Abstract
Smartphone addiction (SPA) affects an increasing number of college students, but it remains poorly understood. This study aimed to explore the psychological mechanism of the decrease in cognitive control ability from cognitive psychology in students with SPA.
The smartphone addition tendency scale (MPATS) was used to identify 64 undergraduates with SPA (MPATS > 45) and 64 sex- and age-matched controls (MPATS < 30).
The 2 groups were well matched for age and sex distribution. The RT of the SPA group under the conflict state was 628.2±59.2 ms vs 549.4 ± 44.2 ms under the consistent state (P < .005). The RT of the control group under the conflict state was 707.5 ± 66.4 vs 582.0 ± 39.4 ms under the consistent state (P < .005). Under the conflict state, errors made by the SPA group were 8.7 ± 5.4, and that of the control group was 6.6 ± 3.7 (P < .05). The reaction delay of the SPA group was 25.6 ± 49.2 vs 110.0 ± 41.8 ms (P < .05).
Correlation analysis showed a clear positive correlation between SPA and the number of mistakes in the conflict state of the Stroop task.
Keywords: cognition, college students, smartphone addiction, stroop effect
1. Introduction
In the network ecological environment, smartphone surfing has become the daily routine of college students. The 45th “Statistical Report on Internet Development in China” shows that as of March 2020, the number of Internet users in China has reached 904 million, with Chinese teenagers accounting for the most significant number, accounting for 26.9%, among which college students accounted for 19.5%.[1] Smartphones are the primary source of internet access for college students. SPA causes problems such as communication barriers, learning and cognition impairments, interpersonal communication obstacles, fragmentation of social relations, and has become an essential factor affecting the development of the social psychology of the individuals affected by SPA.[2–5]
“Smartphone syndrome,” “smartphone dependency,” “smartphone addiction syndrome,” “smartphone anxiety neurosis,” and “smartphone addiction” are all descriptions of the excessive use of smartphones. To a certain extent, the over-reliance on smartphones is consistent with classical “behavioral addictions.”[6,7] Psychiatrists believe that SPA is an “excessive use” behavior, having various adverse effects on physical and mental health as well as the learning progress.[5,8] Psychologists believe that SPA is an “addictive behavior” that causes excitement and pleasure as an extraordinary hobby and habit.[5,6]
The Stroop effect was discovered by the American psychologist John Riddley Stroop in 1935.[9] Stroop merged the 2 stimulus dimensions of word and object (color) into a single whole, creating a comic response conflict scenario. Word recognition and font color recognition are 2 different cognitive processes. The information processing rates of these 2 processes differ. Word reading is always faster than color recognition so that the meaning of the words will be dealt with first. When the color of the word is the same as the meaning of the word, recognition of font color will be promoted; otherwise, font color recognition will be impeded. Therefore, the speed of font color recognition differs, and this difference is the Stroop effect.[10] Stroop tasks are used to investigate the cognitive control status of the subjects through the Stroop effect. In cognitive control, color-word Stroop tasks are commonly used to study the neural mechanism of conflict processing.[11–13] The so-called cognitive control is also called executive control. It refers to the process by which individual stores, plans, and manipulates the relevant information from top to bottom according to the objectives of the current task. Cognitive control ability plays an important role when people need to overcome automatic behavior or the current task is unfamiliar, dangerous, or requires planning and decision-making.[14–17]
In this study, the Stroop effect was used for the cognitive-behavioral measurement of SPA among college students. This research aimed to explore the psychological mechanism of the cognitive control ability from the perspective of cognitive psychology in college students with SPA. The results might provide a scientific basis for moderate intervention and reasonable treatment.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Study design and subjects
This prospective case--control study of students studying at Shaanxi universities was recruited between September 2016 and August 2017. The study was approved by the ethics committee of The First Affiliated Hospital of Xi’an Jiaotong University. All subjects signed the informed consent form.
For the SPA group, the SPA Tendency Scale (MPATS)[18] was used. It uses 5-point scoring for 16 items of 4 dimensions, including abstinence symptoms, prominence behavior, social comfort, and mood alteration. The reliability is 0.834, and the validity coefficient is >0.9. Loads of each factor are 0.51 to 0.79. The cumulative variance contribution rate is 54.3%. Confirmatory factor analysis showed that the 4-factor model in the scale fitted well. The Cronbach α coefficient of the entire scale is 0.83. The Cronbach α coefficients of the 4 factors are between 0.55 and 0.80. The retest reliability of the total scale is 0.91. The retest reliability of the 4 factors is 0.75 to 0.85.[18–20] The questionnaire (Table 1) was filled in a quiet environment. The reliability of the questionnaire was confirmed through phone communication with their parents and by asking classmates in the same dormitory whether the subjects often used smartphones for internet access and whether the roommates were disturbed. The subjects were considered to be SPA when the MPATS score was ≥45.
Table 1.
1. I tend to check for missed calls or messages immediately after being without my smartphone for a while. |
2. I prefer chatting over the smartphone to communicating face to face. |
3. While waiting for others, I always make frequent phone calls to check where they are. Otherwise, I feel anxious. |
4. I feel uncomfortable if not using a smartphone for a while. |
5. I cannot concentrate in class because of phone calls or messages. |
6. I feel lonely in the absence of a smartphone. |
7. I feel more confident when communicating with others over the phone. |
8. I feel uncomfortable after a while without a phone ring and will involuntarily check for missed calls/messages. |
9. I often imagine that my phone is ringing/vibrating. |
10. I feel more satisfied when there are more phone calls and messages. |
11. I am often afraid of the automatic shutdown of my smartphone. |
12. A smartphone is a part of me. In its absence, I feel like I have lost something. |
13. My classmates and friends often say that I am too dependent on the smartphone. |
14. When the smartphone loses internet access or cannot receive signals, I become anxious and irritable. |
15. I tend to focus on the smartphone in class, which affects the lectures. |
16. I feel more comfortable communicating with others over the smartphone. |
For the control group, a total of 64 normal subjects were included. They were matched for sex, age, and education level with the SPA group. The scores of the 64 normal subjects in the MPATS test were <30. During the interview, the subjects used smartphones for internet access <3 times.
2.2. Demographic data
The demographic data of all subjects were collected, including sex, age, years of education, hours online every day (hours/day), days online every week (days/week), the score of the MPATS test, and years using the internet (years).
2.3. Collection of the behavioral data
The Stroop task in this study included three colors (red, blue, and green). There were 2 states in this task, the consistent state when the word and the color were consistent, and the conflict state when the word and the color were inconsistent (Fig. 1). The E-prime software was used to perform the Stroop task. Two runs were included in this experiment. Four consistent states, 4 inconsistent states, and 9 resting states of blocks were included in each run. There were 7 trials in each task-related block. Each stimulus lasted for 1 second, and the interval between stimuli was 2 seconds. Therefore, blocks related to each task lasted 21 secondse. All blocks under the resting state lasted 17 seconds, except for the first one that lasted 19 seconds. Before each block, there was a “color recognition” instruction for all subjects. All instructions lasted 2 seconds. All runs lasted 367 seconds in total. All subjects were told to press the keys using the index finger, middle finger, and ring finger, respectively. They were required to press keys in response to font color. The order of the keys was red, blue, and green. During the transition, the subjects were required to watch the white cross on the screen. In order to ensure data quality, the subjects performed the behavioral tests in quiet rooms.
2.4. Analysis of the behavioral data
The indexes included the reaction time (RT) under conflict state, RT under consistent state, errors under consistent state, errors under conflict state, and reaction delay. The time of conflict state was the RT under conflict state, and the time of consistent state was the RT under consistent state. The Stroop effects of the 2 groups were then calculated. Reaction delay = RT under conflict state - RT under consistent state.
2.5. Statistical analysis
SPSS 19.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY) was used for statistical analysis. Continuous variables were presented as means ± standard deviations and analyzed using the Student t test. Categorical variables were presented as numbers and percentages and analyzed using the Chi-square test. The Pearson Correlation was used to examine the correlations among two variables. Two-sided P-values <.05 were considered statistically significant.
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of the subjects
As summarized in Table 2, there was no significant difference in sex, age, and years of education between the 2 groups (all P > .05). Indexes including hours on the internet per day, days on the internet per week, MPATS, and years using the internet were all significantly higher in the SPA group than in the control group (all P < .005).
Table 2.
Items (unit) | SPA (n = 64) | Control (n = 64) | P |
Age, yr | 20.3 ± 1.2 | 19.8 ± 2.2 | >.05 |
Sex (male) | 42 (53.8%) | 38 (48.7%) | >.05 |
Years of education, yr | 13.3 ± 0.6 | 14.2 ± 0.3 | >.05 |
Hours on internet per day, h/day | 8.6 ± 1.7 | 2.6 ± 0.4 | <.005 |
Days on internet per week, days/wk | 6.2 ± 1.0 | 2.3 ± 0.7 | <.005 |
Smart phone addition tendency scale (MPATS) | 64.4 ± 10.3 | 26.6 ± 3.2 | <.005 |
Years using internet, yr | 6.0 ± 2.7 | 3.3 ± 1.9 | <.005 |
3.2. Behavioral test results
As summarized in Table 3, the RT of the SPA group under the conflict state was 628.2 ± 59.2 vs 549.4 ± 44.2 ms under the consistent state (P < .005). The RT of the control group under the conflict state was 707.5 ± 66.4 vs 582.0 ± 39.4 ms under the consistent state (P < .005). Under the conflict state, errors made by the SPA group were 8.7 ± 5.4, and that of the control group was 6.6 ± 3.7 (P < .05). The reaction delay of the SPA group was 25.6 ± 49.2 ms, and that of the control group was 110.0 ± 41.8 ms (P < .05).
Table 3.
Items | SPA | Control | ||
Consistent state | Conflict state | Consistent state | Conflict state | |
Reaction time, ms | 549.4 ± 44.2 | 628.2 ± 59.2 | 582.0 ± 39.4 | 707.5 ± 66.4 |
Reaction errors (number) | 3.2 ± 2.4 | 8.7 ± 5.4 | 3.0 ± 2.0 | 6.6 ± 3.7 |
Reaction delay, ms | 78.9 ± 45.4 | 125.6 ± 49.2 |
3.3. Correlation between the degrees of SPA and Stroop task performance
Correlation analysis showed a clear positive correlation between SPA and the number of mistakes in the conflict state of the Stroop task. The correlation coefficient was 0.549 (P < .005) (Table 4).
Table 4.
SPA | Stroop task | |
Degrees of SPA Pearson Correlation | 1 | 0.549 |
P | .002 | |
N | 64 | 64 |
Stroop task Pearson Correlation | 0.549∗ | 1 |
P value | 0.002 | |
N | 64 | 64 |
4. Discussion
SPA is affecting an increasing number of college students,[2–5] but it remains poorly understood. Therefore, this study aimed to explore the psychological mechanism of the decrease in cognitive control ability from cognitive psychology in students with SPA. The results suggest that the cognitive control ability of undergraduates with SPA was impaired.
The undergraduate period is a relatively particular developmental stage, accompanied by multiple changes in psychology, physiology, and social attributes.[21] The relatively immature cognitive control ability during this period may make undergraduate students inadaptable to society and vulnerable.[21] Therefore, the risks for college students to face various affective disorders or addictions significantly increases.[22] Several studies showed that SPA had become a significant problem that seriously affects the development of college students’ or teenagers’ social psychology.[2–5,18,23–25]
Yali and Dandan[26] summarized the college students’ reasons for SPA: the specificity of the college students’ development and their personal characteristics, college students’ compliance with group norms, college students’ emotional transfer, and satisfaction of psychological needs. Skierkowski et al[27] suggested that personalities, self-esteem, loneliness, and sense of control greatly impacted SPA. Tang et al[11] found that self-control and vanity were the 2 major subjective factors leading to undergraduate SPA. Hong and Hongli[28] performed tests among college students using the MPATS, loneliness scale, and questionnaire of smartphone use motivation, and showed that loneliness was a vital factor for SPA. Thus, internal factors for college students’ SPA could be summarized as college students’ desire for information, group psychology of college students, college students’ pursuit of fashion, and college students’ emotion transfer.
Liu et al[16] analyzed multiple objective features of smartphones (open, interactive, suppositional, equal, fast, pluralistic, anonymous, instant), the influence of school lifestyle and educational mode, as well as family factors. Skierkowski et al[27] examined social communication according to demographic variables (age, sex, major, grade, and region). Haijun and Ronghui[29] established a linear regression model for analysis and concluded that 4 factors influenced SPA: network social demand factor, traditional social demand factor, self-management demand factor, and entertainment demand factor. Tang et al[11] chose social, environmental factors, and management factors, and showed that society, family, and school significantly impacted undergraduate SPA.
In the present study, behavioral measurement was performed for the cognitive control ability of SPA in college students from the perspective of cognitive psychology. Color-word Stroop tasks were performed for quantitative measurement. The results showed apparent Stroop effects in both the SPA and control groups. Under the conflict state of the Stroop task, the RT of the SPA group was significantly longer than that of the control group, and more errors were made than in the control group. In addition, there was a positive correlation between SPA and the number of errors made under the conflict state of the Stroop tasks. These results strongly suggest that the cognitive control ability of college students with SPA was decreased, and their cognition levels also declined.
This study has limitations. First, the number of subjects was relatively small. In future studies, the sample size needs to be expanded. Second, when subjects were performing behavior tasks, the surrounding environment was not absolutely quiet, which might affect their final behavior. Finally, the dynamic tracking of students with SPA was not performed in this study, so the accuracy of the results was limited in relative time. Those limitations will be addressed in future studies.
In conclusion, using Stroop word-color tasks, the present study strongly suggests that the cognitive control ability of undergraduates with SPA was impaired. From this study, a more comprehensive and in-depth understanding of the impact mechanism of cognitive behavior of SPA was obtained, providing scientific guidance for future intervention and treatment to improve the social development of students with SPA.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: Xiaoli Ni.
Data curation: Ting Zhang, Na Gong, Rui Jia, Huian Li.
Formal analysis: Ting Zhang, Na Gong, Huian Li, Xiaoli Ni.
Investigation: Ting Zhang, Na Gong, Rui Jia, Huian Li.
Methodology: Ting Zhang, Na Gong, Rui Jia.
Project administration: Xiaoli Ni.
Writing – original draft: Ting Zhang, Na Gong, Rui Jia, Huian Li, Xiaoli Ni.
Writing – review & editing: Ting Zhang, Xiaoli Ni.
Footnotes
Abbreviations: MPATS = The smartphone addition tendency scale, RT = reaction time, SPA = Smartphone addiction.
How to cite this article: Zhang T, Gong N, Jia R, Li H, Ni X. Stroop effect in smartphone addiction among college students. Medicine. 2021;100:30(e26741).
Ting Zhang and Na Gong contributed equally to this article.
This work was supported by National Social Science Fund Project (No.19XXW008) and Humanities and Social Science Research Project of the Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China (No. 20YJA860020).
All authors declare that they have no competing interests.
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
P < .005.
References
- [1].Zhang H. From “knowledge Gap” to “Belief gap”: reconstruction of social relations in virtual space. Res Educ Media 2020;4:01. [Google Scholar]
- [2].Park CS. Examination of smartphone dependence: functionally and existentially dependent behavior on the smartphone. Computers Human Behav 2019;123–8. [Google Scholar]
- [3].Derevensky JL, Hayman V, Lynette G. Behavioral addictions: excessive gambling, gaming, Internet, and smartphone use among children and adolescents. Pediatr Clin North Am 2019;66:1163–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Fischer-Grote L, Kothgassner OD, Felnhofer A. Risk factors for problematic smartphone use in children and adolescents: a review of existing literature. Neuropsychiatr 2019;33:179–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Panova T, Carbonell X. Is smartphone addiction really an addiction? J Behav Addict 2018;7:252–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Grant JE, Potenza MN, Weinstein A, Gorelick DA. Introduction to behavioral addictions. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2010;36:233–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Starcevic V, Khazaal Y. Relationships between behavioural addictions and psychiatric disorders: what is known and what is yet to be learned? Front Psychiatry 2017;8:53–153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Punzi EH. Excessive behaviors in clinical practice--a state of the art article. Int J Qual Stud Health Well-being 2016;11:30055–130055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643–62. [Google Scholar]
- [10].Szucs D, Soltész F. Stimulus and response conflict in the color-word Stroop task: a combined electro-myography and event-related potential study. Brain Res 2010;1325:63–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Tang DD, Liu PD, Chen AT. A profile of conflict types in the color-word Stroop task. Adv Psychol Sci 2013;20:1962–70. [Google Scholar]
- [12].Purmann S, Pollmann S. Adaptation to recent conflict in the classical color-word Stroop-task mainly involves facilitation of processing of task-relevant information. Front Hum Neurosci 2015;9:88–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Damen TGE, Strick M, Taris TW, Aarts H. When conflict influences liking: the case of the Stroop task. PLoS One 2018;13:e0199700–1199700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Blasi G, Goldberg TE, Weickert T, et al. Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression. Eur J Neurosci 2006;23:1658–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Matsumoto K, Tanaka K. Neuroscience. Conflict and cognitive control. Science 2004;303:969–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Liu X, Nan W, Wang K. Modular organization of cognitive control. Adv Psychol Sci 2013;21:2091–102. [Google Scholar]
- [17].Soar K, Chapman E, Lavan N, et al. Investigating the effects of caffeine on executive functions using traditional Stroop and a new ecologically-valid virtual reality task, the Jansari assessment of Executive Functions (JEF(©). Appetite 2016;105:156–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Jie X, Zhou ZK, Wu C. Development of the mobile phone Addiction Tendency Scale for College Students. Chin Mental Health J 2012;2012:222–5. [Google Scholar]
- [19].Li G, Xie J, An L, et al. A generalizability analysis of the mobile phone addiction tendency scale for Chinese college students. Front Psychiatry 2019;10:241–1241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Hu Y, Long X, Lyu H, et al. Alterations in white matter integrity in young adults with smartphone dependence. Front Hum Neurosci 2017;11:532–1532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008;1124:111–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Steinberg L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Scientist 2010;24:40–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Dengliang H, Zhifei Q. Psychological analysis of college students’ “Mobile-phone addiction”. Contrmp Youth Res 2005;12:34–8. [Google Scholar]
- [24].Akin A, Altundag Y, Turan ME. The validity and reliability of the Turkish version of the smart phone addiction scale-short form for adolescent. Procedia Soc Behav Sci 2014;152:74–7. [Google Scholar]
- [25].Yuan X, Feng X. Research status of mobile phone demand and usage behavior in youth. China Youth Study 2011;4:78–81. [Google Scholar]
- [26].Yali L, Dandan L. Psychological craving sence of mobile phone dependency among college students. Psychol Res 2016;9:79–84. [Google Scholar]
- [27].Skierkowski D, Wood RM. The importance of text messaging among college-aged youth. Comput Hum Behav 2012;28:744–56. [Google Scholar]
- [28].Hong L, Hongli W. The relationship between college students’ mobile phone addiction and loneness, smart phone usage motivation. J Psychol Sci 2011;34:1453–7. [Google Scholar]
- [29].Haijun D, Ronghui Y. Empirical analysis of college students’ mobile phone dependence and demand. China Youth Study 2014;2014:15–9. [Google Scholar]