Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Aug 9;16(8):e0238663. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238663

Personality, subjective well-being, and the serotonin 1a receptor gene in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)

Alexander Weiss 1,2,*, Chihiro Yokoyama 3,¤, Takuya Hayashi 3, Miho Inoue-Murayama 1
Editor: Nicolas Chaline4
PMCID: PMC8351977  PMID: 34370743

Abstract

Studies of personality traits in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) indicate that there are five or six constructs—Sociability, Dominance, Neuroticism, Openness, and two related to Conscientiousness. The present study attempted to determine whether our earlier study of laboratory-housed individuals only yielded three—Dominance, Sociability, and Neuroticism—because of a low amount of between-subjects variance. To do so, we increased our sample size from 77 to 128. In addition, we ascertained the reliability and validity of ratings and whether polymorphisms related to the serotonin 1a receptor were associated with personality. We found Sociability, Dominance, and Negative Affect factors that resembled three domains found in previous studies, including ours. We also found an Openness and Impulsiveness factor, the latter of which bore some resemblance to Conscientiousness, and two higher-order factors, Pro-sociality and Boldness. In further analyses, we could not exclude the possibility that Pro-sociality and Boldness represented a higher-level of personality organization. Correlations between personality factors and well-being were consistent with the definitions of the factors. There were no significant associations between personality and genotype. These results suggest that common marmoset personality structure varies as a function of rearing or housing variables that have not yet been investigated systematically.

Introduction

Common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are small New-World monkeys that populate a wide range of habitats in South America [1,2]. Because of their size, fast life history, and other physical and physiological characteristics, common marmosets are an increasingly popular animal model in biomedical research [3], although some [e.g., 4] have highlighted the shortcomings of marmoset models.

Common marmosets are also becoming popular subjects for research on cognition and personality. This trend has been driven partly by findings that common marmosets display behaviors and capabilities once believed to be exclusive to humans and great apes. Common marmosets, for example, exhibit high levels of spontaneous cooperative behavior [5,6] and can discriminate between third parties that do and do not reciprocate [7]. These capabilities and others are believed to have evolved in common marmosets because they, like other callitrichids, but unlike other nonhuman primates, are cooperative breeders [see 8 for a review]. In species that are cooperative breeders, rather than disperse and mate, the adult siblings and offspring of mating pairs often stay with the family unit to help raise offspring, and so delay or forego reproduction [9].

Studies of common marmosets have revealed the presence of stable personality traits [10,11], although one study found that these traits can be modified via social facilitation [12]. Studies have also found that personality traits in common marmosets are heritable and related to well-being [13], associated with the strength of laterality [14], and the binding potential of serotonin transporters in the brain [15]. Moreover, personality variables that are based on behavioral observations and those based on ratings are correlated when both assess the same psychological construct, and uncorrelated when they do not, respectively [16,17]. However, at least one study found that this evidence for the convergent and discriminant validity of personality measures does not necessarily generalize across samples [16].

Studies of common marmosets have contributed to the attempt to reconstruct the evolutionary history of personality structure [18,19]. Personality structure refers to the fact that statistical methods, including factor analysis and principal components analysis [20], but also others [e.g., 21], reveal that individual traits group into higher-order factors, which represent personality domains. In humans, for example, factor analysis has shown that traits such as ‘fearful’, ‘vulnerable’ and ‘anxious’ describe the Neuroticism domain while traits such as ‘active’, ‘social’, and ‘assertive’ describe the Extraversion domain [22,23].

Like studies of other nonhuman primate taxa, including Macaca [24], Pan [25], Saimiri [26], Sapajus and Cebus [2729], and other Callitrichids [30], studies of common marmosets [13,17,31,32] have yielded findings consistent with the notion that a species’ socioecology influences that species’ personality structure [see 33 for a discussion]. These studies of common marmosets, despite differences in the origins and housing of subjects, reveal a set of overlapping personality domains (Table 1): all five revealed domains related to sociability [13,17,31,32]; four revealed domains related to aggressiveness and competitive prowess [13,17,31,32]; three revealed domains related to anxiety and vigilance [13,17,32]; two revealed domains related to exploratory tendencies [17,31]; and two revealed domains related to self-control [17,31]. In addition, two of these studies found domains—Perceptual Sensitivity [17] and Patience [31]—that were not found in other species, but which may also be related to self-control.

Table 1. Personality domains found in previous studies grouped by psychological construct.

Construct Study Data Analysis Label
Sociability
Yokoyama et al. (2011) B EFA Sociability
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) R PCA Agreeableness
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) B PCA Agreeableness
Koski et al. (2017) R PCA Agreeableness
Inoue-Murayama et al. (2018) R EFA Sociability
Dominance
Yokoyama et al. (2011) B EFA Aggressiveness
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) R PCA Extraversion
Koski et al. (2017) R PCA Assertiveness
Inoue-Murayama et al. (2018) R EFA Dominance
Neuroticism
Yokoyama et al. (2011) B EFA Social Anxiety
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) R PCA Neuroticism
Inoue-Murayama et al. (2018) R EFA Neuroticism
Openness
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) R PCA Openness
Koski et al. (2017) R PCA Inquisitiveness
Conscientiousness
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) R PCA Conscientiousness
Koski et al. (2017) R PCA Conscientiousness
Unknown, possibly Conscientiousness
Iwanicki and Lehmann (2015) B PCA Perceptual Sensitivity
Koski et al. (2017) R PCA Patience

Column labeled “Data” indicates whether behavioral (B) or rating (R) measures were analyzed. Column labeled “Analysis” indicates whether principal components analysis (PCA) or exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to identify domains.

The most striking finding from these studies is that, although numerous species exhibit individual differences in traits related to self-control [34,35], in common marmosets these traits formed one or two broad domains. Among other primates, similar domains have only been found in humans Homo sapiens [e.g., 36], chimpanzees Pan troglodytes [3742], bonobos Pan paniscus [25], and brown (or tufted) capuchin monkeys Sapajus apella [28], all known for their relatively large brains. Consequently, findings of domains related to self-control in common marmosets led researchers to consider the possibility that the cognitive and behavioral demands associated with cooperative breeding may have led to the evolution of these domains in what is a small-brained primate [31].

The results from the four studies of common marmosets, however, were not entirely consistent. Specifically, although they found that common marmosets possess up to six or seven domains, they varied with respect to which subset was found. This variation across studies may be attributable to the fact that, asides perhaps from one study [31], these studies did not sample enough traits or individuals to capture all the ways in which individuals may differ in their personality. To address whether this was the case, we followed up our earlier study of personality in 77 (68 male and 9 female) common marmosets housed at the Kobe, Japan campus of the Institute of Physical and Chemical Research (RIKEN) [13]. In our earlier study, we did not find a Conscientiousness, Patience, or, for that matter, an Openness domain. One reason why we may not have found those domains is that several items related to those domains were removed because they had interrater reliability estimates that were less than zero [13]. This can happen if there is not enough between-subjects variance in a trait and/or a large amount of error variance [43]. One reason for why a low level of between-subjects variance may come is that unmeasured influences, for example how animals are housed or bred, overwhelms, or makes it difficult to observe or perceive, individual differences in one or more traits. Recent findings that appear to rule out this possibility come from a study that found the same structure in laboratory-housed and wild common marmosets [44]. However, because the captive and wild groups lived in family groups, that study could not exclude the possibility that being raised in other kinds of groups might affect personality structure. A low level of between-subjects variance may also come about because the personalities of individuals conform to that of their group [12].

In the present follow-up study, we attempted to rule out the possibility that low between-subjects variance was responsible for the inconsistency between our previous study and studies that found additional domains, including Conscientiousness. We therefore increased the between-subjects variance in the RIKEN sample by increasing the sample size by approximately two-thirds. Doing so also led to our nearly doubling the ratio of females to males.

In addition to trying to find these additional domains in the common marmosets housed at RIKEN, we examined associations between personality domains and subjective well-being. Previous studies in humans [45,46] and in nonhuman primate species, including chimpanzees [40,47,48], orangutans Pongo spp. [49], rhesus macaques Macaca mulatta [50], brown capuchin monkeys [51], and common marmosets [13], have shown a consistent pattern of relationships between personality and measures of well-being or welfare. Specifically, personality domains associated with gregariousness, assertiveness, activity, and other traits associated with Extraversion [52] were related to higher subjective well-being and personality domains made up of traits associated with vigilance, fearfulness, anxiety, and other traits associated with Neuroticism [53] were associated with lower subjective well-being. By testing for associations between the personality domains and subjective well-being, then, we could assess the degree to which the personality domains we find are measures of distinct psychological constructs.

Finally, we tested whether a set of genetic polymorphisms were associated with personality. Our previous study of common marmosets found that lower Dominance and lower Neuroticism in common marmosets were both associated with the AA genotype of the μ-opioid receptor gene; lower Neuroticism was additionally associated with the short form of the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A gene [13]. For the present study we focused on single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the serotonin receptor 1a gene. A study of chimpanzees identified a SNP (rs25209664: C743A) that caused a proline to glutamine substitution at the 248th amino acid of the serotonin receptor 1a gene. This polymorphism was associated with aggression and sociability: chimpanzees who possessed two C alleles engaged in less social grooming and were rated as more anxious [54]. This study also found evidence for some interactions: males with the CC genotype displayed more often and, of chimpanzees with the AC genotype, mid-ranking individuals had lower proximity scores [54].

Materials and methods

Subjects

Subjects were 128 common marmosets (99 males, 29 females) that ranged in age from 1.6 to 15.1 (mean = 4.8, SD = 2.7). Subjects were recruited in three waves. The 77 subjects from the first wave had taken part in a similar study [13] and included 68 males and 9 females ranging in age from 1.5 to 15.1 years (mean = 6.0, SD = 2.6). Subjects from the second and third waves were born at RIKEN. The 24 subjects from the second wave included 17 males and 7 females ranging in age from 1.7 to 4.5 years (mean = 2.6, SD = 0.7) and the 27 subjects from the third wave included 14 males and 13 females ranging in age from 2.0 to 4.9 years (mean = 3.0, SD = 0.8).

Animal housing and husbandry

Subjects were housed in the RIKEN Center for Biosystems Dynamics Research in Kobe, Japan. One hundred and twelve subjects were born at the center, six were supplied by CLEA Japan Inc. (Tokyo, Japan), and 10 were supplied by Japan Wild Animal Laboratory Limited (Amami, Japan). Subjects sourced from other facilities had lived in the center for at least three years prior to this study.

At RIKEN, subjects were housed in breeding rooms that had a 12 h light-dark cycle (light: 08:00–20:00). Enclosures (1630 × 760 × 831 mm for families, 660 × 650 × 600 or 660 × 450 × 600 mm for pairs or individuals) had wooden perches, a plastic cube-shaped shelter, a food tray, and a water dispenser. There were around twenty cages in each breeding room and so even if animals were individually housed, they were exposed to visual, auditory, and olfactory stimulation from conspecifics. The temperature and humidity in the breeding room were maintained at approximately 28°C and 50%, respectively. In the morning and afternoon, subjects received solid food (CMS-1, CLEA Japan, Inc., Tokyo, Japan) mixed with enough water to soften it, powdered milk formula, honey, gluconic acid, calcium, vitamin C, and lactobacillus probiotic. Food in the afternoon was softened into a paste by soaking it in water and then stirring it. Once a week subjects’ diets were supplemented with chopped and boiled eggs or bananas.

Animal rearing

Animals reared by their parents and/or their family members, including one to five older brothers or sisters, were reared in family cages. At around 14 days after birth, when they were still infants, these subjects were fed a food paste in the afternoon. When these individuals were between 6 and 15 months old, to ensure that they were provided with the required amount of space, they were transferred from their family cage to a home cage (0.21 to 0.43 m2 floor space per animal). Animals living in these home cages were same-sex, mixed-age peers. Individuals that were to be used in brain imaging [55] or in behavioral studies, and individuals that did not get along with their partners, were single housed.

Animals that were not reared by their parents, for example, in the event of a triplet birth or parental neglect, were reared in climate-controlled rearing cages by human caregivers. This procedure has been described elsewhere [13]. In short, these animals were housed in a thermal insulation box and a towel roll from one day to 21 days after birth. Then, from 21 days after birth to weaning, these animals were housed in a wire-mesh box sized 390 × 230 × 300 mm furnished with a hammock, perches, towel roll, feeding dish, and water bottle. These animals were breastfed on the day of their birth and then bottle-fed until weaning. A food paste was introduced at around 28 days and then animals were weaned fully 50 to 70 days after birth. After weaning, these animals were housed in a home cage with peers or individually in the breeding room.

Of the 77 subjects from the first wave, 30, including 23 parent-reared and seven hand-reared subjects, were housed in a family group (n = 13) or with same-sex peers (n = 17). The remaining 47 subjects, including 33 that were parent-reared, 13 that were hand-reared, and one with an unknown rearing history, were single-housed. Of the 24 subjects from the second wave, 22, including 18 that were parent-reared, 3 that were hand-reared, and one with an unknown rearing history, were housed in a family group (n = 7), with an opposite sex marmoset for breeding (n = 2), or with same-sex peers (n = 13). The remaining two subjects from the second wave were parent-reared and single-housed. Of the 27 subjects from the third wave, 25 parent-reared subjects and one hand-reared subject were housed in a family group (n = 1), with an opposite-sex marmoset for breeding (n = 4), with same-sex peers (n = 19), or single-housed (n = 2). The remaining subject was parent-reared and single-housed.

Ratings

Questionnaires

For this study, we used personality and subjective well-being questionnaires that had been translated from English into Japanese using a back-translation procedure. A study of chimpanzees revealed that the translation did not affect the psychometric properties of these questionnaires [40].

For personality, we used the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ; available at https://extras.springer.com/2011/978-1-4614-0176-6.zip). Each of the HPQ’s 54 items consists of a trait adjective paired with one to three sentences that set the adjective in the context of primate behavior. For example, “FEARFUL” (boldface and capitals in the original) is paired with the descriptor sentence “Subject reacts excessively to real or imagined threats by displaying behaviors such as screaming, grimacing, running away or other signs of anxiety or distress.” The HPQ’s instructions ask raters to a) judge the standing of each animal on each trait based on the animal’s behavior and interactions with others, and the rater’s own judgement of what constitutes average behavior for this species, b) assign a rating of 1 (“Displays either total absence or negligible amounts of the trait.”) to 7 (“Displays extremely large amounts of the trait.”) to each item, and c) not discuss their ratings with their fellow raters.

A description of the HPQ’s development can be found elsewhere [56]. Briefly, the HPQ grew out the 48-item Orangutan Personality Questionnaire [49], which grew out of the 43-item Chimpanzee Personality Questionnaire [39]. Forty-one of the HPQ’s 54 items were sampled from Goldberg’s [57] trait terms of the five major domains of human personality [39]. The remaining 13 items were adapted from items [58] or facets [59] from other human personality inventories, or were created by the authors of these instruments [39,40,49].

To assess subjective well-being, we used a four-item scale that was based on a questionnaire that was originally developed for studies of captive chimpanzees [47]. Each item was devised to assess a different concept of subjective well-being that had been described in the human literature [47,6064]. The first item (moods) concerned the extent to which an individual experienced positive versus negative affect. The second item (social) concerned whether the individual experienced pleasure from social interactions. The third item (goals) concerned whether the individual was able to achieve its goals, bearing in mind that different individuals may have different goals. The fourth item (be marmoset) asked raters how “happy” they would be if they were that marmoset for a week and was thus meant to measure global satisfaction. The subjective well-being scale’s instructions asked raters to assign a rating of 1 (“Displays either total absence or negligible amounts of the trait or state.”) to 7 (“Displays extremely large amounts of the trait.”) to each item. The instructions also request that raters do not discuss their ratings.

Raters and ratings

We asked three keepers (two men and one woman) who completed the questionnaires in the first wave of data collection [see 13 for details] to complete the questionnaires for the second and third wave of data collection. The keepers did not know the results of the previous study or the purpose of collecting the data. The keepers had known the subjects they rated for 1.1 to 9.8 years (mean = 3.7 years, SD = 2.2). Two keepers (one man and one woman) rated all 128 subjects and the third rated 81 subjects. This resulted in a total of 337 ratings or an average of 2.63 ratings per subject. There were no missing rating data.

Genotyping

A buccal swab was taken from each subject and kept in a 90% ethanol solution until DNA extraction. DNA was extracted by DNeasy Blood and Tissue kit (Qiagen, CA, USA). PCR amplification was conducted in a 10 μl (the total volume) reaction mixture containing 10ng of DNA template, 0.4 μM of each primer (forward: 5’-tggattcccttcctccgaaa-3’, reverse: 5’-aggtgttgattccctagggt-3’), 0.5U of LA Taq DNA polymerase, 400 μM of dNTPs, and GC buffer I (TaKaRa, Shiga, Japan). After denaturing DNA samples at 95°C for 1 min, we set up 40 cycles of 95°C for 30 seconds, 60°C for 30 seconds, 74°C for 1 minute, and a final extension at 74°C for 10 minutes. A total of 1,473 base pair fragments including whole single exon region were amplified. We then sequenced the polymerase chain reaction products, both forwards and backwards, using 3130xl Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems, CA, USA). The internal primer 5’-tcatgctggttctctatggg-3’ was also used for sequencing. Primers were designed based on the NCBI Reference Sequence NC_013897. In the end, we identified three novel SNPs (G840C, G841A, and T901A) in the third intracellular region of the receptor (Figs 1 and 2). G840C was a synonymous SNP coding alanine at the 280th amino acid sequence, G841A was a nonsynonymous SNP that caused a methionine substitution at the 281st amino acid sequence, and T901A was a nonsynonymous SNP that caused a serine to threonine substitution at the 301st amino acid sequence.

Fig 1. The nucleotide sequence of the common marmoset serotonin receptor 1a gene.

Fig 1

The sequence is based on NC_013897. Primer sequences are underlined. Start and stop codon sequences are in boxes. The third intracellular region is enclosed in the parentheses. Nucleotide substitutions are shown in capital and bold letters: G840C (A280A), G841A (V281M), T901A (S301T).

Fig 2. Locations of the single nucleotide polymorphisms on the single exon of the common marmoset serotonin receptor 1a gene.

Fig 2

The shaded region corresponds to the third intracellular region.

Analyses

We conducted the analyses using version 3.6.3 of R [65]. We used functions from version 1.9.12 of the psych package [66], version 1.0.7 of the EFA.MRFA package [67], and some custom functions.

Item interrater reliabilities

We used a custom function to compute the interrater reliabilities of the ratings of individual items on the HPQ and subjective well-being questionnaire. This function computed two intraclass correlations (ICCs) described by Shrout and Fleiss (43). One, ICC(3,1), indicates the reliability of individual ratings, that is, it is an estimate of the reliability of the rating from a single rater. The other, ICC(3,k), indicates the reliability of the mean rating coming from k raters, which was equal to 2.63 in the present study. We excluded items that had reliabilities that were less than or equal to zero [see 56 for a discussion].

Exploratory factor analyses

We conducted these analyses on the aggregate (mean) of personality ratings for the 128 subjects. Simulation studies [6870] have shown that the number of subjects required for satisfactory recovery of factors is a function of item communalities, item loadings, and the item:factor ratio, and that the subject:item ratio is irrelevant. Previous rating-based studies of common marmoset personality [13,17,31] found that 72% to 97% of questionnaire items were reliable, a total of three to five factors, median salient loadings of around .6 or .7, and communalities that were between wide and high [see ref 69 for definitions of these types of communalities]. As such, we have a large enough sample size to conduct factor analyses on these data [69].

Before extracting factors using the maximum likelihood method, we determined how many factors to extract. To do so, we used the fa.parallel function from the psych package to generate a scree plot, which we inspected, and to conduct a parallel analysis [71] in which we compared eigenvalues from a principal components analysis of our data to the distribution of 1000 eigenvalues generated from principal components analysis of resampled and randomly generated data. We used principal components analysis for our parallel analysis because a recent study showed that the number of dimensions identified in this manner is more likely to recover the correct number of factors [72]. In addition, we used the VSS function from the psych package to determine, for one to eight factor solutions, which had the lowest Bayesian Information Criterion [BIC; 73] and the hullEFA function from the EFA.MRFA package to determine the number of factors via the Hull method [74]. The Hull method is known to perform well with personality data [75]. Finally, we inspected the factors to ensure that they were interpretable.

After we extracted factors using the fa function from the psych package, we applied an oblique (promax) and an orthogonal (varimax) rotation. If the promax rotation yielded factors that were strongly correlated and/or a different structure, we retained and interpreted those factors. We otherwise retained and interpreted the varimax-rotated factors.

In interpreting factors, we specified that salient loadings were those equal to or greater than |0.4|. We labeled factors based on our interpretations of what they were and attempted to find suitable labels from previous studies of common marmosets and, if none were available, studies of nonhuman primates and humans. If we could not find a label from these sources, we devised our own.

In addition to conducting this first-order factor analysis, we found evidence (results to be discussed) suggesting that there may be second-order personality factors underlying these data. We thus conducted an exploratory factor analyses of the inerfactor correlation matrix obtained from the promax-rotated factors. For the same reason, we conducted an additional item-level factor analysis as a robustness check.

We conducted a maximum likelihood factor analysis using the fa function on aggregated (mean) ratings of the four subjective well-being items for all 128 subjects. Previous work in 77 of these subjects revealed a single factor [13].

Unit-weighted factor scores

For the remaining analyses, we used a custom function to compute unit-weighted scores [20,76] for the personality and subjective well-being data. This involved, for each item, finding the largest salient factor loading. If that loading was positive, we assigned it a weight of +1. If that loading was negative, we assigned it a weight of -1. In all other cases, we assigned a weight of zero. After We then summed the weighted item ratings.

Factor reliabilities

For the first- and second-order personality factors, and for subjective well-being, we again computed ICC(3,1) and ICC(3,k). As with the item-level analyses, k was equal to 2.63. In addition, we used the alpha function from the psych package to compute Cronbach’s alpha (α), a measure of the internal consistency reliability of a scale, and the omega function to compute McDonald’s omega hierarchical (ωh), a measure of the degree to which a general factor saturates a scale’s items.

Personality factor comparisons

To compare the first- and second-order factors to factors found in previous studies of common marmosets [13,17,31], we first generated unit-weighted factor scores based on the personality structures described in these other studies. Because there was not a total overlap of questionnaire items across these studies, we sometimes substituted items that were similar in their meaning or in the constructs that they purportedly assessed. Details about how these scores were created can be found in S1 Table. After computing these scores, we obtained correlations between the scores based on the factor loadings from the present study and the scores based on component and factor loadings from previous studies. We compared the absolute magnitudes of these correlations and highlighted the highest correlation or, in the case where the confidence intervals of two or more correlations overlapped, highest correlations.

Personality and subjective well-being associations

We used Pearson correlation coefficients to examine associations between the first- and second-order personality factors and, both, the subjective well-being items and the sum of these items. We used Holm’s method [77] to adjust for familywise error rates.

Personality and genotype associations

To examine the genotype-personality associations, for the first- and second-order personality factors, we fit linear models using the lm function. For these analyses, we standardized the personality factor scores (mean = 0, SD = 1). The variables in the models included sex (male = 1, female = 0), age in years, and a categorical variable that indicated genotype. Because there were problems with genotyping four subjects, these individuals were excluded from the analyses. In addition, the G840C genotypes for two subjects and T901A genotype for one subject were unclear, and so these individuals were not included in tests of associations between personality and the G840C and T901A genotype, respectively. Finally, only one subject had the AA version of G841A and only nine subjects had the GA version of this genotype. We therefore did not examine associations between these genotypes and personality.

Although subjects were related, we did not test for the effect of genotypes within the context of an animal model [cf. 13]. Moreover, because we conducted multiple, sometimes non-independent, tests, we used the Bonferroni correction to adjust for familywise error rates.

Ethics

This study complied with the current laws of Japan, including the Act on Welfare and Management of Animals. All experimental and husbandry procedures were performed in accordance with RIKEN’s Guidelines for Conducting Animal Experiments, and in accordance with the ARRIVE (Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments) guidelines. All procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Kobe Institute of RIKEN (MA2009-10-16).

Results

Interrater reliabilities of items

Personality

The interrater reliabilities of the 54 HPQ items are presented in Table 2. The reliabilities of individual ratings and of mean ratings for the items ‘anxious’, ‘persistent’, ‘quitting’, and ‘unperceptive’ were negative, and so we excluded these items from further analyses. Although the reliability of mean ratings for the items ‘innovative’ and ‘decisive’ were equal to 0.01, the reliabilities of individual ratings for these items were less than 0.01, and so we decided to exclude those items, too.

Table 2. Interrater reliabilities of items from the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire.
Item ICC(3,1) ICC(3,k)
Sociable 0.45 0.68
Sympathetic 0.43 0.66
Solitary 0.43 0.67
Protective 0.42 0.66
Helpful 0.38 0.62
Friendly 0.36 0.60
Aggressive 0.34 0.57
Gentle 0.34 0.58
Irritable 0.33 0.57
Affectionate 0.32 0.55
Stingy/greedy 0.29 0.51
Dominant 0.28 0.50
Individualistic 0.28 0.51
Submissive 0.28 0.51
Imitative 0.28 0.51
Independent 0.27 0.50
Excitable 0.26 0.48
Conventional 0.26 0.48
Bullying 0.23 0.44
Impulsive 0.23 0.44
Dependent/follower 0.23 0.44
Defiant 0.23 0.45
Stable 0.21 0.41
Jealous 0.21 0.41
Cautious 0.19 0.39
Active 0.19 0.38
Fearful 0.18 0.37
Autistic 0.18 0.36
Thoughtless 0.17 0.36
Erratic 0.17 0.35
Playful 0.16 0.33
Intelligent 0.16 0.33
Reckless 0.15 0.32
Curious 0.14 0.30
Sensitive 0.14 0.30
Inquisitive 0.13 0.28
Cool 0.13 0.29
Predictable 0.13 0.28
Lazy 0.11 0.25
Timid 0.10 0.22
Clumsy 0.09 0.21
Disorganized 0.09 0.20
Vulnerable 0.07 0.16
Unemotional 0.06 0.15
Distractible 0.05 0.13
Manipulative 0.05 0.12
Depressed 0.04 0.09
Inventive 0.02 0.06
Innovative 0.00 0.01
Decisive 0.00 0.01
Anxious -0.03 -0.08
Persistent -0.06 -0.19
Quitting -0.08 -0.24
Unperceptive -0.12 -0.39

Of the remaining 48 items, the interrater reliabilities of individual ratings ranged from 0.02 (‘inventive’) to 0.45 (‘sociable’). The mean and standard deviation for these estimates were 0.21 and 0.11, respectively. The interrater reliabilities of mean ratings for the remaining items ranged from 0.06 (‘inventive’) to 0.68 (‘sociable’). The mean and standard deviation for these estimates were 0.40 and 0.16, respectively.

Subjective well-being

The interrater reliabilities of individual ratings were 0.16, 0.10, 0.15, and 0.14 for the moods, social, goals, and global well-being, respectively. The corresponding reliabilities of mean ratings were 0.33, 0.23, 0.32, and 0.30.

Maximum likelihood exploratory factor analyses

Personality

The scree plot (S1 Fig) and parallel analysis indicated that there were five factors. The Hull method (S2 Fig) also indicated that there were five factors, and BIC achieved a minimum with five factors. We therefore extracted five factors.

A promax rotation of the five-factor solution yielded two interfactor correlations that were large (rs ≥ |0.5|) and two that were medium-sized (rs ≥ |0.3|). The mean and standard deviation of the absolute interfactor correlations were 0.26 and 0.20, respectively. Comparison of the varimax- and promax-rotated factors revealed that the congruence coefficients for two factors fell below 0.95 (S2 Table) and an inspection of the loadings indicated that the promax-rotated factors differed some from their varimax-rotated counterparts. Given these results, we interpreted the promax-rotated factors (Table 3), which explained 63% of the variance (the varimax-rotated factors are presented in S3 Table).

Table 3. Pattern matrix from the first-order factor analysis of the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire.
Factor Loadings
Item Soc Dom Imp Opn Neg h 2
Helpful 0.89 0.10 -0.05 0.09 -0.08 0.79
Sympathetic 0.83 -0.01 -0.07 0.07 0.08 0.76
Protective 0.80 0.01 0.02 0.02 -0.15 0.66
Individualistic -0.78 0.09 -0.05 0.13 0.25 0.71
Dependent/follower 0.77 0.06 -0.05 0.23 0.39 0.69
Independent -0.73 0.27 -0.31 0.10 0.08 0.62
Imitative 0.72 0.04 0.07 0.20 0.18 0.49
Solitary -0.71 0.05 -0.04 -0.17 0.34 0.74
Affectionate 0.64 -0.09 -0.16 0.21 0.12 0.65
Sensitive 0.64 -0.08 -0.17 -0.02 0.04 0.61
Sociable 0.62 -0.27 -0.16 0.16 -0.15 0.85
Conventional 0.60 0.11 -0.33 -0.13 0.19 0.62
Intelligent 0.59 0.26 -0.18 0.00 -0.19 0.39
Gentle 0.50 -0.35 -0.24 0.15 0.09 0.83
Reckless -0.50 -0.16 0.39 0.38 -0.03 0.62
Friendly 0.49 -0.46 -0.15 0.13 0.02 0.86
Jealous 0.02 0.92 -0.04 0.21 0.15 0.83
Stingy/greedy -0.08 0.87 -0.06 0.28 0.15 0.86
Bullying -0.04 0.85 0.00 0.11 0.07 0.77
Dominant -0.09 0.79 0.06 0.01 -0.07 0.80
Manipulative 0.21 0.72 -0.10 0.04 -0.33 0.57
Aggressive -0.12 0.69 0.15 -0.05 -0.17 0.80
Defiant -0.09 0.65 0.20 -0.01 -0.20 0.78
Irritable 0.00 0.50 0.48 -0.18 -0.11 0.74
Excitable -0.02 0.16 0.78 -0.08 -0.06 0.78
Unemotional -0.10 0.15 -0.77 0.00 0.26 0.52
Impulsive -0.10 0.06 0.75 0.13 0.13 0.76
Cool 0.18 -0.05 -0.66 -0.06 0.01 0.65
Fearful 0.23 -0.10 0.62 -0.42 0.28 0.51
Distractible -0.11 -0.05 0.53 0.23 0.10 0.40
Disorganized -0.10 0.15 0.52 0.18 0.12 0.51
Stable 0.21 -0.19 -0.46 0.07 -0.38 0.64
Thoughtless -0.22 -0.03 0.41 0.38 0.02 0.45
Predictable 0.12 -0.08 -0.40 0.04 0.02 0.27
Erratic -0.20 0.32 0.35 -0.06 0.20 0.54
Curious 0.14 0.13 0.08 0.73 0.02 0.61
Inquisitive 0.17 0.11 0.06 0.72 0.09 0.55
Inventive 0.27 0.18 -0.05 0.68 0.11 0.51
Playful 0.23 -0.12 0.32 0.65 -0.04 0.60
Cautious 0.47 0.08 0.24 -0.59 0.21 0.53
Active 0.28 0.18 0.41 0.52 -0.13 0.69
Autistic 0.03 -0.13 0.09 0.19 0.68 0.45
Timid 0.14 0.00 0.34 -0.18 0.65 0.58
Depressed -0.17 0.07 -0.27 -0.04 0.64 0.51
Clumsy -0.04 0.17 -0.05 0.00 0.58 0.33
Vulnerable -0.02 -0.19 -0.02 0.04 0.57 0.39
Lazy -0.23 -0.05 -0.45 -0.17 0.50 0.59
Submissive 0.33 -0.20 -0.18 -0.01 0.48 0.61
Proportion of variance 0.20 0.14 0.13 0.08 0.08
Factor Correlations
Factor Soc Dom Imp Opn Neg
Soc 1.00
Dom -0.53 1.00
Imp -0.45 0.57 1.00
Opn 0.06 0.15 0.18 1.00
Neg -0.06 -0.19 -0.04 -0.38 1.00

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

The first factor loaded on items related to high Extraversion and high Agreeableness in humans [e.g., 57]. This factor resembled the Sociability factor in one study [13] and the Agreeableness factor from two other studies [17,31] of common marmosets. To be consistent with our prior study [13], we labeled this factor Sociability.

The second factor loaded on items related to high Extraversion and low Agreeableness in humans [e.g., 57]. Compared to other studies of common marmosets, it is best described as a narrow version of factors labeled Dominance [13], Extraversion [17], and Assertiveness [31]. To be consistent with our prior study [13], we labeled this factor Dominance.

The third factor had positive loadings on items related to high Neuroticism and low Conscientiousness in humans, and also negative loadings on items related to low Neuroticism and high Conscientiousness [e.g., 57]. In a previous study of a subsample of these animals [13], the Dominance and Neuroticism factor loaded on some of these items. Compared to other studies of common marmosets, it resembled most closely the factors labeled Conscientiousness and Patience in one study [31] and the Conscientiousness factor in another [17]. Humans that are high in Neuroticism and low in Conscientiousness are described as having an undercontrolled style of impulse control [78]. We thus labeled this factor Impulsiveness.

With the exception of a negative loading on the item cautious, the fourth factor loaded primarily on items related to high Openness in humans [e.g., 57]. Previous studies of common marmosets have labeled factor such as these Openness [17] and Inquisitiveness [31]. We thus labeled this factor Openness.

The fifth factor loaded on items related to low Extraversion and high Neuroticism in humans [e.g., 57]. In the previous study of a subset of these animals [13], Neuroticism had a positive loading on many of these items. In all three previous studies of this species, factors such as Dominance, Assertiveness, and Conscientiousness had negative loadings on these items [13,17,31]. Given that this factor combined aspects of high Neuroticism and low degrees of Assertiveness or social prowess, we labeled it Negative Affect.

Because there were several non-negligible correlations between the just-described factors, we factor analyzed the factor intercorrelation matrix. Inspection of the scree plot (S3 Fig) and parallel analysis indicated that there were two factors; BIC was lowest for the two-factor solution (we did not conduct a Hull test because hullEFA cannot be used to examine correlation matrices). We also tried to extract a single ‘general’ factor, but this solution exhibited poor fit (root mean square of the residuals = 0.14), and the factor did not have a salient loading on Openness or on Negative Affect. A promax rotation of the two-factor solution indicated that the factors were nearly orthogonal, and the loadings of the varimax-rotated factors were nearly identical to those of the promax-rotated factors (Table 4). We therefore interpreted the varimax-rotated factors, which accounted for 49% of the variance. After reflecting (multiplying loadings by -1) the first factor, it had a positive loading on Sociability and a negative loading on both Dominance and Impulsiveness. We thus labeled this factor Pro-sociality. The second factor had a positive loading on Openness and a negative loading on Negative Affect. We thus labeled this factor Boldness.

Table 4. Pattern matrix from the second-order factor analysis of personality factors.
Promax Rotation Varimax Rotation
First-Order Factor Pro-sociality Boldness Pro-sociality Boldness h 2
Dominance -0.79 0.15 -0.80 0.21 0.68
Sociability 0.72 0.21 0.70 0.15 0.51
Impulsiveness -0.67 0.08 -0.68 0.13 0.48
Negative affect -0.01 0.62 0.04 -0.61 0.38
Openness -0.01 -0.61 -0.06 0.61 0.38
Proportion of variance 0.32 0.17 0.32 0.17

N = 128. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted from the factor correlation matrix from Table 2 using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax and varimax procedures. The Pro-Social factor for both structures were reflected, that is, the loadings were multiplied by -1. The promax-rotated factors correlated -0.15. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

Previous studies of common marmosets did not find evidence higher-order factors, that is, interfactor correlations tended to be modest [13,17,31]. We thus felt it would be worthwhile to follow-up these results with two robustness checks.

The first of robustness check tested whether the higher-order factors reflected the structuring of data collection. Specifically, because different subjects were rated in each wave, this may have led raters to rate the subjects belonging to each wave as resembling one another more than they did subjects in other waves. To test this, we residualized the 48 reliable items on a categorical variable that represented whether the subject was rated in the first, second, or third wave. We then factor analyzed the residualized item scores.

Inspection of the scree plot (S4 Fig) suggested that there were four or five factors and parallel analysis indicated that there were four factors. The Hull test indicated that there were two factors (S5 Fig). The BIC was lowest for a five-factor solution. We therefore examined promax-rotations after extracting two, four, and then five factors.

For the two-factor solution (S4 Table), the first factor loaded predominantly on items onto which the factors Sociability, Dominance, and Impulsiveness had loaded. It therefore resembled the higher-order factor Pro-sociality. The second factor loaded predominantly on items onto which the factors Openness and Negative Affect had loaded. It therefore resembled the higher-order factor Boldness. For the four-factor solution (S5 Table), the first, third, and fourth factors resembled Sociability, Negative Affect, and Openness. The second factor loaded predominantly on items related to high Dominance and high Impulsiveness. The five-factor solution yielded five factors that resembled the five factors that had been found earlier (S6 Table).

The similarity, as indicated by Tucker’s congruence coefficients, between the five factors obtained before and after item scores were residualized were equal to or greater than 0.98, suggesting that these were the same factors (S7 Table). Factor analysis of the residualized item scores, then, revealed either the same structure (the five-factor solution) or structures in which there were stronger associations between factors (the two- and four-factor solutions). These results are not consistent with the possibility that the higher-order factors reflect the fact that we collected these data in three stages.

The second of our robustness checks was to test whether the second-order factors are general evaluative factors used by raters [cf. 79]. To do so, we factor analyzed ratings from each of the three raters separately. We also factor analyzed a weighted correlation matrix from which removed possible effects of raters:

Rw=1Ni=1kRini

where Rw, the weighted correlation matrix, is the sum of the products of the correlation matrices of each of k = 3 raters, Ri, and the subjects, ni rated by that individual rater, divided by the total number of subjects, N.

For ratings from one keeper who rated all the subjects, the scree plot (S6 Fig), parallel analysis, BIC, and Hull method (S7 Fig) indicated that there were five factors. We thus extracted five factors and subjected them to a promax rotation (S8 Table). These factors resembled those obtained in the initial factor analysis and the interfactor correlations were similar in magnitude. We then conducted a second-order factor analysis in which we forced a two-factor solution. Although one second-order factor just missed our criterion for a salient loading on a first-order factor, these factors resembled Pro-sociality (reversed) and Boldness (S9 Table).

For ratings from the second keeper who rated all the subjects, the scree plot (S8 Fig) indicated that there were five factors and parallel analysis, the BIC, and the Hull method (S9 Fig) indicated that there were four factors. We thus extracted four factors and subjected the m to a promax rotation (S10 Table). The first factor appeared to be a Dominance versus Agreeableness, the third factor was Gregariousness (a narrow facet of Extraversion), and the last two factors were difficult to interpret. The interfactor correlations were not consistent with there being a second-order factor.

For ratings from the keeper who rated 81 subjects, the scree plot (S10 Fig) and parallel analysis, the BIC, and Hull method (S11 Fig) indicated that there were three factors. We thus extracted three factors and subjected them to a promax rotation (S11 Table). These factors included Agreeableness versus Dominance, Extraversion, and Negative Affect/Impulsiveness, respectively.

For the weighted correlation matrix, the scree plot indicated that there were five factors (S12 Fig) and both the parallel analysis, and the BIC indicated that there were three factors. We thus extracted three factors and subjected them to a promax rotation (S12 Table). The first and third factors loaded on many of the traits that belonged to the factors that made up the second-order Pro-sociality and Boldness domains, respectively. The second factor was an Impulsiveness factor. The correlation between the first and third factors was low, but the correlation between Impulsiveness and Pro-sociality was between medium and large, and therefore was consistent with the definitions of these factors.

We then extracted five factors and subjected them to a promax-rotation (S13 Table). The five factors resembled those from our initial factor analyses as did the interfactor correlations. The scree plot indicated that there were two factors (S13 Fig) as did both the parallel analysis and the BIC. The first higher-order factor was, when reflected, a Pro-sociality factor. The second higher-order factor was a Boldness factor (S14 Table).

Subjective well-being

Inspection of the scree plot (S14 Fig), parallel analysis, and the BIC all indicated that there was a single factor. The Hull test indicated that there was one factor, but produced a warning, which we suspect was attributable to there being only four items. This factor explained 67% of the variance and had salient loadings on all four items (Table 5).

Table 5. Results from factor analysis of the subjective well-being scale.
Item Loading h 2
Be marmoset 0.98 0.97
Balance of moods 0.86 0.73
Ability to achieve goals 0.82 0.67
Pleasure from social interactions 0.55 0.30

N = 128. h2 = communalities.

Reliabilities of factors

Personality

The interrater reliabilities of the individual ratings for Sociability, Dominance, Impulsiveness, Openness, and Negative Affect were 0.52, 0.39, 0.28, 0.26, and 0.25, respectively. The interrater reliabilities of mean ratings for these factors were 0.74, 0.63, 0.50, 0.48, and 0.46, respectively. For Pro-sociality and Boldness, respectively, the interrater reliabilities of individual ratings were 0.45 and 0.30, and the interrater reliabilities of mean ratings for these second-order factors were 0.69 and 0.53.

The internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α) for Sociability, Dominance, Impulsiveness, Openness, and Negative Affect were 0.95, 0.95, 0.88, 0.85, and 0.81, respectively. The degree to which a general factor saturated these factors (McDonald’s ωh) was 0.81, 0.85, 0.75, 0.80, and 0.68, respectively.

Subjective well-being

For the total subjective well-being score, the interrater reliability of individual ratings was 0.21 and the interrater reliability of the mean of ratings 0.41. Cronbach’s α for this scale was 0.87 and McDonald’s ωh was 0.13.

Personality factor comparisons

Iwanicki and Lehmann [17] found four factors. Compared to our first-order factors, their Extraversion factor overlapped with Dominance and (reversed) Negative Affect, their Agreeableness factor overlapped with Sociability and (reversed) Dominance, their Conscientiousness factor overlapped with Sociability, and their Openness factor overlapped with the Openness factor that we found (Table 6). Compared to our second-order factors, Iwanicki and Lehman’s Extraversion overlapped with Pro-sociality and Boldness; their Agreeableness and Conscientiousness factors overlapped with Pro-sociality; and their Openness factor overlapped with Boldness (Table 7).

Table 6. Correlations between unit-weighted factor scores based on factor loadings in the present study and factor loadings from previous studies of common marmoset personality.

95% Confidence Interval
r Lower Bound Upper Bound
Iwanicki and Lehman (2015)
  Extraversion
    Sociability -0.50 -0.62 -0.36
    Dominance 0.82 0.75 0.87
    Impulsiveness 0.50 0.36 0.62
    Openness 0.54 0.40 0.65
    Negative affect -0.70 -0.78 -0.60
  Agreeableness
    Sociability 0.86 0.81 0.90
    Dominance -0.84 -0.88 -0.77
    Impulsiveness -0.76 -0.82 -0.67
    Openness -0.07 -0.24 0.11
    Negative affect 0.25 0.08 0.41
  Conscientiousness
    Sociability 0.83 0.77 0.88
    Dominance -0.47 -0.59 -0.32
    Impulsiveness -0.63 -0.73 -0.51
    Openness 0.00 -0.17 0.18
    Negative affect 0.05 -0.13 0.22
  Openness
    Sociability 0.11 -0.07 0.28
    Dominance 0.34 0.18 0.49
    Impulsiveness 0.27 0.10 0.42
    Openness 0.96 0.94 0.97
    Negative affect -0.40 -0.53 -0.24
Koski et al. (2017)
  Conscientiousness
    Sociability 0.62 0.51 0.72
    Dominance -0.89 -0.92 -0.84
    Impulsiveness -0.84 -0.89 -0.79
    Openness -0.39 -0.53 -0.23
    Negative affect 0.19 0.01 0.35
  Agreeableness
    Sociability 0.95 0.92 0.96
    Dominance -0.68 -0.77 -0.58
    Impulsiveness -0.72 -0.79 -0.62
    Openness 0.03 -0.14 0.20
    Negative affect 0.13 -0.05 0.29
  Assertiveness
    Sociability -0.67 -0.76 -0.57
    Dominance 0.67 0.57 0.76
    Impulsiveness 0.33 0.16 0.47
    Openness 0.32 0.15 0.47
    Negative affect -0.64 -0.73 -0.53
  Patience
    Sociability 0.74 0.65 0.81
    Dominance -0.37 -0.51 -0.21
    Impulsiveness -0.59 -0.69 -0.47
    Openness 0.21 0.04 0.37
    Negative affect -0.03 -0.21 0.14
  Inquisitiveness
    Sociability 0.34 0.18 0.48
    Dominance 0.21 0.04 0.37
    Impulsiveness 0.17 -0.01 0.33
    Openness 0.82 0.76 0.87
    Negative affect -0.56 -0.67 -0.43
Inoue-Murayama et al. (2018)
  Dominance
    Sociability -0.71 -0.79 -0.62
    Dominance 0.94 0.91 0.96
    Impulsiveness 0.83 0.77 0.88
    Openness 0.34 0.18 0.49
    Negative affect -0.38 -0.52 -0.22
  Sociability
    Sociability 0.93 0.90 0.95
    Dominance -0.40 -0.54 -0.25
    Impulsiveness -0.39 -0.52 -0.23
    Openness 0.40 0.24 0.54
    Negative affect -0.12 -0.29 0.05
  Neuroticism
    Sociability -0.30 -0.45 -0.14
    Dominance -0.03 -0.20 0.14
    mpulsiveness 0.35 0.19 0.49
    Openness -0.34 -0.49 -0.18
    Negative affect 0.82 0.75 0.87

N = 128. The highest correlation between the five factors found in this study and each factor found in a previous study are highlighted in bold. In cases where the confidence intervals of two or more of the highest correlations overlapped, we highlighted both correlations.

Table 7. Correlations between unit-weighted factor scores based on loadings from the second-order factor analysis in the present study and factor loadings from previous studies of common marmoset personality.

95% Confidence Interval
r Lower Bound Upper Bound
Iwanicki and Lehman (2015)
  Extraversion
    Pro-sociality -0.72 -0.80 -0.63
    Boldness 0.73 0.64 0.80
  Agreeableness
    Pro-sociality 0.95 0.93 0.97
    Boldness -0.18 -0.35 -0.01
  Conscientiousness
    Pro-sociality 0.73 0.64 0.81
    Boldness -0.02 -0.20 0.15
  Openness
    Pro-sociality -0.20 -0.36 -0.03
    Boldness 0.83 0.76 0.87
Koski et al. (2017)
  Conscientiousness
    Pro-sociality 0.91 0.88 0.94
    Boldness -0.35 -0.49 -0.19
  Agreeableness
    Pro-sociality 0.90 0.87 0.93
    Boldness -0.05 -0.22 0.12
  Assertiveness
    Pro-sociality -0.67 -0.76 -0.56
    Boldness 0.56 0.43 0.67
  Patience
    Pro-sociality 0.64 0.53 0.73
    Boldness 0.15 -0.02 0.32
  Inquisitiveness
    Pro-sociality -0.02 -0.19 0.16
    Boldness 0.84 0.78 0.88
Inoue-Murayama et al. (2018)
  Dominance
    Pro-sociality -0.97 -0.98 -0.95
    Boldness 0.43 0.28 0.56
  Sociability
    Pro-sociality 0.66 0.55 0.75
    Boldness 0.32 0.16 0.47
  Neuroticism
    Pro-sociality -0.21 -0.37 -0.04
    Boldness -0.68 -0.76 -0.57

N = 128. The highest correlation between the five factors found in this study and each factor found in a previous study are highlighted in bold. In cases where the confidence intervals of two or more of the highest correlations overlapped, we highlighted both correlations.

Koski et al. [31] found five factors. Compared to our first-order factors, their Conscientiousness factor overlapped with (reversed) Dominance and (reversed) Impulsiveness; their Agreeableness factor overlapped with Sociability; their Assertiveness factor overlapped with (reversed) Sociability, Dominance, and (reversed) Negative Affect; their Patience factor overlapped with Sociability; and their Inquisitiveness factor overlapped with Openness (Table 6). Compared to our second-order factors, their Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Patience factors all overlapped with Pro-sociality; their Assertiveness factor overlapped with Boldness and (reversed) Pro-sociality; and their Inquisitiveness factor overlapped with Boldness (Table 7).

Inoue-Murayama et al. [13] found three factors. Compared to our first-order factors, their Dominance factor overlapped with Dominance; their Sociability factor overlapped with Sociability; and their Neuroticism factor overlapped with Negative Affect (Table 6). Compared to our second-order factors, their Dominance and Sociability factors overlapped with low and high Pro-sociality, respectively; their Neuroticism factor overlapped with (reversed) Boldness (Table 7).

Personality and subjective well-being associations

The correlations between the subjective well-being items and the personality factors are presented in Table 8. Sociability was significantly associated with higher scores on all four items and the total subjective well-being score. Dominance was not significantly related to any of the scale’s items or the factor. Impulsiveness was significantly related to lower, and Openness was significantly related to higher, balance of positive versus negative moods, how happy raters thought they would be if they were the marmoset, and the total subjective well-being score. Negative Affect was negatively related to how happy raters thought they would be if they were the marmoset. Of the second-order factors, Pro-sociality was significantly associated with being rated as higher on all items save for the ability to achieve goals, and the total subjective well-being score. Boldness was not significantly associated with the pleasure subjects derived from social interactions, but it was significantly related to being higher in the other three items and in the total subjective well-being score.

Table 8. Correlations between personality and the subjective well-being items and factor.

95% Confidence Interval
r Lower Bound Upper Bound
Moods
    Sociability 0.42 0.26 0.55
    Dominance -0.18 -0.34 < -0.01
    Impulsiveness -0.27 -0.43 -0.11
    Openness 0.38 0.22 0.52
    Negative affect -0.14 -0.30 0.04
    Pro-sociality 0.33 0.16 0.48
    Boldness 0.31 0.15 0.46
Social
    Sociability 0.53 0.40 0.65
    Dominance -0.22 -0.38 -0.05
    Impulsiveness -0.24 -0.40 -0.07
    Openness 0.14 -0.03 0.31
    Negative affect -0.11 -0.27 0.07
    Pro-sociality 0.38 0.22 0.52
    Boldness 0.15 -0.03 0.31
Goals
    Sociability 0.39 0.24 0.53
    Dominance -0.08 -0.25 0.10
    Impulsiveness -0.22 -0.38 -0.05
    Openness 0.25 0.08 0.40
    Negative affect -0.22 -0.38 -0.05
    Pro-sociality 0.26 0.09 0.41
    Boldness 0.28 0.11 0.43
Be marmoset
    Sociability 0.45 0.30 0.58
    Dominance -0.18 -0.34 < -0.01
    Impulsiveness -0.28 -0.43 -0.11
    Openness 0.33 0.16 0.48
    Negative affect -0.27 -0.42 -0.10
    Pro-sociality 0.34 0.18 0.49
    Boldness 0.36 0.20 0.50
Subjective well-being
    Sociability 0.53 0.39 0.65
    Dominance -0.19 -0.36 -0.02
    Impulsiveness -0.30 -0.45 -0.13
    Openness 0.32 0.15 0.47
    Negative affect -0.21 -0.37 -0.04
    Pro-sociality 0.39 0.23 0.53
    Boldness 0.32 0.16 0.47

N = 128. Estimates in bold are significant at P < 0.05 after adjustment using Holm’s method. 95% confidence intervals are based on the nominal (unadjusted) significance level.

Personality and genotype associations

G840C

Twenty-seven subjects had the GG genotype, 23 had the CC genotype, and 72 were heterozygous. In the first set of analyses, we compared subjects with the GC genotype and subjects with the GG genotype to those with the CC genotype. Compared to subjects with the CC genotype, subjects with the GC or GG genotypes were significantly higher in Dominance; these associations, however, did not prevail correction for multiple tests (S15 Table). In a second set of analyses, we compared the 95 subjects who were carriers of the C allele (CC or GC genotype) to the 27 subjects with the GG genotype. None of the comparisons were statistically significant (S16 Table). In a third set of analyses, we compared the 99 subjects who were carriers of the G allele (GG or GC genotype) to the 23 subjects with the CC genotype. Carriers were significantly higher in Dominance, but this effect did not prevail correction for multiple tests (S16 Table).

T901A

Twelve subjects had the TT genotype, 35 had the AA genotype, and 76 were heterozygous. Because of this imbalance in the number of subjects, we only compared the 88 subjects who carried the T allele to the 35 subjects with the AA genotype. None of the comparisons were statistically significant (S17 Table).

Discussion

We found five personality domains—Sociability, Dominance, Impulsiveness, Openness, and Negative Affect—in common marmosets. We also found two higher-order domains. One higher-order factor, Pro-sociality, had a positive loading on Sociability and negative loadings on Dominance and Impulsiveness, and a second, Boldness, which had a positive loading on Openness and a negative loading on Negative Affect. The interrater reliabilities of both sets of domains were comparable to what has been found in studies of other primates [80], including humans [e.g., 81] and they were related to subjective well-being in ways consistent with their meaning. We found no strong evidence that either personality or subjective well-being was associated with the serotonin 1a receptor gene.

The personality domains that we found overlapped, although not completely, with those found in prior studies of common marmosets. Openness resembled eponymous domains, or a domain labeled Inquisitiveness, identified in previous studies [13,17,31,32]. Moreover, although we did not find a clear Conscientiousness factor, as did two previous studies [17,31], Impulsiveness and Pro-sociality overlapped with Conscientiousness in that all three were related to behavioral consistency and reliability, prosociality, tolerance, and low levels of aggression [17,31]. Impulsiveness, however, was also related to emotionality and reactivity whereas Conscientiousness was not. Finally, Dominance, Sociability, and Negative Affect resembled domains found in earlier studies [13,17,31,32]. On the other hand, although they may have been represented by Pro-sociality, we did not find strong evidence for a Patience [31] or a Perceptual Sensitivity [17] domain, although these may be the same construct [31] or related to Conscientiousness.

Unlike past rating-based studies that did not find higher-order factors in common marmosets [13,17,31], we found two such factors. Robustness checks indicated that these higher-order domains partly reflect a tendency for raters to see some traits as more correlated than others. However, these analyses could not exclude the possibility that these factors represented a higher-level of personality organization, perhaps reflecting group personalities [cf. 12]. Although there have been reports of higher-order factors of human personality [e.g., 82], including a so-called “general factor of personality” [e.g., 83], these reports have been criticized [e.g., 8486]. The problems that affect human studies that purportedly find higher-order personality factors were absent in the present study: each animal was rated by two or three keepers, the correlations among latent variables were considerable, and adjusting for rater effects increased rather than decreased some interfactor correlations. Nevertheless, because second-order factors were not identified in other studies of common marmosets [13,17,31,44], including one that included 77 of the same animals, prior to interpreting the meaning of this phenomenon, we urge an attempt to replicate these findings and an analysis of similar data using more flexible modeling techniques [e.g., 11].

The findings from the present study are consistent with the possibility that common marmosets evolved a personality structure that includes a domain or domains associated with self-control, such as the sort that are found in larger-brained primates, including brown capuchin monkeys [28], chimpanzees [39], and humans [59]. As these species share only a very distant common ancestors with Callitrichids and have very different socioecologies, these traits are not likely to be homologous. Instead, the presence of a domain related to self-control in common marmosets likely reflects convergent evolution that was driven by the need for individuals to meet the demands associated with cooperative breeding. Although, it is worth noting that there is still variability between studies in how these traits group, studies that examine the role that factors such as Conscientiousness, Patience, or Impulsivity play in infant rearing, especially by helpers, among common marmosets are needed to test this hypothesis.

As in our study of a subsample of these subjects [13], we found personality-subjective well-being correlations that were consistent with those found in studies of humans [45,46] and nonhuman primates [40,4751]. These findings, and those in humans and great apes that indicate that a common genetic background underlies these traits [8792], are consistent with the possibility that these relationships are ancestral.

Our failure to find association between SNPs related to the serotonin 1a receptor gene and either personality domains is not consistent with previous findings of an association between this genotype and personality in chimpanzees [54]. It is possible that our failure to find associations resulted from the personality measure that we used. However, as the associations between personality and serotonin-related genes in humans are likely false positives [93], we suspect that we did not find any associations because there were none.

This study had shortcomings. First, nearly 40% of the subjects were single housed. Behaviors related to some traits might therefore have been rare or absent, and so we still may not have been capturing enough between-subjects variation. Second, the factor structure was compared to studies that used different, although partly overlapping, instruments. It is unclear to what degree the use of different measures may have obscured similarities or blurred differences between the structures in these studies. This limitation also prevented us from using other statistical methods to directly compare these structures. Third, we judged that it was worth reporting the genetic associations so that they may contribute to future meta-analyses, as we alluded to previously, to identify genetic effects considerably larger sample sizes are needed. Fourth, the interrater reliabilities of the subjective well-being variables were lower than those reported in other nonhuman primate species, for example, chimpanzees [40,47], and the degree to which a general factor saturated the items was low.

The cliché that a study’s findings can yield more questions than answers fits the present case. Nevertheless, these findings highlight the need for (and promise of) large collaborative studies if we are to understand the proximate and ultimate origins of personality structure in common marmosets, and other species, including ours.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S10 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S11 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S12 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S13 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S14 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S1 Table. Definitions of factors from previous studies.

a Substituted for “+ stingy”. b Substituted for “+ bold”. c Substituted for “+ cooperative”. d Substituted for “+ cool”. e Substituted for “− dependent”.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Congruence coefficients for comparison of varimax- and promax-rotated structures.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Pattern matrix from the first-order factor analysis of ratings on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire.

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the varimax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Correlation between factors = 0.07.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Congruence coefficients for comparison of factors from raw and residualized scores.

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 1’s ratings.

N = 128. Imp = Impulsiveness, Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Pattern matrix from second-order factor analysis from rater 1’s ratings.

N = 128. I = Pro-sociality (reversed), II = Boldness, h2 = communalities. 1 When rounded to three decimal places, this loading was equal to 0.995. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Factor correlation = -0.14.

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 2’s ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S11 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 3’s ratings.

N = 81. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S12 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S13 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S14 Table. Pattern matrix from second-order factor analysis from the weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Factor correlation = -0.08.

(DOCX)

S15 Table. Effects of G840C genotype on personality domains.

N = 122.

(DOCX)

S16 Table. Effects of G840C genotype on personality domains.

N = 122.

(DOCX)

S17 Table. Effects of T901A genotype on personality domains.

N = 123.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the Leading graduate program in Primatology and Wildlife Science. We are also grateful to Hiromi Kobayashi for technical support and to Mr. Akihiro Kawasaki, Mr. Takashi Fukuoka, and Ms. Chiho Takeda for rating the animals. AW thanks Kyoto University for hosting him as a Visiting Professor at the Wildlife Research Center.

Data Availability

Data needed to reproduce the analyses are available via the Open Science Foundation website: https://osf.io/ysrja/.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by AMED Grant Number JP20dm0307006 to TH, KAKENHI Grant Numbers 19H04904 and 20H00420 to MI-M, and 18H05090 and18K06372 to CY. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Rylands AB, Coimbra-Filho AF, Mittermeier RA. Systematics, geographic distribution, and some notes on the conservation status of the Callitrichidae. In: Rylands AB, editor. Marmosets and tamarins: Systematics, behaviour, and ecology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 11–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rylands AB, de Faria DS. Habitats, feeding ecology, and home range size in the genus Callithrix. In: Rylands AB, editor. Marmosets and tamarins: Systematics, behaviour, and ecology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 262–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Servick K. Why are U.S. neuroscientists clamoring for marmosets? Science. 2018:383–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Preuss TM. Critique of pure marmoset. Brain Behav Evol. 2019;93(2–3):92–107. doi: 10.1159/000500500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Burkart JM, Fehr E, Efferson C, van Schaik CP. Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(50):19762–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710310104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Burkart JM, Allon O, Amici F, Fichtel C, Finkenwirth C, Heschl A, et al. The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications. 2014;5. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kawai N, Yasue M, Banno T, Ichinohe N. Marmoset monkeys evaluate third-party reciprocity. Biology Letters. 2014;10(5). doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Burkart JM, van Schaick CP. Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Anim Cogn. 2010;13:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Digby LJ, Ferrari SF, Saltzman W. Callitrichines: The role of competition in cooperatively breeding species. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM, editors. Primates in Perspective. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010. p. 91–107. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Šlipogor V, Gunhold-de Oliveira T, Tadić Z, Massen JJM, Bugnyar T. Consistent inter-individual differences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) in Boldness-Shyness, Stress-Activity, and Exploration-Avoidance. Am J Primatol. 2016;78(9):961–73. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Martin JS, Massen JJM, Šlipogor V, Bugnyar T, Jaeggi AV, Koski SE. The EGA+GNM framework: An integrative approach to modelling behavioural syndromes. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2019;10(2):245–57. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.13100 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Koski SE, Burkart JM. Common marmosets show social plasticity and group-level similarity in personality. Scientific Reports. 2015;5. doi: 10.1038/srep08878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Inoue-Murayama M, Yokoyama C, Yamanashi Y, Weiss A. Common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) personality, subjective well-being, hair cortisol level, and AVPR1a, OPRM1, and DAT genotypes. Scientific Reports. 2018. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28112-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tomassetti D, Caracciolo S, Manciocco A, Chiarotti F, Vitale A, De Filippis B. Personality and lateralization in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioural Processes. 2019;167. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Yokoyama C, Kawasaki A, Hayashi T, Onoe H. Linkage between the midline cortical serotonergic system and social behavior traits: positron emission tomography studies of common marmosets. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:2136–45. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Šlipogor V, Burkart JM, Martin JS, Bugnyar T, Koski SE. Personality method validation in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): Getting the best of both worlds. J Comp Psychol. 2020;134(1):52–70. doi: 10.1037/com0000188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Iwanicki S, Lehmann J. Behavioral and trait rating assessments of personality in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Psychol. 2015;129(3):205–17. doi: 10.1037/a0039318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gosling SD, Graybeal A. Tree thinking: A new paradigm for integrating comparative data in psychology. J Gen Psychol. 2007;134(2):259–77. doi: 10.3200/GENP.134.2.259-278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Harvey PH, Pagel MD. The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Gorsuch RL. Factor analysis. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Araya-Ajoy YG, Dingemanse NJ. Characterizing behavioural ’characters’: an evolutionary framework. P R Soc B. 2014;281(1776). doi: 10.1098/Rspb.2013.2645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the Five-Factor Model and its applications. J Pers. 1992;60(2):175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the Five-Factor Model. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:417–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Adams MJ, Majolo B, Ostner J, Schuelke O, De Marco A, Thierry B, et al. Personality structure and social style in macaques. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;109(2):338–53. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Weiss A, Staes N, Pereboom JJM, Inoue-Murayama M, Stevens JMG, Eens M. Personality in bonobos. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(9):1430–9. doi: 10.1177/0956797615589933 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wilson VAD, Inoue-Murayama M, Weiss A. A comparison of personality in the common and Bolivian squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus and Saimiri boliviensis). J Comp Psychol. 2018;132(1):24–39. doi: 10.1037/com0000093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Robinson LM, Morton FB, Gartner MC, Widness J, Paukner A, Essler JL, et al. Divergent personality structures of brown and white-faced capuchins. J Comp Psychol. 2016;130(4):305–12. doi: 10.1037/com0000037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Morton FB, Lee PC, Buchanan-Smith HM, Brosnan SF, Thierry B, Paukner A, et al. Personality structure in brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella): Comparisons with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo spp.), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Comp Psychol. 2013;127(3):282–98. doi: 10.1037/a0031723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fernández-Bolaños M, Delval I, de Oliveira RS, Izar P. Assessing the personality structure of wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus xanthosternos) using trait rating and behavioral coding. J Comp Psychol. 2020;134(3):349–60. doi: 10.1037/com0000219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Masilkova M, Šlipogor V, Weiss A, Konečná M. Comparative assessment of behaviorally derived personality structures in golden-handed tamarins (Saguinus midas), cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus), and common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Psychol. 2020;Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/com0000226 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Koski SE, Buchanan-Smith H, Ash H, Burkart J, Bugnyar T, Weiss A. Common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) personality. J Comp Psychol. 2017;131(4):326–36. doi: 10.1037/com0000089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yokoyama C, Onoe H. Molecular brain imaging of personality traits in nonhuman primates: A study of the common marmoset. In: Inoue-Murayama M, Kawamura S, Weiss A, editors. From Genes to Animal Behavior: Social Structures, Personalities, Communication by Color. Tokyo: Springer Japan; 2011. p. 389–406. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weiss A. Personality traits: A view from the animal kingdom. J Pers. 2017. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Delgado MM, Sulloway FJ. Attributes of conscientiousness throughout the animal kingdom: An empirical and evolutionary overview. Psychol Bull. 2017;143(8):823–67. doi: 10.1037/bul0000107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn CL, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson RC, et al. The evolution of self-control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(20):E2140–E8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323533111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McCrae RR, Terracciano A, 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(3):547–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547 ISI:000227310400009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Weiss A, King JE, Hopkins WD. A cross-setting study of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality structure and development: Zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center. Am J Primatol. 2007;69(11):1264–77. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.King JE, Weiss A, Farmer KH. A chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) analogue of cross-national generalization of personality structure: Zoological parks and an African sanctuary. J Pers. 2005;73(2):389–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00313.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.King JE, Figueredo AJ. The Five-Factor Model plus Dominance in chimpanzee personality. J Res Pers. 1997;31(2):257–71. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1997.2179 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Weiss A, Inoue-Murayama M, Hong K-W, Inoue E, Udono S, Ochiai T, et al. Assessing chimpanzee personality and subjective well-being in Japan. Am J Primatol. 2009;71(4):283–92. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Freeman HD, Brosnan SF, Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Gosling SD. Developing a comprehensive and comparative questionnaire for measuring personality in chimpanzees using a simultaneous top-down/bottom-up design. Am J Primatol. 2013;75(10):1042–53. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dutton DM. Subjective assessment of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality: Reliability and stability of trait ratings. Primates. 2008;49(4):253–9. doi: 10.1007/s10329-008-0094-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979;86(2):420–8. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Šlipogor V, Massen JJM, Schiel N, Souto A, Bugnyar T. Temporal consistency and ecological validity of personality structure in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): A unifying field and laboratory approach. Am J Primatol. 2021;83(2). doi: 10.1002/ajp.23229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.DeNeve KM, Cooper H. The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124(2):197–229. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(1):138–61. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.King JE, Landau VI. Can chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) happiness be estimated by human raters? J Res Pers. 2003;37(1):1–15. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00527-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Robinson LM, Altschul D, Wallace E, Úbeda Y, Llorente M, Machanda Z, et al. Chimpanzees with positive welfare are happier, extraverted, and emotionally stable. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;191:90–7. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Weiss A, King JE, Perkins L. Personality and subjective well-being in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii). J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90(3):501–11. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Weiss A, Adams MJ, Widdig A, Gerald MS. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as living fossils of hominoid personality and subjective well-being. J Comp Psychol. 2011;125(1):72–83. doi: 10.1037/a0021187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Robinson LM, Waran NK, Leach MC, Morton FB, Paukner A, Lonsdorf E, et al. Happiness is positive welfare in brown capuchins (Sapajus apella). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2016;181:145–51. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.029 WOS:000381171300019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wilt J, Revelle W. Extraversion. In: Widiger TA, editor. The Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017. p. 57–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Tackett JL, Lahey BB. Neuroticism. In: Widiger TA, editor. The Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017. p. 39–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Staes N, Sherwood CC, Freeman H, Brosnan SF, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD, et al. Serotonin Receptor 1A Variation Is Associated with Anxiety and Agonistic Behavior in Chimpanzees. Mol Biol Evol. 2019;36(7):1418–29. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msz061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hayashi T, Hou Y, Glasser MF, Autio JA, Knoblauch K, Inoue-Murayama M, et al. The nonhuman primate neuroimaging and neuroanatomy project. Neuroimage. 2021;229:117726. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117726 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Weiss A. Exploring factor space (and other adventures) with the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire. In: Vonk J, Weiss A, Kuczaj S, editors. Personality in Nonhuman Animals. Cham, Switzerland: Springer; 2017. p. 19–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Goldberg LR. An alternative "description of personality": the Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59(6):1216–29. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.6.1216 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. Updating Norman’s “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;49(3):710–21. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.49.3.710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Costa PT Jr., McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125(2):276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;47(5):1105–17. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.47.5.1105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Pavot W, Diener E, Colvin CR, Sandvik E. Further validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale—Evidence for the cross-method convergence of well-being measures. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1991;57(1):149–61. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5701_17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Campbell A. The sense of well-being in America: Recent patterns and trends. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cantor N, Sanderson CA. Life task participation and well-being: The importance of taking part in daily life. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1999. p. 230–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Revelle W. psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. 1.9.12 ed. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Navarro-Gonzalez D, Lorenzo-Seva U. EFA.MRFA: Dimensionality assessment using minimum rank factor analysis. 1.0.9 ed2020.
  • 68.de Winter JCF, Dodou D, Wieringa PA. Exploratory factor analysis with small sample sizes. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2009;44(2):147–81. doi: 10.1080/00273170902794206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods. 1999;4(1):84–99. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.1.84 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Mundfrom DJ, Shaw DG, Ke TL. Minimum sample size recommendations for conducting factor analyses. International Journal of Testing. 2005;5(2):159–68. doi: 10.1207/s15327574ijt0502_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30(2):179–85. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Auerswald M, Moshagen M. How to determine the number of factors to retain in exploratory factor analysis: a comparison of extraction methods under realistic conditions. Psychol Methods. 2019;24(4):468–91. doi: 10.1037/met0000200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics. 1978;6(2):461–4. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Ceulemans E, Kiers HAL. Selecting among three-mode principal component models of different types and complexities: A numerical convex hull based method. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2006;59(1):133–50. doi: 10.1348/000711005X64817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lorenzo-Seva U, Timmerman ME, Kiers HA. The Hull method for selecting the number of common factors. Multivar Behav Res. 2011;46(2):340–64. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.564527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Wainer H. Estimating coefficients in linear models: It don’t make no nevermind. Psychol Bull. 1976;83(2):213–7. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.83.2.213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics. 1979;6(2):65–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Costa PT Jr., McCrae RR. Manual supplement for the NEO-4. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Bäckström M, Björklund F, Larsson MR. Five-factor inventories have a major general factor related to social desirability which can be reduced by framing items neutrally. J Res Pers. 2009;43(3):335–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Freeman HD, Gosling SD. Personality in nonhuman primates: A review and evaluation of past research. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(8):653–71. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20833 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Costa PT Jr., McCrae RR. Cross-sectional studies of personality in a national sample: I. Development and validation of survey measures. Psychology and Aging. 1986;1(2):140–3. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.1.2.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Digman JM. Higher-order factors of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;73(6):1246–56. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Musek J. A general factor of personality: Evidence for the Big One in the Five-Factor Model. J Res Pers. 2007;41(6):1213–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Revelle W, Wilt J. The general factor of personality: A general critique. J Res Pers. 2013;47(5):493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ferguson E, Chamorro-Premuzic T, Pickering A, Weiss A. Five into one doesn’t go: A critique of the General Factor of Personality. In: Chamorro-Premuzic T, von Stumm S, Furnham A, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011. p. 162–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Mutch C. Higher-order factors of the Big Five model of personality: A reanalysis of Digman (1997). Psychol Rep. 2005;96(1):167–77. doi: 10.2466/pr0.96.1.167-177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Weiss A, Bates TC, Luciano M. Happiness is a personal(ity) thing: The genetics of personality and well-being in a representative sample. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:205–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02068.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Weiss A, Baselmans BML, Hofer E, Yang J, Okbay A, Lind PA, et al. Personality polygenes, positive affect, and life satisfaction. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2016;19(5):407–17. doi: 10.1017/thg.2016.65 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hahn E, Gottschling J, König CJ, Spinath FM. The heritability of job satisfaction reconsidered: only unique environmental influences beyond personality. Journal of Business and Psychology. 2016;31(2):217–31. doi: 10.1007/s10869-015-9413-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Hahn E, Johnson W, Spinath FM. Beyond the heritability of life satisfaction–The roles of personality and twin-specific influences. Journal of Research in Personality. 2013;47(6):757–67. doi: 0.1016/j.jrp.2013.07.003 [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Adams MJ, King JE, Weiss A. The majority of genetic variation in orangutan personality and subjective-well being is nonadditive. Behav Genet. 2012;42:675–86. doi: 10.1007/s10519-012-9537-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Weiss A, King JE, Enns RM. Subjective well-being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83:1141–9. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Munafò MR, Freimer NB, Ng W, Ophoff R, Veijola J, Miettunen J, et al. 5-HTTLPR genotype and anxiety-related personality traits: A meta-analysis and new data. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(2):271–81. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30808 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S10 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S11 Fig. Hull test results.

(TIF)

S12 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S13 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S14 Fig. Scree plot.

(TIF)

S1 Table. Definitions of factors from previous studies.

a Substituted for “+ stingy”. b Substituted for “+ bold”. c Substituted for “+ cooperative”. d Substituted for “+ cool”. e Substituted for “− dependent”.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Congruence coefficients for comparison of varimax- and promax-rotated structures.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Pattern matrix from the first-order factor analysis of ratings on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire.

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the varimax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Correlation between factors = 0.07.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of residualized ratings.

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Congruence coefficients for comparison of factors from raw and residualized scores.

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 1’s ratings.

N = 128. Imp = Impulsiveness, Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Pattern matrix from second-order factor analysis from rater 1’s ratings.

N = 128. I = Pro-sociality (reversed), II = Boldness, h2 = communalities. 1 When rounded to three decimal places, this loading was equal to 0.995. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Factor correlation = -0.14.

(DOCX)

S10 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 2’s ratings.

N = 128. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S11 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of rater 3’s ratings.

N = 81. Factors were not assigned labels. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S12 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S13 Table. Pattern matrix from the factor analysis of weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. Soc = Sociability, Dom = Dominance, Imp = Impulsiveness, Opn = Openness, Neg = Negative Affect, h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold.

(DOCX)

S14 Table. Pattern matrix from second-order factor analysis from the weighted correlation matrix (Rw).

N = 128. h2 = communalities. Factors extracted using a maximum likelihood estimation and rotated using the promax procedure. Factor loadings greater than or equal to |0.4| are in bold. Factor correlation = -0.08.

(DOCX)

S15 Table. Effects of G840C genotype on personality domains.

N = 122.

(DOCX)

S16 Table. Effects of G840C genotype on personality domains.

N = 122.

(DOCX)

S17 Table. Effects of T901A genotype on personality domains.

N = 123.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

Data needed to reproduce the analyses are available via the Open Science Foundation website: https://osf.io/ysrja/.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES