Abstract
Background:
Knowledge of characteristics in upper limb dystonia remains limited, derived primarily from small, single-site studies.
Objective:
The objective of this study was to characterize demographic and clinical characteristics of upper limb dystonia from the Dystonia Coalition data set, a large, international, multicenter resource.
Methods:
We evaluated clinical and demographic characteristics of 367 participants with upper limb dystonia from onset, comparing across subcategories of focal (with and without dystonia spread) versus nonfocal onset.
Results:
Focal onset occurred in 80%; 67% remained focal without spread. Task specificity was most frequent in this subgroup, most often writer’s cramp and affecting the dominant limb (83%). Focal onset with spread was more frequent in young onset (<21 years). Focal onset occurred equally in women and men; nonfocal onset affected women disproportionately.
Conclusions:
Upper limb dystonia distribution, focality, and task specificity relate to onset age and likelihood of regional spread. Observations may inform clinical counseling and design, execution, and interpretation of future studies.
Keywords: writer’s cramp, musician’s cramp, task-specific, sensory trick, alleviating maneuver
Adult-onset isolated limb dystonia more commonly affects the upper rather than lower limbs and can be focal or part of segmental, multifocal, hemi-, or generalized dystonia.1 Most studies of adult-onset isolated upper limb dystonia (ULD) focus on subtypes of task-specific focal dystonia such as writer’s cramp,2–5 typist’s cramp,6 musician’s cramp,7–11 or sports-related dystonia.12 In contrast, the demographic and clinical features of ULD are primarily derived from relatively small populations of predominantly young-onset, generalized dystonia.13,14 Prior studies reported that body region of onset, positive family history, and a younger age at onset may increase the risk of spread of dystonia.13,15–20 In fact, prospective analysis in a relatively small adult-onset component of the current data set demonstrated a relationship between initial body region affected (including hand) and risk of spread.21 Thus, clinical and demographic features are critical for understanding important subtypes of ULD. Such clinical characteristics are essential for design and implementation of future studies that aim to test symptomatic or disease-modifying interventions in isolated limb dystonia.21,22 The primary aim of this report is to characterize clinical features and demographics of isolated ULD where the upper limb was involved at onset. Data are derived from the Dystonia Coalition database, the largest standardized, multicenter cohort ever assembled.
Methods
Data were obtained from the Dystonia Coalition database (https://www.rarediseasesnetwork.org/cms/dystonia/). A detailed description of the study population and dystonia classification is provided in the Supporting Information. All participants gave written informed consent at the recruiting site according to the Declaration of Helsinki and The Common Rule. Analysis of deidentified aggregate data was approved by the Emory University Human Subjects Review Board and the Human Research Protection Office at Washington University. We only included data from the first visit if a participant had multiple visits.
Eligibility for this study required a diagnosis of isolated dystonia and an age of at least 18 years. A clinician-administered standardized questionnaire and video were collected for each participant according to the standard protocol at the time of enrollment.23 All participants were evaluated by movement disorders specialists either in person or by a standardized video to note involvement of each affected body region (upper face, lower face, tongue, larynx, neck, shoulder, upper arm, hand, trunk, pelvis, upper leg, foot), the affected side of the body, and Burke-Fahn-Marsden dystonia rating scores. Spread was determined by any change in the participants’ recall of onset regions and the body regions affected at study intake. In addition, video recordings included each participant’s demonstration of a sensory trick.23
We performed comparisons of clinical and demographic variables across subgroups that included focal versus nonfocal onset, focal onset without spread versus focal onset with spread, and young (<21 years old) versus adult (≥21 years old) onset according to established criteria.24 The following variables were compared across subgroups: gender, mean age of onset, duration of dystonia (excluding focal vs. nonfocal onset subgroups), frequency of task specificity and sensory tricks, and Burke-Fahn-Marsden score. We applied independent 2-tailed Student’s t tests for comparisons of continuous data and Pearson χ2 statistics to test the main effect of associations between nominal clinical characteristic variables across each dystonia subgroup with the null hypothesis of no association. All statistics were considered significant with P < 0.05, adjusted using Bonferroni correction to control for family-wise error rate in independent subgroup analyses. We performed all statistics with IBM (Armonk, NY) SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 25.0.
Results
Clinical Characteristics
A total of 367 participants reported ULD at onset (demographics in Supporting Information Table S1). Focal onset occurred in 292/367 (80%), of which 195 (67%) remained focal without spread. Adult onset was reported in 286/367 (78%). Limb onset affected the dominant upper limb in 303/367 (83%), preferentially involving the hand with (85%) or without arm (21%) at onset. Table 1 displays clinical characteristics for all participants across subgroups. The dominant limb was affected more frequently (χ2 = 4.5, P = 0.034) in those with (212/246, 85%) versus without (92/121, 76%) task-specific dystonia. Gene mutations thought to be associated with the dystonia were reported in 13/367 participants, including 12 with onset before age 21 years (Supporting Information Table S2).
TABLE 1.
All | Focal Onset | Versus | Nonfocal Onset | χ2 or t | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of participants | 367 | 292 (80%) | 75 (20%) | |||
Female | 210 (57%) | 157 (54%)a | 53 (71%)a | 7.0 | 0.008 | |
Mean age of onset, y | 37.5 ± 18.1 | 35.8 ± 17.5a | 44.4 ± 18.4a | −3.6 | 0.001 | |
Task specificity | 246 (67%) | 205 (70%) | 41 (55%) | 6.5 | 0.011 | |
Sensory trick | 153 (42%) | 107 (37%)a | 46 (61%)a | 15.0 | <0.001 | |
Onset, dominant limb | 303 (83%) | 239 (82%) | 64 (85%) | 0.5 | 0.478 | |
BFM total | 10.2 ± 11.2 | 9.6 ± 11.6 | 12.2 ± 9 | −2.3 | 0.023 | |
No Spread | Versus | Spread | χ2 or t | P | ||
Number of participants | 195 | 97 | ||||
Female | 105 (54%)b | 52 (54%) | 0.001 | 0.969 | ||
Mean age of onset, y | 38.9 ± 15.4b | 29.5 ± 19.6b | 4.1 | <0.001 | ||
Dystonia duration, y | 14.5 ± 12.9b | 24.1 ± 16b | −5.1 | <0.001 | ||
Sensory trick | 57 (29%)b | 50 (52%)b | 13.9 | <0.001 | ||
Onset dominant limb | 164 (84%) | 75 (77%)b | 2.0 | 0.157 | ||
BFM total | 4.9 ± 4.2b | 18.6 ± 15.6b | −8.4 | <0.001 |
Significant differences across focal versus nonfocal upper limb onset following focal upper limb onset.
Significant differences across no spread versus spread following focal upper limb onset.
All participants reporting upper limb involvement at onset, then divided into (top) focal onset versus nonfocal (limb plus another body region) onset and (bottom) without and with spread to other body regions. Pearson χ2 or t statistic are reported for nominal or continuous group comparisons, respectively. Significant results appear in bold font. Data are n (%) or mean ± standard deviation.
Abbreviation: BFM, Burke-Fahn-Marsden scale.
Young Onset Versus Adult Onset
Table 2 compares participants reporting young-onset (age of onset <21) and adult-onset (≥21 years) ULD. The proportion of those with focal onset was similar across young onset versus adult onset, but the young-onset group had more frequent regional spread (Supporting Information Figure S1).
TABLE 2.
Young Onset | Versus | Adult Onset | χ2 or t | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of participants | 81 | 286 | |||
Female | 44 (54%) | 166 (58%) | 0.3 | 0.566 | |
Mean age of onset, y | 11.6 ± 5.3a | 44.9 ± 12.9a | −34.4 | <0.001 | |
Dystonia duration, y | 31.6 ± 17.2a | 13.3 ± 10.6* | 9.1 | <0.001 | |
Task specificity | 37 (45%)a | 208 (73%)a | 21.2 | <0.001 | |
Sensory trick | 34 (42%) | 119 (42%) | 0.001 | 0.972 | |
BFM total | 15.8 ± 14.8a | 8.6 ± 9.3a | 4.1 | <0.001 | |
Focal onset | 71 (86%) | 221 (76%) | 4.2 | 0.040 | |
Spread after onset | 41 (49%)a | 56 (19%)a | 25.2 | <0.001 | |
Onset, dominant limb | 64 (79%) | 238 (84%) | 0.9 | 0.347 | |
Focal | 27 (33%) | 160 (56%) | |||
Segmental | 22 (27%) | 92 (32%) | |||
Multifocal | 12 (15%) | 18 (6%) | |||
Generalized | 19 (23%) | 14 (5%) |
significant differences across onset age subgroups.
Young (age <21 years) onset and adult (age ≥21). Pearson χ2 or t statistic are reported for nominal or continuous group comparisons, respectively. Percentage focal onset and spread after onset reflect a percentage of upper limb onset, whereas all other percentages reflect the percentage of participants in young versus adult onset. Significant results appear in bold font. Data are n (%) or mean ± standard deviation.
BFM, Burke-Fahn-Marsden scale.
Task Specificity
Participants with task specificity (n = 246) reported writer’s cramp (n = 194), musician’s cramp (n = 30), typist cramp (n = 25), and sport-related cramp (n = 4). Participants may have had more than 1 type (n = 7), including writer’s cramp and typist cramp (n = 3), musician’s cramp and writer’s cramp (n = 1), and musician’s cramp and typist cramp (n = 3). Musician’s cramp was more commonly associated with focal onset without spread (27/30 instances) than focal onset with spread (3/30). Speech-related (laryngeal) dystonia was present in those with nonfocal ULD onset (10/13 instances) more frequently than focal onset with spread (3/13 instances).
Sensory Tricks
Of participants reporting a sensory trick, 57/57 (100%) with focal onset and no spread, and 32/50 (64%) with focal onset and spread reported sensory tricks isolated to the limb. The remaining 18 participants with spread reported sensory tricks relieved dystonia in other body parts, but not limb dystonia.
Discussion
Demographic and clinical characteristics from the largest multicenter, international cohort of patients with standardized evaluations of ULD suggest the existence of distinct subgroups based on focal versus nonfocal onset and focal onset with and without spread. Specifically, age of onset, sex, and frequency of task specificity and sensory tricks differed across subgroups. Consideration of such features may be necessary for accurate patient prognosis and in the design and interpretation of treatments with symptomatic or disease-modifying properties.
ULD onset in our entire cohort occurs across the age spectrum, with peak frequency in the fifth decade. Subgroup analysis indicates increased prevalence of focal onset and spread among young onset, consistent with prior observations following lower limb onset.13,14,25 In fact, 12/13 participants in our cohort with an identified gene recall onset in youth, 8 with TOR1A-associated dystonia. Although this supports the fact that TOR1Aassociated and other genetic dystonia commonly contribute to ULD onset in youth, we advise caution given likely clinical selection bias in testing this population. A prospective assessment of 47 adults with hand dystonia (included in the current study) indicated a mean span of 3.3 years from onset until spread to another body region.21 This relatively short time to spread and a plateau of the Kaplan-Meier curve in conjunction with our current data support the notion that ULD spread is not solely related to dystonia duration but also age of onset. One must consider inherent study bias when comparing duration of dystonia in our study to others15,19,20 because we only recruited those at least 18 years of age and asked for recall regarding onset region(s).
Our data indicate that a higher proportion of women to men are affected by ULD, but this ratio is nearly 1:1 in the focal-onset group, independent of spread. A recent meta-review suggests a predominance of women with isolated focal dystonia, except for ULD.26 Others have previously observed gender differences across various forms of task-specific limb dystonia. For instance, a higher prevalence of men may be affected by musician’s cramp.7,27 This contrasts with a report that 62% of 105 consecutive patients with writer’s cramp were women.4 Inconsistences in the gender ratios across various forms of task-specific dystonia may represent true differences between men and women, but may also reflect population differences in the number of men versus women who are musicians, for instance. Indeed, 77% of participants with musician’s dystonia were men in our study, whereas 38% and 48% were men among those with writer’s cramp and typist cramp, respectively. Thus, future studies should account for onset site and spread as well as task specificity when attempting to assess the potential contribution of sex differences in dystonia.
Task specificity is most frequent in focal-onset ULD without spread and least frequent with nonfocal onset. When we analyzed 324 participants in an independent Dystonia Coalition cohort without limb involvement at onset that later developed ULD, only 21% reported task specificity. Thus, task-specific ULD appears to favor those with focal onset without regional spread. Across all subgroups analyzed, the frequency of writer’s cramp outnumbered all additional forms of task specificity. This probably reflects the high prevalence of writer’s cramp in the general population, although there are no well-designed epidemiological studies estimating its prevalence. The observation that ULD affects the dominant limb in 85% supports this concept and may support prior reports that relatively intensive use of a body region contributes to task specificity.4,28,29 Our findings indicate that studies of task specificity should focus on focal ULD without regional spread to better differentiate the pathophysiological basis of altered sensory input,30,31 maladaptive plasticity,32 and disruption of large-scale neural connectivity.33
There is relative paucity of upper arm involvement that influences classification of body distribution (eg, segmental vs. multifocal). Whether scarceness of upper arm dystonia results from discrepancies in the sensitivity of examination or anatomical or pathophysiological mechanisms produce such differences remains uncertain. Muscles controlling the upper arm extend across shoulder girdles, thus complicating clear demarcation of “segmental” dystonia and application of currently available scales for dystonia.34 Future efforts should focus on improving diagnostic criteria for detecting upper segment limb dystonia.
Study limitations include retrospective recall of the body region affected at dystonia onset. In addition, those with mild disease that are either underrecognized or not seeking treatment are less likely to attend one of our recruiting sites. The Dystonia Coalition does not require neuroimaging of the neural axis to exclude potentially overlooked secondary structural lesions that may contribute to a small percentage of those categorized as isolated dystonia for study entry.35,36 Furthermore, all examinations were performed at least 2 months after botulinum toxin injection, but lingering effects could still alter dystonia manifestations.37 Nevertheless, these data on ULD focality, task specificity, and likelihood of regional spread may inform clinical counseling and future efforts in the design, execution, and interpretation of clinical trials.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments:
The authors would like to thank Meghan Campbell, PhD, and Susan Nielsen, PhD, for helpful discussion regarding statistical applications.
Funding agencies: Dystonia Coalition (U54NS065701, U54TR001456, U54NS116025), a part of the Rare Diseases Clinical Research Network, an initiative of the Office of Rare Diseases Research, National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS). The Dystonia Coalition is funded through collaboration between NCATS and the National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Stroke.
Footnotes
Supporting Data
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site.
Relevant conflicts of interests/financial disclosures: Nothing to report.
References
- 1.Pont-Sunyer C, Marti MJ, Tolosa E. Focal limb dystonia. Eur J Neurol 2010;17(suppl 1):22–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Das CP, Prabhakar S, Truong D. Clinical profile of various subtypes of writer’s cramp. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2007;13(7): 421–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jhunjhunwala K, Lenka A, Pal PK. A clinical profile of 125 patients with writer’s cramp. Eur Neurol 2015;73(5–6):316–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Roze E, Soumare A, Pironneau I, et al. Case-control study of writer’s cramp. Brain 2009;132(Pt 3):756–764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sheehy MP, Marsden CD. Writers’ cramp-a focal dystonia. Brain 1982;105(Pt 3):461–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ham JH, Kim SJ, Song SK, et al. A prognostic factor in focal hand dystonia: typist’s cramp cases and literature review. J Neurol Sci 2016;371:85–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Brandfonbrener AG, Robson C. Review of 113 musicians with focal dystonia seen between 1985 and 2002 at a clinic for performing artists. Adv Neurol 2004;94:255–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gatto EM, Pardal MM, Reisin RC, Pardal AM. Playing harp, another unusual task-specific dystonia. Mov Disord 2001;16(4): 778–779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ragothaman M, Sarangmath N, Jayaram S, Swaminath PV, Muthane U. Task-specific dystonia in tabla players. Mov Disord 2004;19(10):1254–1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schuele SU, Lederman RJ. Long-term outcome of focal dystonia in instrumental musicians. Adv Neurol 2004;94:261–266. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bianchi S, Fuertinger S, Huddleston H, Frucht SJ, Simonyan K. Functional and structural neural bases of task specificity in isolated focal dystonia. Mov Disord 2019;34(4):555–563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dhungana S, Jankovic J. Yips and other movement disorders in golfers. Mov Disord 2013;28(5):576–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Marsden CD, Harrison MJ. Idiopathic torsion dystonia (dystonia musculorum deformans). A review of forty-two patients. Brain 1974;97(4):793–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Marsden CD, Harrison MJ, Bundey S. Natural history of idiopathic torsion dystonia. Adv Neurol 1976;14:177–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Abbruzzese G, Berardelli A, Girlanda P, et al. Long-term assessment of the risk of spread in primary late-onset focal dystonia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79(4):392–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Defazio G, Berardelli A, Abbruzzese G, et al. Risk factors for spread of primary adult onset blepharospasm: a multicentre investigation of the Italian movement disorders study group. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67(5):613–619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Martino D, Berardelli A, Abbruzzese G, et al. Age at onset and symptom spread in primary adult-onset blepharospasm and cervical dystonia. Mov Disord 2012;27(11):1447–1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Svetel M, Pekmezovic T, Jovic J, et al. Spread of primary dystonia in relation to initially affected region. J Neurol 2007;254(7):879–883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Svetel M, Pekmezovic T, Tomic A, Kresojevic N, Kostic VS. The spread of primary late-onset focal dystonia in a long-term follow up study. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2015;132:41–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Weiss EM, Hershey T, Karimi M, et al. Relative risk of spread of symptoms among the focal onset primary dystonias. Mov Disord 2006;21(8):1175–1181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Berman BD, Groth CL, Sillau SH, et al. Risk of spread in adult-onset isolated focal dystonia: a prospective international cohort study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2020;91(3):314–320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Balint B, Mencacci NE, Valente EM, et al. Dystonia. Nat Rev Dis Primers 2018;4(1):25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Yan L, Hicks M, Winslow K, et al. Secured web-based video repository for multicenter studies. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2015;21(4): 366–371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Albanese A, Bhatia K, Bressman SB, et al. Phenomenology and classification of dystonia: a consensus update. Mov Disord 2013;28(7): 863–873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Greene P, Kang UJ, Fahn S. Spread of symptoms in idiopathic torsion dystonia. Mov Disord 1995;10(2):143–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jinnah HA, Berardelli A, Comella C, et al. The focal dystonias: current views and challenges for future research. Mov Disord 2013;28 (7):926–943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Epidemiological Study of Dystonia in Europe Collaborative Group. A prevalence study of primary dystonia in eight European countries. J Neurol 2000;247(10):787–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Le Floch A, Vidailhet M, Flamand-Rouviere C, et al. Table tennis dystonia. Mov Disord 2010;25(3):394–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Chang FC, Frucht SJ. Motor and sensory dysfunction in musician’s dystonia. Curr Neuropharmacol 2013;11(1):41–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Conte A, Defazio G, Hallett M, Fabbrini G, Berardelli A. The role of sensory information in the pathophysiology of focal dystonias. Nat Rev Neurol 2019;15(4):224–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rosenkranz K, Williamon A, Butler K, Cordivari C, Lees AJ, Rothwell JC. Pathophysiological differences between musician’s dystonia and writer’s cramp. Brain 2005;128(Pt 4):918–931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gallea C, Herath P, Voon V, et al. Loss of inhibition in sensorimotor networks in focal hand dystonia. NeuroImage Clinical 2018;17:90–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jin SH, Lin P, Hallett M. Abnormal reorganization of functional cortical small-world networks in focal hand dystonia. PloS One 2011;6(12):e28682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.LeDoux MS. Meige syndrome: what’s in a name? Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2009;15(7):483–489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Marsden CD, Obeso JA, Zarranz JJ, Lang AE. The anatomical basis of symptomatic hemidystonia. Brain 1985;108(Pt 2):463–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gupta N, Pandey S. Post-thalamic stroke movement disorders: a systematic review. Eur Neurol 2018;79(5–6):303–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mittal SO, Lenka A, Jankovic J. Botulinum toxin for the treatment of tremor. Parkinsonism Relat Disorder 2019;63:31–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.