Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
editorial
. 2021 Jun 24;16(8):739–744. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab073

Computational methods in social neuroscience: recent advances, new tools and future directions

Carolyn Parkinson 1,
PMCID: PMC8343570  PMID: 34101815

Abstract

Recent years have seen a surge of exciting developments in the computational tools available to social neuroscientists. This paper highlights and synthesizes recent advances that have been enabled by the application of such tools, as well as methodological innovations likely to be of interest and utility to social neuroscientists, but that have been concentrated in other sub-fields. Papers in this special issue are emphasized—many of which contain instructive materials (e.g. tutorials and code) for researchers new to the highlighted methods. These include approaches for modeling social decisions, characterizing multivariate neural response patterns at varying spatial scales, using decoded neurofeedback to draw causal links between specific neural response patterns and psychological and behavioral phenomena, examining time-varying patterns of connectivity between brain regions, and characterizing the social networks in which social thought and behavior unfold in everyday life. By combining computational methods for characterizing participants’ rich social environments—at the levels of stimuli, paradigms and the webs of social relationships that surround people—with those for capturing the psychological processes that undergird social behavior and the wealth of information contained in neuroimaging datasets, social neuroscientists can gain new insights into how people create, understand and navigate their complex social worlds.

Keywords: computational social neuroscience, multivoxel pattern analysis, social network analysis, social decision-making, naturalistic neuroimaging

Introduction

Creating, navigating and thriving within humans’ distinctively complex social environments are thought to entail some of the most significant computational challenges for the human brain and may have played a significant role in shaping its evolution (Dunbar, 2003). Scientific understanding of how the human brain meets such challenges is particularly likely to benefit from methodological tools that are capable of capturing the rich, multivariate and interdependent nature of the processes involved in social thought and behavior. Recent years have seen a surge of new developments in the methodological tools available to and used by social neuroscientists. Researchers studying how the human brain represents and navigates the social world increasingly integrate theory and methods from social psychology and neuroscience with computational approaches from network science, machine learning and other fields. There is also an abundance of methodological innovations from other areas of neuroscience that hold great promise for advancing our understanding of the social brain. This special issue, in tandem with other recent work in Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (SCAN), highlights and synthesizes advances in social neuroscience that have been enabled by the application of diverse computational methods, as well as methodological innovations likely to be of particular interest and utility to researchers studying how the brain supports social thought and behavior.

Modeling social decisions and behaviors

To successfully navigate the social world, the human brain must perform a number of computations in parallel (e.g. tracking others’ behaviors, intentions, social identities and interactions, and using such information to shape one’s own behavior). In this issue, Molapour et al. (2021) outline a set of key computations with which the human brain must contend in any social interaction and emphasize the importance not only of examining each of these distinct computations individually, but also of understanding how the brain integrates information across computations and modalities to support social interactions. Examinations of how the brain supports social cognition and behavior using methods from computational neuroscience often involve creating a model of a particular social decision-making or learning process and relating parameters of that model to neural responses measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). For example, in this issue, Park et al. (2021) use this approach to shed light on the role of the right temporoparietal junction in updating impressions of close others and strangers. Much insight into the computations and mechanisms that guide human social thoughts, feelings and behavior has been achieved with such approaches. Also, in this issue Lockwood and Klein-Flügge (2021) provide a comprehensive and accessible primer on using one type of computational models (specifically, reinforcement learning models) to study social cognition. In this piece, Lockwood and Klein-Flügge (2021) describe theoretical and practical issues regarding the implementation of such models, promising directions for future research and ways in which applications of this approach can yield new hypotheses about how the human brain navigates everyday social situations. The authors also provide links to publicly available resources with tutorials and example code to assist readers interested in getting started with fitting reinforcement learning models to their data.

A growing body of computational social neuroscience research also uses approaches from ethology and behavioral ecology (Mobbs et al., 2018). Such approaches contextualize social decisions in the kinds of scenarios that individuals regularly face in their natural social environment. In so doing, such research complements and extends findings that have been achieved using more constrained, and sometimes relatively decontextualized, paradigms. In this issue, Gabay and Apps (2021) provide an overview of one particular framework—marginal value theorem (Charnov, 1976)—that holds particular promise for examining questions about social cognition and behavior. Marginal value theorem characterizes decisions that individuals must make regarding when to abandon a current location and move onto a new setting when foraging for rewards. In addition to providing an overview of this framework, Gabay and Apps (2021) provide guidance for researchers seeking to implement such techniques in their own research and discuss how various aspects of social cognition and behavior can be conceptualized in this way (e.g. as foraging for social information), and thus, could be fruitfully studied through the lens of marginal value theorem. Continuing to integrate approaches from ethology and behavioral ecology into social neuroscience promises to enrich our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying social thought and behavior by facilitating the precise quantification of decisions and behavior in increasingly complex situations that resemble those that individuals encounter in everyday life.

Probing neural representations with multivariate approaches

Multivariate pattern analysis: fundamentals and methodological considerations.

Another area in which the integration of computational methods has expanded the questions that social neuroscientists can ask is multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) of fMRI data. Recent ‘Tools of the Trade’ articles in SCAN have provided comprehensive overviews of MVPA in general (Weaverdyck et al., 2020) and of representational similarity analysis (RSA) in particular (Popal et al., 2019). MVPA approaches can afford sensitivity to information carried in distributed neural response patterns, provide insight into how particular brain regions organize information (i.e. what rules govern which stimuli or mental states are treated as relatively similar or distinct from one another, in terms of evoked neural response patterns), and elucidate the significance of overlapping activations across different tasks, domains or contexts (Weaverdyck et al., 2020). There are many important considerations for design, analysis and interpretation that should be taken into account when using MVPA, as discussed in depth in the aforementioned papers (Popal et al., 2019; Weaverdyck et al., 2020). One particular consideration pertains to the spatial scale of the representations targeted for study, as discussed briefly in Weaverdyck et al. (2020) and in great detail in this issue by Jolly and Chang (2021). Of note, Jolly and Chang (2021) discuss the assumptions and consequences associated with using MVPA approaches that are sensitive to effects carried at different spatial scales (e.g. searchlight analyses, region-of-interest analyses and whole-brain predictive modeling). They also provide concrete guidelines regarding how researchers can make an informed choice about which analytic technique to use in order to afford maximal sensitivity to the effects that they aim to capture.

Using MVPA to answer questions of interest to social neuroscientists.

This issue also contains examples of how MVPA methods can be applied to answer questions of particular interest to social neuroscientists. For example, Thornton and Tamir (2021) examine the multivoxel response patterns evoked when viewing naturalistic action sequences and suggest a framework (the ACT-FAST model) that the brain may use to represent others’ actions that are currently being observed and to predict future actions. Additionally, Londerée and Wagner (2021) apply RSA to demonstrate that the orbitofrontal cortex encodes multiple dimensions of value for a particular class of stimuli (here, the tastiness and health value of food) and demonstrate that this region contains finer-grained representations of relative tastiness at the upper end of that dimension. While Londerée and Wagner (2021) focus on non-social appetitive stimuli (food), there is great promise in applying such an approach when probing the representation of other people. For example, what dimensions organize our representations of social partners during interactions and how might this change across contexts? What determines which people are represented as particularly distinct from one another? Do we always simultaneously encode multiple dimensions of social value when encountering other people? How might the answers to these questions differ across brain regions?

More generally, Brooks et al. (2021) review how the application of MVPA to fMRI data has afforded insights into the neural basis of social perception. For example, by elucidating the ways in which different brain regions’ representations are organized, MVPA can shed light on how social information is transformed at different stages of processing and how particular brain regions may undergird particular social-perceptual processes.

Establishing causal links between brain and behavior.

An often-noted limitation of functional neuroimaging methods like fMRI is that such methods only allow for establishing correlational relationships between neural and psychological and/or behavioral phenomena. However, an exciting new approach leverages MVPA methods to allow researchers to establish causal links between particular brain states and particular cognitive, affective and behavioral outcomes: fMRI-based decoded neurofeedback. In this issue, Taschereau-Dumouchel et al. (2021) provide an accessible overview of this approach and guidelines for researchers seeking to employ the technique in their own research.

Briefly, in decoded neurofeedback studies, researchers target occurrences of a particular multivoxel response pattern by providing participants with real-time feedback about the activation likelihood of that response pattern, which is also associated with a reward (and, in some cases, the presentation of particular stimuli, to create new associations between the targeted pattern and such stimuli). Participants are not provided with any particular training strategy to evoke the desired patterns and, thus, can be kept unaware of the phenomena with which the elicited patterns are typically associated (e.g. participants could be taught to evoke a neural response pattern associated with seeing a spider without ever knowing that the response pattern that they are evoking typically accompanies seeing a spider). Participants then show specific and robust effects on physiological and behavioral outcomes related to the targeted neural representations. For example, in the aforementioned example, the participant’s fear of spiders would be reduced after repeatedly eliciting neural representations similar to those evoked when viewing spiders, without their awareness of the meaning of the neural patterns that they had evoked (Koizumi et al., 2017; Taschereau-Dumouchel et al., 2018). Across a range of recent studies, researchers have directly and unconsciously shaped a variety of psychological processes (e.g. metacognition, threat reactivity, learning, and emotion perception) by targeting specific neural response patterns through decoded neurofeedback (Taschereau-Dumouchel et al., 2021).

The exciting implications of decoded neurofeedback are not limited to therapeutic interventions (e.g. fear reduction, Koizumi et al., 2017; Taschereau-Dumouchel et al., 2018). This approach also establishes that the induced response patterns caused the observed psychological outcomes, rather than merely establishing a correlational link. Researchers have begun to apply this approach to study social phenomena in a small handful of studies (e.g. Moll et al., 2014; Shibata et al., 2016; Ramot et al., 2017) and holds great promise, particularly given that other methods for establishing causal links between neural and psychological phenomena in humans do so at a much coarser level of granularity. For example, whereas anatomical lesion studies or transcranial magnetic stimulation can causally link entire brain regions to particular psychological or behavioral phenomena, decoded neurofeedback approaches can reveal the causal effects of specific multivoxel response patterns (e.g. Taschereau-Dumouchel et al., 2018) or specific functional connectivity patterns (e.g. Ramot et al., 2017) on particular psychological and behavioral phenomena.

Connectivity and the social brain

Recent advances in social neuroscience have also involved characterizing functional and structural connectivity between brain regions. These have included functional connectome-based ‘fingerprinting’ methods that characterize patterns of functional connectivity between brain regions that are diagnostic of individual identity (Finn et al., 2015) and that are also predictive of various traits (e.g. fluid intelligence, attentional abilities and personality traits, Finn et al., 2015; Rosenberg et al., 2016; Hsu et al., 2018), dimensions of psychopathology (Xia et al., 2018), and people’s social relationships with one another (Hyon et al., 2020b). Other methods for characterizing functional and structural brain that are particularly promising for future social neuroscience applications include examinations of time-varying functional connectivity (Calhoun et al., 2014), which can capture how brain states evolve over time, as well as edge-centric functional network representations, which can capture how connections between brain regions interact with one another (Faskowitz et al., 2020), and multimodal approaches that demonstrate, for example, how the functional profiles of brain regions relate to their underlying patterns of structural connectivity (Saygin et al., 2016; Tovar and Chavez, 2021).

This issue contains two articles focused on a subset of the methods described above, with concrete guidance on how social neuroscience researchers new to such methods can apply them in their own research. First, a ‘Tools of the Trade’ article by Iraji et al. (2021) provides a gentle introduction to time-varying connectivity methods, including guidance on how to use an openly accessible toolbox for capturing properties of dynamic functional connectivity and on interpreting measures that are calculated in such analyses. Second, Tovar and Chavez (2021) link the functional organization of the medial prefrontal cortex (based on patterns of functional coactivation) to its structural organization (based on patterns of structural connectivity) and find similar parcellations of this region using both approaches. These results shed light on the internal organization of a brain region that is consistently implicated in social and affective phenomena—the medial prefrontal cortex (Lieberman et al., 2019)—and provide evidence in support of the notion that the structural connectivity of brain regions constrains their functional profiles. The authors supplement their empirical article with an online tutorial to facilitate the adoption of such approaches by other researchers.

While functional connectivity is often characterized during rest (i.e. in the absence of external stimulation), a growing body of research highlights the value of characterizing patterns of functional connectivity during particular kinds of naturalistic stimulation (e.g. while viewing audiovisual movies). Using data acquired during movie-viewing, rather than rest, yields more reliable estimates of functional connectivity (Wang et al., 2017) that are more predictive of trait-level variables than resting-state functional connectivity (Finn and Bandettini, 2021), particularly when participants are shown movies that are rich in social content. Thus, future work could likely productively extend this approach to gain insight into how individual differences in functional connectivity between brain regions relate to individual differences in how people interact with, think about and emotionally respond to one another.

Examining the brain in its social context

Naturalistic stimuli.

In recent years, there has been considerable interest in examining the neural basis of social and affective processes in contexts that more closely resemble the rich, dynamic perceptual experiences that populate everyday life than traditional fMRI paradigms. The tasks that have traditionally been used in many functional neuroimaging studies have tended to prioritize experimental control, which has often entailed stripping stimuli of their surrounding context (e.g. by presenting participants with images or trials of particular tasks, preceded and followed by fixation crosses or blank screens, Sonkusare et al., 2019). Such approaches have yielded, and continue to yield, many important insights about human brain function. In contrast, naturalistic neuroimaging studies often use exceptionally engaging, context-rich stimuli where narrative meaning unfolds over time (e.g. audiovisual movies and audio recordings of short stories, Sonkusare et al., 2019). Because data from each time point are inextricably linked to its surrounding temporal and narrative context in such paradigms, and since many stimulus-level features often vary in tandem with one another within and across naturalistic stimuli, it is often difficult to ascertain what aspects of stimuli, paradigms or evoked mental processes drive observed effects or to isolate targeted perceptual, cognitive or affective constructs. However, while naturalistic paradigms sacrifice experimental control, they also yield significant benefits, such as increased ecological validity and the ability to study how stimuli are processed when embedded in the narrative and temporal contexts that typically surround them. Examining how brain activity evolves over time in response to complex, dynamic stimuli in which meaning unfolds over the course of minutes or hours, rather than milliseconds, may be particularly valuable for social neuroscientists, given that many of the brain regions that play important roles in social cognition (e.g. regions of the default mode network) have relatively long temporal receptive windows—i.e. are attuned to information that unfolds over relatively long periods of time (Hasson et al., 2008). Thus, the stimuli and data analytic approaches often used in naturalistic neuroimaging studies are particularly well-suited to studying information processing within these regions and the aspects of social functioning that they support.

Researchers often analyze data from naturalistic fMRI studies by linking inter-subject correlations of time series of neural response magnitudes to similarities in subjective understanding, emotional responding, memory or other outcomes (e.g. Yeshurun et al., 2017; Nguyen et al., 2018); see Nastase et al. (2019) for a recent overview of methodological considerations for inter-subject correlation analyses. Recent work has also examined how multivoxel response patterns fluctuate over time (e.g. Baldassano et al., 2017; Hyon et al., 2020a; Chang et al., 2021), as well as patterns of functional connectivity (e.g. Finn and Bandettini, 2021), during naturalistic stimulation.

Inter-subject similarities of neural response time series during naturalistic stimulation have been linked to individual difference variables related to social cognition, such as trait paranoia (Finn et al., 2018) and the development of theory of mind (Richardson et al., 2018), as well as to friendships and the proximity between people in their real-world social networks (Parkinson et al., 2018; Hyon et al., 2020a). Further underscoring the relevance of this approach to social neuroscientists, as described in the preceding section (‘Connectivity and the social brain’), patterns of functional connectivity evoked during naturalistic stimulation are most successful in predicting trait-level variables when the naturalistic stimuli used are rich with social content, such as people, faces and dialog (Finn and Bandettini, 2021).

Social interactions.

Another characteristic that distinguishes how social cognitive processes unfold in everyday life from how they have often been studied in neuroimaging research is their interactive nature. Even when participants in fMRI studies view social stimuli or perform social tasks, they often do so while in a room (e.g. a scanner suite) by themselves, in the absence of social interaction. Sometimes, participants interact with real or virtual social partners who are located outside of the scanner (Redcay et al., 2010), allowing researchers to examine one half of a dyadic social interaction, which is sufficient to fruitfully examine many phenomena, such as the neural basis of making socially guided decisions during interactions (Carter et al., 2012). Examining how brains dynamically interact during social interactions, however, demands different approaches to data acquisition and analysis.

From the perspective of data acquisition, some studies have adopted hyper-scanning approaches to fMRI, where two or more participants can communicate with each other while being scanned simultaneously (for a recent review, see Misaki et al., 2021); others have examined inter-brain coupling in social contexts using electroencephalography (e.g. Dumas et al., 2010; Dikker et al., 2017; Goldstein et al., 2018) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) (for a recent review, see Burns and Lieberman, 2019). fNIRS is a particularly promising method for studying social interactions in contexts that bear a strong resemblance to everyday life, given that participants can move, express themselves and interact relatively freely, allowing for more natural behavior. Moreover, fNIRS equipment is often quite portable, affording the ability to collect data in a greater diversity of settings than most other neuroimaging modalities would allow (Burns et al., 2019; Burns and Lieberman, 2019).

From the perspective of data analysis, while many studies have looked at inter-subject correlations of neural response time series during social interactions, mirroring common practices in naturalistic neuroimaging more generally, it is clear that such an approach does not capture all aspects of interpersonal coupling that take place during social interactions (Hasson and Frith, 2016). Advancing our understanding of how the human brain supports dynamic, reciprocal social interactions will require the further adoption and development of data analytic approaches that are capable of capturing other aspects of interpersonal complementarity during social interactions and relating such complementarity to properties of social interactions, to the construction of shared meaning, and to other individual-, dyad- and group-level outcomes; see Redcay and Schilbach (2019) for a thoughtful recent discussion of relevant issues and methods.

Social networks.

A growing body of research also integrates approaches from social neuroscience for characterizing information processing within individual brains with approaches for characterizing the social networks that people inhabit (e.g. Zerubavel et al., 2015; Parkinson et al., 2017; Schmälzle et al., 2017; Peer et al., 2021); for recent reviews, see Falk and Bassett (2017) and Weaverdyck and Parkinson (2018). Integrating approaches for collecting and analyzing real-world social network data into social neuroscience can provide opportunities to gain insight into how people understand, shape and are shaped by the structure of their social worlds. In this issue, Baek et al. (2021) provide an overview for social neuroscientists of key theoretical and methodological issues in social network analysis, with examples of how approaches from social neuroscience and social network analysis can be productively combined, tutorials to aid in the practical implementation of many key concepts, and a discussion of outstanding issues and questions in this area of research.

Approaches using naturalistic stimuli, interactive paradigms and characterizations of real-world social networks can greatly enrich our understanding of how the brain supports social processes in everyday life. By taking into account more aspects of the contexts in which social processing unfolds in the real world, such approaches promise to complement the continued use of more constrained experimental paradigms that afford greater internal validity and that have provided much of the foundational knowledge on which the research discussed in this section has been built.

Conclusions

The set of computational tools employed in social neuroscience continues to grow and advance. Such tools provide new ways to richly characterize the psychological processes that underlie social decisions, neural responses at different levels of granularity, and the real-world social contexts in which social cognition and behavior unfold. Efforts to continue to advance our understanding of the neural basis of social thought and behavior will no doubt benefit from integrative approaches, both in terms of examining how different cues and computations are integrated by the brain to support social interactions (Molapour et al., 2021) and in terms of integrating computational approaches that capture the information contained in multivariate neural data, in modeled approximations of social thought and behavior, and in our complex social surroundings. Such interdisciplinary endeavors are increasingly recognized as necessary for addressing fundamental questions about human behavior (for a thoughtful recent discussion of the challenges associated with the increasingly interdisciplinary nature of social science and potential solutions to such challenges, see Buyalskaya et al., 2021). Continuing to adopt, develop and combine computational methods for characterizing the patterns of social ties that surround people, the psychological processes supporting social behavior and the wealth of information contained in neuroimaging data promises to advance our understanding of how people create, make sense of and navigate their social worlds.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (grant numbers 1835239 and 2048212).

Conflict of interest

The authors declared that they had no conflict of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

References

  1. Baek E.C., Porter M.A., Parkinson C. (2021). Social network analysis for social neuroscientists. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 883–901.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baldassano C., Chen J., Zadbood A., Pillow J.W., Hasson U., Norman K.A. (2017). Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron, 95(3), 709–21.e5.doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brooks J.A., Stolier R.M., Freeman J.B. (2021). Computational approaches to the neuroscience of social perception. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,16(8), 827–837.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Burns S.M., Barnes L.N., McCulloh I.A., et al. (2019). Making social neuroscience less WEIRD: using fNIRS to measure neural signatures of persuasive influence in a middle east participant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(3), e1–1.doi: 10.1037/pspa0000144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Burns S.M., Lieberman M. (2019). The use of fNIRS for unique contributions to social and affective neuroscience. PsyArXiv. 10.31234/osf.io/kygbm. [Google Scholar]
  6. Buyalskaya A., Gallo M., Camerer C.F. (2021). The golden age of social science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(5), e2002923118.doi: 10.1073/pnas.2002923118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Calhoun V.D.D., Miller R., Pearlson G., Adali T., Adalı T. (2014). The chronnectome: time-varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery. Neuron, 84(2), 262–74.doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carter R.M., Bowling D.L., Reeck C., Huettel S.A. (2012). A distinct role of the temporal-parietal junction in predicting socially guided decisions. Science, 337(6090), 109–11.doi: 10.1126/science.1219681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chang L.J., Jolly E., Cheong J.H., et al. (2021). Endogenous variation in ventromedial prefrontal cortex state dynamics during naturalistic viewing reflects affective experience. Science Advances, 7(17), 7129–52.doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abf7129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Charnov E.L. (1976). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9(2), 129–36.doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dikker S., Wan L., Davidesco I., et al. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Current Biology, 27(9), 1375–80.doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dumas G., Nadel J., Soussignan R., Martinerie J., Garnero L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One, 5(8), 12166.doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dunbar R.I.M. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32(1), 163–81.doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Falk E.B., Bassett D.S. (2017). Brain and social networks: fundamental building blocks of human experience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 674–90.doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Faskowitz J., Esfahlani F.Z., Jo Y., Sporns O., Betzel R.F. (2020). Edge-centric functional network representations of human cerebral cortex reveal overlapping system-level architecture. Nature Neuroscience, 23(12), 1644–54.doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00719-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Finn E.S., Shen X., Scheinost D., et al. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 1664–71.doi: 10.1038/nn.4135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Finn E.S., Corlett P.R., Chen G., Bandettini P.A., Constable R.T. (2018). Trait paranoia shapes inter-subject synchrony in brain activity during an ambiguous social narrative. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1–13.doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04387-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Finn E.S., Bandettini P.A. (2021). Movie-watching outperforms rest for functional connectivity-based prediction of behavior. NeuroImage, 235, 117963.doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gabay A.S., Apps M.A.J. (2021). Foraging optimally in social neuroscience: computations and methodological considerations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 782–794doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldstein P., Weissman-Fogel I., Dumas G., Shamay-Tsoory S.G. (2018). Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(11), E2528–37.doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hasson U., Yang E., Vallines I., Heeger D.J., Rubin N. (2008). A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(10), 2539–50.doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5487-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hasson U., Frith C.D. (2016). Mirroring and beyond: coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150366.doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hsu W.T., Rosenberg M.D., Scheinost D., Constable R.T., Chun M.M. (2018). Resting-state functional connectivity predicts neuroticism and extraversion in novel individuals. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(2), 224–32.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hyon R., Kleinbaum A.M., Parkinson C. (2020a). Social network proximity predicts similar trajectories of psychological states: evidence from multi-voxel spatiotemporal dynamics. NeuroImage, 216, 116492.doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hyon R., Youm Y., Kim J., Chey J., Kwak S., Parkinson C. (2020b). Similarity in functional brain connectivity at rest predicts friendship in the social network of an entire village. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(52), 33149–60.doi: 10.1073/pnas.2013606117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Iraji A., Faghiri A., Lewis N., Fu Z., Rachakonda S., Calhoun V.D. (2021). Tools of the trade: estimating time-varying connectivity patterns from fMRI data. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 849–874.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jolly E., Chang L.J. (2021). Multivariate spatial feature selection in fMRI. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 795–806.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Koizumi A., Amano K., Cortese A., et al. (2017). Fear reduction without fear through reinforcement of neural activity that bypasses conscious exposure. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(1), 1–7.doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lieberman M.D., Straccia M.A., Meyer M.L., Du M., Tan K.M. (2019). Social, self, (situational), and affective processes in medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC): causal, multivariate, and reverse inference evidence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 99, 311–28.doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lockwood P.L., Klein-Flügge M.C. (2021). Computational modelling of social cognition and behaviour—a reinforcement learning primer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 761–771.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Londerée A.M., Wagner D.D. (2021). The orbitofrontal cortex spontaneously encodes food health and contains more distinct representations for foods highest in tastiness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 816–826.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Misaki M., Kerr K.L., Ratliff E.L., et al. (2021). Beyond synchrony: the capacity of fMRI hyperscanning for the study of human social interaction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(1–2), 84–92.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mobbs D., Trimmer P.C., Blumstein D.T., Dayan P. (2018). Foraging for foundations in decision neuroscience: insights from ethology. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 19(7), 419–27.doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0010-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Molapour T., Hagan C.C., Silston B., et al. (2021). Seven computations of the social brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 745–760.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moll J., Weingartner J.H., Bado P., et al. (2014). Voluntary enhancement of neural signatures of affiliative emotion using fMRI neurofeedback. PLoS One, 9(5), 97343.doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nastase S.A., Gazzola V., Hasson U., Keysers C. (2019). Measuring shared responses across subjects using intersubject correlation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(6), 669–87.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nguyen M., Vanderwal T., Hasson U. (2018). Shared understanding of narratives is correlated with shared neural responses. NeuroImage, 184, 161–70.doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Park B., Fareri D., Delgado M., Young L. (2021). The role of right temporoparietal junction in processing social prediction error across relationship contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 772–781.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Parkinson C., Kleinbaum A.M., Wheatley T. (2017). Spontaneous neural encoding of social network position. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0072.doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0072. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Parkinson C., Kleinbaum A.M., Wheatley T. (2018). Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nature Communications, 9(1), 332.doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Peer M., Hayman M., Tamir B., Arzy S. (2021). Brain coding of social network structure. Journal of Neuroscience. JN-RM-2641-20, 41(22), 4897–909.doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2641-20.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Popal H., Wang Y., Olson I.R. (2019). A guide to representational similarity analysis for social neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(11), 1243–53.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ramot M., Kimmich S., Gonzalez-Castillo J., et al. (2017). Direct modulation of aberrant brain network connectivity through real-time NeuroFeedback. eLife, 6, e28974.doi: 10.7554/eLife.28974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Redcay E., Dodell-Feder D., Pearrow M.J., et al. (2010). Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: a new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1639–47.doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Redcay E., Schilbach L. (2019). Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 20(8), 495–505.doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Richardson H., Lisandrelli G., Riobueno-Naylor A., Saxe R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1–12.doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosenberg M.D., Finn E.S., Scheinost D., et al. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 165–71.doi: 10.1038/nn.4179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Saygin Z.M., Osher D.E., Norton E.S., et al. (2016). Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nature Neuroscience, 19(9), 1250–5.doi: 10.1038/nn.4354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schmälzle R., Brook O’Donnell M., Garcia J.O., et al. (2017). Brain connectivity dynamics during social interaction reflect social network structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(20), 5153–8.doi: 10.1073/pnas.1616130114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shibata K., Watanabe T., Kawato M., Sasaki Y. (2016). Differential activation patterns in the same brain region led to opposite emotional states. PLoS Biology, 14(9), e1002546.doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sonkusare S., Breakspear M., Guo C. (2019). Naturalistic stimuli in neuroscience: critically acclaimed. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(8), 699–714.doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Taschereau-Dumouchel V., Cortese A., Chiba T., Knotts J.D., Kawato M., Lau H. (2018). Towards an unconscious neural reinforcement intervention for common fears. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(13), 3470–5.doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721572115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Taschereau-Dumouchel V., Cortese A., Lau H., Kawato M. (2021). Conducting decoded neurofeedback studies. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 838–848.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Thornton M.A., Tamir D.I. (2021). Perceiving actions before they happen: psychological dimensions scaffold neural action prediction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 807–815.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tovar D.T., Chavez R.S. (2021). Large-scale functional coactivation patterns reflect the structural connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(8), 875–882.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang J., Ren Y., Hu X., et al. (2017). Test–retest reliability of functional connectivity networks during naturalistic fMRI paradigms. Human Brain Mapping, 38(4), 2226–41.doi: 10.1002/hbm.23517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Weaverdyck M.E., Lieberman M.D., Parkinson C. (2020). Multivoxel pattern analysis in fMRI: a practical introduction for social and affective neuroscientists. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(4), 487–509.doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Weaverdyck M.E., Parkinson C. (2018). The neural representation of social networks. Current Opinion in Psychology, 24, 58–66.doi: 10.1016/J.COPSYC.2018.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Xia C.H., Ma Z., Ciric R., et al. (2018). Linked dimensions of psychopathology and connectivity in functional brain networks. Nature Communications, 9, 3003.doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05317-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yeshurun Y., Swanson S., Simony E., et al. (2017). Same story, different story: the neural representation of interpretive frameworks. Psychological Science, 28(3), 307–19.doi: 10.1177/0956797616682029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Zerubavel N., Bearman P.S., Weber J., Ochsner K.N. (2015). Neural mechanisms tracking popularity in real-world social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(49), 15072–7.doi: 10.1073/pnas.1511477112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES