Abstract
Harmful algal blooms produce potent neurotoxins that accumulate in seafood and are hazardous to human health. Developmental exposure to the harmful algal bloom toxin, domoic acid (DomA), has behavioral consequences well into adulthood, but the cellular and molecular mechanisms of DomA developmental neurotoxicity are largely unknown. To assess these, we exposed zebrafish embryos to DomA during the previously identified window of susceptibility and used the well-known startle response circuit as a tool to identify specific neuronal components that are targeted by exposure to DomA. Exposure to DomA reduced startle responsiveness to both auditory/vibrational and electrical stimuli, and even at the highest stimulus intensities tested, led to a dramatic reduction of one type of startle (short-latency c-starts). Furthermore, DomA-exposed larvae had altered kinematics for both types of startle responses tested, exhibiting shallower bend angles and slower maximal angular velocities. Using vital dye staining, immunolabeling, and live imaging of transgenic lines, we determined that although the sensory inputs were intact, the reticulospinal neurons required for short-latency c-starts were absent in most DomA-exposed larvae. Furthermore, axon tracing revealed that DomA-treated larvae also showed significantly reduced primary motor neuron axon collaterals. Overall, these results show that developmental exposure to DomA targets large reticulospinal neurons and motor neuron axon collaterals, resulting in measurable deficits in startle behavior. They further provide a framework for using the startle response circuit to identify specific neural populations disrupted by toxins or toxicants and to link these disruptions to functional consequences for neural circuit function and behavior.
Keywords: domoic acid, harmful algal blooms, harmful algal bloom toxins, developmental toxicity, startle response, escape response, startle circuit
Harmful algal blooms (HABs) have increased in frequency, duration, and geographic extent in recent decades (Anderson et al., 2002). Some HABs produce potent toxins that contaminate drinking water, seafood, and air. One such toxin, domoic acid (DomA), is produced primarily by algae in the genus Pseudo-nitzschia. Increasing blooms of toxic Pseudo-nitzschia species, resulting in part from changing climatic conditions, have been documented in recent years on both the west and east coasts of the United States, where they have led to shellfish harvest closures (Bates et al., 2018; Clark et al., 2019; McKibben et al., 2017).
DomA is a structural analog of glutamate and is known to exert its toxicity by binding and activating ionotrophic glutamate receptors (Cendes et al., 1995; Hampson and Manalo, 1998). Exposure to DomA in adults occurs primarily through the consumption of contaminated seafood. To prevent acute toxicity to humans, a regulatory limit has been set at 20 mg of DomA per kg shellfish tissue (Wekell et al., 2010). However, this limit may not be protective for earlier life stages, which are more sensitive to DomA than adult stages (Doucette et al., 2004; Tryphonas et al., 1990; Xi et al., 1997).
DomA is an established developmental neurotoxin. It has been shown to cross the placenta, readily accumulate in amniotic fluids, and distribute to fetal brains in nonhuman mammals (Brodie et al., 2006; Lefebvre et al., 2018; Maucher and Ramsdell, 2007). This provides a means by which fetuses can continue to be exposed to DomA after it is cleared from the mother’s plasma (Brodie et al., 2006; Shum et al., 2020). Further exposures may occur in postnatal development through consumption of contaminated breast milk (Maucher and Ramsdell, 2005; Rust et al., 2014).
Although regulatory limits to protect shellfish consumers from acute toxicity have been established, developmental exposures to doses of DomA that do not lead to acute neurotoxic phenotypes (“asymptomatic doses”) can have long-term consequences. In rodents, both prenatal and postnatal exposures to these asymptomatic doses have been shown to lead to changes in neuron receptor densities (Perry et al., 2009), altered neural connectivity (Dakshinamurti et al., 1993; Mills et al., 2016), aberrant brain morphology (Bernard et al., 2007; Doucette et al., 2004), and ultimately behavioral deficits through adulthood. Following exposure to a range of DomA doses, behavioral deficits include impaired interlimb coordination (Shiotani et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2000), aberrant exploratory behavior (Levin et al., 2005; Shiotani et al., 2017; Tanemura et al., 2009), reduced socialization (Mills et al., 2016; Zuloaga et al., 2016), and the inability to cope with novel environments (Doucette et al., 2004; Perry et al., 2009). Many of these behavioral and histological phenotypes are latent and progressive, either appearing for the first time or increasing in severity as the animal ages. Developmental DomA toxicity also can be silent until unmasked by further challenges such as chemical exposures in adulthood, making the animals more susceptible to developing seizures and memory deficits (Gill et al., 2010; Levin et al., 2005; Tiedeken and Ramsdell, 2007).
Although behavioral deficits resulting from low-level, developmental exposure to DomA have been extensively characterized, the cellular and molecular mechanisms that underlie these behavioral effects are poorly understood. We recently showed that exposure to DomA during a specific period in zebrafish neurodevelopment (2 dpf) can lead to myelin sheath defects and aberrant startle behavior, supporting the use of zebrafish as a model for investigating the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying DomA-induced developmental neurotoxicity (Panlilio et al., 2020). Here, we build on these findings by using the well-characterized startle response circuit as a tool (Sive, 2011).
The startle response is an appropriate tool to investigate the behavioral effects of DomA exposure. Aberrant startle response patterns have been associated with myelination defects (Pogoda et al., 2006), which we identified as a feature of DomA toxicity (Panlilio et al., 2020). In addition, aberrant glutamate signaling is known to alter startle response kinematics (McKeown et al., 2012).
Startle responses in teleosts are elicited by sudden stimuli (auditory/vibrational, tactile, visual). Auditory/vibrational stimuli can lead to either of the 2 types of startle responses—a short-latency c (SLC)-type startle response or a long-latency c (LLC)-type startle response, which are distinguished by their onset time and kinematics. SLC responses occur shortly after exposure to a stimulus (about 15 ms or less, depending on temperature) and tend to lead to more pronounced bend angles, whereas LLCs occur later and produce shallower bend angles (Burgess and Granato, 2007b; Troconis et al., 2016). Increasing the intensity of auditory/vibrational stimuli biases fish to perform SLC responses, which requires the activation of a hindbrain neuron called the Mauthner cell (Eaton et al., 1977; Zottoli, 1977). Conversely, lower-intensity stimuli are more likely to lead to LLC startles, which are Mauthner cell-independent (Jain et al., 2018; Marquart et al., 2019).
The underlying startle response circuits are well known in zebrafish, making it is possible to link startle behavioral data to underlying structural and cellular targets. Auditory/vibrational stimuli are perceived by both the lateral line and hair cells in the inner ear which transmit information to the statoacoustic ganglia (Faber et al., 1989; Kohashi et al., 2012; Mirjany et al., 2011). For SLC startles, the statoacoustic ganglia cranial nerve synapses to the Mauthner cell in the hindbrain, which fires a single action potential that is propagated down its axons in the spinal cord and synapses to the primary motor neurons (Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Korn and Faber, 2005). The near-simultaneous activation of the primary motor neurons results in a ‘c’ shaped bend. In contrast to auditory/vibrational stimuli, direct-electric field stimulation bypasses the sensory system altogether, directly activating the Mauthner neurons and the downstream circuits (Tabor et al., 2014).
The goal of this study was to use this well-known startle response circuit as a tool to (1) determine which sensory and motor processes are disrupted by DomA exposure and (2) identify specific neuronal populations within the circuit that are affected by DomA.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Fish husbandry and lines used
These experiments were approved by the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution Animal Care and Use Committee (Assurance D16-00381 from the NIH Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare). Zebrafish (Danio rerio) embryos were maintained at 28°C–28.5°C with a 14:10 light-dark cycle in 0.3x Danieau’s medium (17.4 mM NaCl, 0.21 mM KCl, 0.12 mM MgSO4, 0.18 mM Ca(NO3)2•4H2O, 1.50 mM HEPES, pH 7.6). The following transgenic lines were used: Tg(mbp:EGFP-CAAX) (Almeida et al., 2011), Tg(sox10:RFP) (Kucenas et al., 2008), and Tg(mbp:EGFP) (gift from Dr Kelly Monk, generated by Dr Charles Kaufman in the laboratory of Dr. Leonard Zon, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts).
Generation of the Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX) line
The Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX) line was generated by Gibson assembly. Using previously published primers, the cntn1b promoter region was amplified from AB wild-type fish (Czopka et al., 2013). The cntn1b promoter was then assembled with EGFP-CAAX (cloned out of Tg(mbp:EGFP-CAAX) fish) into the vector backbone pkHR7, which contains ISCe-1 restriction sites (Hoshijima et al., 2016). To generate the stable line, the plasmid containing cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX was coinjected along with I-SceI in the 1–2 cell stage (Soroldoni et al., 2009; Thermes et al., 2002).
Domoic acid exposure paradigm
Domoic acid (D6152-5MG; Lot # SLBN1744V; ≥ 90% pure; Sigma-Aldrich, Missouri) stock solutions were prepared as we described earlier (Panlilio et al., 2020) and aliquots were stored at −20°C. Working solutions were prepared fresh prior to microinjection by diluting the stock in 0.2× Danieau’s to obtain the appropriate doses. Details on microinjection supplies and protocols were outlined previously (Panlilio et al., 2020).
DomA (nominal mass of 0.14 ng) was intravenously microinjected into the common posterior cardinal vein at around 48 53 hpf. The dose and timing of the exposure were chosen based on previous work that showed myelin defects and startle deficits (Panlilio et al., 2020). Controls from the same breeding clutch were injected with the saline vehicle (0.2× Danieau’s). To perform intravenous microinjections, fish were anesthetized with 0.10% w/v Tricaine mesylate (MS222) and placed laterally on dishes coated with 1.5% agarose (Cianciolo Cosentino et al., 2010). An injection was deemed successful if there was a visible displacement of blood cells and no disruption of the yolk during the injection process. Any fish that showed evidence of being incorrectly injected were removed from the study. Following injections, zebrafish were placed back in clean embryo media and monitored daily.
Measuring startle responses
Two different types of stimuli were used to elicit startle behavior: auditory/vibrational (A/V) and direct-electric field stimuli.
Auditory/vibrational (A/V) stimulation
A/V stimuli were generated by a minishaker (Brüel and Kjaer, Vibration Exciter 4810) connected to an amplifier (Brüel and Kjaer, Power Amplifier Type 2718) (Burgess and Granato, 2007a; Wolman et al., 2011). Larvae (6 dpf) were tested in a 16-well acrylic plate (40 × 40 mm) that rested in a petri dish (100 mm × 100 mm). The 16-well plate itself comprised of laser cut acrylic pieces that were fused together using acrylic cement (Weld-On #3; IPS) (Wolman et al., 2011). An LED backlight was placed below the dish to illuminate the well plate (Adafruit #1622). The petri dish was epoxied to a thumbscrew (#10-32 UNF threads) that was fitted to the base of the minishaker (Supplementary Figure 1A).
Groups of larvae (6 dpf) were subjected to 4 stimulus intensities (32, 38, 41, and 43 dB re 1 m/s2) that were given in increasing order (3-ms pulses). The software output frequency was 1000 Hz, though the shaker output of these pulses was more broadband, with variable peaks from 1 to 2000 Hz (Supplementary Figure 1B). Each stimulus intensity was given 4 to 7 times, spaced 20 ms apart to prevent habituation (Supplementary Table 1) (Wolman et al., 2011). For a given stimulus intensity, startle kinematics were shown to be statistically indistinguishable between the first and the fourth stimulus given, indicating that the 20 ms wait period was a sufficient interstimulus interval (Supplementary Figure 2). A high-speed video camera (Edgertronic, CA) was set at a 10% pretrigger rate to capture 13 frames prior to the stimulus being elicited, while recording larval movements at 1000 frames per second (Panlilio et al., 2020).
Measuring startle vibration
The startle vibration was measured as described earlier (Panlilio et al., 2020). Briefly, vibration was measured using a 3-axis accelerometer (PCB Piezotronics, W356B11). The accelerometer signal was low-pass filtered (10 kHz low-pass cutoff frequency) and amplified with a 30 dB gain before being digitized. Data were converted into acceleration units (m/s2) using manufacturer sensitivity values for each axis of the accelerometer and accounting for gain. Data were then band-pass filtered between 1 and 5000 Hz using an eighth order Butterworth filter. The Euclidian norm (vector sum) for the 3 acceleration signals was calculated to get the total 3D magnitude of acceleration. The maximum value (peak) of each impulse was calculated in decibels (dB) using the following equation:
where Lz-pk is the zero-to-peak acceleration level in dB re 1 m/s2, and x is the maximum acceleration level.
Direct-electric field stimulation
Larvae (7 dpf) were head-mounted in 35-mm glass-bottom dishes that were modified by gluing jumper wires to the edges of the dish to create a defined surface area for the electric field. Larvae were positioned rostral-caudally to the electrodes to provide the highest probability of eliciting a startle (Tabor et al., 2014). To head-mount, fish were anesthetized in MS222 (0.16%) and mounted upright (ventral side on the dish) using 1.5% low-melt agarose. Once the agarose solidified, an insect pin (size #000) was used to carefully carve out the agarose so that the region below the head was free to move. The dish was then flooded with embryo media (0.3× Danieau’s) and the fish were allowed to recover in a water bath heated to approximately 26°C. Using an “embryo poker” (a piece of 0.41 mm fishing line glued to a glass pipette tip), the fish’s tail was brushed lightly to confirm that it recovered from anesthesia prior to beginning the trial. A 4.4 V/cm, 2-ms square wave was generated by the stimulus generator, PulsePal (Sansworks, New York). This was delivered simultaneously as a TTL pulse that triggered the high-speed camera (Edgertronic, California), which recorded the tail movements at 1000 frames per second. Tail bend angle and latency were then tracked using the Flote software (Burgess and Granato, 2007b).
Startle behavioral analysis
High-speed videos were converted into jpeg files (.mov files with a minimal resolution of 720 × 720, 1/1008 shutter speed, and a frame rate of 1000 frames/s). To reduce the noise and tracking errors, the background was subtracted using a custom script in MATLAB (Panlilio et al., 2020). Flote software (Burgess and Granato, 2007b) was then used to analyze the jpeg files. To analyze startle in response to Auditory/vibrational stimuli, we used the ‘fourier’ analysis with the angle threshold set at 2, while filtering out fish that were swimming prior to the stimulus (selecting for ‘Ignore swimmers’) (Supplemental File 1) Quantitative attributes of the startle response measured include startle responsiveness (whether larvae responded or not), latency (time between stimulus and startle), bend angle, and maximal angular velocity during startle (Panlilio et al., 2020).
Using mixture models to identify latency cutoffs for SLC versus LLC responses
In response to auditory/vibrational stimuli, zebrafish perform 2 types of startle responses: short-latency c-bends (SLCs) and long-latency c-bends (LLCs) (Supplementary Figure 1C). These 2 types of startle responses have different latencies, distinct kinematics, and are driven by separate underlying neural circuits, making it necessary to distinguish the 2 startle types (Burgess and Granato, 2007a; 2007b; Higashijima et al., 2003; Marquart et al., 2019). To distinguish between the 2 types of startle responses, we identified a latency cutoff using a Gaussian mixture model, which fitted 2 Gaussian distributions, and assigned each latency data point a probability of belonging to either of the 2 distributions (R package, mixtools) (Benaglia et al., 2009; Panlilio et al., 2020). The cutoff for assigning a response as an SLC was 14 ms—the latency with a greater than 50% probability of belonging to the first fitted Gaussian distribution (Supplementary Figure 1D). Startle responses that had latencies greater than 14 ms were classified as LLCs.
Statistical modeling of startle responsiveness
Larvae were given 4–7 replicate auditory/vibrational stimuli or 7 replicate direct electric field stimuli per stimulus intensity tested. For all instances where a fish was successfully tracked, response rates were recorded. Response rates for individual fish were calculated (% responsiveness = number of times the fish responded/number of successfully tracked videos). For modeling responsiveness following auditory/vibrational stimuli, a mixed effects logistic regression model was then used to identify treatment differences in percent responsiveness, with the treatment as a fixed effect and the replicate experimental trials as a random effect (glmer(), lme4 R package) (Bates et al., 2015). To determine whether responsiveness differed by myelin severity score , a generalized linear model with a quasibinomial link function was used. Post-hoc pairwise Dunnett comparisons were then done (glht(), multcomp R package) (Hothorn et al., 2008).
Imaging myelin sheaths in the spinal cord following DomA exposures
Our previous results (Panlilio et al., 2020) showed pronounced myelin defects following exposures to DomA at 2 dpf. To determine whether there is a relationship between the behavioral findings and the myelin phenotype, Tg(mbp:EGFP-CAAX) embryos were exposed to DomA at 2 dpf and screened for myelin defects (at 5 or 6 dpf) prior to behavioral analyses at 6 or 7 dpf. The severity of the myelin defect was qualitatively scored (Supplementary Figure 3). To simplify the analyses, 4 categories were used: (0) Normal phenotype––dorsal and ventral regions had labeled myelin sheaths. The myelin sheath surrounding the Mauthner axon was visible. (1) Myelin sheaths were present but disorganized. In some cases, myelinated axons that were normally found ventrally were located more dorsally. In others, the myelinated axons terminated prematurely with distal ends located more dorsally. (2) Myelin was labeled in both the dorsal and ventral regions of the spinal cord, but there are some noticeable deficits. Although the ventral spinal cord was labeled, it had noticeably less myelin labeled compared with controls. (3) The loss of labeled myelin in the ventral spinal cord resulted in large, observable gaps between myelinated axons. Circular myelin membranes were present both in the ventral and dorsal spinal cord. For one out of the nine trials, larvae from the experiments performed in Panlilio et al., 2020 were used for behavioral analysis after they were scored for myelin defects in that study.
Statistical modeling of the distribution of startle response type due to treatment
When fish responded to a stimulus, they performed either an SLC or an LLC startle, which was defined by the latency cutoff of 14 ms. To determine whether DomA exposure alters the proportion of SLC to LLC responses, a mixed effects logistic regression model was used with treatment as a fixed effect and the replicate experimental trials as a random effect.
DomA-exposed fish were also classified by the severity of myelin defects. To determine whether there is an association between the myelin severity and the likelihood that a fish performs an SLC rather than an LLC, a separate mixed effects logistic regression model was used, with myelin severity as the fixed effect and the replicate experimental trials as a random effect.
Calculating the startle bias index
The Startle Bias Index was calculated as described previously (Jain et al., 2018). Bias per individual fish was calculated as the (frequency of SLC—frequency of LLC)/total responses where +1 represents the value in which all responses were SLC startles, and −1 represents the value in which all were LLC startles. Following this, mean behavioral biases for a treatment group were calculated.
Analysis of treatment differences in startle response kinematics
Kinematic responses from the 2 types of startle responses (SLC vs LLC) were analyzed separately based on previous research showing that they are driven by distinct neural circuits and have distinct kinematic characteristics (Burgess and Granato, 2007a; Marsden and Granato, 2015; O’Malley et al., 1996). The median response of individual fish for each startle type was then calculated for each intensity level (Panlilio et al., 2020).
To determine whether DomA alters kinematic responses to startle, we performed the Aligned Ranked Transformed ANOVA test, with the treatment group as a fixed factor (DomA vs control). Nine separate repeated experiments (trials) were combined for the analysis (art(), ARTool R package) (Wobbrock et al., 2011) (Supplementary Table 1). To account for potential differences in response due to the variations between trials, the experimental trial was incorporated into the model as a random factor.
To determine whether there was an association between the myelin phenotypes and the startle kinematics, we used a nonparametric multiple comparisons test with Dunnett-type intervals (nparcomp(), nparcomp R package) (Konietschke et al., 2015).
Immunohistochemistry
Fish were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight at 4°C. In some cases, whole brains were dissected, and in other cases, whole embryos were used. Antigen retrieval was done by placing tissue in 150 mM Tris HCl (pH 9.0) in a 70°C water bath for 15 min (Inoue and Wittbrodt, 2011). Brain tissue from 5 to 7 dpf larvae was permeabilized using proteinase K (10 μg/ml for 3 min) then post-fixed for 20 min using 4% paraformaldehyde. Whole-mount embryos (2.5 dpf) were permeabilized using ice-cold acetone (7 min). Samples were then blocked in 10% normal goat serum and 1% DMSO, followed by 1–3 day incubations in primary antibodies (α-3A10—1:100 dilution, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, antibody registry ID: AB 531874, prepared 7/28/16, 51 µg/ml; α-acetylated tubulin—1:500 dilution, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, SC 23950 sample HS417, 10 µg/50 ml). After several washes in phosphate-buffered saline with 0.01% triton-X, samples were then incubated in secondary antibodies (1:400 Alexa Fluor 488 Goat α-mouse, Catalog A11001, 1752514 or Alexa Fluor 594 Goat α-mouse; Abcam, GR196875). Samples were then placed in antifade mountant (Prolong or SlowFade Diamond mountant, Invitrogen), and placed between bridged #1.5 coverslips for imaging.
Anti-acetylated tubulin was used to assess primary neuron axons, early sensory neurons (Rohon-beard cells), and the peripheral lateral line, whereas anti-3A10 was used to characterize Mauthner cell bodies and hindbrain and midbrain axonal tracks. Brain dissections required for 3A10 staining sometimes led to reticulospinal axons (those that extend down to the spinal cord) being removed from the preparation. (Supplementary Figure 4 shows the range of phenotypes found in controls). Thus, for the antibody labeling experiment, the presence or absence of the Mauthner cell was determined solely based on the presence of the Mauthner cell body and lateral dendrite.
DASPEI labeling
Sensory neuromasts were labeled using the vital dye, DASPEI ((2-(4-(dimethylamino)styryl) -N-ethylpyridinium iodide, Biotium Inc., Freemont, California) (700018 Lot # 9D0403). Zebrafish larvae (5 dpf) were incubated in 0.005% DASPEI in embryo media (0.3× Danieau’s) for 15–20 min, then washed and placed in anesthetic (0.16% MS222 in 0.3× Danieau’s). Larvae were then mounted in low-melt agarose (1%–1.5%) either laterally or ventrally and imaged with a widefield microscope. Neuromasts were manually counted on blinded files. To identify treatment differences in counts, a random coefficient Poisson regression was done, with repeated experiments (trials) modeled as a random factor (glmer(), lme4 R package) (Bates et al., 2015).
Reticulospinal backfills
Larvae (7 dpf) were anesthetized using MS222 (0.16% in 0.3× Danieau’s), then mounted ventrally in 1.5% low-melt agarose. Dissection spring scissors were dipped into Texas red dextran (3000 MW) and the spinal cord was transected at the level of the anus (Moens et al., 1996). Larvae were placed back into anesthetic and screened for dye uptake using epifluorescence microscopy (Zeiss Axiovert inverted microscope). Roughly an hour after spinal cord transections, larvae were placed in 4% paraformaldehyde and fixed overnight in 4°C. Following several washes in PBS, the whole brain was dissected (Turner et al., 2014). Whole brains were mounted using the same technique used with brains that were labeled with antibodies. The presence of the Mauthner cell, along with any labeled neurons in rhombomere 5 (r5) and rhombomere 6 (r6)—the location of MiDcm2, MiDcm3—was identified. Following this classification, ordered logistic regression was done to determine whether treatment alters the number of neurons found in each location (polr(), Mass R package, R) (Venables and Ripley, 2002).
Primary motor neuron live imaging and tracing
Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX) embryos were exposed to DomA at around 48–53 hpf, and then imaged starting around 2.5 dpf. Embryos were anesthetized (0.16% MS222 in 0.3× Danieau’s), and embedded laterally in 1%–1.5% low-melt agarose (in 0.3× Danieau’s) in glass-bottom microscopy dishes. Embryos were imaged using a confocal microscope (LSM 710 or LSM 780) with the 40× water immersion objective (C-Apochromat, 40×, NA 1.1). Caudal primary motor neurons located approximately at myotome 20–23 were imaged. Images were blinded prior to image analysis. A single primary motor neuron from the image stack was chosen. If its axons were intact, its main axon along with the axon collaterals were traced from the image stacks using the ImageJ plugin, Simple Neurite Tracer (Longair et al., 2011). The total length of the traced axon collaterals was calculated from the tracing files. Welch’s analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to determine whether there was an effect of treatment on the total length of the axon collaterals (oneway.test(var.equal = FALSE), R).
RESULTS
DomA-Exposed Fish Were Less Responsive, and Less Likely to Perform SLC Startles
We first determined whether DomA exposure at 2 dpf alters startle responsiveness at 6 dpf over the range of stimulus intensities tested. At the lowest stimulus intensity (32 dB), most control and DomA-treated fish did not respond (Figure 1A, Supplementary Table 2). Furthermore, both DomA and control fish that did respond were more likely to perform LLCs rather than SLC startles. Although this was true for both control and DomA-exposed fish, DomA-exposed fish were even less likely to respond (Estimate = −0.474, p = .0132, Supplementary Table 3), and even less likely to perform SLC startles rather than LLC startles compared with controls (Estimate = −1.076, p = 5.84E−15, Supplementary Table 4).
With higher stimulus intensities (≥ 38 dB), a greater proportion of both the control and DomA-treated fish performed startles rather than not responding (Figure 1A, Supplementary Table 2). However, as seen at the lowest stimulus intensity, DomA-exposed fish were significantly less likely to respond and significantly less likely to perform an SLC startle versus an LLC startle, compared with control fish (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4).
SLC loss is further illustrated by calculating the startle bias index (Jain et al., 2018), a measure of the frequency with which individual fish performed an LLC versus an SLC response at a given stimulus intensity. At intensities of 38 dB and higher, control fish preferentially performed SLC rather than LLC responses (Figure 1B). In contrast, DomA-exposed fish preferentially performed LLC rather than SLC responses at all stimulus intensities tested.
Fish with Myelin Defects Were Less Likely to Perform SLC Startles Than Those without Defects
Previously, we showed the DomA-exposed larvae had myelin deficits (Panlilio et al., 2020). To assess the relationship between startle bias and the observed myelin sheath defects, we classified a subset of the larvae based on myelin defects prior to the behavioral assessment. Similar to the controls, DomA-exposed fish with normal myelin sheaths (category 0) performed more SLC startles at higher stimulus intensities (38 dB and higher) and did not differ from controls in the likelihood of performing LLCs over SLCs at the highest intensity tested (43 dB) (Figure 1C, Supplementary Table 5). In contrast, fish with any noticeable myelin defects (Categories 1–3) had a reduced likelihood of switching from LLC to SLC startles at the highest stimulus intensity, as compared with controls (p < .0004, Supplementary Table 5). Furthermore, DomA-exposed fish with the most severe defects (Category 3) were also less likely to switch from LLC to SLC startles at 43 dB compared with DomA-exposed fish with any of the less severe myelin defects (Categories 0–2) (DomA-exposed fish (Category 3) Estimate= −3.196, p = 4.81E−14; Supplementary Table 5).
DomA-Exposed Fish That Had Myelin Defects Also Had Startle Response Kinematic Deficits
For the fish that responded to a given stimulus, the startle kinematics (bend angle, maximal angular velocity) were assessed. In agreement with our previous findings (Panlilio et al., 2020), DomA-exposed animals had both smaller bend angles and lower maximal angular velocities (Mav) compared with control animals at all the intensities tested (Supplementary Figure 5; p < 2.22e−16). This was true for both LLC (Supplementary Figs. 5A and 5B and Table 6) and SLC (Supplementary Figs. 5C and 5D) startles.
We then determined whether myelin defect severity was correlated with startle kinematic performance. A majority of DomA-exposed larvae were classified as category 3 (the most severe myelin defects). When performing either SLC or LLC startle responses, these larvae had significantly reduced bend angles and Mavs for all stimulus intensities tested (Figure 2, Supplementary Tables 6 and 7).
Fish with intermediate myelin defects (categories 1–2) also consistently had kinematic deficits when performing SLC startles across all stimulus intensities tested (Figs. 2A and 2B, Supplementary Table 6). However, it was only fish with the most severe myelin defects (category 3) that had reduced bend angles and slower maximal angular velocities during LLC startles at all stimulus intensities tested (Figs. 2C and 2D, Supplementary Table 7).
Like DomA-treated fish in other categories, DomA-treated fish with control-like myelin sheaths (category 0) also had kinematic deficits when performing SLC startle responses at the 3 higher intensities (Figs. 2A and 2B, Supplementary Table 6). In contrast, DomA-treated fish with category 0 phenotypes did not have any measurable kinematic deficits when performing LLC startle responses at all stimulus intensities tested (Figs. 2C and 2D, Supplementary Table 7).
These results suggest that exposures to DomA that led to distinguishable myelin defects also led to startle deficits. The severity of the myelin defect was correlated with the severity of the behavioral deficit; fish with the most severe myelin defects were also less likely to perform SLCs and had shallower bend angles and slower maximal angular velocities relative to controls.
DomA-Exposed Fish Were Less Responsive and Had Aberrant Kinematics Following Direct-Electric Field Stimulation
To assess the contribution of the sensory system to the observed startle deficits, we used direct-electric field stimulation, which bypasses the sensory system, directly activating the Mauthner cell, leading to ultra-rapid startle responses (Tabor et al., 2014) (Figure 3A). DomA-treated larvae were significantly less responsive to electrical stimulation than control fish (p < 1e−16). A majority of control fish (70/74) responded to all of the 7 replicate stimuli (Figure 3B), whereas a majority of the DomA-treated fish (47/74) did not respond to any of the replicate stimuli. Furthermore, control fish responded rapidly to electrical stimulation, with a median latency of 2 ms (Figure 3C). Strikingly, when DomA-treated fish did respond, they had significantly longer latencies (control median = 2 ms, DomA median= 49 ms, IQR= 2 s, estimated relative effect [est] = 0.78, 95% CI [0.633, 0.927], p = .001). When DomA-treated fish did respond, they also had shallower bend angles and slower maximal angular velocities compared with controls (bend angle: median =83.5° in controls, median = 41.9° in DomA treated, Coefficient= 0.173, 95% CI [0.059, 0.286], p < e−16; mav: median = 3.78°/ms in controls, median = 2.28°/ms in DomA treated, Coefficient = 0.21, 95% CI [0.083, 0.338], p < e−16) (Figs. 3D and 3E).
Like auditory/vibrational startle deficits, deficits in electric field-induced startle were also correlated with myelin defects; DomA-treated fish with more severe myelin defects also had more severe deficits in electric field-induced startle (Figure 4). DomA-treated fish that had no visible myelin defects had no differences in responsiveness, bend angles, Mavs, or latencies compared with controls (Figs. 4A–E). In contrast, DomA-treated fish with any visible myelin defect, even in its least severe form (category 1), had significantly reduced responsiveness compared with controls (Dunnet post-hoc test, p < e−9) (Figure 4E). Furthermore, DomA-exposed fish that had defects of intermediate severity (category 2) also had significantly longer latencies (est = 0.989, p < 3.28e−13), but no statistically significant differences in bend angle or Mav (Figs. 4C and 4D; Supplementary Table 8). Finally, DomA-exposed fish that had the most severe myelin defects (category 3) had deficits for all startle attributes measured (Figs. 4A–E; Supplementary Table 8).
To further identify the potential cellular and structural defects underlying these observed behavioral deficits, we imaged different components of the startle circuit, starting with the sensory system.
Sensory Inputs for Acoustic/Vibrational Startle
We assessed the key sensory inputs that are required for performing auditory/vibrational startle responses. When an auditory/vibrational stimulus is provided, hair cells in the inner ear, and in some circumstances within neuromasts in the lateral line, are activated. When activated, these hair cells lead to the activation of the statoacoustic ganglia (in the inner ear) or the lateral line ganglia, which in turn send the sensory information to the hindbrain where the information is integrated (Nicolson, 2017; Nicolson et al., 1998). To assess whether DomA disrupts the sensory system, we sought to determine whether DomA reduced the number of neuromasts or disrupted statoacoustic ganglia or lateral line structures.
Using the vital dye DASPEI, we labeled the neuromasts at 5 dpf and found no differences in the number of neuromasts in both the cranial and the trunk region after exposure to DomA (Figs. 5A and 5B). By exposing transgenic Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX) fish in which the peripheral lateral line and the statoacoustic ganglia are labeled, we also found no discernible differences in the presence of these structures in DomA-exposed fish at 5 dpf compared with controls (Figs. 5D and 5E).
Reticulospinal Neurons Integrate Sensory Information
To determine whether DomA disrupts hindbrain neurons responsible for integrating sensory information, we performed spinal backfills at 7 dpf. This allowed us to label the Mauthner cells—paired neurons required for eliciting SLC responses—and their homologs MiDcm2 and MiDcm3, which are active during LLC responses (Marsden and Granato, 2015; O’Malley et al., 1996). Most of the control larvae had both Mauthner cells and all 4 neurons within rhombomere 5 (r5) and rhombomere 6 (r6), the position where MiDcm2 and MiDcm3 reside (Figure 6). In contrast, a majority of DomA-exposed larvae had neither Mauthner cell, and 0 or 1 (out of the 4) neurons in positions r5 and r6 (Figs. 6A–C). In fact, fish exposed to DomA were 99.3% less likely than controls to have Mauthner cells (odds ratio [OR] = 0.007, p = 1.7e−14), and had an even lower probability of having neurons in r5 and r6 (OR = 0.001, p = 1.6e−09).
To confirm that the neurons are absent rather than unable to take up dye from their axons, we also performed whole-brain 3A10 antibody staining, which labels the Mauthner cell bodies along with other midbrain and hindbrain axonal tracks (1–7) (Figure 7A). In agreement with the spinal backfill data, a majority of DomA-exposed larvae (5 dpf) did not have Mauthner cell bodies, whereas a majority of controls did (Figs. 7B and 7C). Importantly, other axonal tracts in the midbrain and hindbrain appeared to be intact in both control and exposed animals. Both control and DomA-exposed animals had medial longitudinal fasciculi, and there were no significant differences in the number of hindbrain axonal tracts (Figs. 7B and 7D).
Primary Motor Neurons That Innervate Muscles That Generate the Response
We then determined whether DomA disrupts primary motor neurons that synapse with the hindbrain reticulospinal neurons and innervate the muscles required to elicit startle. To determine whether DomA alters the main primary motor neuron axons, we stained the trunk region of 2.5-dpf embryos using α-acetylated tubulin (Figure 8A). There were no differences in the presence of the primary motor neurons in DomA-exposed larvae versus controls (Figs. 8B and 8C). In the same tissue, there were also no differences in the appearance of sensory neuron cell bodies or the peripheral lateral line (Figs. 8B and 8C).
By using the transgenic line Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX), we were then able to examine the axon collaterals that branch from the main caudal primary motor neuron axon. Although staining with α-acetylated tubulin showed no differences in the presence of the main branches in primary motor neurons (Figs. 8B and 8C), tracings of the motor neuron axon collaterals in DomA-treated Tg(cntn1b:EGFP-CAAX) larvae (2.5 dpf) (Figure 8D) showed a significant reduction in the length of the branches (Figs. 8E and 8F) (Control [mean ± SE] = 1457 μm ± 43, DomA [mean ± SE] = 527 μm ± 29, F(1, 74.6)= 315.87, p < 2.2e−16).
DISCUSSION
Developmental exposure to DomA leads to persistent behavioral deficits, but the cellular and molecular mechanisms that underlie these effects are largely unknown. We used the zebrafish model and its well-characterized startle response circuit as tools to identify the cell types that are preferentially targeted by developmental DomA exposure and link these to sensorimotor processing deficits that occur during startle. Our results reveal that DomA exposure alters sensorimotor function, reducing responsiveness to stimuli and disrupting kinematics during startle. By imaging the neuronal components of the circuit, we found that DomA exposure led to the loss of reticulospinal neurons and to the reduction in axon collaterals in caudal primary motor neurons, while not affecting the presence of sensory ganglia or other hindbrain axon tracks. Taken together, these results suggest that DomA exposure leads to the loss of specific cells that are necessary for startle circuit function, resulting in behavioral startle deficits.
Mauthner Cells Are Targeted by DomA Exposure
DomA prevents specific types of startle response from occurring in a manner consistent with the loss of a type of hindbrain reticulospinal neuron known as the Mauthner cell. The Mauthner cell is required for performing both electric field-induced startle and A/V-induced SLC startles, but not LLC startles (Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Marsden and Granato, 2015; Tabor et al., 2014). Behaviors that require the Mauthner cell-mediated startle were lost in a majority of the DomA-exposed larvae. Furthermore, DomA-exposed larvae preferentially performed LLC startles versus SLC startles even with high-intensity A/V stimuli. These behavioral results suggest that the Mauthner cell is disrupted by DomA. Imaging results supported this; using 2 complementary approaches (spinal backfills and antibody staining for the neurofilament 3A10), we confirmed that the Mauthner cells were absent in a majority of DomA-treated larvae.
DomA Induces Motor Axon Collateral Loss
DomA-exposed larvae showed kinematic deficits such as smaller bend angles and slower Mavs when performing all forms of startle tested (SLC, LLC, and electric field-induced startles). We hypothesized that this shared phenotype (altered startle kinematics) may be a result of disruptions to a cell type that is common in the neural circuits of these 3 types of startle responses. One of the shared neuronal components is the downstream primary motor neuron that activates the trunk and tail muscles required for the startle responses. Although the main caudal primary motor neuron axons were present in DomA-exposed fish, the collaterals of these motor neuron axons were severely reduced. The reduction in the collaterals could translate to fewer synapses to the trunk muscles which could consequently have led to the observed shallower bend angles (McLean and Dougherty, 2015).
Sensory System Is an Unlikely Primary Target for DomA
Developmental DomA exposure does not completely disrupt sensory processing during startle, consistent with the morphological data showing intact sensory ganglia. The sensory system is bypassed following direct-electric field stimulation, and yet DomA-exposed larvae still exhibited kinematic deficits under these conditions. This result indicates that the observed behavioral deficits cannot be due to solely deficits in the sensory system. DomA-exposed larvae increased their responsiveness with increasing stimulus intensities, suggesting that DomA-exposed larvae have the ability to encode graded sensory information that translates to different degrees of responsiveness. Indeed, when assessing different components of the sensory circuit, we found that DomA did not reduce the number of neuromasts or disrupt the presence of important sensory ganglia necessary for perceiving A/V stimuli, supporting the idea that DomA may not disrupt sensory system structures. However, DomA-exposed larvae were less responsive compared with controls fish for a given stimuli. This may be in part due to functional deficits in the sensory system even if the structures were present. Both afferent sensory neurons and hair cell function have been previously shown to be affected by other glutamate receptor agonists such as AMPA and KA whose application led to the reduced firing of afferent sensory neurons, swelling of hair cell afferent synapses, and hair cell loss (Sebe et al., 2017; Sheets, 2017). It is conceivable that DomA exposure alters the sensory system in a similar manner, contributing to the reduced responsiveness observed in DomA-exposed larvae. Taken together, these results indicate that DomA does not alter the overall structures in the sensory system, nor does it affect the ability of larvae to increase their responsiveness with higher stimulus intensities. However, DomA-exposed fish are generally less responsive relative to control fish, which may be due to subtle perturbations to sensory system function that have yet to be investigated.
Behavioral Deficits Are Correlated with the Severity of Myelin Phenotypes
The startle circuit is heavily myelinated to achieve rapid signaling. We showed previously that myelination deficits result from developmental exposure to DomA (Panlilio et al., 2020). Here, by imaging Tg(mbp:EGFP-CAAX) larvae prior to behavioral tests, we were able to correlate the degree of severity (score) of both myelin and axonal defects to the behavioral phenotypes.
We found that behavioral deficits observed were largely correlated to the severity of axon and myelin defects. DomA-exposed fish with the most severe axonal and myelin defects (category 3) had deficits in all behavioral attributes tested (bend angle, Mav, latency, responsiveness, SLC). In contrast, DomA-exposed fish with “control-like” axons and myelin sheaths (category 0) had startle behaviors that were mostly indistinguishable from controls, with a few exceptions such as bend angle during SLC kinematics.
EGFP expression in Tg(mbp:EGFP-CAAX) fish was a proxy for the degree (score) of axonal and myelin defects because its expression is dependent both on intact myelin and on the presence of the axons (with no axons, there is nothing for oligodendrocytes to myelinate). Imaging results (Figs. 6 and 7) indicated that by the larval stages, reticulospinal neurons are lost in DomA-exposed fish, suggesting that the aberrant EGFP expression at these later developmental stages is due to the loss of axons. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that the loss of these axons was secondary to, or exacerbated by, the myelination defects. Our previous work (Panlilio et al., 2020) showed that as early as 2.5 dpf—the time at which axons are first wrapped—myelination is already perturbed. Thus, future studies are necessary to see whether DomA targets the axons first, followed by the myelination, or whether the loss of myelin leads to the axonal defects later.
DomA Is Not Just a General Neurotoxin
More generally, these findings suggest that DomA is not simply a nonspecific neurotoxin that targets all neural precursors and neurons, but instead one that targets specific neuronal and glial subtypes. The most apparent targets of DomA were the reticulospinal neurons in the hindbrain (including the Mauthner cell) and the caudal primary motor neurons. DomA had no apparent effects on other neurons and neuronal precursors examined, including the medial longitudinal fasciculus, the hindbrain axonal tracts, the Rohon-beard sensory cell bodies, or the neural precursors in the spinal cord. While both Mauthner cells and caudal primary motor neurons express the ionotrophic glutamate receptors to which DomA binds (Patten and Ali, 2007; Todd et al., 2004; Warp et al., 2012), so too do other neurons and glial cells (Hoppmann et al., 2008), suggesting that other factors may be important in mediating susceptibility of subclasses of neurons.
The sensitivity of hindbrain reticulospinal neurons and motor neurons to DomA may be due to their intrinsic properties. Both of these neuron types have large axons, some with extensive axonal arbors that are located in either the spinal cord or periphery (Kimmel et al., 1982; Myers, 1985). Any perturbation that disrupts homeostatic mechanisms, such as slowing of axonal transport, could have important effects on these neurons, making these cells more susceptible to disease and environmental insults. In fact, motor neurons are known for being selectively vulnerable to insult because they are large, continuously active, and heavily rely on mitochondrial respiration processes (Lewinski and Keller, 2005).
Implications for Human Health
Although these studies were done in zebrafish, the findings have implications for human health. The dosages of DomA used in our studies are lower than those known to cause acute neurotoxicity in adult humans (discussion in Panlilio et al., 2020). What is not yet known is the exposure experienced by fetuses or infants of people consuming shellfish with DomA levels below regulatory limits. More generally, our behavioral data indicate that DomA exposure disrupts motor control. Motor deficits have been previously characterized from both incidental human exposures and animal exposure models (Shiotani et al., 2017; Teitelbaum et al., 1990; Wang et al., 2000). Adult humans acutely exposed to DomA developed sensorimotor neuropathy and axonopathy as assessed by electromyography (Teitelbaum et al., 1990). A subset of primates exposed orally at or near the accepted daily tolerable dose of 0.075 mg/kg developed visible hand tremors (Petroff et al., 2019). Rodents prenatally exposed to DomA (PND 10–17) developed aberrant gait patterns (Shiotani et al., 2017). These results suggest that motor deficits may be an important functional endpoint for DomA toxicity; future studies should assess both reflex and fine motor skills. One recent study in nonhuman primates found that in utero exposure to DomA had no impact on early survival reflexes in neonates (Grant et al., 2019). It would be useful to continue to trace motor skill development in these neonates to determine whether there are any potential latent effects, or whether more subtle motor skill deficits emerge.
The study also provides insights into potential cell targets for developmental exposures to DomA, demonstrating that DomA selectively targets specific neurons—the primary motor neurons and reticulospinal neuron subclasses. Although humans do not have Mauthner cells, the intrinsic characteristics that make this cell type more vulnerable to toxins may be shared with other neurons in humans. Identifying these general characteristics (large axons, extensive arbors, location of the axon in the spinal cord) may also provide some useful hints as to which neurons are targeted in humans. Although the identification of other candidates would require other animal models, our results using the zebrafish startle response can help guide further investigation of the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying the behavioral deficits caused by early-life exposure to DomA.
CONCLUSION
This study characterized the effects of developmental DomA exposure on the startle response. The startle response circuit is well characterized, and the neural circuits that drive it are well known. Utilizing this knowledge, we identified specific neural populations that may be more sensitive to early-life exposure to DomA. Furthermore, this study illustrates the potential of using the startle response circuit as a tool to identify neuronal populations targeted by toxin or toxicant exposures.
SUPPLEMENTARY DATA
Supplementary data are available at Toxicological Sciences online.
DECLARATION OF CONFLICTING INTERESTS
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank H.E. Rivera and A.R. Solow for their advice on the statistical analysis, H.A. Burgess for providing the Flote software for startle kinematic analysis, B.G. Merrick for his help designing and building the startle apparatus, and N.R. Brun and L. Kerr (MBL microscopy facility) for microscopy training and advice. They also thank the labs who generously provided us with zebrafish transgenic lines to make this work possible: B. Appel (University of Colorado, Denver), K.R. Monk (Oregon Health & Science University), C.K. Kaufman (Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis), L. Zon (Harvard University), S. Kucenas (University of Virginia), and D.A. Lyons (University of Edinburg). This paper includes data submitted by J.M.P. in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Oceanography and Applied Ocean Science and Engineering at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution (Panlilio, 2019).
Funding
This research was supported by a WHOI Von Damm and Ocean Ridge Initiative Fellowships to J.M.P. and the Woods Hole Center for Oceans and Human Health (NIH: P01ES021923 and P01ES028938; NSF: OCE-1314642 and OCE-1840381).
REFERENCES
- Almeida R. G., Czopka T., Ffrench-Constant C., Lyons D. A. (2011). Individual axons regulate the myelinating potential of single oligodendrocytes in vivo. Development 138, 4443–4450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson D. M., Glibert P. M., Burkholder J. M. (2002). Harmful algal blooms and eutrophication: Nutrient sources, composition, and consequences. Estuaries 25, 704–726. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bates S. S., Hubbard K. A., Lundholm N., Montresor M., Leaw C. P. (2018). Pseudo-nitzschia, Nitzschia, and domoic acid: New research since 2011. Harmful Algae 79, 3–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benaglia T., Chauveau, D., Hunter, D., and Young, D. (2009). mixtools: An R package for analyzing finite mixture models. J. Stat. Softw. 32, 1–29. [Google Scholar]
- Bernard P. B., Macdonald D. S., Gill D. A., Ryan C. L., Tasker R. A. (2007). Hippocampal mossy fiber sprouting and elevated trkB receptor expression following systemic administration of low dose domoic acid during neonatal development. Hippocampus 17, 1121–1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodie E. C., Gulland F. M. D., Greig D. J., Hunter M., Jaakola J., Leger J. S., Leighfield T. A., Van Dolah F. M. (2006). Domoic acid causes reproductive failure in California Sea lions (Zalophus Californianus). Mar. Mammal Sci. 22, 700–707. [Google Scholar]
- Burgess H. A., Granato M. (2007a). Modulation of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish during light adaptation. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 2526–2539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess H. A., Granato M. (2007b). Sensorimotor gating in larval zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 27, 4984–4994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cendes F., Andermann F., Carpenter S., Zatorre R. J., Cashman N. R. (1995). Temporal lobe epilepsy caused by domoic acid intoxication: Evidence for glutamate receptor-mediated excitotoxicity in humans. Ann. Neurol. 37, 123–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cianciolo Cosentino C., Roman, B. L., Drummond, I. A., and Hukriede, N. A. (2010). Intravenous microinjections of zebrafish larvae to study acute kidney injury. J. Vis. Exp. (42), 2079. doi: 10.3791/2079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark S., Hubbard K. A., Anderson D. M., McGillicuddy D. J., Ralston D. K., Townsend D. W. (2019). Pseudo-nitzschia bloom dynamics in the Gulf of Maine: 2012–2016. Harmful Algae 88, 101656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czopka T., Ffrench-Constant C., Lyons D. A. (2013). Individual oligodendrocytes have only a few hours in which to generate new myelin sheaths in vivo. Dev. Cell 25, 599–609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dakshinamurti K., Sharma S. K., Sundaram M., Watanabe T. (1993). Hippocampal changes in developing postnatal mice following intrauterine exposure to domoic acid. J. Neurosci. 13, 4486–4495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doucette T. A., Bernard P. B., Husum H., Perry M. A., Ryan C. L., Tasker R. A. (2004). Low doses of domoic acid during postnatal development produce permanent changes in rat behaviour and hippocampal morphology. Neurotox. Res. 6, 555–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton R. C., Bombardieri R. A., Meyer D. L. (1977). The Mauthner-initiated startle response in teleost fish. J. Exp. Biol. 66, 65–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faber D. S., Fetcho J. R., Korn H. (1989). Neuronal networks underlying the escape response in goldfish. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 563, 11–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill D. A., Bastlund J. F., Watson W. P., Ryan C. L., Reynolds D. S., Tasker R. A. (2010). Neonatal exposure to low-dose domoic acid lowers seizure threshold in adult rats. Neuroscience 169, 1789–1799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant K. S., Crouthamel B., Kenney C., McKain N., Petroff R., Shum S., Jing J., Isoherranen N., Burbacher T. M. (2019). Preclinical modeling of exposure to a global marine bio-contaminant: Effects of in utero Domoic acid exposure on neonatal behavior and infant memory. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 73, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampson D. R., Manalo J. L. (1998). The activation of glutamate receptors by kainic acid and domoic acid. Nat. Toxins 6, 153–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higashijima S-I., Masino M. A., Mandel G., Fetcho J. R. (2003). Imaging neuronal activity during zebrafish behavior with a genetically encoded calcium indicator. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3986–3997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoppmann V., Wu J. J., Søviknes A. M., Helvik J. V., Becker T. S. (2008). Expression of the eight AMPA receptor subunit genes in the developing central nervous system and sensory organs of zebrafish. Dev. Dyn. 237, 788–799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoshijima K., Jurynec M. J., Grunwald D. J. (2016). Precise editing of the zebrafish genome made simple and efficient. Dev. Cell 36, 654–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hothorn T., Bretz F., Westfall P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical J. 50, 346–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue D., Wittbrodt J. (2011). One for all–a highly efficient and versatile method for fluorescent immunostaining in fish embryos. PLoS One 6, e19713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jain R. A., Wolman M. A., Marsden K. C., Nelson J. C., Shoenhard H., Echeverry F. A., Szi C., Bell H., Skinner J., Cobbs E. N., et al. (2018). A forward genetic screen in zebrafish identifies the G-protein-coupled receptor CaSR as a modulator of sensorimotor decision making. Curr. Biol. 28, 1357–1369.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmel C. B., Powell S. L., Metcalfe W. K. (1982). Brain neurons which project to the spinal cord in young larvae of the zebrafish. J. Comp. Neurol. 205, 112–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohashi T., Nakata N., Oda Y. (2012). Effective sensory modality activating an escape triggering neuron switches during early development in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 32, 5810–5820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohashi T., Oda Y. (2008). Initiation of Mauthner- or non-Mauthner-mediated fast escape evoked by different modes of sensory input. J. Neurosci. 28, 10641–10653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konietschke F., Placzek, M., Schaarschmidt, F., and Hothorn, L. A. (2015). An R software package for nonparametric multiple comparisons and simultaneous confidence intervals. J. Stat. Softw. 64, 1–17. [Google Scholar]
- Korn H., Faber D. S. (2005). The Mauthner cell half a century later: A neurobiological model for decision-making? Neuron 47, 13–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucenas S., Takada N., Park H.-C., Woodruff E., Broadie K., Appel B. (2008). CNS-derived glia ensheath peripheral nerves and mediate motor root development. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 143–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefebvre K. A., Hendrix A., Halaska B., Duignan P., Shum S., Isoherranen N., Marcinek D. J., Gulland F. M. D. (2018). Domoic acid in California sea lion fetal fluids indicates continuous exposure to a neuroteratogen poses risks to mammals. Harmful Algae 79, 53–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin E. D., Pizarro K., Pang W. G., Harrison J., Ramsdell J. S. (2005). Persisting behavioral consequences of prenatal domoic acid exposure in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 27, 719–725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinski F. V., Keller B. U. (2005). Ca2+, mitochondria and selective motoneuron vulnerability: Implications for ALS. Trends Neurosci. 28, 494–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longair M. H., Baker D. A., Armstrong J. D. (2011). Simple Neurite Tracer: Open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics 27, 2453–2454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquart G. D., Tabor K. M., Bergeron S. A., Briggman K. L., Burgess H. A. (2019). Prepontine non-giant neurons drive flexible escape behavior in zebrafish. PLoS Biol 17, e3000480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsden K. C., Granato M. (2015). In vivo Ca(2+) imaging reveals that decreased dendritic excitability drives startle habituation. Cell Rep. 13, 1733–1740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maucher J. M., Ramsdell J. S. (2005). Domoic acid transfer to milk: Evaluation of a potential route of neonatal exposure. Environ. Health Perspect. 113, 461–464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maucher J. M., Ramsdell J. S. (2007). Maternal-fetal transfer of domoic acid in rats at two gestational time points. Environ. Health Perspect. 115, 1743–1746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown K. A., Moreno R., Hall V. L., Ribera A. B., Downes G. B. (2012). Disruption of Eaat2b, a glutamate transporter, results in abnormal motor behaviors in developing zebrafish. Dev. Biol. 362, 162–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKibben S. M., Peterson W., Wood A. M., Trainer V. L., Hunter M., White A. E. (2017). Climatic regulation of the neurotoxin domoic acid. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 239–244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean D. L., Dougherty K. J. (2015). Peeling back the layers of locomotor control in the spinal cord. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 63–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills B. D., Pearce H. L., Khan O., Jarrett B. R., Fair D. A., Lahvis G. P. (2016). Prenatal domoic acid exposure disrupts mouse pro-social behavior and functional connectivity MRI. Behav. Brain Res. 308, 14–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirjany M., Preuss T., Faber D. S. (2011). Role of the lateral line mechanosensory system in directionality of goldfish auditory evoked escape response. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 3358–3367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moens C. B., Yan Y. L., Appel B., Force A. G., Kimmel C. B. (1996). valentino: A zebrafish gene required for normal hindbrain segmentation. Development 122, 3981–3990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers P. Z. (1985). Spinal motoneurons of the larval zebrafish. J. Comp. Neurol. 236, 555–561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson T. (2017). The genetics of hair-cell function in zebrafish. J. Neurogenet. 31, 102–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson T., Rüsch A., Friedrich R. W., Granato M., Ruppersberg J. P., Nüsslein-Volhard C. (1998). Genetic analysis of vertebrate sensory hair cell mechanosensation: The zebrafish circler mutants. Neuron 20, 271–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Malley D. M., Kao, Y. H., and Fetcho, J. R. (1996). Imaging the functional organization of zebrafish hindbrain segments during escape behaviors. Neuron 17, 1145–1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panlilio J. M. (2019). Impacts of developmental exposures to the harmful algal bloom toxin domoic acid on neural development and behavior.
- Panlilio J. M., Aluru N., Hahn M. E. (2020). Developmental neurotoxicity of the harmful algal bloom toxin domoic acid: Cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying altered behavior in the zebrafish model. Environ. Health Perspect. 128, 117002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patten S. A., Ali D. W. (2007). AMPA receptors associated with zebrafish Mauthner cells switch subunits during development. J. Physiol. 581, 1043–1056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry M. A., Ryan C. L., Tasker R. A. (2009). Effects of low dose neonatal domoic acid administration on behavioural and physiological response to mild stress in adult rats. Physiol. Behav. 98, 53–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petroff R., Richards T., Crouthamel B., McKain N., Stanley C., Grant K. S., Shum S., Jing J., Isoherranen N., Burbacher T. M., et al. (2019). Chronic, low-level oral exposure to marine toxin, domoic acid, alters whole brain morphometry in nonhuman primates. Neurotoxicology 72, 114–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pogoda H.-M., Sternheim N., Lyons D. A., Diamond B., Hawkins T. A., Woods I. G., Bhatt D. H., Franzini-Armstrong C., Dominguez C., Arana N., et al. (2006). A genetic screen identifies genes essential for development of myelinated axons in zebrafish. Dev. Biol. 298, 118–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. (2013). A language and environment for statistical computing.
- Rust L., Gulland F., Frame E., Lefebvre K. (2014). Domoic acid in milk of free living California marine mammals indicates lactational exposure occurs. Mar. Mammal Sci. 30, 1272–1278. [Google Scholar]
- Sebe J. Y., Cho, S., Sheets, L., Rutherford, M. A., Von Gersdorff, H., and Raible, D. W. (2017). Ca2+ -permeable AMPARs mediate glutamatergic transmission and excitotoxic damage at the hair cell ribbon synapse. J. Neurosci. 37, 6162–6175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Sheets L. (2017). Excessive activation of ionotropic glutamate receptors induces apoptotic hair-cell death independent of afferent and efferent innervation. Sci. Rep. 7, 41102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiotani M., Cole T. B., Hong S., Park J. J. Y., Griffith W. C., Burbacher T. M., Workman T., Costa L. G., Faustman E. M. (2017). Neurobehavioral assessment of mice following repeated oral exposures to domoic acid during prenatal development. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 64, 8–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shum S., Jing J., Petroff R., Crouthamel B., Grant K. S., Burbacher T. M., Isoherranen N. (2020). Maternal-fetal disposition of domoic acid following repeated oral dosing during pregnancy in nonhuman primate. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 398, 115027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sive H. (2011). ‘Model’ or ‘tool’? New definitions for translational research. DMM Dis. Model. Mech. 4, 137–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soroldoni D., Hogan, B.M., and Oates, A. C. (2009). Simple and Efficient Transgenesis with Meganuclease Constructs in Zebrafish. In Zebrafish Methods and Protocols (G.J. Lieschke, et al., Eds.), pp. 117–130. Humana Press, New York, NY. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabor K. M., Bergeron, S. A., Horstick, E. J., Jordan, D. C., Aho, V., Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Haspel, G., and Burgess, H. A. (2014). Direct activation of the Mauthner cell by electric field pulses drives ultra-rapid escape responses 2 3 4. J. Neurophysiol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanemura K., Igarashi K., Matsugami T.-R., Aisaki K-i., Kitajima S., Kanno J. (2009). Intrauterine environment-genome interaction and Children’s development (2): Brain structure impairment and behavioral disturbance induced in male mice offspring by a single intraperitoneal administration of domoic acid (DA) to their dams. J. Toxicol. Sci. 34, SP279–SP286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teitelbaum J. S., Zatorre R. J., Carpenter S., Gendron D., Evans A. C., Gjedde A., Cashman N. R. (1990). Neurologic sequelae of domoic acid intoxication due to the ingestion of contaminated mussels. N. Engl. J. Med. 322, 1781–1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thermes V., Grabher C., Ristoratore F., Bourrat F., Choulika A., Wittbrodt J., Joly J.-S. (2002). I-SceI meganuclease mediates highly efficient transgenesis in fish. Mech. Dev. 118, 91–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiedeken J. A., Ramsdell J. S. (2007). Embryonic exposure to domoic acid increases the susceptibility of zebrafish larvae to the chemical convulsant pentylenetetrazole. Environ. Health Perspect. 115, 1547–1552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd K. J., Slatter C. A. B., Ali D. W. (2004). Activation of ionotropic glutamate receptors on peripheral axons of primary motoneurons mediates transmitter release at the zebrafish NMJ. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 828–840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troconis E. L., Ordoobadi, A. J., Sommers, T. F., Aziz-Bose, R., Carter, A. R., and Trapani, J. G. (2016). Intensity-dependent timing and precision of startle response latency in larval zebrafish. J. Physiol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tryphonas L., Truelove J., Nera E., Iverson F. (1990). Acute neurotoxicity of domoic acid in the rat. Toxicol. Pathol. 18, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner K. J., Bracewell, T. G., and Hawkins, T. A. (2014). Anatomical Dissection of Zebrafish Brain Development, pp. 197–214. Humana Press, Totowa, NJ. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venebles, W. N., and Ripley B. (2018). Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th ed. Springer, New York.
- Wang G. J., Schmued L. C., Andrews A. M., Scallet A. C., Slikker W., Binienda Z. (2000). Systemic administration of domoic acid-induced spinal cord lesions in neonatal rats. J. Spinal Cord Med. 23, 31–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warp E., Agarwal G., Wyart C., Friedmann D., Oldfield C. S., Conner A., Del Bene F., Arrenberg A. B., Baier H., Isacoff E. Y., et al. (2012). Emergence of patterned activity in the developing zebrafish spinal cord. Curr. Biol. 22, 93–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wekell J. C., Hurst, J., and Lefebvre, K. A. (2010). The origin of the regulatory limits for PSP and ASP toxins in shellfish. J. Shellfish Res. 23, 927–930. [Google Scholar]
- Wobbrock J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., and Higgins, J. J. (2011). The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only anova procedures. In Proceedings of the 2011 Annual Conference on Human Factors in Computing systems - CHI 2011, p. 143. ACM Press, New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
- Wolman M. A., Jain R. A., Liss L., Granato M. (2011). Chemical modulation of memory formation in larval zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 15468–15473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xi D., Peng Y.-G., Ramsdell J. S. (1997). Domoic acid is a potent neurotoxin to neonatal rats. Nat. Toxins 5, 74–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zottoli S. J. (1977). Correlation of the startle reflex and Mauthner cell auditory responses in unrestrained goldfish. J. Exp. Biol. 66, 243–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuloaga D. G., Lahvis G. P., Mills B., Pearce H. L., Turner J., Raber J. (2016). Fetal domoic acid exposure affects lateral amygdala neurons, diminishes social investigation and alters sensory-motor gating. Neurotoxicology 53, 132–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.