Skip to main content
Pharmaceuticals logoLink to Pharmaceuticals
. 2021 Jun 23;14(7):607. doi: 10.3390/ph14070607

Nrf2/ARE Activators Improve Memory in Aged Mice via Maintaining of Mitochondrial Quality Control of Brain and the Modulation of Gut Microbiome

Irina S Sadovnikova 1, Artem P Gureev 1,2,*, Daria A Ignatyeva 1, Maria V Gryaznova 1,2, Ekaterina V Chernyshova 1, Ekaterina P Krutskikh 1, Anastasia G Novikova 1, Vasily N Popov 1,2
Editors: Lusine Danielyan, Huu Phuc Nguyen
PMCID: PMC8308546  PMID: 34201885

Abstract

Aging is one of the most serious factors for central nervous dysfunctions, which lead to cognitive impairment. New highly effective drugs are required to slow the development of cognitive dysfunction. This research studied the effect of dimethyl fumarate (DMF), methylene blue (MB), and resveratrol (RSV) on the cognitive functions of 15-month-old mice and their relationship to the maintenance of mitochondrial quality control in the brain and the bacterial composition of the gut microbiome. We have shown that studied compounds enhance mitochondrial biogenesis, mitophagy, and antioxidant defense in the hippocampus of 15-month-old mice via Nrf2/ARE pathway activation, which reduces the degree of oxidative damage to mtDNA. It is manifested in the improvement of short-term and long-term memory. We have also shown that memory improvement correlates with levels of Roseburia, Oscillibacter, Christensenellaceae R-7, Negativibacillus, and Faecalibaculum genera of bacteria. At the same time, long-term treatment by MB induced a decrease in gut microbiome diversity, but the other markers of dysbiosis were not observed. Thus, Nrf2/ARE activators have an impact on mitochondrial quality control and are associated with a positive change in the composition of the gut microbiome, which together lead to an improvement in memory in aged mice.

Keywords: aging, cognitive dysfunction, Nrf2/ARE pathway, mitochondrial biogenesis, mitophagy, antioxidants, resveratrol, dimethyl fumarate, methylene blue, memory, gut–brain axis, gut microbiome

1. Introduction

Aging is one of the most serious factors for central nervous dysfunctions such as mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and various neurodegenerative diseases [1]. Most patients with dementia frequently encounter various problems in their daily lives. Even mild cognitive impairment causes trouble to embarrass both the patients and their families [2]. New high effective drugs are required to slow the development of cognitive dysfunction. These drugs should be directed not at symptomatic treatment, but at eliminating the cause of cognitive dysfunctions, which are based on biochemical and molecular genetic causes. At the same time, drug discovery is a time-consuming, laborious, costly, and high-risk process. The success of developing a new molecular drug is about 2%, on average [3]. Drug repositioning is a highly effective, low-cost, and riskless strategy for finding new indications for existing drugs [4].

Some drugs have the potential to slow down neurodegenerative processes, inhibit the development of dementia, or improve the cognitive phenotype in mild cognitive impairment. In this research, we tested three compounds, which are used as drugs or dietary supplements. Dimethyl fumarate (DMF) is the methyl ester of fumaric acid [5] (Figure 1A). DMF undergoes rapid conversion to its active metabolite, monomethyl fumarate, whose terminal half-life is 1 h [6]. DMF has been approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and European Medicines Agency (EMA) as a drug for the treatment of multiple sclerosis (trade name Tecfidera) and for the treatment of moderate-to-severe plaque psoriasis (trade name Skilarence) [7]. Methylene blue (MB), a thiazine dye derivative of phenothiazine, has been described as “the first fully synthetic drug used in medicine” [8] (Figure 1B). Estimated terminal half-life of MB is 5.25 h [9]. Today, MB is the first-line treatment for methemoglobinemia, which is approved by FDA [10]. In addition, MB has been previously used to treat malaria [11]. In recent years, there have been clinical trials of MB for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. The first clinical trials [12] were unsuccessful, probably due to incorrect planning of phase III trials and inappropriate use of low doses of MB as a placebo [13]. Today, MB is undergoing clinical trials with other placebo pills [14,15]. Resveratrol (RSV, trans-3,5,4′-trihydroxystilbene) is a polyphenolic phytoalexin that was isolated from different edible parts of plants such as grapes, berries, peanuts, pistachios, and plums (Figure 1C). RSV has received considerable attention for its beneficial effects to health over the past three decades [16]. Unlike the other two drugs, it has not yet been approved by the FDA and is only used as a commercially available supplement. In addition, RSV has a fairly long terminal elimination half-life (8.52 ± 2.27 h) [17]. Clinical trials are currently underway on the effects of RSV supplementation on cognitive function in healthy adults [18].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Chemical formula of studied compounds: (A) dimethyl fumarate (DMF); (B) methylene blue (MB); (C) resveratrol (RSV).

Common to all three compounds is their ability to activate the factor 2 signaling pathway associated with the NF-E2/antioxidant-responsive element (Nrf2/ARE). Activation of this pathway protects cells from oxidative stress by regulation of the expression of genes that encode proteins with cytoprotective properties, for example, antioxidant enzymes, phase II proteins of xenobiotic detoxification, and anti-inflammatory enzymes as well as metabolic enzymes and regulators involved in maintaining redox homeostasis [19]. Thus, Nrf2/ARE activation may be one of the most promising directions in the treatment of cognitive impairments. Nrf2/ARE activators can provide antioxidant protection and maintain mitochondrial homeostasis associated with the regulation of the number and functionality of mitochondria; they can also suppress degenerative processes in the brain, significantly slowing down the rate of dementia development [20].

However, in addition to mitochondrial dysfunctions, there are several factors that can indirectly influence cognitive changes during aging. In recent years, accumulating data confirm that gut microbiome may modulate host brain function via a microbiome-gut–brain axis. This effect was shown for mild cognitive impairments [21], Alzheimer’s disease [22], Parkinson’s disease [23], depression [24], and schizophrenia [25]. The effect of RSV on the composition of the gut microbiome is well understood [26]. DMF and MB are not well understood in comparison with RSV. There are few studies of the effect of MB on the mouse gut microbiome [27] and DMF effect on the gut microbiota profile of patients with multiple sclerosis [28].

The goal of this study was to comprehensively study the effect of Nrf2/ARE activators on the cognitive abilities of middle-aged mice and their relationship with mitochondrial brain function including mitochondrial biogenesis, mitophagy, antioxidant defense, and mtDNA integrity in parallel with the study of gut microbiome composition in a context of microbiome–gut–brain axis.

The aim of this work was to study the effect of Nrf2/ARE activators on the cognitive abilities of middle-aged mice and their relationship with mitochondrial brain function including mitochondrial biogenesis, mitophagy, antioxidant defense, mtDNA integrity as well as to study gut microbiome composition in the context of the microbiome–gut–brain axis.

2. Results

2.1. Physiological Tests

None of the studied compounds exhibited a strong effect on behavioral parameters to indicate a change in stressful or exploratory behavior. However, there were changes in the grooming time (F (3, 26) = 5.6928, p < 0.01). Post-hoc test showed that RSV increased grooming time compared with the control group (Table 1). In addition, RSV reduced the time that mice spent in the open compartment of the dark–light box (F (3, 27) = 81.163, p < 0.001; post-hoc test showed differences between the control and RSV group, p < 0.001). DMF decreased the number of transitions between compartments of the dark–light box (F (3, 27) = 5.4463, p < 0.01; post-hoc test showed differences between the control and DMF group, p < 0.05) (Table 1). There were no differences between groups in the elevated plus-maze and string test (Table 1).

Table 1.

Results of physiological tests: open field, dark-light box, elevated plus-maze (EPM), string test.

Test Open Field
Indicator Horizontal Activity (s) Time in the Center (s) Entering in the Center (Number) Rearing (Number) Hole-Poking (Number) Grooming
(s)
Control 114.62 ± 14.49 19.87 ± 7.13 5.87 ± 1.78 15.25 ± 2.25 4.5 ± 0.91 10.62 ± 1.44
DMF 124.00 ± 7.41 16.00 ± 4.07 5.12 ± 0.67 12.75 ± 1.23 3.12 ± 0.69 9.25 ± 2.61
MB 83.67 ± 14.27 10.5 ± 2.66 4.83 ± 1.51 14.00 ± 2.67 2.66 ± 0.80 9.00 ± 3.38
RSV 90.75 ± 12.89 10.66 ± 3.38 4.5 ± 1.18 7.62 ± 2.09 5.5 ± 2.38 24.25 ± 4.38*
Test Open Field Dark-Light Box EPM String
Indicator Grooming
(Number)
Defecation Time in the Open Compartment (s) Transition between Compartments (Number) Time in the Open Arm (s) Scores
(Number)
Control 1.75 ± 0.16 1.62 ± 0.86 235.25 ± 8.64 15.75 ± 2.68 4.37 ± 1.61 2.8 ± 0.33
DMF 4.12 ± 2.42 0.25 ± 0.25 253.22 ± 9.16 7.67 ± 1.43 * 8.44 ± 2.53 1.82 ± 0.39
MB 1.5 ± 0.22 1.5 ± 0.5 231.83 ± 7.50 16.83 ± 1.42 6.83 ± 2.09 3.5 ± 0.43
RSV 2.75 ± 0.65 2.5 ± 1.44 64.37 ± 12.64 *** 8.37 ± 2.14 5.75 ± 4.34 3.2 ± 0.40

The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 8. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

2.2. Memory

Nrf2/ARE activators changed the values of short-term memory F (3, 647) = 5.6069, p < 0.001. The post-hoc test showed that MB and DMF increased the number of scores by about 20% (both p < 0.05) compared with the control (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Number of scores in the Water Marris Maze for the assessment of short-term memory. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. * p < 0.05, comparison of control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

The studied compounds did not affect the distance and time that mice spent for the platform search and the time spent in the quadrant with the platform on the days of the study (6th and 12th days) (Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4). Differences were observed during learning. During the acquisition phase of the test, there were differences in the distance that mice swam for the platform search on the 4th day (F (3, 27) = 3.7849, p < 0.05). The post-hoc test showed differences between the MB and DMF group (p < 0.05) and there were no differences versus the control group. In contrast, during the reversal phase of the test, all compounds tested caused a decrease in the distance that mice spent searching for the platform. On the 7th day, F (3, 27) = 8.1537, p < 0.001; on the 8th day, F (3, 27) = 5.3290, p < 0.01; on the 9th day F (3, 27) = 8.2114, p < 0.001; on the 10th day F (3, 27) = 5.8029, p < 0.01; and on the 11th day F (3, 27) = 3.1380, p < 0.05 (Table 4, Figure 3).

Table 2.

The distance that mice swam for the platform search.

Acquisition Learning Probe
Start SW Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6
N; E; SE; NW SE; N; NW; E NW; SE; E; N E; NW; N; SE N; SE; E; NW NW
Control 2611 ± 514 1852 ± 306 1511 ± 266 1956 ± 313 1265 ± 232 1401 ± 410
DMF 2512 ± 230 2018 ± 151 2130 ± 201 2035 ± 187 1887 ± 222 2897 ± 359
MB 2573 ± 266 1960 ± 257 2174 ± 276 1134 ± 192 1654 ± 295 1682 ± 464
RSV 1896 ± 238 1894 ± 220 1724 ± 236 1461 ± 195 1544 ± 223 1376 ± 385
Reversal Learning Probe
Start NE Day 7 Day 8 Day 9 Day 10 Day 11 Day 12
S; W; NW; SE NW; S; SE; W SE; NW; W; S W; SE; S; NW S; NW; W; SE SE
Control 2774 ± 423 2306 ± 398 2726 ± 420 2212 ± 213 2064 ± 498 1324 ± 443
DMF 1228 ± 184 ** 1491 ± 195 996 ± 174 *** 1122 ± 153 ** 1142 ± 154 * 1062 ± 401
MB 1113 ± 217 ** 920 ± 188 ** 1335 ± 266* 1195 ± 179 * 682 ± 116 *** 1838 ± 334
RSV 1430 ± 195 * 1107 ± 165 * 1229 ± 201 ** 1160 ± 163 ** 1076 ± 168 * 1691 ± 338

The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 8. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

Table 3.

The time that mice spent on the platform search.

Acquisition Learning Probe
Start SW Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6
N; E; SE; NW SE; N; NW; E NW; SE; E; N E; NW; N; SE N; SE; E; NW NW
Control 38.3 ± 6.2 28.4 ± 5.4 35.5 ± 10.0 29.5 ± 4.5 34.5 ± 6.5 24.5 ± 8.5
DMF 47.3 ± 3.3 49.9 ± 2.6 46.7 ± 3.4 44.2 ± 3.3 36.9 ± 3.8 49.2 ± 5.6
MB 45.9 ± 4.0 34.3 ± 4.6 37.3 ± 4.5 24.3 ± 4.1 30.7 ± 5.6 29.3 ± 9.7
RSV 41.2 ± 4.3 37.6 ± 3.8 31.6 ± 4.3 29.2 ± 3.9 24.5 ± 3.6 29.4 ± 8.9
Reversal Learning Probe
Start NE Day 7 Day 8 Day 9 Day 10 Day 11 Day 12
S; W; NW; SE NW; S; SE; W SE; NW; W; S W; SE; S; NW S; NW; W; SE SE
Control 19.7 ± 3.6 17.6 ± 3.5 20.9 ± 3.3 21.5 ± 2.6 19.8 ± 5.5 22.1 ± 7.4
DMF 21.7 ± 3.3 31.7 ± 3.9 17.4 ± 3.2 21.6 ± 3.4 23.0 ± 3.5 16.8 ± 6.6
MB 17.7 ± 3.1 15.1 ± 3.0 19.1 ± 3.8 18.4 ± 2.8 13.1 ± 2.5 31.0 ± 3.9
RSV 26 ± 3.8 22.4 ± 3.6 26.1 ± 4.3 23.1 ± 3.8 25.9 ± 3.7 33.0 ± 8.6

The results expressed as means ± SEM.

Table 4.

The time that mice spent in the quadrant with the platform.

Acquisition Learning Probe
Start SW Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6
N; E; SE; NW SE; N; NW; E NW; SE; E; N E; NW; N; SE N; SE; E; NW NW
Control 10.3 ± 1.62 9.3 ± 1.38 11.4 ± 2.95 7.5 ± 0.98 5.9 ± 0.87 8.5 ± 3.09
DMF 11 ± 0.99 17.4 ± 1.52 10.9 ± 0.98 14.2 ± 1.55 10.8 ± 1.30 10.6 ± 1.83
MB 11.8 ± 1.72 13.9 ± 2.29 12.2 ± 1.42 13.9 ± 2.68 10.1 ± 1.81 7.8 ± 2.40
RSV 9.7 ± 0.97 12.7 ± 1.66 10.9 ± 1.61 9.5 ± 1.47 8.3 ± 1.27 10.9 ± 3.79
Reversal Learning Probe
Start NE Day 7 Day 8 Day 9 Day 10 Day 11 Day 12
S; W; NW; SE NW; S; SE; W SE; NW; W; S W; SE; S; NW S; NW; W; SE SE
Control 5.7 ± 0.90 5.2 ± 0.43 7.2 ± 0.94 6.5 ± 0.51 8.3 ± 1.23 4.6 ± 1.05
DMF 7.9 ± 1.14 7.6 ± 0.95 5.7 ± 0.79 4.9 ± 0.79 6.0 ± 0.80 5.6 ± 1.83
MB 6.3 ± 1.08 5.3 ± 0.86 5.3 ± 0.76 5.8 ± 0.84 3.9 ± 0.56 10.7 ± 2.16
RSV 7.6 ± 1.18 6.4 ± 1.19 5.2 ± 1.01 5.4 ± 1.15 5.1 ± 0.73 9.5 ± 3.37

The results expressed as means ± SEM.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The distance that mice swam for the platform search during acquisition and reversal learning. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 8.

2.3. Gene Expression

All three studied compounds stimulated a 2–3-fold increase in the expression of the nuclear factor erythroid 2-related factor 2 (Nfe2l2) gene in the hippocampus of 15-month-old mice (all p < 0.05). DMF increased expression of sirtuin 1 (Sirt1) (p < 0.05). MB increased expression of mitophagy-related genes p62 and PTEN-induced kinase 1 (Pink1) (both p < 0.05) and nuclear respiratory factor 1 (Nrf1) (p < 0.05). RSV increased expression of PPARG coactivator 1 alpha (Ppargc1a) (p < 0.01), Sirt1, Nrf1, and p62 (all p < 0.05). All three studied Nrf2/ARE activators stimulated more than 50% increase in the expression of the fork box O1 (FoxO1) protein compared to the control, but there were no statistically significant changes (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Expression of transcriptional factors in the hippocampus. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, comparison of control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

Nrf2/ARE activators affected the expression of antioxidant genes in the hippocampus of 15-month-old mice. DMF induced more than a 2-fold increase in superoxide dismutase 2 (Sod2) expression (p < 0.05). MB stimulated expression of catalytic subunit of glutamate cysteine ligase (Gclc) and heme oxygenase 1 (Hmox1). RSV increased expression of thioredoxin reductase (TrxR), peroxiredoxin-5 (Prdx5), and Sod2 gene. The effect of the tested compounds on the expression of glutathione peroxidase (Gpx) were not shown (Figure 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Expression of antioxidants in the hippocampus. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. * p < 0.05, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

2.4. Copy Number of mtDNA

MB induced aa 3-fold increase in mtDNA copy number in the hippocampus (p < 0.05). DMF and RSV did not affect the copy number mtDNA in the hippocampus (Figure 6A). All three studied Nrf2/ARE activators increased the copy number of mtDNA in the cortex. It should be noted that DMF increased the number of mtDNA copies in the cerebral cortex by eight times compared to the control (p < 0.05) (Figure 6B). Mice that received DMF and RSV had a 2.5-fold increased number of mtDNA in the mid-brain (both p < 0.05). MB also stimulated an increase in the number of copies of mtDNA, but the changes were not statistically significant (Figure 6C).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Copy number of mtDNA in the (A) hippocampus, (B) cortex, (C) mid-brain. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. * p < 0.05, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

2.5. mtDNA Damage

MB more than 2-fold reduced the amount of mtDNA damage in the hippocampus of 15-month-old mice (p < 0.001) (Figure 7A). A decrease in the number of damage was observed in fragments 2, 4, 5, 6 (Table 5), which corresponded to the 16s-ND1 region and ND5-ND6-CytB-D-loop region, while RSV in the first and second fragments (12s-16s-ND1 region), in contrast, stimulated an increase in the number of lesions, and DMF caused damage in fragment 3 (ND1-ND2 region) (Table 5).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Amount of mtDNA damage in the (A) hippocampus, (B) cortex, (C) mid-brain. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 8. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

Table 5.

Amount of mtDNA damage.

Hippocampus
Control DMF MB RSV
1 fragment 2.05 ± 0.30 2.97 ± 0.30 1.20 ± 0.34 3.68 ± 0.19 ***
2 fragment 2.33 ± 0.24 3.83 ± 0.71 0.80 ± 0.34 ** 4.63 ± 0.26 ***
3 fragment 2.19 ± 0.54 3.46 ± 0.21 * 1.50 ± 0.48 3.29 ± 0.28
4 fragment 3.84 ± 0.28 4.03 ± 0.20 1.72 ± 0.61 ** 3.82 ± 0.27
5 fragment 3.84 ± 0.23 4.09 ± 0.18 1.39 ± 0.32 *** 4.22 ± 0.24
6 fragment 4.75 ± 0.30 5.17 ± 0.17 1.82 ± 0.59 *** 4.39 ± 0.21
Forebrain
1 fragment 2.26 ± 0.35 1.86 ± 0.47 3.16 ± 0.35 3.03 ± 0.42
2 fragment 5.35 ± 0.34 3.60 ± 0.79 * 6.23 ± 0.24 5.51 ± 0.59
3 fragment 4.74 ± 0.14 2.94 ± 0.51 *** 4.72 ± 0.16 4.32 ± 0.44
4 fragment 1.71 ± 0.47 0.47 ± 0.69 1.26 ± 0.53 1.23 ± 0.48
5 fragment 2.50 ± 0.74 0.97 ± 0.67 3.00 ± 0.62 3.17 ± 0.53
6 fragment 5.92 ± 0.45 3.59 ± 1.05 * 1.55 ± 1.24 ** 1.47 ± 0.98 ***
Mid-Brain
1 fragment 2.30 ± 0.37 2.90 ± 0.29 3.35 ± 0.35 * 3.16 ± 0.46
2 fragment 7.54 ± 0 2.27 ± 0.72 *** 2.48 ± 0.88 *** 2.43 ± 0.88 ***
3 fragment 4.15 ± 0.45 2.88 ± 0.55 2.98 ± 0.59 2.62 ± 0.52 *
4 fragment 2.71 ± 0.38 3.45 ± 0.29 3.14 ± 0.17 3.39 ± 0.24
5 fragment 3.1 ± 0.53 2.71 ± 0.57 3.60 ± 0.35 2.74 ± 0.45
6 fragment 3.1 ± 0.65 4.28 ± 0.59 5.05 ± 0.45 * 5.84 ± 0.48 **

The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 9, MB = 6, RSV = 7. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, comparison of the control group and treated groups using Tukey’s post-hoc test.

DMF decreased the number of mtDNA damage in the cortex (p < 0.01) due to the reduced damages in the 2nd, 3rd, and 6th fragments (12s-16s-ND1 and D-loop regions) (Figure 7B, Table 5). MB and RSV decreased the amount of mtDNA damage in the D-loop fragment only. For this reason, there was no statistically significant decrease in the average number of damage in the cortex of mice that received MB and RSV (Table 5).

DMF only also decreased the number of mtDNA damage in the midbrain, mainly due to reducing the number of damage in the 2nd fragment. Treatment with MB and RSV also led to a decrease in the amount of mtDNA damage in the 2nd fragment, but these compounds stimulated damage in the D-loop region (Figure 7C, Table 5).

2.6. Bacterial Composition of Gut Microbiome

PCR analysis showed that more than 95% of the bacteria in the gut microbiome were Bacteroidetes and Firmicutes (Table 6). ANOVA showed that Nrf2/ARE activators affect the percentage of bacteria of the Firmicutes phylum (F (3, 24) = 4.2200, p < 0.05). However, the post-hoc test did not show differences between the control and treated groups. A tendency to an increase in Firmicutes level was shown for the MB group (p = 0.069) and RSV group (p = 0.07) (Figure 8).

Table 6.

Phyla of bacteria in gut microbiome. Results of PCR analysis.

Control DMF MB RSV
Bacteroidetes 85.20 ± 2.74 84.19 ± 2.31 75.44 ± 4.31 79.70 ± 3.01
Firmicutes 9.45 ± 1.76 9.48 ± 2.07 18.18 ± 2.97 17.60 ± 2.86
Actinobacteria 0.26 ± 0.04 0.25 ± 0.08 0.26 ± 0.08 0.11 ± 0.03
Betaproteobacteria 2.24 ± 1.05 1.20 ± 0.36 0.90 ± 0.24 0.75 ± 0.17
Epsilonproteobacteria 0.46 ± 0.29 0.30 ± 0.07 0.20 ± 0.07 0.60 ± 0.21
Delta- and Gammaproteobacteria 1.26 ± 0.30 2.24 ± 0.59 2.78 ± 0.69 2.05 ± 0.90
«Candidatus Saccharibacteria» 0.42 ± 0.12 0.80 ± 0.29 0.55 ± 0.15 0.47 ± 0.11
Deferribacteres 0.29 ± 0.11 0.69 ± 0.36 1.60 ± 0.88 0.52 ± 0.23
Tenericutes 0.00 ± 0.00 0.02 ± 0.01 0.02 ± 0.01 0.00 ± 0.00
Verrucomicrobia 0.41 ± 0.15 0.82 ± 0.67 0.06 ± 0.01 0.17 ± 0.04

Figure 8.

Figure 8

Level of Firmicutes phylum level in the gut microbiome. The results expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 8, MB = 6, RSV = 6.

There was a negative correlation between the level of Verrucomicrobia and distance (rs = −0.494) and time (rs = −0.465) that mice spent to search the goal platform on 12th day. At the same time, Verrucomicrobia negatively correlated with the time that mice spent to search the goal platform on 12th day (rs = −0.398). Tenericutes level positively correlated with distance (rs = 0.474) and time (rs = −0.436) that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 6th day. Time spent in the quadrant with the platform positively correlated with Deferribacteres level (rs = 0.407) and “Candidatus Saccharibacteria” (rs = 0.387) (Table S1). Multivariate correlation analysis showed a negative correlation between scores of short-term memories and Epsilonproteobacteria level (R = −0.504) (Table S3).

NGS analysis showed that genus Prevotella (Bacteroidetes phylum) was dominant in the control group (0.135 ± 0.031), DMF group (0.135 ± 0.058), and RSV group (0.160 ± 0.027). In the MB group, genus Lachnospiraceae UCG-001 (Firmicutes phylum) was dominant (0.147 ± 0.062), while the Prevotella amount was lower (0.096 ± 0.018) (Table 7).

Table 7.

Gut microbiome (NGS).

Phylum Class Family Genus Control DMF MB RSV
Abditibacteriota Abditibacteriaceae Abditibacteriaceae Abditibacterium 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Actinobacteriota Actinobacteria Propionibacteriaceae Cutibacterium 0.022 ± 0.022 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Bifidobacteriaceae Bifidobacterium 0.005 ± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.003 0.003 ± 0.001 0.013 ± 0.007
Corynebacteriaceae Corynebacterium 0.002 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Coriobacteriia Atopobiaceae Olsenella 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Proteobacteria Alphaproteobacteria Rhizobiales Methylobacterium-Methylorubrum 0.005 ± 0.005 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Caulobacterales Asticcacaulis 0.002 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Gammaproteobacteria Burkholderiales Parasutterella 0.085 ± 0.025 0.082 ± 0.055 0.054 ± 0.013 0.088 ± 0.021
Burkholderia-Caballeronia-Paraburkholderia 0.006 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Enterobacteriaceae Escherichia-Shigella 0.002 ± 0.002 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Xanthomonadaceae Pseudoxanthomonas 0.004 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Moraxellaceae Acinetobacter 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Rhodanobacteraceae Rudaea 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Bacteroidota Bacteroidia Prevotellaceae Prevotella 0.135 ± 0.031 0.135 ± 0.058 0.096 ± 0.018 0.160 ± 0.027
Prevotellaceae UCG-001 0.023 ± 0.005 0.031 ± 0.014 0.014 ± 0.004 0.032 ± 0.008
Prevotellaceae Ga6A1 group 0.029 ± 0.008 0.015 ± 0.009 0.030 ± 0.011 0.023 ± 0.010
Alloprevotella 0.022 ± 0.007 0.022 ± 0.021 0.010 ± 0.002 0.012 ± 0.003
Marinifilaceae Odoribacter 0.026 ± 0.006 0.028 ± 0.013 0.014 ± 0.004 0.025 ± 0.004
Bacteroidaceae Bacteroides 0.074 ± 0.025 0.027 ± 0.009 0.018 ± 0.003 0.029 ± 0.004
Muribaculaceae Muribaculum 0.033 ± 0.006 0.020 ± 0.010 0.017 ± 0.004 0.038 ± 0.013
Tannerellaceae Parabacteroides 0.008 ± 0.002 0.005 ± 0.003 0.004 ± 0.001 0.005 ± 0.001
Rikenellaceae Rikenella 0.006 ± 0.001 0.006 ± 0.004 0.007 ± 0.001 0.006 ± 0.001
Alistipes 0.025 ± 0.005 0.019 ± 0.011 0.011 ± 0.003 0.023 ± 0.006
Rikenellaceae RC9 gut group 0.035 ± 0.004 0.052 ± 0.029 0.050 ± 0.027 0.067 ± 0.025
Campilobacterota Campylobacteria Helicobacteraceae Helicobacter 0.038 ± 0.007 0.095 ± 0.070 0.058 ± 0.021 0.050 ± 0.007
Firmicutes Clostridia Lachnospiraceae Lachnospiraceae UCG-001 0.080 ± 0.025 0.037 ± 0.017 0.147 ± 0.062 0.034 ± 0.014
Herbinix 0.014 ± 0.004 0.012 ± 0.016 0.021 ± 0.015 0.007 ± 0.001
[Ruminococcus] gnavus group 0.013 ± 0.002 0.029 ± 0.022 0.023 ± 0.008 0.016 ± 0.005
Acetatifactor 0.006 ± 0.001 0.010 ± 0.014 0.034 ± 0.019 0.031 ± 0.012
Stomatobaculum 0.002 ± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.002 0.003 ± 0.001 0.008 ±0.003
Lachnospiraceae NK4A136 group 0.027 ± 0.005 0.031 ± 0.020 0.036 ± 0.006 0.052 ± 0.011
Tuzzerella 0.004 ± 0.001 0.006 ± 0.006 0.002 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001
Tyzzerella 0.003 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Roseburia 0.008 ± 0.006 0.003 ± 0.002 0.003 ± 0.001 0.007 ± 0.004
GCA-900066575 0.003 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001
ASF356 0.002 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.002 0.002 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Blautia 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.002 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.001
[Eubacterium] hallii group 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
A2 0.002 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001
Lachnospiraceae UCG-003 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000
[Eubacterium] xylanophilum group 0.003 ± 0.002 0.003 ± 0.002 0.002 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001
[Eubacterium] fissicatena group 0.002 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.000
Mobilitalea 0.001 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Lachnoanaerobaculum 0.004 ± 0.001 0.007 ± 0.006 0.005 ± 0.002 0.007 ± 0.001
Oscillospiraceae Oscillibacter 0.014 ± 0.003 0.018 ± 0.013 0.007 ± 0.001 0.011 ± 0.001
Colidextribacter 0.024 ± 0.005 0.030 ± 0.012 0.016 ± 0.001 0.029 ± 0.002
UCG-003 0.004 ± 0.001 0.009 ± 0.005 0.003 ± 0.001 0.007 ± 0.001
Flavonifractor 0.003 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.002 0.002 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001
UCG-002 0.002 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Intestinimonas 0.009 ± 0.002 0.010 ± 0.007 0.006 ± 0.001 0.009 ± 0.002
Ruminococcaceae Ruminococcus 0.018 ± 0.004 0.019 ± 0.015 0.013 ± 0.004 0.011 ± 0.005
Incertae Sedis 0.000 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.002 0.004 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.002
Negativibacillus 0.004 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001
Angelakisella 0.002 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Paludicola 0.002 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Anaerotruncus 0.004 ± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.002 0.004 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000
[Eubacterium] siraeum group 0.003 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.008 ± 0.004
Clostridiaceae Clostridium sensu stricto 1 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001 0.003 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000
Anaerovoracaceae [Eubacterium] nodatum group 0.002 ± 0.001 0.002 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Family XIII UCG-001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Monoglobaceae Monoglobus 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.002 0.002 ± 0.002
Christensenellaceae Christensenellaceae R-7 group 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Peptococcaceae Peptococcus 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.001 0.000 ± 0.000
Bacilli Erysipelotrichaceae Allobaculum 0.077 ± 0.020 0.070 ± 0.069 0.200 ± 0.079 0.067 ± 0.017
Ileibacterium 0.017 ± 0.006 0.019 ± 0.030 0.011 ± 0.004 0.038 ± 0.016
Dubosiella 0.003 ± 0.002 0.004 ± 0.002 0.004 ± 0.002 0.007 ± 0.002
Faecalibaculum 0.001 ± 0.000 0.004 ± 0.003 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001
Lactobacillaceae Lactobacillus 0.012 ± 0.005 0.008 ± 0.002 0.015 ± 0.003 0.008 ± 0.003
Atopostipes 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Streptococcaceae Streptococcus 0.004 ± 0.003 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.001
Acholeplasmataceae Anaeroplasma 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.003 0.003 ± 0.003 0.001 ± 0.000
Staphylococcaceae Staphylococcus 0.002 ± 0.002 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Jeotgalicoccus 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.003 ± 0.002 0.001 ± 0.000
Exiguobacteraceae Exiguobacterium 0.004 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Erysipelatoclostridiaceae Candidatus Stoquefichus 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000 0.004 ± 0.003
Erysipelatoclostridium 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Streptococcaceae Lactococcus 0.001 ± 0.001 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001
Mycoplasmataceae Ureaplasma 0.000 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.002 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001
Mycoplasma 0.002 ± 0.000 0.001± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Negativicutes Acidaminococcaceae Phascolarctobacterium 0.034 ± 0.021 0.019 ± 0.035 0.002 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000
Sporomusaceae Pelosinus 0.001 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Deferribacterota Deferribacteres Deferribacteraceae Mucispirillum 0.010 ± 0.003 0.038 ± 0.052 0.014 ± 0.011 0.006 ± 0.001
Desulfobacterota Desulfovibrionia Desulfovibrionaceae Desulfovibrio 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.002 0.001 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000
Bilophila 0.001 ± 0.000 0.002 ± 0.001 0.001 ± 0.000 0.001 ± 0.000
Spirochaetota Leptospirae Leptospiraceae Leptospira 0.002 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000 0.000 ± 0.000
Brachyspirae Brachyspiraceae Brachyspira 0.004 ± 0.001 0.025 ± 0.040 0.004 ± 0.003 0.002 ± 0.001
Patescibacteria Saccharimonadia Saccharimonadaceae Candidatus Saccharimonas 0.005 ± 0.001 0.008 ± 0.005 0.003 ± 0.001 0.008 ± 0.001
Verrucomicrobiota Verrucomicrobiae Akkermansiaceae Akkermansia 0.012 ± 0.006 0.009 ± 0.013 0.010 ± 0.006 0.023 ± 0.014

There was a negative correlation between the level of Mycoplasma and time on the 12th day (rs = −0.453), between time in the goal quadrant on the 12th day (rs = −0.451) and on the 6th day (rs = 0.506). In addition, we observed a positive correlation between the level of Mycoplasma and number of scores of short-term memory (rs = 0.456). Level of Roseburia negatively correlated with distance (rs = −0.493) and time (rs = −0.425) that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 12th day. Level of Oscillibacter negatively correlated with distance (rs = −0.407) and time (rs = −0.474) that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 12th day. Level of Christensenellaceae R-7 group bacteria negatively correlated with distance (rs = −0.421) and time (rs = −0.412) that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 12th day. There was a negative correlation between level of Muribaculum and time on the 6th day (rs = −0.470), between time in the goal quadrant on the 6th day (rs = −0.400) and distance on the 6th day (rs = −0.464). Level of Bilophila negatively correlated with distance (rs = −0.549) and time in the goal quadrant (rs = −0.562) that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 12th day. There was a negative correlation between the level of Negativibacillus and distance on the 6th day (rs = −0.453). There was a positive correlation between time in a goal quadrant on 6th day and level of Faecalibaculum (rs = 0.415) (Table S2).

In our research, a number of bacterial genera was associated with memory impairment. We observed a negative correlation between time in a goal quadrant on the 6th day and level of Streptococcus (rs = −0.397) and level of Odoribacter (rs = −0.434). Level of Acinetobacter was correlated with distance (rs = 0.462) and time (rs = 0.471) that mice spent searching the goal platform on the 6th day as well as Clostridium (rs = 0.442 with distance and rs = 0.467 with time). There was a positive correlation between time that mice spent to search the goal platform on the 12th day and level of Dubosiella (rs = 0.422) and level of Desulfovibrio (rs = 0.403). Time that mice spent searching the goal platform on the 6th day correlated with Prevotellaceae UCG-001 (rs = 0.397). The distance that mice swam for the platform search correlated with Tyzzerella level (rs = 0.413) and UCG-002 (Family Oscillospiraceae) level (rs = 0.419) (Table S2). Multivariate correlation analysis showed strong positive correlation between Bifidobacterium and time that mice spent in goal quadrant at day 12 (R = 0.795). Other average and weak correlations are represented in Table S4.

3. Discussion

Analysis of the expression data shows that DMF, MB, and RSV activate the Nrf2/ARE signal pathway in the hippocampus of 15-month-old mice. All three studied compounds doubled the expression of the Nfe2l2 gene, which encodes the Nrf2 protein (Figure 4). Nfe2l2 contains ARE-like sequences in the promoter region and Nrf2 may activate its own gene expression, leading to increased production of Nrf2 protein [29]. It is well known that DMF, MB, and RSV activate the Nrf2/ARE signal pathway (Figure 9) [30,31,32]. DMF covalently interacts with the reactive cysteines lying in the BTB and IVR regions of Kelch-like ECH-associated protein 1 (Keap1). In addition, DMF binds to the Nrf2-binding site (bottom region of Keap1-DC), and to the top region of Keap1-DC, near blade II [30]. RSV forms H-bonds with Ser49, Asn100, Arg101, Leu196, and Ser288 of the Kelch domain and Pi–cation interaction with Arg101 (motifs in zebrafish) [31]. RSV also activates Nrf2 through stimulation of the Sirt1/FoxO1 pathways [32]. These data have been confirmed by qPCR. We showed that RSV increases the expression of the Sirt1 gene and there is a tendency to increase the expression of FoxO1, but the changes are not statistically significant (Figure 4). In addition, RVS activation of Nrf2 may be due to p62-dependent autophagic degradation of Keap1 [33] (Figure 9). We showed that RSV induced an increase in p62 expression (Figure 4).

Figure 9.

Figure 9

Scheme of the activation of the Nrf2/ARE signal pathway by dimethyl fumarate (DMF); methylene blue (MB); resveratrol (RSV).

There are currently no data showing that MB directly activates Nrf2 due to interaction with Keap1 or glycogen synthase kinase 3 beta (GSK3β). However, there are several indirect mechanisms that can lead to the activation of Nrf2. It is known that MB can accept electrons from NADH and perform alternative electron transport in the electron–transport chain [34]. This leads to a shift in the NAD+/NADH equilibrium toward NAD+. This leads to the activation of AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK), which can activate the Nrf2/ARE signaling pathway [35] (Figure 9). In addition, MB in the electron transfer process can increase the H2O2 production rate [36,37]. We have previously shown that this process is not associated with a significant increase in oxidative stress in the brain, but can cause compensatory effects [37,38]. H2O2 can activate Nrf2 by disrupting its interaction with both Keap1 and GSK3β (Figure 9).

There are various consequences of the activation of the Nrf2/ARE signaling pathway. It is known that this signaling pathway plays an important role in the regulation of antioxidant defense [39], which is manifested in the fact that Nrf2 activators to varying degrees activated the expression of antioxidant genes (Figure 5). This is actually the target for the drug because there is an age-dependent decrease in antioxidant mechanisms [40]. It should be noted that not all Nrf2 activators increase the expression of all studied antioxidants, most of all increased by RSV treatment (4-fold increase in TrxR and Prdx5 expression and 5-fold increase in Sod2 expression) (Figure 5). This is not surprising because RSV is a phenolic stilbenoid compound. Generally, phenolic compounds are powerful antioxidants [41].

Regulation of mitochondrial pool volume in the cell is an important part of the maintenance of mitochondrial quality control [42]. For a long time, it was believed that “PGC-1α is a master regulator of mitochondrial biogenesis” [43]. In recent years, evidence has begun to accumulate that this is not clear and that there are other ways of mitochondrial biogenesis regulation, one of which is the Nrf2/ARE signaling pathway [44]. We found that one of the Nrf2/ARE activators (RSV) could activate the Sirt1/PGC-1α axis due to the increase in expression of both Sirt1 and Ppargc1a (PGC-1α encoded gene) (Figure 9). In addition, we observed that RSV increased Nrf1 expression (Figure 4), which plays a critical role in the transcriptional regulation of mtDNA transcription process [45]. At the same time, the level of mtDNA in the hippocampus was not increased in mice treated with RSV. Hippocampal mtDNA increased in MB-treated mice (Figure 6a). Similarly, we found a MB-induced increase in Nrf1 expression. However, no increase in the expression of genes involved in the activation of the Sirt1/PGC-1α axis was found (Figure 4). These data confirm that PGC-1α is not major regulator of mitochondrial biogenesis and other pathways can regulate the level of mitochondria in the cell [44]. It should be noted that in the forebrain, all three studied compounds increased the number of mtDNA copies, while in the mid-brain, the increase in the number of mtDNA copies was caused by DMF and RSV (Figure 6).

Mitophagy is another important process that regulates the number of mitochondria in a cell. [46]. MB and RSV stimulate expression of both p62 and Pink1 genes (Figure 4), which may indicate that mitophagy processes are activated. Mitophagy plays an important role in maintaining the integrity of mitochondria and various mitochondrial components [46]. We found that MB stimulated a decrease in the amount of mtDNA damage in the hippocampus (Figure 7, Table 5). It is likely that this may be due to the activation of the mitophagy process. Surprisingly, RSV did not induce a decrease in the amount of mtDNA damage (Figure 7, Table 5), although it also activated the expression of genes associated with mitophagy and stimulated antioxidant defense more strongly than other Nrf2 activators (Figure 5). Obviously, there are many other factors that can affect the integrity of mtDNA and other mitochondrial components.

We showed that Nrf2/ARE activators, in general, improve some parameters of the memory of 15-month-old mice where DMF and MB increased score numbers in short-term memory (Figure 2). We did not observe a direct improvement in the values of long-term memory, which may be induced by Nrf2/ARE activators. We showed that mice treated with DMF, MB, and RSV swam less distance before finding the platform compared with the control group of mice during reversal learning (Table 2, Figure 3). Reversal learning in the water Morris maze reveals whether or not animals can extinguish their initial learning of the platform’s position and acquire a direct path to the new goal position [47]. Thus, DMF, MB, and RSV allow for a more rapid switch away from the old goal location to the new goal compared with the control. However, by the 6th day after the change in platform position, the control mice showed the same result as the treated mice (Table 2, Figure 3).

Early on, it has been shown that RSV treatment significantly improved the learning and memory in rats with vascular dementia [48], rats with H-89-induced deficits on spatial memory [49], rats with bilateral common carotid artery occlusion [50], juvenile mice with high-calorie diet-induced memory dysfunction [51], and mice with AlCl3 and D-galactose-induced cognitive deficits [52]. Additionally, RSV improved learning, memory, and mood function among 25-month-old mice [53]. Nevertheless, a meta-analysis of 225 patients showed that RSV had no significant impact on factors related to memory and cognitive performance including learning ability, delayed recall, retention, and recognition [54]. DMF treatment alleviated long-term memory deficits induced by lipopolysaccharide [55] and prevented the disruption of spatial reference and working memory induced by streptozotocin [56]. The neuroprotective effect of MB began to be studied in the late 1970s when it was shown that MB acts as an inhibitory avoidance response [57]. Furthermore, it has been shown that MB administration during the memory consolidation period restored the memory retention impaired by the inhibition of cytochrome oxidase [58]. MB was associated with a 7% increase in correct responses during memory retrieval [59].

We hypothesize that the cognitive improvements that the studied compounds stimulate are due to a number of factors. We have studied several aspects of mitochondrial quality control in the brain. However, cognitive functions are closely related to other, at first glance, non-obvious factors. Recently, the gut–brain axis has been actively discussed. Communication between the gut microbiome and the cognitive function can occur via neuronal, immunological, and endocrine pathways. For example, it is well known that the microbiota regulates the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis [60]. PCR analysis showed that Nrf2/ARE activators impact the Firmicutes level (Table 6, Figure 8). Previously, it has been shown that the Firmicutes level was significantly correlated with higher scores on the cognitive test on neurologically-healthy older adult people [61].

Spearman’s rank correlation analysis showed a positive correlation between Verrucomicrobia level and cognitive function, and a negative correlation between Tenericutes level and cognitive function (Table S1). A positive correlation between Verrucomicrobia level and cognitive functions was noted for the neurologically-healthy older adult people [61]. Increases in Verrucomicrobia level suppress neurodegeneration [62]. Verrucomicrobia reverses cognitive dysfunction including impaired spatial working memory and recognition of new objects, restores brain metabolism [63], and alleviates memory impairment caused by high fat in mice [64]. Patients with cognitive impairment had a lower abundance of Tenericutes [65]. This is at odds with our data that showed a positive correlation between Tenericutes level and distance (rs = 0.474) and time (rs = 0.436) spent on the platform search in the water Morris maze (Table S1). Multivariate correlation analysis showed a negative correlation between scores of short-term memories and Epsilonproteobacteria level (R= −0.504) (Table S3). Not all bacteria in this phillum are pathogenic, but the increase in their number suggests that there may be damage to organs or organ systems, which leads to memory impairment [66].

We showed positive correlation between memory values and level of Roseburia, Oscillibacter, Christensenellaceae R-7, Negativibacillus, and Faecalibaculum genera (Table S2). It has earlier been shown that cognitive decline coincided with a decrease in Roseburia [66] and opposite memory improvement was associated with a significant increase in the abundance of Roseburia [67]. Improved cognitive function of APP/PS1 mice was associated with increase in Oscillibacter level [68]. Level of Faecalibaculum was also positively associated with cognitive function [68,69,70,71]. The role of Christensenellaceae R-7 and Negativibacillus in cognition has not been previously considered (Table S2).

We showed positive correlation between memory values and level of Streptococcus, Odoribacter, Acinetobacter, Clostridium, Dubosiella, Desulfovibrio, Prevotellaceae UCG-001, Tyzzerella, and UCG-002 level (Family Oscillospiraceae). Odoribacter level increased significantly in APP/PS1 [72] and aged [73] mice with impaired spatial learning. Desulfovibrio was also associated with memory decline, induced by a high-fat diet [74,75]. At the same time, some of our data are at odds with the previously obtained results. Increased Streptococcus was associated with probiotic-induced prevention of diet-induced memory deficits [76], while our data showed negative correlation with time that mice spent in a goal quadrant (Table S2). Additionally, decrease in Prevotellaceae was associated with memory deficits [77], while our data showed positive correlation with time that mice spent for the platform search (Table S2).

Some bacteria genera (Mycoplasma, Bilophila, Muribaculum) showed contradictory effects in the various values of memory search (Suppl. 2). Early, positive correlation between cognition and bacteria level were shown for Bilophila [78] and Muribaculum [71,79]. Previous studies have found no link between cognitive performance and Mycoplasma levels [80,81,82], which is partly confirmed by our contradictory data. It should be kept in mind that we have not shown a direct effect of drugs on the content of these bacteria genera (Table 7), therefore, we cannot say that the addition of Nrf2/ARE activators has a strong effect on the bacterial composition of the intestinal microbiota.

It is known that even non-antibiotic drugs have a notable impact on the overall architecture of the intestinal microbiome [83]. This implies the long-term treatment by drugs that are aimed at slowing down the processes of cognitive impairment, which can increase the degree of the effect of the drug on the intestinal composition of the microbiome. Recently, we found that treatment by a high concentration of MB (50 mg/kg/day) during one month induced dysbiosis, which manifested in an increase in Proteobacteria level [27]. Classically thought to be markers of dysbiosis in inflammatory bowel disease are increase in the Proteobacteria and Bacteroidetes phyla bacteria, decrease in the number of Firmicutes phylum bacteria, and decrease in the alpha-diversity index [84]. In this research (MB concentration 15 mg/kg/day for 3 months), we showed no changes in the Proteobacteria and Bacteroidetes level, and a tendency to increase in Firmicutes level (Table 6, Figure 8). However, MB-treated mice had a tendency decrease in alfa-diversity. Shannon index for MB group was 2.699 ± 0.169, while for the control group it was 3.063 ± 0.108; for the DMF group it was 3.100 ± 0. 069; for RSV it was 3.067 ± 0.023 (Figure 10). The ANOVA test showed that F (3, 22) = 2.9073, p = 0.0574. The post-hoc test did not show differences between the control and MB-treated groups, but there was a tendency toward a decrease in Shannon index (p = 0.0832). It is believed that normal diversity is when Shannon index ≥3.0 [85]. In the other groups of mice (control group and DMF, RSV groups), the Shannon index was more than 3.0 (Figure 10). MB mediated decrease of bacterial diversity in the gut microbiome also manifested in the decreasing number of predominant genera. Gut microbiome of MB-treated mice contained 11 genera with OTU of more than 0.02, while the control mice and RSV-treated mice contained 17 genera and DMF-treated mice contained 16 genera (Figure 11). Thus, these results and our previous data [27] show some evidence of MB-induced dysbiosis, but we cannot say that MB causes dysbiosis, since the other markers of dysbiosis are not observed. It is known that dysbiosis altered brain function and induced depressive-like behavior [86]. We also did not find that MB could increase stress levels or suppress exploratory behavior in mice (Table 1), though the PROVEPHARM SAS company, which manufactures ProVayBlue (MB-based FDA-approved drug for acquired methemoglobinemia) warns that MB can cause a confusional state and warns against driving or operating machines (Drugs FDA: FDA-Approved Drugs; New Drug Application (NDA): 204630) [87].

Figure 10.

Figure 10

Value of Shannon index (H) of alfa-diversity. The results are expressed as means ± SEM. Control n = 8, DMF = 6, MB = 6, RSV = 6.

Figure 11.

Figure 11

Predominant genera in the gut microbiome. Control n = 8, DMF = 6, MB = 6, RSV = 6.

In contrast, not one marker of dysbiosis was shown for the DMF and RSV treated groups (Table 6 and Table 7). Previously, the pilot study did not detect a major effect of DMF on the gut microbiota composition [28]. RSV, in contrast, had a prebiotic effect on bacteria, which could have a positive effect on body weight and fat mass [88,89].

Thus, we can conclude that DMF, MB, and RSV can activate Nrf2/ARE signaling, which leads to an enhancement of mitochondrial biogenesis, mitophagy, and antioxidant defense in the hippocampus of aged mice. This is manifested in the improvement of short-term and long-term memory during normal aging-associated cognitive deterioration. We cannot directly extrapolate these results to cognitive dysfunction and dementia, which can be caused by neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, learning behavior disorder, progressive supranuclear palsy, frontotemporal dementia and others, which have specific pathogenetic mechanisms that differ from normal aging. These results expand our understanding of the effect of Nrf2/ARE activators on mitochondrial quality control, its relationship with the change in the composition of the gut microbiome, and improvement in memory in health aged mice.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Animals

Fifteen-month-old mice (C57Bl/6 strain) were used in the experiment. Mice were obtained from Stolbovaya Nursery (Moscow region, Russia) and kept under standard conditions of vivarium at t = 25 °C, relative humidity of at least 40%, and 12-h light/dark cycle. Standard laboratory diet (Ssniff Spezialdiaten GmbH, Soest, Germany) and drinking water were available ad libitum. Mice were divided into four groups: (1) control group (mice received pure water, n = 8); (2) DMF group (mice received 10 mg/kg/day DMF dissolved in water, n = 9); (3) MB group (mice received 15 mg/kg/day MB dissolved in water, n = 6); and (4) RSV group (mice received 20 mg/kg/day RSV dissolved in water, n = 8). All chemicals were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO, USA). All experiments with animals were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Voronezh State University Ethical Committee on Biomedical Research (Animal Care and Use Section, protocol N42-01a dated 16 March 2020).

Mice received the compound for 50 days. On the 50th day of the experiment, an open field test was carried out. On the 52nd day of the experiment, a dark–light box test was carried out. On the 54th day of the experiment, an elevated-plus-maze test was carried out. On the 56th day of the experiment, a string test was carried out. Water Morris test for assessing long-term memory was performed between days 57 and 68 days. Morris water-maze test for assessing short-term memory was performed between days 69 and 89 days. On the 90th day of the experiment, mice feces were collected for microbiome analysis. Further mice were sacrificed at the age of 18 months.

Mice were sacrificed by a mixture (1 mL/kg) of xylazine (10 mg/kg) and ketamine (90 mg/kg) administered intraperitoneally. Afterward, mice were decapitated and brains were dissected. In this research, we used hippocampus compartment for biochemical and expression analysis. A MtDNA study was conducted on the hippocampus, forebrain, and ventral mid-brain.

4.2. Physiological Tests

For assessment of anxiety-related and exploratory behavior, we used an open field test. The mice were placed in the corner of the open arena (60 × 60 × 40 cm) with five randomly distributed holes (0.5 cm diameter) for 5 min. We evaluated locomotor activity of mice (s), time spent in the center (s), number of exits to the center, number of hole-pocking acts, the number of rearing acts and defecation acts, the duration (s), and the number of grooming acts.

For assessment of anxiety-related behavior, we used a dark–light box. A box contained two compartments. The light compartment (24 × 20 × 25 cm) took up 2/3 of the device and did not have a lid, while the dark compartment (12 × 20 × 25 cm) took up 1/3 and was covered with a lid. The compartments were connected by a door (4 × 5 cm). The animal was placed in the light compartment and had the ability to move freely between the compartments for 5 min. We evaluated the time spent in the light or dark part of the chamber, and the number of transitions between compartments.

For exploratory behavior assessment, the elevated plus-maze test was used. The maze contained two open arms (30 × 5 cm) and two enclosed arms (30 × 5 × 15 cm). Each mouse was placed on the crossing arms and the 5 min test session was initiated. We evaluated the time spent in the open arms.

For assessment of the strength and endurance of mice, we used the string test. The mice, holding their forelimbs, were suspended on a string 50 cm long at a height of 50 cm above the surface. A cushion underneath was also provided to prevent injury to falling mice. Next, the mice were rated according to the following criteria: 1—hang by two forepaws; 2—attempt to climb onto the string; 3—two forepaws + one or both hind paws; and 4—four paws + tail around the string. If the mice fell, they received from 0 to 0.9 points (depending on a time of falling). For example, a mouse received 0.25 points if it fell in 15 s of the trial or 0.5 points if it fell in 30 s of trial. If a mouse reached the end of the string, it received from 5 to 5.9 points (depending on a time of escape). For example, a mouse received 5.75 points if it escaped in 15 s of the trial or 5.5 points if it escaped in 30 s of the trial. For each mouse, there were two trials with 30 min between trials.

4.3. Morris Water Maze

To study the short-term and long-term memory of the mice, the Morris water maze was used. The experiment was performed in a round pool (154 cm diameter) with a round platform (15 cm diameter) in the target quadrant. The pool was filled with colored water so that the platform was hidden below the water surface (0.5 cm). The study of long-term memory took place in two stages: learning and probe. During learning, the animals were trained for five days and four trials, and each animal had to remember the location of the platform. On day 6 (probe), each animal was given one trial to reach the platform under 1 min. The second stage (reversal) of the study of reference memory includes the same stages as the first, with the only difference being that the platform should be located in the opposite quadrant of the arena. On day 12, each animal was given one trial to reach the platform under 1 min. To study the long-term memory, the following parameters were measured: distance from the starting point to platform (cm); time that mice spent for the platform search (s); and the time the animal spent in the quadrant with the platform (s).

To study short-term memory, the animal was given only two attempts with an interval of 15 s in order to find the platform. The study lasted 21 days. The difference in time (s) to reach the platform is an important indicator. The level of short-term memory was expressed in scores, which were calculated according to Equation (1) when latency of the second trial was more than the latency of the first trial and according to Equation (2) when the latency of the second trial was less than the latency of the first. When a mouse did not find the platform, it received 0 scores.

Score=1(2nd trial1001st trial1001) (1)
Score=1+(2nd trial1001st trial100) (2)

4.4. DNA and RNA Isolation

Total DNA from the brain compartment was isolated using the Proba-GS Kit (DNA-Technology, Moscow, Russia) according to the protocol. Isolation of total RNA was performed using the ExtractRNA Kit (Evrogen, Moscow, Russia) according to the protocol. Qualitative analysis of DNA and RNA was carried out using electrophoresis in 2% agarose gel in 1× TAE-buffer.

4.5. Measurement of mtDNA Copy Number

The amount of mtDNA copy number was estimated by quantitative PCR of the mtDNA fragment using the Bio-Rad CFX96TM Real-Time System and 1× qPCR mix-HS SYBR kit (Evrogen, Russia). The mtDNA primer sequences were as follows:

F: 5′-ACGAGGGTCCAACTGTCTCTTA-3′;

R: 5′-AGCTCCATAGGGTCTTCTCGT-3′.

The Gapdh and 18s rRNA genes of nuclear DNA were used as a reference. The primer sequences were as follows:

Gapdh—F: 5′-GGCTCCCTAGGCCCCTCCTG-3′;

R: 5′-TCCCAACTCGGCCCCCAACA-3′;

18s—F: 5′-CGGCTACCACATCCAAGGAA-3′;

R: 5′-GCTGGAATTACTGTGGCT-3′.

qPCR cycling conditions were: initial denaturation at 95 °C for 3 min followed by 35 cycles: denaturation 95 °C for 10 s, primer annealing at 59 °C for 30 s, and elongation at 72 °C for 30 s.

Normalized mtDNA level relative to nuclear DNA was calculated using standard Equation (3):

mtDNA level=2(ΔΔCq) (3)

4.6. mtDNA Damage Measurement

The amount of mtDNA damage was estimated by quantitative long-range PCR. Each PCR reaction contained 1× of Encyclo polymerase, 1× Encyclo buffer, 0.2 mM of each dNTP (all Evrogen, Russia), 1× SYBR GreenMasterMix (BioDye, Moscow, Russia), and a mix of forward and reverse primers in a total volume of 20 μL. qPCR cycling conditions were: initial denaturation at 95 °C for 3 min; 35 cycles: denaturation 95 °C for 30 s, primer annealing at 59 °C for 30 s, and elongation at 72 °C for 270 s.

The primers were designed previously [90]. The amount of mtDNA damage was calculated per 10,000 bp according to Equation (4).

mtDNA damage=1(2(ΔlongΔshort ))10000/fragment lenght (4)

where Δ long = Cq control − Cq experiment for the long fragment and Δ short = Cq control − cq experiment for the short fragment.

4.7. Gene Expression Analysis

RNA was used to obtain cDNA using MMLV reverse transcriptase (Evrogen, Russia) on an Eppendorf Mastercycler personal thermal cycler (Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany). qPCR cycling conditions were: initial denaturation at 95 °C for 3 min followed by 35 cycles: denaturation 95 °C for 10 s, primer annealing at 59 °C for 30 s, and elongation at 72 °C for 30 s. The primer sequences are presented in Table 8. For calculation of normalized expression, standard Bio-Rad CFX Manager software was used.

Table 8.

Primer sequences for expression measurement.

Gene Forward Primer 5′–3′ Reverse Primer 5′–3′
18s CGGCTACCACATCCAAGGAA GCTGGAATTACTGTGGCT
Gapdh GGCTCCCTAGGCCCCTCCTG TCCCAACTCGGCCCCCAACA
Ppargc1a ATGTGTCGCCTTCTTGCTCT CACGACCTGTGTCGAGAAAA
Sirt1 CTGTTTCCTGTGGGATACCTGACT ATCGAACATGGCTTGAGGATCT
FoxO1 GGGTCTGTCTCCCTTTCCTC TCAGTGGCATTCAGCAGGTA
Nfe2l2 CTCTCTGAACTCCTGGACGG GGGTCTCCGTAAATGGAAG
Nrf1 AGCACGGAGTGACCCAAA TGTACGTGGCTACATGGACCT
P62 GCCAGAGGAACAGATGGAGT TCCGATTCTGGCATCTGTAG
Pink1 GAGCAGACTCCCAGTTCTCG GTCCCACTCCACAAGGATGT
Gclc GCAGCTTTGGGTCGCAAGTAG TGGGTCTCTTCCCAGCTCAGT
Gpx AGTCCACCGTGTATGCCTTCT GAGACGCGACATTCTCAATGA
Txnr2 GATCCGGTGGCCTAGCTTG TCGGGGAGAAGGTTCCACAT
Prdx5 GGCTGTTCTAAGACCCACCTG GGAGCCGAACCTTGCCTTC
Sod2 CAGACCTGCCTTACGACTATGG CTCGGTGGCGTTGAGATTGTT
Hmox1 CACGCATATACCCGCTACCT CCAGAGTGTTCATTCGAGCA

4.8. Analysis of Gut Microbiome Using PCR

Bacteria in the mice feces were analyzed according to Yang et al. (2015) by quantitative PCR of the mtDNA fragment using a Bio-Rad CFX96TM Real-Time System and 1× qPCR mix-HS SYBR Kit (Evrogen, Russia). The content of bacteria of a particular phylum was determined using the Equation (5).

% bacteria=EUnivCqUnivESpecCqSpec × 100% (5)

where EUniv is PCR efficiency with the universal primers; ESpec is PCR efficiency with the phylum-specific primers; CqUniv is the number of quantitation cycle with the universal primers; and CqSpec is the number of quantitation cycle with the phylum-specific primers

4.9. Analysis of Gut Microbiome Using NGS

We selected the variable region V3 of the 16s rRNA gene to study the microbiome using sequencing on the Ion Torrent PGM. Bacterial DNA was amplified with the universal direct 337F forward primer and reverse 518R primer. The primer sequences were as follows:

337F: 5′-GACTCCTACGGGAGGCWGCAG-3′;

518R:5′-GTATTACCGCGGCTGCTGG-3′.

PCR was performed using a 5X ScreenMix-HS Master Mix (Evrogen, Russia) in the following regime: 94 °C for 4 min followed by 37 cycles of 94 °C for 30 s, 53 °C for 30 s, and 72 °C for 30 s with the final elongation at 72 °C for 5 min.

PCR products were purified with AMPure XP magnetic beads (Beckman Coulter, Brea, CA, USA) and used for constructing sequencing libraries using NEBNext Fast DNA Library Prep (New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA, USA) as recommended by the manufacturer. Barcoding was performed using Ion Xpress barcode adapters (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). Library DNA concentration was determined by qPCR using Library Quantification Kit Ion Torrent Platforms (Kapa Biosystems, Wilmington, MA, USA).

Sequencing was performed on the Ion Torrent PGM system using Ion PGM Hi-Q View Sequencing Kit, Ion OneTouch 2System, and Ion PGM Hi-Q View OT2 Kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Wilmington, MA, USA). Libraries were sequenced using an Ion Chip 318.

4.10. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using Statistica 10 (StatSoft. Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA). The results were expressed as means ± SEM. The results of the physiological tests and bacterial composition of gut microbiome were analyzed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Tukey’s post-hoc test was used to determine the significance level. Correlation analysis was performed using Spearman’s rank correlation (rs). For calculation of normalized expression and copy number of mtDNA, standard Bio-Rad CFX Manager software was used.

Sequencing results were obtained as binary alignment map (BAM) files that were converted into the FASTQ format using the SAMtools v.1.2 software. The reads were filtered according to the reading quality based on the expected number of errors using the maximum expected error cutoff of 1.0 [91]. The samples were pooled and unique sequences were identified before searching for the operational taxonomic units (OTUs). We searched for the OTUs using the UNOISE2 algorithm, which reduces the noise through error correction [92,93]. We combined all reads for all samples to generate OTUs and compile an OTU table.

Filtration of reads, identification of unique sequences, and clusterization in order to search for the OTUs were performed using either USEARCH v.10.0.240 or VSEARCH v.2.8.2 software. Microbial genus in the samples were identified using the SILVA database v.123 (https://www.arb-silva.de (accessed on 29 April 2021)).

Microbiota diversity was quantified using Shannon index (H) according Equation (6).

H=i=1Spi(lnpi) (6)

where pi is often the proportion of bacteria genus belonging to the ith species in the microbiome.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ph14070607/s1, Table S1: Correlation between bacterial composition of gut microbiome (PCR results) and values of memory; Table S2: Correlation between bacterial composition of gut microbiome (PCR results) and values of memory; Table S3: Multivariate correlation between bacterial composition of gut microbiome (PCR results) and values of memory; Table S4: Multivariate correlation between bacterial composition of gut microbiome (PCR results) and values of memory; Table S5: Open field results; Table S6: Dark-light box results; Table S7: Elevated plus maze results; Table S8: String test results; Table S9: Long-term memory. Distance; Table S10: Long-term memory. Time; Table S11: Long-term memory. Time in goal quadrant; Table S12: Short-term memory. Time; Table S13: Short-term memory. Scores; Table S14: Microbiome. PCR; Table S15: Microbiome. NGS.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.P.G. and V.N.P.; Methodology, I.S.S.; Software, D.A.I.; Validation, A.P.G., I.S.S. and V.N.P.; Investigation, M.V.G., E.P.K. and E.V.C.; Data curation, D.A.I.; Writing—original draft preparation, I.S.S. and A.P.G.; Visualization, I.S.S. and A.G.N.; Supervision, V.N.P.; Project administration, A.P.G. and V.N.P.; Funding acquisition, A.P.G. and V.N.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the scholarship of the President of the Russian Federation for young scientists and PhD Students (Project SP-2802.2021.4) to A.P.G.; by the RF Ministry of Science and Higher Education in the framework of the national project “Science” (Agreement 075-03-2020-088, Unique number of the register of State tasks 075001 × 39782002) to V.N.P.; and by the President grant for support of leading scientific school (Agreement NSh 2535.2020.11).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according with the guidelines of the Voronezh State University Ethical Committee on Biomedical Research (Animal Care and Use Section, protocol N42-01a dated 16 March 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

For studies not involving humans.

Data Availability Statement

Data available as Supplementary Materials.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Edwards G.A., III, Gamez N., Escobedo G., Jr., Calderon O., Moreno-Gonzalez I. Modifiable Risk Factors for Alzheimer’s Disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2019;11:146. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Terada S., Nakashima M., Wakutani Y., Nakata K., Kutoku Y., Sunada Y., Kondo K., Ishizu H., Yokota O., Maki Y., et al. Social problems in daily life of patients with dementia. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2019;19:113–118. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Yeu Y., Yoon Y., Park S. Protein localization vector propagation: A method for improving the accuracy of drug repositioning. Mol. Biosyst. 2015;11:2096–2102. doi: 10.1039/C5MB00306G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Xue H., Li J., Xie H., Wang Y. Review of Drug Repositioning Approaches and Resources. Int. J. Biol. Sci. 2018;14:1232–1244. doi: 10.7150/ijbs.24612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mrowietz U., Morrison P.J., Suhrkamp I., Kumanova M., Clement B. The Pharmacokinetics of Fumaric Acid Esters Reveal Their In Vivo Effects. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 2018;39:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2017.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cada D.J., Levien T.L., Baker D.E. Dimethyl fumarate. Hosp. Pharm. 2013;48:668–679. doi: 10.1310/hpj4808-668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kourakis S., Timpani C.A., de Haan J.B., Gueven N., Fischer D., Rybalka E. Dimethyl Fumarate and Its Esters: A Drug with Broad Clinical Utility? Pharmaceuticals. 2020;13:306. doi: 10.3390/ph13100306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schirmer R.H., Adler H., Pickhardt M., Mandelkow E. “Lest we forget you--methylene blue…”. Neurobiol. Aging. 2011;32:2325.e7–2325.e16. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Peter C., Hongwan D., Küpfer A., Lauterburg B.H. Pharmacokinetics and organ distribution of intravenous and oral methylene blue. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2000;56:247–250. doi: 10.1007/s002280000124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Skold A., Cosco D.L., Klein R. Methemoglobinemia: Pathogenesis, diagnosis, and management. South Med. J. 2011;104:757–761. doi: 10.1097/SMJ.0b013e318232139f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lu G., Nagbanshi M., Goldau N., Mendes Jorge M., Meissner P., Jahn A., Mockenhaupt F.P., Müller O. Efficacy and safety of methylene blue in the treatment of malaria: A systematic review. BMC Med. 2018;16:59. doi: 10.1186/s12916-018-1045-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01689246. [(accessed on 29 April 2021)]; Available online: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01689246.
  • 13.Hashweh N.N., Bartochowski Z., Khoury R., Grossberg G.T. An evaluation of hydromethylthionine as a treatment option for Alzheimer’s disease. Expert Opin. Pharmacother. 2020;21:619–627. doi: 10.1080/14656566.2020.1719066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03446001. [(accessed on 29 April 2021)]; Available online: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03446001.
  • 15.ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02380573. [(accessed on 29 April 2021)]; Available online: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02380573.
  • 16.Singh A.P., Singh R., Verma S.S., Rai V., Kaschula C.H., Maiti P., Gupta S.C. Health benefits of resveratrol: Evidence from clinical studies. Med. Res. Rev. 2019;39:1851–1891. doi: 10.1002/med.21565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lin H.S., Ho P.C. Preclinical pharmacokinetic evaluation of resveratrol trimethyl ether in sprague-dawley rats: The impacts of aqueous solubility, dose escalation, food and repeated dosing on oral bioavailability. J. Pharm. Sci. 2011;100:4491–4500. doi: 10.1002/jps.22588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT04314739. [(accessed on 29 April 2021)]; Available online: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04314739.
  • 19.Dinkova-Kostova A.T., Abramov A.Y. The emerging role of Nrf2 in mitochondrial function. Pt BFree Radic. Biol. Med. 2015;88:179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2015.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Gureev A.P., Shaforostova E.A., Popov V.N. Regulation of Mitochondrial Biogenesis as a Way for Active Longevity: Interaction between the Nrf2 and PGC-1α Signaling Pathways. Front. Genet. 2019;10:435. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2019.00435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Saji N., Murotani K., Hisada T., Tsuduki T., Sugimoto T., Kimura A., Niida S., Toba K., Sakurai T. The relationship between the gut microbiome and mild cognitive impairment in patients without dementia: A cross-sectional study conducted in Japan. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:19227. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55851-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zhao Y., Jaber V., Lukiw W.J. Secretory Products of the Human GI Tract Microbiome and Their Potential Impact on Alzheimer’s Disease (AD): Detection of Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) in AD Hippocampus. Front. Cell. Infect. Microbiol. 2017;7:318. doi: 10.3389/fcimb.2017.00318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Caputi V., Giron M.C. Microbiome-Gut-Brain Axis and Toll-Like Receptors in Parkinson’s Disease. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018;19:1689. doi: 10.3390/ijms19061689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cepeda M.S., Katz E.G., Blacketer C. Microbiome-Gut-Brain Axis: Probiotics and Their Association with Depression. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2017;29:39–44. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.15120410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kanji S., Fonseka T.M., Marshe V.S., Sriretnakumar V., Hahn M.K., Müller D.J. The microbiome-gut-brain axis: Implications for schizophrenia and antipsychotic induced weight gain. Eur. Arch Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2018;268:3–15. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017-0820-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chaplin A., Carpéné C., Mercader J. Resveratrol, Metabolic Syndrome, and Gut Microbiota. Nutrients. 2018;10:1651. doi: 10.3390/nu10111651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gureev A.P., Syromyatnikov M.Y., Ignatyeva D.A., Valuyskikh V.V., Solodskikh S.A., Panevina A.V., Gryaznova M.V., Kokina A.V., Popov V.N. Effect of long-term methylene blue treatment on the composition of mouse gut microbiome and its relationship with the cognitive abilities of mice. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0241784. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Storm-Larsen C., Myhr K.M., Farbu E., Midgard R., Nyquist K., Broch L., Berg-Hansen P., Buness A., Holm K., Ueland T., et al. Gut microbiota composition during a 12-week intervention with delayed-release dimethyl fumarate in multiple sclerosis—A pilot trial. Mult. Scler. J. Exp. Transl. Clin. 2019;5:2055217319888767. doi: 10.1177/2055217319888767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Li R., Jia Z., Zhu H. Regulation of Nrf2 Signaling. React. Oxyg Species. 2019;8:312–322. doi: 10.20455/ros.2019.865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Unni S., Deshmukh P., Krishnappa G., Kommu P., Padmanabhan B. Structural insights into the multiple binding modes of Dimethyl Fumarate (DMF) and its analogs to the Kelch domain of Keap1. FEBS J. 2021;288:1599–1613. doi: 10.1111/febs.15485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Raghunath A., Nagarajan R., Sundarraj K., Palanisamy K., Perumal E. Identification of compounds that inhibit the binding of Keap1a/Keap1b Kelch DGR domain with Nrf2 ETGE/DLG motifs in zebrafish. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 2019;125:259–270. doi: 10.1111/bcpt.13222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Farkhondeh T., Folgado S.L., Pourbagher-Shahri A.M., Ashrafizadeh M., Samarghandian S. The therapeutic effect of resveratrol: Focusing on the Nrf2 signaling pathway. Biomed. Pharmacother. 2020;127:110234. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2020.110234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zhao Y., Song W., Wang Z., Wang Z., Jin X., Xu J., Bai L., Li Y., Cui J., Cai L. Resveratrol attenuates testicular apoptosis in type 1 diabetic mice: Role of Akt-mediated Nrf2 activation and p62-dependent Keap1 degradation. Redox Biol. 2018;14:609–617. doi: 10.1016/j.redox.2017.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wen Y., Li W., Poteet E.C., Xie L., Tan C., Yan L.J., Ju X., Liu R., Qian H., Marvin M.A., et al. Alternative mitochondrial electron transfer as a novel strategy for neuroprotection. J. Biol. Chem. 2011;286:16504–16515. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M110.208447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Atamna H., Atamna W., Al-Eyd G., Shanower G., Dhahbi J.M. Combined activation of the energy and cellular-defense pathways may explain the potent anti-senescence activity of methylene blue. Redox Biol. 2015;6:426–435. doi: 10.1016/j.redox.2015.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tretter L., Horvath G., Hölgyesi A., Essek F., Adam-Vizi V. Enhanced hydrogen peroxide generation accompanies the beneficial bioenergetic effects of methylene blue in isolated brain mitochondria. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 2014;77:317–330. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2014.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gureev A.P., Shaforostova E.A., Laver D.A., Khorolskaya V.G., Syromyatnikov M.Y., Popov V.N. Methylene blue elicits non-genotoxic H2O2 production and protects brain mitochondria from rotenone toxicity. J. Appl. Biomed. 2019;17:107–114. doi: 10.32725/jab.2019.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gureev A.P., Syromyatnikov M.Y., Gorbacheva T.M., Starkov A.A., Popov V.N. Methylene blue improves sensorimotor phenotype and decreases anxiety in parallel with activating brain mitochondria biogenesis in mid-age mice. Neurosci. Res. 2016;113:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2016.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Satoh T., McKercher S.R., Lipton S.A. Reprint of: Nrf2/ARE-mediated antioxidant actions of pro-electrophilic drugs. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 2014;66:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Miller C.J., Gounder S.S., Kannan S., Goutam K., Muthusamy V.R., Firpo M.A., Symons J.D., Paine R., 3rd, Hoidal J.R., Rajasekaran N.S. Disruption of Nrf2/ARE signaling impairs antioxidant mechanisms and promotes cell degradation pathways in aged skeletal muscle. Biochim. Biophys. Acta. 2012;1822:1038–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Oh W.Y., Shahidi F. Antioxidant activity of resveratrol ester derivatives in food and biological model systems. Food Chem. 2018;261:267–273. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2018.03.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Picca A., Mankowski R.T., Burman J.L., Donisi L., Kim J.S., Marzetti E., Leeuwenburgh C. Mitochondrial quality control mechanisms as molecular targets in cardiac ageing. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 2018;15:543–554. doi: 10.1038/s41569-018-0059-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Scarpulla R.C. Transcriptional paradigms in mammalian mitochondrial biogenesis and function. Physiol. Rev. 2008;88:611–638. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00025.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Islam H., Hood D.A., Gurd B.J. Looking beyond PGC-1α: Emerging regulators of exercise-induced skeletal muscle mitochondrial biogenesis and their activation by dietary compounds. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2020;45:11–23. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2019-0069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kiyama T., Chen C.K., Wang S.W., Pan P., Ju Z., Wang J., Takada S., Klein W.H., Mao C.A. Essential roles of mitochondrial biogenesis regulator Nrf1 in retinal development and homeostasis. Mol. Neurodegener. 2018;13:56. doi: 10.1186/s13024-018-0287-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Han S., Jeong Y.Y., Sheshadri P., Su X., Cai Q. Mitophagy regulates integrity of mitochondria at synapses and is critical for synaptic maintenance. EMBO Rep. 2020;21:e49801. doi: 10.15252/embr.201949801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Vorhees C.V., Williams M.T. Morris water maze: Procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:848–858. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zhang Y., Li Y., Wang Y., Wang G., Mao L., Zhang D., Wang J. Effects of resveratrol on learning and memory in rats with vascular dementia. Mol. Med. Rep. 2019;20:4587–4593. doi: 10.3892/mmr.2019.10723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tabrizian K., Musavi S.S., Rigi M., Hosseindadi F., Kordi S., Shamshirgaran F., Bazi A., Shahraki J., Rezaee R., Hashemzaei M. Behavioral and molecular effects of intrahippocampal infusion of auraptene, resveratrol, and curcumin on H-89-induced deficits on spatial memory acquisition and retention in Morris water maze. Hum. Exp. Toxicol. 2019;38:775–784. doi: 10.1177/0960327119839160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shen D., Tian X., Sang W., Song R. Effect of Melatonin and Resveratrol against Memory Impairment and Hippocampal Damage in a Rat Model of Vascular Dementia. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2016;23:318–331. doi: 10.1159/000454681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Xu B.L., Zhang H., Ma L.N., Dong W., Zhao Z.W., Zhang J.S., Wang Y.L., Zhang X., Wang R. Resveratrol prevents high-calorie diet-induced learning and memory dysfunction in juvenile C57BL/6J mice. Neurol. Res. 2018;40:709–715. doi: 10.1080/01616412.2018.1471118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Labban S., Alghamdi B.S., Alshehri F.S., Kurdi M. Effects of melatonin and resveratrol on recognition memory and passive avoidance performance in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 2021;402:113100. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kodali M., Parihar V.K., Hattiangady B., Mishra V., Shuai B., Shetty A.K. Resveratrol prevents age-related memory and mood dysfunction with increased hippocampal neurogenesis and microvasculature, and reduced glial activation. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:8075. doi: 10.1038/srep08075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Farzaei M.H., Rahimi R., Nikfar S., Abdollahi M. Effect of resveratrol on cognitive and memory performance and mood: A meta-analysis of 225 patients. Pharmacol. Res. 2018;128:338–344. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2017.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Paraiso H.C., Kuo P.C., Curfman E.T., Moon H.J., Sweazey R.D., Yen J.H., Chang F.L., Yu I.C. Dimethyl fumarate attenuates reactive microglia and long-term memory deficits following systemic immune challenge. J. Neuroinflammation. 2018;15:100. doi: 10.1186/s12974-018-1125-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Majkutewicz I., Kurowska E., Podlacha M., Myślińska D., Grembecka B., Ruciński J., Plucińska K., Jerzemowska G., Wrona D. Dimethyl fumarate attenuates intracerebroventricular streptozotocin-induced spatial memory impairment and hippocampal neurodegeneration in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2016;308:24–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Martinez J.L., Jensen R.A., Vasquez B.J., McGuinness T., McGaugh J.L. Methylene blue alters retention of inhibitory avoidance responses. Physiol. Psychol. 1978;6:387–390. doi: 10.3758/BF03326744. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Callaway N.L., Riha P.D., Wrubel K.M., McCollum D., Gonzalez-Lima F. Methylene blue restores spatial memory retention impaired by an inhibitor of cytochrome oxidase in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;332:83–86. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00827-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rodriguez P., Zhou W., Barrett D.W., Altmeyer W., Gutierrez J.E., Li J., Lancaster J.L., Gonzalez-Lima F., Duong T.Q. Multimodal Randomized Functional MR Imaging of the Effects of Methylene Blue in the Human Brain. Radiology. 2016;281:516–526. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2016152893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Gareau M.G. Microbiota-gut-brain axis and cognitive function. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2014;817:357–371. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-0897-4_16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Manderino L., Carroll I., Azcarate-Peril M.A., Rochette A., Heinberg L., Peat C., Steffen K., Mitchell J., Gunstad J. Preliminary Evidence for an Association Between the Composition of the Gut Microbiome and Cognitive Function in Neurologically Healthy Older Adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017;23:700–705. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Wang L., Lu J., Zeng Y., Guo Y., Wu C., Zhao H., Zheng H., Jiao J. Improving Alzheimer’s disease by altering gut microbiota in tree shrews with ginsenoside Rg1. FEMS Microbiol. Lett. 2020;367:fnaa011. doi: 10.1093/femsle/fnaa011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Higarza S.G., Arboleya S., Arias J.L., Gueimonde M., Arias N. Akkermansia muciniphila and environmental enrichment reverse cognitive impairment associated with high-fat high-cholesterol consumption in rats. Gut Microbes. 2021;13:1–20. doi: 10.1080/19490976.2021.1880240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wu F., Guo X., Zhang M., Ou Z., Wu D., Deng L., Lu Z., Zhang J., Deng G., Chen S., et al. An Akkermansia muciniphila subtype alleviates high-fat diet-induced metabolic disorders and inhibits the neurodegenerative process in mice. Anaerobe. 2020;61:102138. doi: 10.1016/j.anaerobe.2019.102138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Zhang Y., Lu S., Yang Y., Wang Z., Wang B., Zhang B., Yu J., Lu W., Pan M., Zhao J., et al. The diversity of gut microbiota in type 2 diabetes with or without cognitive impairment. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2021;33:589–601. doi: 10.1007/s40520-020-01553-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Jeon W.J., Dong H.J., Shin J.H., Kim I.Y., Ho H., Oh S.H., Yoon Y.M., Choi Y.K., Suh J.G., Nam K.H., et al. Helicobacter apodemus sp. nov., a new Helicobacter species identified from the gastrointestinal tract of striped field mice in Korea. J. Vet. Sci. 2015;16:475–481. doi: 10.4142/jvs.2015.16.4.475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gu C., Zhou W., Wang W., Xiang H., Xu H., Liang L., Sui H., Zhan L., Lu X. ZiBuPiYin recipe improves cognitive decline by regulating gut microbiota in Zucker diabetic fatty rats. Oncotarget. 2017;8:27693–27703. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.14611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Xiao Y., Dong J., Yin Z., Wu Q., Zhou Y., Zhou X. Procyanidin B2 protects against d-galactose-induced mimetic aging in mice: Metabolites and microbiome analysis. Food Chem. Toxicol. 2018;119:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.fct.2018.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wang G., Zhou H.H., Luo L., Qin L.Q., Yin J., Yu Z., Zhang L., Wan Z. Voluntary wheel running is capable of improving cognitive function only in the young but not the middle-aged male APPSwe/PS1De9 mice. Neurochem. Int. 2021;145:105010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2021.105010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.D’Amato A., Di Cesare Mannelli L., Lucarini E., Man A.L., Le Gall G., Branca J., Ghelardini C., Amedei A., Bertelli E., Regoli M., et al. Faecal microbiota transplant from aged donor mice affects spatial learning and memory via modulating hippocampal synaptic plasticity- and neurotransmission-related proteins in young recipients. Microbiome. 2020;8:140. doi: 10.1186/s40168-020-00914-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Liu J., Yu C., Li R., Liu K., Jin G., Ge R., Tang F., Cui S. High-altitude Tibetan fermented milk ameliorated cognitive dysfunction by modified gut microbiota in Alzheimer’s disease transgenic mice. Food Funct. 2020;11:5308–5319. doi: 10.1039/C9FO03007G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Shen L., Liu L., Ji H.F. Alzheimer’s Disease Histological and Behavioral Manifestations in Transgenic Mice Correlate with Specific Gut Microbiome State. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017;56:385–390. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Scott K.A., Ida M., Peterson V.L., Prenderville J.A., Moloney G.M., Izumo T., Murphy K., Murphy A., Ross R.P., Stanton C., et al. Revisiting Metchnikoff: Age-related alterations in microbiota-gut-brain axis in the mouse. Brain Behav. Immun. 2017;65:20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2017.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wang S., Huang X.F., Zhang P., Wang H., Zhang Q., Yu S., Yu Y. Chronic rhein treatment improves recognition memory in high-fat diet-induced obese male mice. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2016;36:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2016.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Leigh S.J., Kaakoush N.O., Westbrook R.F., Morris M.J. Minocycline-induced microbiome alterations predict cafeteria diet-induced spatial recognition memory impairments in rats. Transl. Psychiatry. 2020;10:92. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0774-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Beilharz J.E., Kaakoush N.O., Maniam J., Morris M.J. Cafeteria diet and probiotic therapy: Cross talk among memory, neuroplasticity, serotonin receptors and gut microbiota in the rat. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:351–361. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Li W., Zhao Q., Wang J., Wang Y., Wen T. Dcf1 deletion presents alterations in gut microbiota of mice similar to Parkinson’s disease. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2020;529:1137–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2020.06.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sun Y., Tang Y., Xu X., Hu K., Zhang Z., Zhang Y., Yi Z., Zhu Q., Xu R., Zhang Y., et al. Lead exposure results in defective behavior as well as alteration of gut microbiota composition in flies and their offsprings. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2020;80:699–708. doi: 10.1002/jdn.10067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Leigh S.J., Kaakoush N.O., Bertoldo M.J., Westbrook R.F., Morris M.J. Intermittent cafeteria diet identifies fecal microbiome changes as a predictor of spatial recognition memory impairment in female rats. Transl. Psychiatry. 2020;10:36. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0734-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Swanepoel T., Harvey B.H., Harden L.M., Laburn H.P., Mitchell D. Simulated systemic recurrent Mycoplasma infection in rats induces recurrent sickness responses without residual impairment in spatial learning and memory. Physiol. Behav. 2012;105:800–808. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Swanepoel T., Sabbar M., Baartman T.L., Laburn H.P., Mitchell D., Dukhan T., Harden L.M. Simulated acute central Mycoplasma infections in rats induce fever, anorexia, body mass stunting and lethargy but spare memory. Physiol. Behav. 2016;163:294–304. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Swanepoel T., Harvey B.H., Harden L.M., Laburn H.P., Mitchell D. Dissociation between learning and memory impairment and other sickness behaviours during simulated Mycoplasma infection in rats. Brain Behav. Immun. 2011;25:1607–1616. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Le Bastard Q., Al-Ghalith G.A., Grégoire M., Chapelet G., Javaudin F., Dailly E., Batard E., Knights D., Montassier E. Systematic review: Human gut dysbiosis induced by non-antibiotic prescription medications. Aliment. Pharmacol. Ther. 2018;47:332–345. doi: 10.1111/apt.14451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Yin L., Wan Y.D., Pan X.T., Zhou C.Y., Lin N., Ma C.T., Yao J., Su Z., Wan C., Yu Y.W., et al. Association Between Gut Bacterial Diversity and Mortality in Septic Shock Patients: A Cohort Study. Med. Sci. Monit. 2019;25:7376–7382. doi: 10.12659/MSM.916808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Qiao Y., Sun J., Xia S., Tang X., Shi Y., Le G. Effects of resveratrol on gut microbiota and fat storage in a mouse model with high-fat-induced obesity. Food Funct. 2014;5:1241–1249. doi: 10.1039/c3fo60630a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Rogers G.B., Keating D.J., Young R.L., Wong M.L., Licinio J., Wesselingh S. From gut dysbiosis to altered brain function and mental illness: Mechanisms and pathways. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:738–748. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Drugs FDA: FDA-Approved Drugs. New Drug Application (NDA): 204630. [(accessed on 29 April 2021)]; Available online: https://www.accessdata.fda.gov/scripts/cder/daf/index.cfm?event=overview.process&varApplNo=204630.
  • 88.Danilova N.A., Abdulkhakov S.R., Grigoryeva T.V., Markelova M.I., Vasilyev I.Y., Boulygina E.A., Ardatskaya M.D., Pavlenko A.V., Tyakht A.V., Odintsova A.K., et al. Markers of dysbiosis in patients with ulcerative colitis and Crohn’s disease. Ter. Arkh. 2019;91:17–24. doi: 10.26442/00403660.2019.04.000211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Jung M.J., Lee J., Shin N.R., Kim M.S., Hyun D.W., Yun J.H., Kim P.S., Whon T.W., Bae J.W. Chronic Repression of mTOR Complex 2 Induces Changes in the Gut Microbiota of Diet-induced Obese Mice. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:30887. doi: 10.1038/srep30887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Gureev A.P., Shaforostova E.A., Starkov A.A., Popov V.N. Simplified qPCR method for detecting excessive mtDNA damage induced by exogenous factors. Toxicology. 2017;382:67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tox.2017.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Yang Y.W., Chen M.K., Yang B.Y., Huang X.J., Zhang X.R., He L.Q., Zhang J., Hua Z.C. Use of 16S rRNA Gene-Targeted Group-Specific Primers for Real-Time PCR Analysis of Predominant Bacteria in Mouse Feces. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 2015;81:6749–6756. doi: 10.1128/AEM.01906-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Edgar R.C., Flyvbjerg H. Error filtering, pair assembly and error correction for next-generation sequencing reads. Bioinformatics. 2015;31:3476–3482. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btv401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Edgar R.C. Search and clustering orders of magnitude faster than BLAST. Bioinformatics. 2010;26:2460–2461. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

Data available as Supplementary Materials.


Articles from Pharmaceuticals are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES