution fast spin-echo T 1-weighted imaging. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2015;15:334–340. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ayzenberg I., Nastos I., Strassburger-Krogias K., Obermann M., Gold R., Krogias C. Hypoechogenicity of brainstem raphe nuclei is associated with increased attack frequency in episodic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2016;36:800–806. doi: 10.1177/0333102415617415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Li Z., Zhou J., Lan L., Cheng S., Sun R., Gong Q., Wintermark M., Zeng F., Liang F. Concurrent brain structural and functional alterations in patients with migraine without aura: An fMRI study. J. Headache Pain. 2020;21:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s10194-020-01203-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Supprian T., Reiche W., Schmitz B., Grunwald I., Backens M., Hofmann E., Georg T., Falkai P., Reith W. MRI of the brainstem in patients with major depression, bipolar affective disorder and normal controls. Psychiatry Res. 2004;131:269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lee H.-Y., Tae W.S., Yoon H.-K., Lee B.-T., Paik J.-W., Son K.-R., Oh Y.-W., Lee M.-S., Ham B.-J. Demonstration of decreased gray matter concentration in the midbrain encompassing the dorsal raphe nucleus and the limbic subcortical regions in major depressive disorder: An optimized voxel-based morphometry study. J. Affect. Disord. 2011;133:128–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.De Marco M., Venneri A. Volume and Connectivity of the Ventral Tegmental Area are Linked to Neurocognitive Signatures of Alzheimer’s Disease in Humans. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018;63:167–180. doi: 10.3233/JAD-171018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Khan A.R., Hiebert N.M., Vo A., Wang B.T., Owen A.M., Seergobin K.N., Macdonald P.A. Biomarkers of Parkinson’s disease: Striatal sub-regional structural morphometry and diffusion MRI. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;21:101597. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Liu J., Chen L., Chen X., Hu K., Tu Y., Lin M., Huang J., Liu W., Wu J., Qiu Z., et al. Modulatory effects of different exercise modalities on the functional connectivity of the periaqueductal grey and ventral tegmental area in patients with knee osteoarthritis: A randomised multimodal magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Anaesth. 2019;123:506–518. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2019.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Chen Z.-Y., Chen X.-Y., Liu M.-Q., Ma L., Yu S.-Y. Volume Gain of Brainstem on Medication-Overuse Headache Using Voxel-Based Morphometry. Chin. Med. J. 2018;131:2158–2163. doi: 10.4103/0366-6999.240807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Schmitz T.W., Initiative T.A.D.N., Spreng R.N. Basal forebrain degeneration precedes and predicts the cortical spread of Alzheimer’s pathology. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13249. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.George S., Mufson E., Leurgans S., Shah R., Ferrari C., Detoledo-Morrell L. MRI-based volumetric measurement of the substantia innominata in amnestic MCI and mild AD. Neurobiol. Aging. 2011;32:1756–1764. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Schulz J., Pagano G., Bonfante J.A.F., Wilson H., Politis M. Nucleus basalis of Meynert degeneration precedes and predicts cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2018;141:1501–1516. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Jethwa K., Dhillon P., Meng D., Auer D., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Are Linear Measurements of the Nucleus Basalis of Meynert Suitable as a Diagnostic Biomarker in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer Disease? Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2019;40:2039–2044. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A6313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Colloby S.J., Elder G.J., Rabee R., O’Brien J.T., Taylor J. Structural grey matter changes in the substantia innominata in Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies: A DARTEL-VBM study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2016;32:615–623. doi: 10.1002/gps.4500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ray N.J., Bradburn S., Murgatroyd C., Toseeb U., Mir P., Kountouriotis G.K., Teipel S.J., Grothe M.J. In vivo cholinergic basal forebrain atrophy predicts cognitive decline in de novo Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2018;141:165–176. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Cantero J.L., Zaborszky L., Atienza M. Volume Loss of the Nucleus Basalis of Meynert is Associated with Atrophy of Innervated Regions in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;27:3881–3889. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Muth K., Schönmeyer R., Matura S., Haenschel C., Schröder J., Pantel J. Mild Cognitive Impairment in the Elderly is Associated with Volume Loss of the Cholinergic Basal Forebrain Region. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67:588–591. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Van Hooren R.W., Verhey F.R., Ramakers I.H., Jansen W.J., Jacobs H.I. Elevated norepinephrine metabolism is linked to cortical thickness in the context of Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Neurobiol. Aging. 2021;102:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Tombaugh T.N. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Gaudino E.A., Geisler M.W., Squires N.K. Construct validity in the trail making test: What makes part B harder? J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1995;17:529–535. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Aston-Jones G., Waterhouse B. Locus coeruleus: From global projection system to adaptive regulation of behavior. Brain Res. 2016;1645:75–78. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Palomero-Gallagher N., Amunts K., Zilles K. Transmitter Receptor Distribution in the Human Brain. Brain Mapp. 2015:261–275. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Talwar N., Churchill N.W., Hird M.A., Tam F., Graham S.J., Schweizer T.A. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of the trail-making test in older adults. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0232469. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Varjacic A., Mantini D., Demeyere N., Gillebert C.R. Neural signatures of Trail Making Test performance: Evidence from lesion-mapping and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. 2018;115:78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Shindo A., Terada S., Sato S., Ikeda C., Nagao S., Oshima E., Yokota O., Uchitomi Y. Trail Making Test Part A and Brain Perfusion Imaging in Mild Alzheimer’s Disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. Extra. 2013;3:202–211. doi: 10.1159/000350806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Zakzanis K.K., Mraz R., Graham S.J. An fMRI study of the Trail Making Test. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1878–1886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Lezak M.D., Howieson D.B., Loring D.W., Hannay H.J., Fischer J.S. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. Oxford University Press; New York, NY, USA: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Bowie C.R., Harvey P.D. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:2277–2281. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Gannon M., Wang Q. Complex noradrenergic dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: Low norepinephrine input is not always to blame. Brain Res. 2019;1702:12–16. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Kang S.S., Liu X., Ahn E.H., Xiang J., Manfredsson F.P., Yang X., Luo H.R., Liles L.C., Weinshenker D., Ye K. Norepinephrine metabolite DOPEGAL activates AEP and pathological Tau aggregation in locus coeruleus. J. Clin. Investig. 2019;130:422–437. doi: 10.1172/JCI130513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Petersen R.C., Aisen P.S., Beckett L.A., Donohue M., Gamst A.C., Harvey D.J., Jack C.R., Jagust W.J., Shaw L.M., Toga A.W., et al. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): Clinical characterization. Neurology. 2009;74:201–209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Keren N.I., Lozar C.T., Harris K., Morgan P., Eckert M.A. In vivo mapping of the human locus coeruleus. NeuroImage. 2009;47:1261–1267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Szot P., Leverenz J.B., Peskind E.R., Kiyasu E., Rohde K., Miller M.A., Raskind M.A. Tyrosine hydroxylase and norepinephrine transporter mRNA expression in the locus coeruleus in Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Brain Res. 2000;84:135–140. doi: 10.1016/S0169-328X(00)00168-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Matchett B.J., Grinberg L.T., Theofilas P., Murray M.E. The mechanistic link between selective vulnerability of the locus coeruleus and neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 2021;141:631–650. doi: 10.1007/s00401-020-02248-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Ishimatsu M., Williams J.T. Synchronous Activity in Locus Coeruleus Results from Dendritic Interactions in Pericoerulear Regions. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:5196–5204. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05196.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Janitzky K. Impaired Phasic Discharge of Locus Coeruleus Neurons Based on Persistent High Tonic Discharge—A New Hypothesis with Potential Implications for Neurodegenerative Diseases. Front. Neurol. 2020;11 doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Keren N.I., Taheri S., Vazey E.M., Morgan P., Granholm A.-C.E., Aston-Jones G.S., Eckert M.A. Histologic validation of locus coeruleus MRI contrast in post-mortem tissue. NeuroImage. 2015;113:235–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Tona K.-D., Keuken M., De Rover M., Lakke E., Forstmann B.U., Nieuwenhuis S., Van Osch M.J.P. In vivo visualization of the locus coeruleus in humans: Quantifying the test–retest reliability. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017;222:4203–4217. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1464-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Betts M.J., Cardenas-Blanco A., Kanowski M., Jessen F., Düzel E. In vivo MRI assessment of the human locus coeruleus along its rostrocaudal extent in young and older adults. NeuroImage. 2017;163:150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Liu K.Y., Acosta-Cabronero J., Cardenas-Blanco A., Loane C., Berry A., Betts M., Kievit R.A., Henson R., Düzel E., Howard R., et al. In vivo visualization of age-related differences in the locus coeruleus. Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;74:101–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Rong Y., Rua C., O’Callaghan C., Jones P.S., Hezemans F., Kaalund S.S., Tsvetanov K.A., Rodgers C.T., Williams G., Passamonti L., et al. An in vivo Probabilistic Atlas of the Human Locus Coeruleus at Ultra-high Field. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.02.03.932087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Beliveau V., Svarer C., Frokjaer V., Knudsen G.M., Greve D.N., Fisher P.M. Functional connectivity of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei at rest. NeuroImage. 2015;116:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Baker K.G., Törk I., Hornung J.-P., Halasz P. The human locus coeruleus complex: An immunohistochemical and three di-mensional reconstruction study. Exp. Brain Res. 1989;77:257–270. doi: 10.1007/BF00274983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.German D.C., Walker B.S., Manaye K., Smith W.K., Woodward D.J., North A.J. The human locus coeruleus: Computer re-construction of cellular distribution. J. Neurosci. 1988;8:1776–1788. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-05-01776.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Ohm T., Busch C., Bohl J. Unbiased Estimation of Neuronal Numbers in the Human Nucleus Coeruleus during Aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 1997;18:393–399. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00034-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Chan-Palay E. Asan. Quantitation of catecholamine neurons in the locus coeruleus in human brains of normal young and older adults and in depression. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989;287:357–372. doi: 10.1002/cne.902870307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Fernandes P., Regala J., Correia F., Gonçalves-Ferreira A.J. The human locus coeruleus 3-D stereotactic anatomy. Surg. Radiol. Anat. 2012;34:879–885. doi: 10.1007/s00276-012-0979-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Pauli W.M., Nili A.N., Tyszka J.M. A high-resolution probabilistic in vivo atlas of human subcortical brain nuclei. Sci. Data. 2018;5:180063. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2018.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Zaborszky L., Hoemke L., Mohlberg H., Schleicher A., Amunts K., Zilles K. Stereotaxic probabilistic maps of the magnocellular cell groups in human basal forebrain. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1127–1141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Forstmann B.U., Keuken M., Jahfari S., Bazin P.-L., Neumann J., Schäfer A., Anwander A., Turner R. Cortico-subthalamic white matter tract strength predicts interindividual efficacy in stopping a motor response. Neuroimage. 2012;60:370–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Collins D.L., Holmes C.J., Peters T.M., Evans A.C. Automatic 3-D model-based neuroanatomical segmentation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1995;3:190–208. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Mazziotta J., Toga A., Evans A., Fox P., Lancaster J., Zilles K., Woods R., Paus T., Simpson G., Pike B. A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM) Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2001;356:1293–1322. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Makris N., Goldstein J.M., Kennedy D., Hodge S.M., Caviness V.S., Faraone S.V., Tsuang M.T., Seidman L.J. Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2006;83:155–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Frazier J.A., Chiu S., Breeze J.L., Makris N., Lange N., Kennedy D.N., Herbert M.R., Bent E.K., Koneru V.K., Dieterich M.E., et al. Structural Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Limbic and Thalamic Volumes in Pediatric Bipolar Disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:1256–1265. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Desikan R.S., Ségonne F., Fischl B., Quinn B.T., Dickerson B.C., Blacker D., Buckner R.L., Dale A.M., Maguire R.P., Hyman B.T., et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based re-gions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Goldstein J.M., Seidman L.J., Makris N., Ahern T., O’Brien L.M., Caviness V.S., Jr., Kennedy D.N., Faraone S.V., Tsuang M.T. Hypo-thalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: Sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61:935–945. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Liu A.K.L., Chang R.C.-C., Pearce R.K.B., Gentleman S.M. Nucleus basalis of Meynert revisited: Anatomy, history and differential involvement in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 2015;129:527–540. doi: 10.1007/s00401-015-1392-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Kilimann I., Grothe M., Heinsen H., Alho E.J.L., Grinberg L., Jr E.A., Dos Santos G.A.B., Da Silva R.E., Mitchell A.J., Frisoni G.B., et al. Subregional Basal Forebrain Atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Multicenter Study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014;40:687–700. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Koulousakis P., Andrade P., Visser-Vandewalle V., Sesia T. The Nucleus Basalis of Meynert and Its Role in Deep Brain Stimulation for Cognitive Disorders: A Historical Perspective. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2019;69:905–919. doi: 10.3233/jad-180133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Le T., Kuplicki R.T., McKinney B.A., Yeh H.-W., Thompson W.K., Paulus M., Aupperle R.L., Bodurka J., Cha Y.-H., Tulsa 1000 Investigators et al. A Nonlinear Simulation Framework Supports Adjusting for Age When Analyzing BrainAGE. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:317. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Aletrino M., Vogels O., Van Domburg P., Donkelaar H.T. Cell loss in the nucleus raphes dorsalis in alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 1992;13:461–468. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(92)90073-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Krashia P., Nobili A., D’Amelio M. Unifying Hypothesis of Dopamine Neuron Loss in Neurodegenerative Diseases: Focusing on Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2019;12:123. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2019.00123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Bozzali M., D’Amelio M., Serra L. Ventral tegmental area disruption in Alzheimer’s disease. Aging. 2019;11:1325–1326. doi: 10.18632/aging.101852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Herrera A.Y., Wang J., Mather M. The gist and details of sex differences in cognition and the brain: How parallels in sex differences across domains are shaped by the locus coeruleus and catecholamine systems. Prog. Neurobiol. 2019;176:120–133. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Braak H., Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Koychev I., Hofer M., Friedman N.C. Correlation of Alzheimer Disease Neuropathologic Staging with Amyloid and Tau Scintigraphic Imaging Biomarkers. J. Nucl. Med. 2020;61:1413–1418. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.119.230458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Bachman S.L., Dahl M.J., Werkle-Bergner M., Düzel S., Forlim C.G., Lindenberger U., Kühn S., Mather M. Locus coeruleus MRI contrast is associated with cortical thickness in older adults. Neurobiol. Aging. 2021;100:72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Francis P.T., Palmer A.M., Sims N.R., Bowen D.M., Davison A.N., Esiri M.M., Neary D., Snowden J.S., Wilcock G.K. Neurochemical Studies of Early-Onset Alzheimer’s Disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 1985;313:7–11. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198507043130102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Hoogendijk W.J.G., Sommer I.E.C., Pool C.W., Kamphorst W., Hofman M.A., Eikelenboom P., Swaab D.F. Lack of association between depression and loss of neurons in the locus coeruleus in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1999;56:45–51. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Matthews K.L., Chen C.P.-H., Esiri M.M., Keene J., Minger S.L., Francis P.T. Noradrenergic changes, aggressive behavior, and cognition in patients with dementia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2002;51:407–416. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01235-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Gannon M., Che P., Chen Y., Jiao K., Roberson E., Wang Q. Noradrenergic dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 2015;9:220. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Aston-Jones G., Rajkowski J., Cohen J. Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility. Biol. Psychiatry. 1999;46:1309–1320. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00140-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Shine J.M. Neuromodulatory Influences on Integration and Segregation in the Brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23:572–583. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.O’Callaghan C., Walpola I.C., Shine J.M. Neuromodulation of the mind-wandering brain state: The interaction between neuromodulatory tone, sharp wave-ripples and spontaneous thought. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2021;376:20190699. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Jacobs H.I.L., Riphagen J.M., Ramakers I.H.G.B., Verhey F.R.J. Alzheimer’s disease pathology: Pathways between central norepinephrine activity, memory, and neuropsychiatric symptoms. Mol. Psychiatry. 2021;26:897–906. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0437-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Löwe L.C., Gaser C., Franke K., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative The Effect of the APOE Genotype on Individual BrainAGE in Normal Aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s Disease. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0157514. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Kaufmann T., Van Der Meer D., Doan N.T., Schwarz E., Lund M.J., Agartz I., Alnæs D., Barch D.M., Baur-Streubel R., Bertolino A., et al. Publisher Correction: Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;23:295. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0553-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Cole J.H. Multimodality neuroimaging brain-age in UK biobank: Relationship to biomedical, lifestyle, and cognitive factors. Neurobiol. Aging. 2020;92:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Wilcock G.K., Esiri M.M., Bowen D.M., Hughes A.O. The differential involvement of subcortical nuclei in senile dementia of Alzheimer’s type. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1988;51:842–849. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.6.842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Rüb U., Del Tredici K., Schultz C., Thal D., Braak E., Braak H. The evolution of Alzheimer’s disease-related cytoskeletal pathology in the human raphe nuclei. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2000;26:553–567. doi: 10.1046/j.0305-1846.2000.00291.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Rodríguez J.J., Noristani H., Verkhratsky A. The serotonergic system in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 2012;99:15–41. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Grinberg L.T., Rüb U., Ferretti R.E.L., Nitrini R., Farfel J.M., Polichiso L., Gierga K., Jacob-Filho W., Heinsen H. The dorsal raphe nucleus shows phospho-tau neurofibrillary changes before the transentorhinal region in Alzheimer’s disease. A precocious onset? Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2009;35 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2990.2008.00997.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Michelsen K.A., Schmitz C., Steinbusch H.W. The dorsal raphe nucleus—From silver stainings to a role in depression. Brain Res. Rev. 2007;55:329–342. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Aznar S., Qian Z.-X., Knudsen G.M. Non-serotonergic dorsal and median raphe projection onto parvalbumin- and calbindin-containing neurons in hippocampus and septum. Neuroscience. 2004;124:573–581. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Baker K.G., Halliday G.M., Hornung J.P., Geffen L.B., Cotton R.G. Distribution, morphology and number of monoamine-synthesizing and substance P-containing neurons in the human dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroscience. 1991;42:757–775. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90043-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Kirby L., Pernar L., Valentino R., Beck S. Distinguishing characteristics of serotonin and non-serotonin-containing cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus: Electrophysiological and immunohistochemical studies. Neuroscience. 2003;116:669–683. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00584-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Ordway G.A., Stockmeier C.A., Cason G.W., Klimek V. Pharmacology and Distribution of Norepinephrine Transporters in the Human Locus Coeruleus and Raphe Nuclei. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:1710–1719. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-05-01710.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Farley I.J., Hornykiewicz O. Noradrenaline distribution in subcortical areas of the human brain. Brain Res. 1977;126:53–62. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90214-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Steffener J., Habeck C., O’Shea D., Razlighi Q., Bherer L., Stern Y. Differences between chronological and brain age are related to education and self-reported physical activity. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;40:138–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Anatürk M., Kaufmann T., Cole J.H., Suri S., Griffanti L., Zsoldos E., Filippini N., Singh-Manoux A., Kivimäki M., Westlye L.T., et al. Prediction of brain age and cognitive age: Quantifying brain and cognitive maintenance in aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2021;42:1626–1640. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Edmonds E.C., Delano-Wood L., Galasko D.R., Salmon D.P., Bondi M.W. Subjective Cognitive Complaints Contribute to Misdiagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2014;20:836–847. doi: 10.1017/S135561771400068X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Edmonds E.C., Delano-Wood L., Clark L.R., Jak A.J., Nation D.A., McDonald C.R., Libon D.J., Au R., Galasko D., Salmon D.P., et al. Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false-positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2015;11:415–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Edmonds E.C., Eppig J., Bondi M.W., Leyden K.M., Goodwin B., Delano-Wood L., McDonald C.R., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Heterogeneous cortical atrophy patterns in MCI not captured by conventional diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2016;87:2108–2116. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Vuoksimaa E., McEvoy L.K., Holland D., Franz C.E., Kremen W.S., Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Modifying the minimum criteria for diagnosing amnestic MCI to improve prediction of brain atrophy and progression to Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;14:787–796. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-0019-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Hölzel B.K., Carmody J., Vangel M., Congleton C., Yerramsetti S.M., Gard T., Lazar S. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. 2011;191:36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Chapman S.B., Aslan S., Spence J.S., Keebler M.W., Defina L.F., Didehbani N., Perez A.M., Lu H., D’Esposito M. Distinct Brain and Behavioral Benefits from Cognitive vs. Physical Training: A Randomized Trial in Aging Adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:338. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Chalermpalanupap T., Kinkead B., Hu W.T., Kummer M.P., Hammerschmidt T., Heneka M.T., Weinshenker D., Levey A.I. Targeting norepinephrine in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2013;5:21–29. doi: 10.1186/alzrt175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Phillips C., Fahimi A., Das D., Mojabi F.S., Ponnusamy R., Salehi A. Noradrenergic System in Down Syndrome and Alzheimer’s Disease A Target for Therapy. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2015;13:68–83. doi: 10.2174/1567205012666150921095924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Dockree P.M., Barnes J.J., Matthews N., Dean A., Abe R., Nandam L.S., Kelly S.P., Bellgrove M.A., O’Connell R.G. The Effects of Methylphenidate on the Neu-ral Signatures of Sustained Attention. Biol. Psychiatry. 2017;82:687–694. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Segal S.K., Cotman C.W., Cahill L.F. Exercise-Induced Noradrenergic Activation Enhances Memory Consolidation in Both Normal Aging and Patients with Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2012;32:1011–1018. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Choudhary P., Pacholko A.G., Palaschuk J., Bekar L.K. The locus coeruleus neurotoxin, DSP4, and/or a high sugar diet induce behavioral and biochemical alterations in wild-type mice consistent with Alzheimers related pathology. Metab. Brain Dis. 2018;33:1563–1571. doi: 10.1007/s11011-018-0263-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Pacholko A.G., Wotton C.A., Bekar L.K. Poor Diet, Stress, and Inactivity Converge to Form a “Perfect Storm” That Drives Alzheimer’s Disease Pathogenesis. Neurodegener. Dis. 2019;19:60–77. doi: 10.1159/000503451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Engelborghs S., Gilles C., Ivanoiu A., Vandewoude M. Rationale and clinical data supporting nutritional intervention in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Clin. Belg. 2014;69:17–24. doi: 10.1179/0001551213Z.0000000006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Seidl S.E., Santiago J.A., Bilyk H., Potashkin J.A. The emerging role of nutrition in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:36. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Scarmeas N., Luchsinger J.A., Mayeux R., Stern Y. Mediterranean diet and Alzheimer disease mortality. Neurology. 2007;69:1084–1093. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000277320.50685.7c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Barnard N.D., Bush A., Ceccarelli A., Cooper J., de Jager C.A., Erickson K.I., Fraser G., Kesler S., Levin S.M., Lucey B., et al. Dietary and lifestyle guidelines for the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35:S74–S78. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Poly C., Massaro J., Seshadri S., Wolf P.A., Cho E., Krall E., Jacques P.F., Au R. The relation of dietary choline to cognitive performance and white-matter hyperintensity in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011;94:1584–1591. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.008938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Ylilauri M.P.T., Voutilainen S., Lönnroos E., Virtanen H.E.K., Tuomainen T.-P., Salonen J.T., Virtanen J.K. Associations of dietary choline intake with risk of incident dementia and with cognitive performance: The Kuopio Ischaemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019;110:1416–1423. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Spiers P.A., Myers D., Hochanadel G.S., Lieberman H.R., Wurtman R.J. Citicoline Improves Verbal Memory in Aging. Arch. Neurol. 1996;53:441–448. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1996.00550050071026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Kühn S., Düzel S., Colzato L., Norman K., Gallinat J., Brandmaier A.M., Lindenberger U., Widaman K.F. Food for thought: Association between dietary tyrosine and cognitive performance in younger and older adults. Psychol. Res. 2019;83:1097–1106. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0957-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Aliev G., Shahida K., Gan S.H., Firoz C., Khan A., Abuzenadah A., Kamal W., Kamal M., Tan Y., Qu X., et al. Alzheimer Disease and Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus: The Link to Tyrosine Hydroxylase and Probable Nutritional Strategies. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets. 2014;13:467–477. doi: 10.2174/18715273113126660153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Forssell L.G., Eklöf R., Winblad B., Forssell L. Early Stages of Late Onset Alzheimer’s Disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 1989;79:27–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1989.tb04875.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Rasmussen D.D., Yen S.S.C., Ishizuka B., Quigley M.E. Effects of Tyrosine and Tryptophan Ingestion on Plasma Catecholamine and 3,4-Dihydroxyphenylacetic Acid Concentrations. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1983;57:760–763. doi: 10.1210/jcem-57-4-760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Agharanya J.C., Alonso R., Wurtman R.J. Changes in catecholamine excretion after short-term tyrosine ingestion in normally fed human subjects. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1981;34:82–87. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/34.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Mahoney C.R., Castellani J., Kramer F.M., Young A., Lieberman H.R. Tyrosine supplementation mitigates working memory decrements during cold exposure. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:575–582. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Thomas J.R., Lockwood P.A., Singh A., Deuster P.A. Tyrosine Improves Working Memory in a Multitasking Environment. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1999;64:495–500. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00094-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Deijen J., Orlebeke J. Effect of tyrosine on cognitive function and blood pressure under stress. Brain Res. Bull. 1994;33:319–323. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Deijen J., Wientjes C., Vullinghs H., Cloin P., Langefeld J. Tyrosine improves cognitive performance and reduces blood pressure in cadets after one week of a combat training course. Brain Res. Bull. 1999;48:203–209. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00163-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Lieberman H.R., Corkin S., Spring B., Wurtman R.J., Growdon J.H. The effects of dietary neurotransmitter precursors on human behavior. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1985;42:366–370. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/42.2.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Melnychuk M.C., Dockree P.M., O’Connell R., Murphy P.R., Balsters J.H., Robertson I.H. Coupling of respiration and attention via the locus coeruleus: Effects of meditation and pranayama. Psychophysiology. 2018;55:e13091. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Craigmyle N.A. The beneficial effects of meditation: Contribution of the anterior cingulate and locus coeruleus. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:731. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Luders E., Toga A.W., Lepore N., Gaser C. The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. NeuroImage. 2009;45:672–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Vestergaard-Poulsen P., van Beek M., Skewes J., Bjarkam C., Stubberup M., Bertelsen J., Roepstorff A. Long-term meditation is associated with increased gray matter density in the brain stem. Neuroreport. 2009;20:170–174. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328320012a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Singleton O., Hölzel B.K., Vangel M., Brach N., Carmody J., Lazar S.W. Change in Brainstem Gray Matter Concentration Following a Mindfulness-Based Intervention is Correlated with Improvement in Psychological Well-Being. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Fox K.C., Nijeboer S., Dixon M.L., Floman J.L., Ellamil M., Rumak S.P., Christoff K. Is meditation associ-ated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation prac-titioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;43:48–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Tang Y.-Y., Lu Q., Geng X., Stein E.A., Yang Y., Posner M.I. Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:15649–15652. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011043107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Tang R., Friston K.J., Tang Y.-Y. Brief Mindfulness Meditation Induces Gray Matter Changes in a Brain Hub. Neural Plast. 2020;2020:1–8. doi: 10.1155/2020/8830005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

* Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu accessed on 15 January 2021). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf (accessed on 15 January 2021).


Articles from Cells are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES