Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2021 Jul 22;12:714627. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.714627

The 100 Top-Cited Studies on Dyslexia Research: A Bibliometric Analysis

Shijie Zhang 1, Hong Fan 1,*, Yonggang Zhang 2,3,*
PMCID: PMC8339432  PMID: 34366943

Abstract

Background: Citation analysis is a type of quantitative and bibliometric analytic method designed to rank papers based on their citation counts. Over the last few decades, the research on dyslexia has made some progress which helps us to assess this disease, but a citation analysis on dyslexia that reflects these advances is lacking.

Methods: A retrospective bibliometric analysis was performed using the Web of Science Core Collection database. The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were retrieved after reviewing abstracts or full-texts to May 20th, 2021. Data from the 100 top-cited studies were subsequently extracted and analyzed.

Results: The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were cited between 245 to 1,456 times, with a median citation count of 345. These studies were published in 50 different journals, with the “Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America” having published the most (n = 10). The studies were published between 1973 and 2012 and the most prolific year in terms of number of publications was 2000. Eleven countries contributed to the 100 top-cited studies, and nearly 75% articles were either from the USA (n = 53) or United Kingdom (n = 21). Eighteen researchers published at least two different studies of the 100 top-cited list as the first author. Furthermore, 71 studies were published as an original research article, 28 studies were review articles, and one study was published as an editorial material. Finally, “Psychology” was the most frequent study category.

Conclusions: This analysis provides a better understanding on dyslexia and may help doctors, researchers, and stakeholders to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of classic studies, new discoveries, and trends regarding this research field, thus promoting ideas for future investigation.

Keywords: dyslexia, bibliometric analysis, top-cited, citation analysis, citation

Introduction

Dyslexia is a common learning disorder that affects between 4 and 8% of children (13), and often persists into adulthood (4, 5). This neurodevelopmental disorder is characterized by reading and spelling impairments that develop in a context of normal intelligence, educational opportunities, and perceptual abilities (4, 6). Reading and spelling abilities can be affected together or separately. The learning abilities of children with dyslexia are significantly lower than those of their unaffected pairs of the same age. Generally, difficulties begin to show during the early school years. Dyslexia is a complex multifactorial disorder whose etiology has not been fully elucidated, and it has caused great social and economic burdens. Over the last few decades, the research on dyslexia has made some progress. For example, some studies have shown that dyslexia has a strong genetic background that can affect brain anatomy (7, 8) and function (9, 10). But a citation analysis on dyslexia that reflects these advances is lacking.

The publication of study results in scientific journals is the most effective strategy to disseminate new research findings. A high number of citations can indicate the potential of a paper to influence the research community and to generate meaningful changes in clinical practice (11). Citation analysis is a type of quantitative and bibliometric analytic method designed to rank papers based on their citation counts. The latest and up-to-date research findings on dyslexia are well-reflected in recent scientific papers (12), particularly in the most cited ones (13, 14). By analyzing the most cited studies, especially the 100 top-cited studies, we can gain better insight into the most significant advances made in the field of dyslexia research over the course of the past several decades (15). This retrospective bibliometric approach has been used for many other diseases, such as diabetes (16), endodontics (17), cancer (18). However, to date, no bibliometric analyses have been conducted in the field of dyslexia. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to analyze the 100 top-cited studies in the field of dyslexia.

Materials and Methods

Search Method and Inclusion Criteria

This retrospective bibliometric analysis was conducted using the Web of Science Core Collection database. The Web of Science Core Collection is a multidisciplinary database with searchable authors and abstracts covering a vast science journal literature (19). It indexes the major journals of more than 170 subject categories, providing access to retrospective data between 1945 and the present (20). On May 20th, 2021, we conducted an exhaustive literature retrieval, regardless of the country of origin, publication year, and language. The only search term used was “dyslexia” and the search results were sorted by the number of citations.

Article Selection

Two authors independently screened the abstracts or full-texts to identify the 100 top-cited articles about dyslexia. Disagreements were resolved through discussion. Only studies that focused on dyslexia were included in subsequent analyses. Studies that only mentioned dyslexia in passing were excluded.

Data Extraction

The final list of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia was determined by total article citation counts. We extracted the following data for each article: title, authors, journal, language, total citation count, publication year, country, journal impact factor, type of article, and Web of Science subject category. If the reprint author had two or more affiliations from different countries, we used the first affiliation as the country of origin. If one article was listed in more than one subject category, the first category was selected. If one article had more than one author, we selected the first-ranked author as the first author and the last-ranked author as the last-author.

Data Analysis

SPSS 11.0 (Chicago, IL, USA) was used to count the frequency. We analyzed the following data: citation count, year of publication, country, the first author, journal, language, type of study, and Web of Science subject category.

Results

Citation Analysis

The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia based on total citations are listed in Table 1. The total citation count for these 100 articles combined was 42,222. The total citation count of per study ranged from 245 to 1,456 times, with a median citation count of 345. Only 3 studies were cited more than 1,000 times, and the rest of the studies were cited between 100 and 1,000 times. The title of the top-cited study, which also had the largest mean citation per year count (n = 91), was “Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory,” which was published by Ziegler et al. in Psychological Bulletin in 2005 (21). The second top-cited study, which also had the second-highest mean citation per year count (n = 80), was published by Vellutino et al. (22). In addition, we also identified the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia based on mean citation per year, whose results were shown in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 1.

The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia based on total citations.

Ranking Title Journal Total citation Publication year Mean citation per year Country Impact factor in the year of publication*
1 Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory Psychological Bulletin 1,456 2005 91 France 9.746
2 Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1,359 2004 80 USA 2.782
3 The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias Journal of Educational Psychology 1,048 1999 48 USA 1.893
4 Rapid automatized naming (ran)—dyslexia differentiated from other learning-disabilities Neuropsychologia 970 1976 22 USA N/A
5 Developmental dyslexia–4 consecutive patients with cortical anomalies Annals of Neurology 955 1985 27 USA N/A
6 A definition of dyslexia Annals of Dyslexia 863 2003 48 USA 1.261
7 Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults Brain 832 2003 46 France 7.967
8 To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia Trends in Neurosciences 724 1997 30 UK 17.084
9 Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: same or different? Psychological Bulletin 698 2004 41 UK 7.701
10 Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 668 1991 22 USA N/A
11 Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity Science 665 2001 33 Italy 23.329
12 Varieties of developmental dyslexia Cognition 635 1993 23 Australia N/A
13 Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia Biological Psychiatry 631 2002 33 USA 5.915
14 Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models Psychological Review 598 1999 27 USA 6.803
15 Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 598 1998 26 USA 9.821
16 Deep dyslexia—a case-study of connectionist neuropsychology Cognitive Neuropsychology 586 1993 21 USA N/A
17 Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: immediate and long-term outcomes from two Instructional approaches Journal of Learning Disabilities 550 2001 28 USA 1.333
18 Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific reading disability Journal of Educational Psychology 537 1996 21 USA N/A
19 Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis Trends in Neurosciences 511 2001 26 UK 16.475
20 Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: evidence from functional MRI Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 496 2003 28 USA 10.272
21 Developmental dyslexia—diagnostic approach based on 3 atypical reading-spelling patterns Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 492 1973 10 USA N/A
22 The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 490 1995 19 Canada N/A
23 Current concepts—dyslexia New England Journal of Medicine 477 1998 21 USA 28.660
24 What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 477 1997 20 Canada 4.844
25 Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging Nature 477 1996 19 USA N/A
26 Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 469 2003 26 France 9.727
27 Persistence of dyslexics phonological awareness deficits Developmental Psychology 467 1992 16 Canada N/A
28 Dyslexia (specific reading disability) Biological Psychiatry 461 2005 29 USA 6.779
29 Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal-distribution of reading-ability New England Journal of Medicine 459 1992 16 USA N/A
30 Brain morphology in developmental dyslexia and attention-deficit disorder hyperactivity Archives of Neurology 454 1990 15 USA N/A
31 Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system Applied Psycholinguistics 443 1993 16 Austria N/A
32 Quantitative trait locus for reading-disability on chromosome-6 Science 436 1994 16 USA N/A
33 Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 432 1998 19 USA 9.821
34 The non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia—a review Reading Research Quarterly 432 1992 15 UK N/A
35 Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from pet scanning Brain 412 1996 16 UK N/A
36 Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics—a search for wernicke's wortschatz? Brain 403 1999 18 UK 7.374
37 A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia Trends in Cognitive Sciences 401 2011 40 UK 12.586
38 Lesioning an attractor network—investigations of acquired dyslexia Psychological Review 401 1991 13 Canada N/A
39 Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis Cognition 399 2007 29 France 3.831
40 The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia—an overview and working hypothesis Brain 398 2000 19 Canada 7.303
41 Developmental lag vs. deficit models of reading disability: a longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis Journal of Educational Psychology 397 1996 16 USA N/A
42 Word-form dyslexia Brain 396 1980 10 UK N/A
43 Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia Reading and Writing 391 1993 14 Canada N/A
44 Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia—case-study Annals of Neurology 391 1979 9 USA N/A
45 Cognitive profiles of reading-disability—comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions Journal of Educational Psychology 388 1994 14 USA N/A
46 Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit Developmental Neuropsychology 387 2005 24 USA 2.443
47 Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15 American Journal of Human Genetics 376 1997 16 USA 10.244
48 The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: a German-English comparison Cognition 373 1997 16 Austria 2.973
49 Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia) Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 358 2000 17 USA 0.800
50 Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung 348 1981 9 UK N/A
51 Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 342 2000 16 UK 2.940
52 Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing Trends in Cognitive Sciences 340 2010 31 Australia 9.686
53 Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 336 2005 21 USA 1.725
54 Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia Trends in Cognitive Sciences 336 2001 17 Finland 11.606
55 Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 329 2002 17 UK 10.700
56 Rapid automatized naming (ran) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities Annual Review of Psychology 328 2012 36 USA 15.265
57 Automaticity—a new framework for dyslexia research Cognition 319 1990 10 UK N/A
58 Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill Child Development 310 2003 17 UK 3.324
59 Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: the importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items Reading Research Quarterly 309 1998 13 Denmark 1.541
60 Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts non-word reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers Current Biology 303 1998 13 UK 7.855
61 Dyslexia in children and young-adults–3 independent neuropsychological syndromes Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 303 1975 7 USA N/A
62 Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability Biological Psychiatry 301 2003 17 USA 6.039
63 On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia Cognition 299 1996 12 USA N/A
64 Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders Annual Review of Psychology 295 2009 25 USA 22.750
65 Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability Journal of Communication Disorders 295 2001 15 USA 0.640
66 Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia Developmental Psychology 295 1990 10 Canada N/A
67 Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture Nature 294 2004 17 China 32.182
68 Developmental dyslexia Lancet 293 2004 17 France 21.713
69 Subtypes of reading disability: variability around a phonological core Journal of Educational Psychology 291 1998 13 USA 1.595
70 Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct Journal of Educational Psychology 290 2006 19 China 2.025
71 On the specifics of specific reading disability and specific language impairment Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 286 2000 14 UK 2.940
72 DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 285 2005 18 USA 10.231
73 Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience Science 284 2009 24 USA 29.747
74 Toward a definition of dyslexia Annals of Dyslexia 284 1995 11 USA N/A
75 Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: a research-based model and its clinical implementation Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 282 2001 14 USA 0.558
76 Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia Child Development 281 2001 14 USA 2.583
77 Developmental dyslexia Lancet 279 2012 31 USA 39.060
78 Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies Human Brain Mapping 278 2009 23 Austria 6.256
79 Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls Vision Research 276 1995 11 UK N/A
80 Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 275 1994 10 USA N/A
81 Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 274 2000 13 USA 2.940
82 Persistence of dyslexia: the Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence Pediatrics 270 1999 12 USA 3.487
83 Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America 269 2007 19 USA 9.598
84 Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training Neurology 268 2002 14 USA 5.340
85 A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America 264 2003 15 Finland 10.272
86 Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 263 2011 26 USA 9.681
87 Developmental dyslexia in women—neuropathological findings in 3 patients Annals of Neurology 262 1990 8 USA N/A
88 MRI evaluation of the size and symmetry of the planum-temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia Brain and Language 260 1990 8 Norway N/A
89 Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia Development and Psychopathology 257 2008 20 USA 5.483
90 Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography Annals of Neurology 257 1996 10 Finland N/A
91 Developmental dyslexia in different languages: language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 253 2003 14 France 1.482
92 The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: the evidence from contrast sensitivity Vision Research 253 2000 12 USA 2.000
93 Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study Neuroreport 252 2001 13 USA 2.374
94 The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex Psychological Science 251 2000 12 USA 2.565
95 Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 250 1997 10 UK 1.333
96 Surface dyslexia Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A-Human Experimental Psychology 250 1983 7 UK N/A
97 Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses Brain and Language 248 2000 12 USA 1.473
98 Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait Nature Reviews Neuroscience 247 2002 13 UK 24.047
99 Semantic access dyslexia Brain 246 1979 6 UK N/A
100 Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 245 2000 12 UK 2.940

USA, the United States of America; UK, the United Kingdom.

*

“N/A” was assigned when the journal impact factor was not available or had not been assigned in the year of publication.

Journals

The different journals of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia and their associated impact factors are listed in Table 2. The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were published in 50 different journals, with the top three in frequency being “Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America” (n = 10), “Brain” (n = 6), and “Journal of Educational Psychology” (n = 6).

Table 2.

Journals of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia.

Journal Total citation times Number of studies Average citation times per study Impact factor (2019)*
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 3,879 10 388 9.412
Brain 2,687 6 448 11.337
Journal of Educational Psychology 2,951 6 492 5.028
Cognition 2,025 5 405 3.294
Annals of Neurology 1,865 4 466 9.037
Biological Psychiatry 1,393 3 464 12.095
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1,878 3 626 7.035
Science 1,385 3 462 41.846
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1,077 3 359 15.218
Annals of Dyslexia 1,147 2 574 1.595
Annual Review of Psychology 623 2 312 18.111
Brain and Language 508 2 254 2.339
Child Development 591 2 296 4.891
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 795 2 398 4.406
Developmental Psychology 762 2 381 3.063
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 628 2 314 7.035
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 503 2 252 2.301
Lancet 572 2 286 60.390
Nature 771 2 386 42.779
New England Journal of Medicine 936 2 468 74.699
Psychological Bulletin 2,154 2 1,077 20.838
Psychological Review 999 2 500 6.844
Reading Research Quarterly 741 2 371 3.543
Trends In Neurosciences 1,235 2 618 12.891
Vision Research 529 2 265 2.610
American Journal of Human Genetics 376 1 376 10.502
Applied Psycholinguistics 443 1 443 1.412
Archives of Neurology 454 1 454 7.419
Cognitive Neuropsychology 586 1 586 2.396
Current Biology 303 1 303 9.601
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 469 1 469 6.263
Development and Psychopathology 257 1 257 3.385
Developmental Neuropsychology 387 1 387 1.477
Human Brain Mapping 278 1 278 4.421
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 477 1 477 3.105
Journal of Communication Disorders 295 1 295 1.315
Journal of Learning Disabilities 550 1 550 2.144
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 336 1 336 1.873
Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 282 1 282 1.740
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 358 1 358 3.800
Nature Reviews Neuroscience 247 1 247 33.654
Neurology 268 1 268 8.770
Neuropsychologia 970 1 970 2.652
Neuroreport 252 1 252 1.394
Pediatrics 270 1 270 5.359
Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung 348 1 348 2.419
Psychological Science 251 1 251 5.367
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 490 1 490 3.910
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A-Human Experimental Psychology 250 1 250 2.449
Reading and Writing 391 1 391 1.445
*

Impact factors were extracted from the journal citation report of 2019. If the journal did not have an impact factor for 2019, its impact factor was expressed for the last year.

The journal with the highest total citation count was “Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.” However, the highest average citation count per study belonged to the journal “Psychological Bulletin.” The journal impact factors of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia ranged from 1.315 to 74.699. Of the 100 top-cited studies, 29 were published in a journal with an impact factor greater than 10. The standard “CNS” journals, with the exception of “Cell,” “Nature,” and “Science” published 2 and 3 studies, respectively. Regarding the top four medical journals, while the “New England Journal of Medicine” and “Lancet” published 2 studies each, no top-cited study was published by the “Journal of the American Medical Association” or the “British Medical Journal.”

Language and Year of Publication

The 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were all published in English and were published between 1973 [by Boder et al. (23)] and 2012 [by Norton et al. (24) and Peterson et al. (25)] (Table 3). The most productive years were 2000, 2001 and 2003, with 9, 8 and 8 published articles, respectively. The year of 2003 had the most total citations with a total count of 3,788 and an average citation count per study of 474.

Table 3.

Publication year of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia.

Year Number of studies Total citation times Average citation times
2000 9 2,655 295
2001 8 3,172 397
2003 8 3,788 474
1996 6 2,379 397
1998 6 2,410 402
1990 5 1,590 318
1997 5 2,200 440
2005 5 2,925 585
1993 4 2,055 514
1999 4 2,319 580
2002 4 1,475 369
2004 4 2,644 661
1992 3 1,358 453
1994 3 1,099 366
1995 3 1,050 350
2009 3 857 286
1979 2 637 319
1991 2 1,069 535
2007 2 668 334
2011 2 664 332
2012 2 607 304
1973 1 492 492
1975 1 303 303
1976 1 970 970
1980 1 396 396
1981 1 348 348
1983 1 250 250
1985 1 955 955
2006 1 290 290
2008 1 257 257
2010 1 340 340

Countries and Authors

Eleven countries contributed articles to the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia (Table 4). Most of the articles were from the USA (n = 53), United Kingdom (n = 21), Canada (n = 7), and France (n = 6). In addition, the USA had the highest total citation count (23,129), and Italy had the highest average citation count per study (665).

Table 4.

Countries of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia.

Country* Number of studies Total citation times Average citation times
USA 53 23,129 436
UK 21 7,728 368
Canada 7 2,919 417
France 6 3,702 617
Austria 3 1,094 365
Finland 3 857 286
Australia 2 975 488
China 2 584 292
Denmark 1 309 309
Italy 1 665 665
Norway 1 260 260

USA, the United States of America; UK, the United Kingdom.

*

The country distribution of 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia research were extracted from the corresponding author.

As shown in Table 5, there were 18 first-authors and 13 last-authors who published more than one of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia. Among them, Shaywitz SE published the most top 100 articles (n = 7) on dyslexia as the first author, followed by Galaburda AM (n = 3) and Pugh KR (n = 3). And for the last author, 8 studies of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia research were published by Shaywitz BA who was the most productive.

Table 5.

Authors with at least two first-author or last-author publications in the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia.

First-author Last-author
Name Number of studies Name Number of studies
Shaywitz SE 7 Shaywitz BA 8
Galaburda AM 3 Frith U 5
Pugh KR 3 Gabrieli JDE 5
Bruck M 2 Gore JC 4
Catts HW 2 Shallice T 4
Goswami U 2 Goswami U 3
Hoeft F 2 Snowling MJ 3
Lyon GR 2 Bruck M 2
Nicolson RI 2 Defries JC 2
Pennington BF 2 Galaburda AM 2
Paulesu E 2 Pennington BF 2
Ramus F 2 Wimmer H 2
Snowling MJ 2 Wolf M 2
Temple E 2
Vellutino FR 2
Warrington EK 2
Willcutt EG 2
Ziegler JC 2

Publication Type and Web of Science Subject Categories

As shown in Table 6, there were 71 studies in the form of an original research article, 28 studies in the form of a review article, and one study in the form of an editorial material publication. The total citation counts for each publication type were 27,812, 13,899, and 511, respectively. Although the type of original research article had the highest total citation count, it had the lowest average citation count per study. In addition, a total of 12 Web of Science subject categories were extracted. Among them, “Psychology” was the most frequent category associated with studies [35], followed by “Clinical Neurology” [15], and “Multidisciplinary Sciences” [15], “Neurosciences” [12], and “Education” [6]. Consistent with the number of studies, the subject categories of “Psychology” and “Clinical Neurology” also had the highest total citation counts (15,683 and 6,427, respectively). The “Behavioral Sciences” subject category had the highest average citation count.

Table 6.

Type of study and subject categories for the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia.

Variable Number of studies Total citation times Average citation times per study
Type of study
Article 71 27,812 392
Review 28 13,899 496
Editorial Material 1 511 511
Web of Science categories*
Psychology 35 15,683 448
Clinical Neurology 15 6,427 428
Multidisciplinary Sciences 15 6,035 402
Neurosciences 12 4,880 407
Education 6 2,829 472
Audiology & Speech-Language Pathology 5 1,421 284
Behavioral Sciences 4 2,047 512
Medicine 4 1,508 377
Biochemistry & Molecular Biology 1 303 303
Genetics & Heredity 1 376 376
Linguistics 1 443 443
Pediatrics 1 270 270
*

Web of Science subject categories were extracted from the Web of Science. If one article was listed in more than one category, the first category was used for data analysis.

Discussion

Although retrospective bibliometric approach has been conducted in many other diseases, to our knowledge, no citation analyses have examined publications on dyslexia. Therefore, this study is the first comprehensive analysis summarizing several features of the most influential studies on dyslexia. It has been suggested that a highly cited study can be considered as a milestone study in a related field and has the potential to generate meaningful changes in clinical practice (26). We believe that the present analysis of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia may be beneficial to the research community for the following reasons. First, the present study not only provides a historical projection of the scientific progress with regards to dyslexia research, but it also shows associated research trends and gaps in the field (27). Second, our findings provide critical quantitative information about how both the classic studies and recent advancements in the field have improved our understanding of dyslexia (28). Third, the present analysis may help journal editors, funding agencies, and reviewers critically evaluate studies and funding applications (28).

Our analysis discovered that the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were published in 50 different journals. This may reflect the fact that the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were very multidisciplinary in nature, unlike the top studies of other fields (e.g., psoriatic arthritis) where there is a more inherent researcher bias for journal selection (29). Of the 100 top-cited studies, 29 were published in a journal with an impact factor >10, and 62 studies were published in journal with an impact factor >5. However, there were only five studies published in the standard “CNS” journals and only four published in the top four medical journals, which suggests that most dyslexia researchers are more inclined to choose the most influential journals in their respective professional fields when submitting articles (30). This is in marked contrast with some other fields (e.g. vaccines), where the majority of top-cited articles are published in either the standard “CNS” journals or in the top four medical journals (15). Several other factors, such as the review turnaround time, likelihood of manuscript acceptance, publication costs, journal publication frequency, will all invariably also affect a researcher's journal selection (13, 20).

According to the results of our analysis, nearly 80% of the 100 top-cited studies on dyslexia were published between 1990 and 2005, and the years of 2000 was found to have the most publications. The increase of landmark publications between 1990 and 2005 might reflect an increase in the interest in dyslexia research or that researchers had made some important scientific breakthroughs during this time period. All the top-cited studies on dyslexia were published in English, likely because English is the most commonly used language for knowledge dissemination in the world.

The top countries with regards to total citation count and number of papers in the top 100 list were the USA (n = 53) and United Kingdom (n = 21), which accounted for ~75% of the 100 top-cited studies. The USA published the most studies from the list, and this is probably because some of the world's top research centers are located in the USA and likely also the USA receives more research funding (31). Furthermore, the most prolific first-author (Shaywitz SE) and last-author (Shaywitz BA) were also from the USA. It is also worth mentioning that China had two studies on the top 100 list, which attests to the improvement of our national scientific research community with regards to knowledge dissemination.

In the present study, there were more original research articles (n = 71) than review articles (n = 28), but the latter had higher average citation counts per study. These results indicate that even though researchers pay significant attention to new findings on dyslexia, they regularly use information from review articles to convey relevant points in their own papers. We found that “Psychology” was the most frequent subject category associated with the top 100 articles, which indicates that researchers have been working to find effective treatments for people with dyslexia and that research in this field will continue to progress.

Like with other bibliometric analyses, there are some study limitations that should be highlighted. First, the 100 top-cited studies were extracted from the Web of Science Core Collection, which might have excluded some top-cited studies from other databases, such as Scopus and Google Scholar. Second, there was no citation data for recently published studies. Third, self-citations might have substantially influenced the results of the citation analysis. Moreover, this was a cross-sectional study, which implies that the identified 100 top-cited studies could change in the future. Despite these limitations, this descriptive bibliometric study could contribute new information about the scientific interest in dyslexia.

In conclusion, the present analysis is the first analysis to recognize the 100 top-cited studies in the field of dyslexia. This analysis provides a better understanding on dyslexia and may help doctors, researchers, and stakeholders to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of classic studies, new discoveries, and trends regarding this research field. As new data continue to emerge, this bibliometric analysis will become an important quantitative instrument to ascertain the overall direction of a given field, thus promoting ideas for future investigation.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

YZ and HF designed the study. SZ and YZ acquired the data and performed statistical analyses. SZ, YZ, and HF drafted the manuscript. All authors critically revised the article and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. This study was partly supported by National Clinical Research Center for Geriatrics, West China Hospital, Sichuan University (Z2018B016).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.714627/full#supplementary-material

References

  • 1.Fortes IS Paula CS Oliveira MC Bordin IA de Jesus Mari J Rohde LA . A cross-sectional study to assess the prevalence of DSM-5 specific learning disorders in representative school samples from the second to sixth grade in Brazil. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 25:195–207. 10.1007/s00787-015-0708-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Landerl K, Moll K. Comorbidity of learning disorders: prevalence and familial transmission. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. (2010) 51:287–94. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02164.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Castillo A, Gilger JW. Adult perceptions of children with dyslexia in the USA. Ann Dyslexia. (2018) 68:203–17. 10.1007/s11881-018-0163-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. (2015) 11:283–307. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cavalli E, Colé P, Brèthes H, Lefevre E, Lascombe S, Velay JL. E-book reading hinders aspects of long-text comprehension for adults with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. (2019) 69:243–59. 10.1007/s11881-019-00182-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wang LC, Liu D, Xu Z. Distinct effects of visual and auditory temporal processing training on reading and reading-related abilities in Chinese children with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. (2019) 69:166–85. 10.1007/s11881-019-00176-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Skeide MA, Kraft I, Müller B, Schaadt G, Neef NE, Brauer J, et al. NRSN1 associated grey matter volume of the visual word form area reveals dyslexia before school. Brain. (2016) 139:2792–803. 10.1093/brain/aww153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kraft I, Schreiber J, Cafiero R, Metere R, Schaadt G, Brauer J, et al. Predicting early signs of dyslexia at a preliterate age by combining behavioral assessment with structural MRI. NeuroImage. (2016) 143:378–86. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Neef NE, Müller B, Liebig J, Schaadt G, Grigutsch M, Gunter TC, et al. Dyslexia risk gene relates to representation of sound in the auditory brainstem. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2017) 24:63–71. 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Männel C, Meyer L, Wilcke A, Boltze J, Kirsten H, Friederici AD. Working-memory endophenotype and dyslexia-associated genetic variant predict dyslexia phenotype. Cortex. (2015) 71:291–305. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Perazzo MF, Otoni ALC, Costa MS, Granville-Granville AF, Paiva SM, Martins-Júnior PA. The top 100 most-cited papers in paediatric dentistry journals: a bibliometric analysis. Int J Paediatr Dentist. (2019) 29:692–711. 10.1111/ipd.12563 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Daley EM, Vamos CA, Zimet GD, Rosberger Z, Thompson EL, Merrell L. The feminization of HPV: reversing gender biases in US human papillomavirus vaccine policy. Am J Public Health. (2016) 106:983–4. 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kolkailah AA, Fugar S, Vondee N, Hirji SA, Okoh AK, Ayoub A, et al. Bibliometric analysis of the top 100 most cited articles in the first 50 years of heart transplantation. Am J Cardiol. (2019) 123:175–86. 10.1016/j.amjcard.2018.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zhao X, Guo L, Lin Y, Wang H, Gu C, Zhao L, et al. The top 100 most cited scientific reports focused on diabetes research. Acta Diabetol. (2016) 53:13–26. 10.1007/s00592-015-0813-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zhang Y, Quan L, Xiao B, Du L. The 100 top-cited studies on vaccine: a bibliometric analysis. Hum Vacc Immunother. (2019) 15:3024–31. 10.1080/21645515.2019.1614398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Beshyah WS, Beshyah SA. Bibliometric analysis of the literature on Ramadan fasting and diabetes in the past three decades (1989-2018). Diabetes Res Clin Pract. (2019) 151:313–22. 10.1016/j.diabres.2019.03.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Adnan S, Ullah R. Top-cited articles in regenerative endodontics: a bibliometric analysis. J Endodontics. (2018) 44:1650–64. 10.1016/j.joen.2018.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gao Y, Shi S, Ma W, Chen J, Cai Y, Ge L, et al. Bibliometric analysis of global research on PD-1 and PD-L1 in the field of cancer. Int Immunopharmacol. (2019) 72:374–84. 10.1016/j.intimp.2019.03.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yu T, Jiang Y, Gamber M, Ali G, Xu T, Sun W. Socioeconomic status and self-rated health in China: findings from a cross-sectional study. Medicine. (2019) 98:e14904. 10.1097/MD.0000000000014904 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yoon DY, Yun EJ, Ku YJ, Baek S, Lim KJ, Seo YL, et al. Citation classics in radiology journals: the 100 top-cited articles, 1945-2012. AJR Am J Roentgenol. (2013) 201:471–81. 10.2214/AJR.12.10489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ziegler JC, Goswami U. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:3–29. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. (2004) 45:2–40. 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Boder E. Developmental dyslexia: a diagnostic approach based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1973) 15:663–87. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1973.tb05180.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Norton ES, Wolf M. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Ann Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:427–52. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet. (2012) 379:1997–2007. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Van Noorden R, Maher B, Nuzzo R. The top 100 papers. Nature. (2014) 514:550–3. 10.1038/514550a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fardi A, Kodonas K, Gogos C, Economides N. Top-cited articles in endodontic journals. J Endodontics. (2011) 37:1183–90. 10.1016/j.joen.2011.05.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gondivkar SM, Sarode SC, Gadbail AR, Gondivkar RS, Choudhary N, Patil S. Citation classics in cone beam computed tomography: the 100 top-cited articles. Int J Dentistry. (2018) 2018:9423281. 10.1155/2018/9423281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Berlinberg A, Bilal J, Riaz IB, Kurtzman DJB. The 100 top-cited publications in psoriatic arthritis: a bibliometric analysis. Int J Dermatol. (2019) 58:1023–34. 10.1111/ijd.14261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bullock N, Ellul T, Bennett A, Steggall M, Brown G. The 100 most influential manuscripts in andrology: a bibliometric analysis. Basic Clin Androl. (2018) 28:15. 10.1186/s12610-018-0080-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Shadgan B, Roig M, Hajghanbari B, Reid WD. Top-cited articles in rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabilit. (2010) 91:806–15. 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES