Abstract
Hippocampal pyramidal cells encode an animal’s location by single action potentials and complex spike bursts. These elementary signals are believed to play distinct roles in memory consolidation. The timing of single spikes and bursts is determined by intrinsic excitability and theta oscillations (5–10 Hz). Yet contributions of these dynamics to place fields remain elusive due to the lack of methods for specific modification of burst discharge. In mice lacking Kcnq3-containing M-type K+ channels, we find that pyramidal cell bursts are less coordinated by the theta rhythm than in controls during spatial navigation, but not alert immobility. Less modulated bursts are followed by an intact post-burst pause of single spike firing, resulting in a temporal discoordination of network oscillatory and intrinsic excitability. Place fields of single spikes in one- and two-dimensional environments are smaller in the mutant. Optogenetic manipulations of upstream signals reveal that neither medial septal GABA-ergic nor cholinergic inputs alone, but rather their joint activity, is required for entrainment of bursts. Our results suggest that altered representations by bursts and single spikes may contribute to deficits underlying cognitive disabilities associated with KCNQ3-mutations in humans.
Subject terms: Ion channels in the nervous system, Neural circuits, Excitability
Hippocampal pyramidal cells encode an animal’s location by single action potentials and complex spike bursts. The authors show that Kcnq3-containing M-channels synergistically with GABAergic inputs coordinate complex spike bursts during theta oscillations, which is a key mechanism for spatial coding by single spikes.
Introduction
How is the balance between reliability and precision of neuronal communication regulated in the brain? Hippocampal pyramidal cells fire about half of their action potentials in complex spike bursts1—several spikes with accommodating amplitudes and interspike intervals (ISI) of 3–15 ms. Bursts increase the probability of transmitter release, leading to more reliable postsynaptic responses than single spikes2, yet are less informative about the timing of presynaptic activity. Single spikes and bursts provide therefore complementary trade-offs between precision and reliability of information transfer3. In the CA1 area, cells participating in engrams of new experiences fire more bursts and encode less precise spatial representations, in contrast to robust spatial coding by neurons with a higher firing probability of single spikes4, genetic suppression of which does not interfere with hippocampus-dependent learning3.
Firing of place cells in the dorsal CA1 area5 appears to be driven by signals of self-motion and spatial cues from entorhinal cortex grid and border cells, respectively6–8. The firing of upstream CA3 place cells is more experience-dependent and has been shown to encode substantially different environments9. Subcortical afferents, including those from the midline thalamus, the medial septum (MS), and aminergic neurons signal to hippocampus information about sensory stimuli relevant for learning and goal-directed behaviors10–12. The translation of input signals from cortical regions into location-specific hippocampal output involves subcortical regulation of excitability in the CA1 area by theta rhythm, activity-dependent plasticity, local inhibition, and intrinsic excitability13–16.
A slowly activating and non-inactivating potassium conductance, mediated by heteromeric Kcnq2/Kcnq3 (Kv7.2/Kv7.3) and Kcnq5/Kcnq3 (Kv7.5/Kv7.3) voltage-gated K+ channels, contributes to resting membrane potential in hippocampal pyramidal cells17. These ‚M-type’ channels can be inhibited by acetylcholine via M1 receptors18,19, which promote repetitive firing of pyramidal cells18,20. Together with Kcnq2, Kcnq3 subunits are targeted to axon initial segments. Here Kcnq3-containing channels regulate the functional availability of Na+ channels and affect spontaneous spiking, action potential amplitude, and propagation17,21,22. Kcnq/M-currents in somata and dendrites contribute to medium after-hyperpolarization, reduce excitability, prolong interspike intervals (ISIs) and regulate synaptic integration, and subthreshold resonance22–26. In contrast to Kcnq5, which modulates hippocampal information processing by shunting inhibitory postsynaptic currents in the CA3 area27, Kcnq3 is strongly expressed also in the CA1 area28. Mutations in the genes encoding either subunit of heteromeric KCNQ2/3 voltage-gated potassium channels have been linked to childhood epilepsy29–32. Recently, cognitive impairments including autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability associated with recurrent seizures have been associated with mutations and polymorphisms of the human KCNQ3 gene33–37. Yet the significance of the Kcnq3 subunit for cognitive information processing in vivo remains unknown.
Here, capitalizing on a crucial position of Kcnq3 in the control of high-frequency discharge of pyramidal cells, we used constitutive as well as pyramidal cell-specific Kcnq3 gene ablation along with electrophysiological and optogenetic approaches to study cellular, network and circuit mechanisms of spatial representations by bursts and single action potentials. We found specific effects of Kcnq3 on burst discharge of pyramidal cells ex vivo and in vivo, and studied the timing of bursts in cells lacking Kcnq3 during spatial navigation and alert immobility. These behavioral states differ in levels of cholinergic stimulation provided to hippocampus by MS afferents. Changes of bursts timing during theta oscillations were accompanied by altered representations of animal’s position by single spikes and could be reproduced by optogenetic manipulations of MS afferents. These findings highlight the role of theta oscillations in the cholinergic modulation of hippocampal representations38 and demonstrate a mechanism for the interaction of coding by bursts and single spikes.
Results
Kcnq3-containing M-channels attenuate burst discharge of pyramidal cells
To investigate the impact of burst discharge on the information processing in the CA1 area we generated Kcnq3-/- mice39 (Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). Disruption of Kcnq3 abolished the expression of both the Kcnq3 mRNA (Supplementary Fig. 1c) and the Kcnq3 protein (Supplementary Fig. 1d), but did not lead to a compensatory upregulation of Kcnq2 expression (Supplementary Fig. 1c). Hence, Kcnq3−/− mice likely form increased levels of Kcnq2 homomeric at the expense of Kcnq2/3 heteromeric channels. This is expected to result in a large decrease of M-current magnitude since currents through heteromeric Kcnq2/3 channels are much larger than those mediated by homomeric Kcnq2 channels40,41 (Fig. 1a,b). Furthermore, homomeric Kcnq2 channels are less modulated by M1-receptors, which act by decreasing levels of phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate that binds preferentially to Kcnq342,43. Ex vivo, Kcnq3−/− and Kcnq3+/+ pyramidal cells did not differ in their resting membrane potential and input resistance (Table 1, Supplementary Fig. 2), possibly because homomeric Kcnq2 channels that remain in Kcnq3−/− neurons prevent a significant change of these parameters. They also displayed similar amplitudes of small conductance calcium-activated potassium (SK-) channel independent (apamin-insensitive) medium after-hyperpolarization (mAHP, Supplementary Fig. 2), in agreement with previous reports24,26. Mutant and control pyramidal cells fired action potentials of a similar amplitude and with a similar rate of depolarization (Table 1). However, the rate of repolarization was higher and action potentials were shorter in the mutant (Table 1). In response to the same amount of injected current, Kcnq3−/− pyramidal cells fired more repetitive action potentials than controls (Fig. 1c, d). Furthermore, trains of action potentials evoked by current injections displayed overall higher frequencies in mutant cells (Fig. 1e). Accordingly, pyramidal cells in vivo spontaneously fired bursts which mostly consisted of a larger number of spikes in Kcnq3−/− than in control mice (Fig. 1f, g). Surprisingly, the rate of burst occurrence did not differ between genotypes (Kcnq3+/+, 0.23 ± 0.01 Hz, n = 306 cells from 6 mice; Kcnq3−/−, 0.24 ± 0.01 Hz, n = 413 cells from 4 mice; p = 0.08, Mann–Whitney-U-Test), but the rate of single spikes and, consequently, the average firing rate of pyramidal cells were reduced in the mutant (single spikes: Kcnq3+/+, 0.80 ± 0.03 Hz, n = 309 cells; Kcnq3−/−, 0.49 ± 0.02 Hz, n = 413 cells; p < 0.0001; all spikes: Kcnq3+/+, 1.31 ± 0.04 Hz; Kcnq3−/−, 1.12 ± 0.03 Hz; p < 0.0001, Mann–Whitney-U-Test).
Table 1.
Kcnq3+/+, n = 11, N = 4 | Kcnq3−/−, n = 12, N = 4 | |
---|---|---|
Resting membrane potential, mV | −62.8 ± 1.1 | −63.9 ± 1.1 |
Action potential amplitude, mV | 103.3 ± 2.6 | 103.7 ± 2.1 |
Action potential half-width, ms | 0.90 ± 0.03 | 0.83 ± 0,02 * |
Depolarization rate during action potential, mV/ms | 362.1 ± 19.8 | 354.4 ± 18.1 |
Repolarization rate during action potential, mV/ms | −90.4 ± 2.5 | −100.0 ± 2.7 * |
Data are presented as mean ± SEM, *, p < 0.05, t test.
In control mice the initial three spikes of either short or long bursts were emitted in vivo with progressively increasing ISIs, resulting in frequency accommodation by ~20 Hz (Fig. 1f,h). In Kcnq3-/- mice, frequency accommodation was dramatically diminished (Fig. 1f,h,i). Furthermore, bursts in the mutant had overall higher frequency than in the control (interspike intervals: Kcnq3+/+, 7.42 ± 0.04 ms, n = 308 cells; Kcnq3−/−, 6.67 ± 0.02 ms, n = 413 cells; F1,7604 = 195.2, p < 0.0001, ANOVA).
The more prominent burst firing in Kcnq3−/− pyramidal cells was accompanied by a reversed relationship between burst length and intraburst frequency. In contrast to controls, in the mutant, longer bursts had higher frequency of spikes (i.e., shorter ISIs) than shorter bursts (Fig. 1f, h, i). Hence Kcnq3-containing channels attenuate high-frequency firing of pyramidal cells and influence spike timing during complex spike bursts.
Kcnq3 loss alters timing of complex spike bursts during spatial navigation
Kcnq3-containing channels may influence the modulation of excitability caused by cholinergic inputs which are known to be active during theta oscillations19,38 (Fig. 2a). We recorded the activity of 722 putative pyramidal cells (single units, Supplementary Fig. 3a) from the hippocampal CA1 area in freely behaving Kcnq3−/− and wild-type mice using silicon probes and computed cross-correlations of burst times in pairs of pyramidal cells during theta oscillations. In line with earlier reports44, pyramidal cells of control mice generated bursts in a temporally coordinated fashion with an average interval between bursts of ~140 ms, i.e., at the theta band frequency of ~7 Hz (Fig. 2b). The ensemble theta-rhythmicity of bursts was abolished in Kcnq3−/− mice (Fig. 2b,c).
To reveal changes in the activity of individual pyramidal cells, we examined modulation of burst firing during two distinct forms of theta oscillations. We distinguished movement-related theta oscillations (type 1 theta) during spatial navigation (Fig. 2d), and immobility-related theta (Fig. 2e), which occurs in absence of movement during high alertness and sensory processing (type 2 theta45). The two types of theta oscillations were initially characterized based on the changes of the mesoscopic rhythm upon systemic application of muscarinic antagonists. These leave the power of local field potential (LFP) theta type 1 largely unaffected (atropine-resistant theta) while abolishing the type 2 (atropine-sensitive) rhythm46. Atropine sensitivity of type 2 theta is owed to its reliance on the cholinergic excitation of PV-cells in the MS47,48, which are crucial for theta rhythm. In line with cholinergic modulation of encoding new information38, hippocampal acetylcholine levels during behaviors associated with type 1 theta are high49. Behaviors connected to type 2 theta are associated with more variable levels of cholinergic stimulation49,50 which, similar to actions of acetylcholine in the cortex, may play a role in changes of information processing during increased attention38. In controls, pyramidal cells generated the majority of bursts close to troughs of both movement- and immobility-related theta (Fig. 2d,e). Unlike the situation in WT mice, the population probability of burst firing in Kcnq3−/− mice was not entrained by movement-related theta oscillations (Fig. 2d). These changes were brought about, on the one hand, by a reduced number of cells locked to theta oscillations in the mutant (76%, control, 60%, mutant, χ²(1, n = 400) = 11.2, p < 0.0008, χ² - test) and, on the other hand, by a larger spread of preferred phases of locked cells. Their phase distribution was not modulated by the theta rhythm in the mutant (Kcnq3+/+: n = 145 cells, p < 0.0001; Kcnq3−/−: n = 126 cells, p = 0.14, Rayleigh test, Supplementary Fig. 3b) and was better described by a mixture of two circular distributions (Kcnq3+/+: p = 0.374, Kcnq3−/−: p = 0.032, Silverman’s bootstrap test). During immobility-related theta oscillations, mutant and control pyramidal cells fired bursts at phases that were close to oscillation troughs (Kcnq3+/+: 344°, n = 50 cells, p < 0.0001, Rayleigh test; Kcnq3−/−: 315°, n = 115 cells, p < 0.0001; Supplementary Fig. 3b). In agreement with the preserved phase-locking of individual cells, auto-correlations of burst times from combined theta-epochs during movement and immobility indicated a similar theta rhythmicity in individual cells of both genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 3c). Thus, the loss of Kcnq3-containing M-channels altered periodic temporal coordination of burst discharge in CA1 pyramidal cells during spatial navigation, but not during immobility-related theta oscillations (Fig. 2f).
Dysfunction of M-type K+-channels can impair coordination of burst discharge by interfering either with the generation or the readout of theta rhythm. This may involve many mechanisms, prominently including the activity of hippocampal interneurons (Fig. 2a). Average firing rates of putative fast spiking interneurons, a population controlling excitability of pyramidal cells and their synchronization during network oscillations, were unchanged in Kcnq3−/− mice (Kcnq3−/−, n = 85 cells; Kcnq3+/+, n = 106 cells, p = 0.68, Mann–Whitney-U-Test). The power of theta oscillations in Kcnq3−/− mice did not differ from controls either (Supplementary Fig. 4a). To study whether selective Kcnq3 ablation in pyramidal cells affects theta oscillations we generated Emx1-ΔKcnq3 mice in which the Kcnq3 gene was preferentially disrupted in pyramidal cells (Supplementary Fig. 4b). LFP theta oscillations were not affected in behaving Emx1-ΔKcnq3 mice (Supplementary Fig. 4c). Collectively these results suggest that the readout rather than the generation of the theta rhythm is influenced by the loss of Kcnq3 in pyramidal cells.
Temporal disconnection of intrinsic and network-driven excitability impairs place fields generated by single spikes
Next, we investigated how the organization of burst discharge during theta-oscillations translates into spatial representations by firing rates. Pyramidal cells fired selectively in portions of environment (“place fields”) while an animal explored a rectangular arena or a circular track (Fig. 3a). Place field sizes did not differ significantly between genotypes in either enclosure (Fig. 3b, Supplementary Information, Statistical Analysis). To study spatial representations by bursts and single spikes, we computed spatial firing maps separately for spikes emitted in, or outside, of bursts. Sizes of these place fields were represented as a fraction of place fields generated by all spikes (Fig. 3a, b). Strikingly, the fields defined by the firing of single spikes were markedly smaller in Kcnq3−/− than in control mice (Fig. 3a, b). These changes were found both in the arena (Kcnq3+/+: 0.46 ± 0.03, n = 76 cells from 5 mice; Kcnq3−/−: 0.30 ± 0.03, n = 95 cells from 4 mice, p = 0.0004, Mann–Whitney-U-Test) and on the track (Kcnq3+/+: 0.53 ± 0.03, n = 78 cells from 5 mice; Kcnq3−/−: 0.40 ± 0.02, n = 94 cells from 4 mice, p < 0.0001, Mann–Whitney-U-Test).
How was the spatial coding by bursts influenced by the ablation of Kcnq3? The sizes of place fields of bursts were slightly larger in the mutant, but only on the circular track (Fig. 3b). On the other hand, peak as well as average rates within place fields were considerably higher in both enclosures, and the discharge during bursts was more diffusely distributed (sparse) on the track in Kcnq3−/− mice (Table 2). Furthermore, on the track, spatial information conveyed by spikes fired either within or outside bursts, was reduced in Kcnq3−/− mice (Table 2).
Table 2.
Arena | Kcnq3+/+, n = 79, N = 5 | Kcnq3−/−, n = 96, N = 4 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All spikes | Burst | Single spikes | All spikes | Burst | Single spikes | |
Peak rate, Hz | 4.93 ± 0.36 | 3.52 ± 0.30 | 2.60 ± 0.16 | 6.80 ± 0.46*** | 5.49 ± 0.44**** | 3.01 ± 0.20 |
In-field rate, Hz | 2.25 ± 0.10 | 1.86 ± 0.08 | 1.58 ± 0.05 | 2.51 ± 0.09 | 2.27 ± 0.09*** | 1.69 ± 0.06 |
Sparsity | 0.49 ± 0.03 | 0.38 ± 0.0317 | 0.54 ± 0.04 | 0.59 ± 0.04 | 0.45 ± 0.04 | 0.63 ± 0.04 |
Inform. density, bit/spike | 1.06 ± 0.18 | 1.33 ± 0.20 | 0.82 ± 0.16 | 0.64 ± 0.09 | 1.01 ± 0.11 | 0.47 ± 0.06 |
Track | Kcnq3+/+, n = 78, N = 5 | Kcnq3−/−, n = 94, N = 4 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All spikes | Burst | Single spikes | All spikes | Burst | Single spikes | |
Peak rate, Hz | 13.29 ± 0.97 | 9.74 ± 0.82 | 6.54 ± 0.53 | 15.28 ± 0.77 | 13.41 ± 0.78*** | 5.95 ± 0.41 |
In-field rate, Hz | 3.73 ± 0.17 | 3.20 ± 0.16 | 2.69 ± 0.19 | 4.32 ± 0.21 | 4.31 ± 0.23**** | 2.47 ± 0.09 |
Sparsity | 0.43 ± 0.04 | 0.25 ± 0.03 | 0.44 ± 0.04 | 0.52 ± 0.04* | 0.37 ± 0.03**** | 0.50 ± 0.03 |
Inform. density, bit/spike | 1.31 ± 0.18 | 2.18 ± 0.25 | 1.20 ± 0.17 | 0.86 ± 0.12** | 1.23 ± 0.12*** | 0.86 ± 0.13* |
Data are presented as mean ± SEM, *, p < 0.025 (significance level is adjusted for comparisons of the same cells in the two enclosures), **, p < 0.01, ***, p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001, Mann–Whitney-U-Test.
An earlier study suggested that a subpopulation of cells fires bursts at highest rates close to the center of place fields, where bursts overlap with single spikes51. In line with those results51, which were obtained in rats, place fields of bursts and of single spikes in Kcnq3+/+ mice were overall correlated (z-transformed Pearson’s r, arena: 0.63 ± 0.09, n = 43 cells; track: 0.46 ± 0.05, n = 68 cells). The spatial coordination was particularly high in a fraction of cells (Supplementary Fig. 5a,b). The spatial correlation of burst and single spike firing rates was markedly reduced in Kcnq3−/− mice compared to controls (arena: 0.42 ± 0.05, n = 73 cells, p = 0.0006; track: 0.29 ± 0.03, n = 90 cells, p = 0.002, Mann–Whitney-U-Test).
To investigate how interactions between bursts and single spikes at time scales of theta oscillations can influence spatial representations by the discharge rate we examined the influence of a burst on the probability of an ensuing single spike. In line with earlier reports44,51, firing of a single spike increased, and later decreased again, the probability of a subsequent burst (Supplementary Fig. 5c). Furthermore, in both genotypes, the probability of single spike firing following a burst was reduced during a period of up to 50–60 ms (Fig. 3c). These time windows of lower intrinsic excitability match the ascending part of the theta cycle when firing probabilities of pyramidal cells are overall low due to the theta-rhythmic increase of inhibition. Accordingly, the recovery of intrinsic excitability after a burst coincides with times of increasing excitability during the theta cycle. The temporal match of an increased intrinsic and theta-oscillation related excitability can therefore facilitate the firing of single spikes during spatial navigation (Fig. 3c). Conversely, the timing of bursts which were no longer phase-locked during spatial navigation due to Kcnq3 ablation (Fig. 2) disrupted the temporal match of intrinsic and theta rhythm-driven excitability. Specifically, when bursts fired more often during descending phases in Kcnq3-/- mice (Fig. 2d, Supplementary Fig. 3b), the firing of single spikes fell on ascending theta phases associated with high inhibition. Hence, the firing of single spikes was reduced in Kcnq3−/− mice (Fig. 3d) and this resulted in impaired spatial representations.
Medial septal inputs entrain complex spike bursts
Which neural pathways contribute to the cholinergic modulation of bursts during theta oscillations? M-type channels are inhibited by acetylcholine released from cholinergic afferents from the MS48. On the other hand, burst firing of pyramidal cells can be effectively controlled by perisomatic inhibition. During theta oscillations, this inhibition is rhythmically modulated by inputs from parvalbumin-positive (GABA+) MS neurons52. To gain insights into the role of these hippocampal afferents in the theta entrainment of bursts, we recorded the discharge of putative CA1 pyramidal cells while optogenetically stimulating different inputs from MS in Kcnq3+/+ mice. To specifically manipulate inhibitory projections from MS to the hippocampus, we introduced ChR2 in parvalbumin-positive (GABA+) MS cells using a Cre-dependent virus in PV-Cre mice (Fig. 4a, b). During baseline recordings the probability of complex spike bursts changed according to the phase of theta oscillations (Fig. 4c). Optogenetic stimulation of GABA+ projections phase-locked hippocampal theta oscillations (Fig. 4b and ref. 53), in accord with results obtained with somatic stimulation of PV+ MS cells48. Surprisingly, the optogenetic stimulation did not entrain complex spike bursts of pyramidal cells (Fig. 4c). Hence, theta-rhythmic activity of inhibitory MS projections may not be sufficient for reliable burst entrainment.
To reveal effects of a coordinated theta-rhythmic activity of GABAergic (GABA+) and non-GABAergic (GABA-) neurons projecting to the hippocampus, we targeted in wild-type mice ChR2 to all MS cell types under the control of an ubiquitously active synthetic CAG promotor (Supplementary Fig. 6a), or to all types of MS neurons under the control of the neuron-specific human synapsin promotor (Fig. 4d). Similar to the GABA+ stimulation, simultaneous optogenetic stimulation of both GABA+ and GABA- MS inputs to the hippocampus at theta frequencies entrained theta oscillations (Fig. 4e, f) and reduced running speed and its variability depending on the entrainment fidelity (Supplementary Fig. 6b,c). This observation agrees with effects of the stimulation of GABA+ - projections reported earlier53. However, in contrast to the selective stimulation of GABA+ inputs, theta phase-locking of both GABA- and GABA+ MS projections entrained burst firing of pyramidal cells similar to spontaneous theta oscillations (Fig. 4g,h, Supplementary Fig. 6d).
MS features two interconnected GABA- neuronal populations projecting to the hippocampus: cholinergic (ChAT+) and glutamatergic cells54. Cholinergic cells prominently synapse on CA1 area pyramidal cells55 and may therefore directly influence burst discharge. To investigate firing of ChAT+ cells in behaving mice we expressed ChR2 in the MS of ChAT-Cre mice using a Cre-dependent virus (Fig. 4i). MS units were recorded during spatial navigation and optogenetically identified as putative ChAT+ or ChAT- cells based on their brief responses to series of light pulses applied at the end of each recording session (Fig. 4j, Supplementary Fig. 6e). Spike waveforms of photoresponsive cells did not change during optogenetic stimulation (Fig. 4j). During movement, a fraction of MS cells displayed a clear modulation according to the phase of locally recorded LFP theta oscillation (Fig. 4k). The theta-rhythmic activity was observed in ~50% of both putative ChAT- and ChAT+ cells (Fig. 4l).
To study the impact of cholinergic inputs from ChAT+ cells on theta oscillations and complex spike bursts we recorded neuronal discharge in the CA1 area during theta-rhythmic ChR2-stimulation of cholinergic projections during navigation (Fig. 4m). Somatic optogenetic stimulation of MS ChAT+ cells evokes theta oscillations48,54 via intra-MS relays48. Accordingly the stimulation of MS-hippocampal afferents, which did not engage intra-MS projections of ChAT+ cells, failed to substantially affect the power of theta oscillations and did not entrain theta oscillations in a phase-synchronized fashion (Fig. 4n,o). ChAT+ inputs, temporally uncoupled by the stimulation from activity of GABA+-afferents, did not affect the probability of burst firing in pyramidal cells at time scales relevant for theta oscillations (Fig. 4p). During 30 ms light pulses none of the recorded pyramidal cells fired a number of burst spikes different from those observed during baseline epochs of the same duration (0.051 < = p < = 0.983, permutation test). Within 30 ms after the light pulse, two out of 18 recorded cells fired significantly more spikes within bursts (p = 0.018, 0.044; for non-responding 16 cells 0.082 = < p < = 0.881, permutation test). Taken together these results suggest that the co-activation of MS GABA+ and ChAT+ inputs to the CA1 area at the time scale of the theta oscillation is necessary and sufficient for theta-rhythmic entrainment of complex spike bursts during spatial navigation.
Discussion
In this study, we have manipulated complex spike bursts by eliminating the Kcnq3 K+ channel subunit and by the stimulation of main inputs from MS. We found that proper timing of complex spike bursts during theta oscillations is required for intact spatial coding by single action potentials. Kcnq3-containing channels are essential for theta entrainment of bursts during spatial navigation, but not during alert immobility. This entrainment is controlled by upstream theta-rhythmic coordination of GABAergic and cholinergic inputs from the MS to the hippocampus.
Theta-periodic burst firing by pyramidal cell populations44 is an intriguing yet poorly understood aspect of hippocampal information processing. Theta-burst stimulation induces synaptic plasticity, suggesting that it may be relevant for the information processing involved in learning and memory. The readout of this activity can be further supported by network oscillations in downstream regions. Burst firing is sufficient for learning of hippocampus-dependent tasks3. Participation of neurons in temporally organized collective activity16 depends on subthreshold resonance in individual cells, on firing rate38, and on the modulation of membrane conductances.
Actions of acetylcholine on pyramidal cells include the inhibition of Kcnq3-containing M-channels, of slow AHP and of “leak” potassium currents19,38,56. Muscarinic antagonists abolish burst firing in hippocampal pyramidal cells ex vivo and in vivo20,57. Conversely, reduction of M-currents in mice expressing a dominant negative mutant of Kcnq222 results in a higher number of spikes during bursts in neonatal mice58. Postsynaptic M1 receptors also enhance Ih and Ca2+-dependent cation currents while presynaptic M4 receptors modulate Kir3 K+-channels and voltage-dependent Ca2+-channels38. The latter effects of acetylcholine limit the spread of excitation in the CA3 region59. This increases the precision of spatial coding and facilitates pattern-completion during memory retrieval (reviewed in ref. 38). High levels of acetylcholine selectively inhibit, via presynaptic M4 receptors on CA3 pyramidal cells, excitatory recurrent connections in CA3 and CA3 afferents to the CA1 area60–62. On the other hand, entorhinal inputs can be facilitated by acetylcholine via nicotinic receptors63. Modulatory actions of acetylcholine enable long-term potentiation of recently modified synapses and facilitate encoding64,65. The lack of theta-rhythmic offsets between bursts in pyramidal cells with impaired cholinergic modulation (Fig. 2b,c) reported here for Kcnq3−/− mice mimics a more correlated bursting in the absence of cholinergic stimulation during slow wave sleep44. Low levels of acetylcholine during slow wave sleep were suggested to result in the spread of excitatory activity and hence in the facilitation of auto-associative activity involved in retrieval66. While bursts are probably not directly caused by the cholinergic stimulation, but rather driven by entorhinal and CA3 inputs67,68, the present results implicate cholinergic modulation of pyramidal cells in a proper timing of bursts within theta oscillation cycles. This adjusts population responses to afferent inputs by setting timing of bursts across pyramidal cells and aligns the activity dynamics of individual cells with the period of the theta rhythm enabling spatial coding by single spikes.
While M/Kcnq-currents contribute to mAHP24,69, according to previous reports70 and the present results, their amplitudes are not affected by the lack of Kcnq3 in CA1 pyramidal cells. mAHP is therefore unlikely to mediate alterations of intrinsic bursts’ features in Kcnq3−/− mice. The reduced spike frequency accommodation and the inverse coupling of burst frequency with spiking output in the mutant were not caused by changes in resting membrane potential or input resistance which, agreeing with a previous report26, were unchanged in the mutant. On the other hand, the observed changes in typical spiking dynamics during complex spike bursts might result from a functional interplay of Kcnq3-containing channels with other channels, e.g., large and small conductance calcium-activated K+ channels32,71. Further investigations will be required to reveal the role of such interactions in the organization of discharge during complex spike bursts.
Cholinergic MS cells are part of a distributed theta rhythm-generating network. Similar to hippocampal interneurons, they are innervated by theta-rhythmic MS GABAergic cells55. Rhythmic activity of cholinergic projections from MS to hippocampus played a central role in earlier models of the generation of theta rhythm, that postulated an important physiological role of cholinergic innervation of pyramidal cells in this process45. However, later studies showed that GABAergic rather than cholinergic projections are involved in theta generation53,72. The temporal pattern of cholinergic activity in behaving animals could not be reliably investigated before cholinergic cells in the basal forebrain could be identified with optogenetic methods73. Depending on behavior, acetylcholine concentration changes dynamically at the seconds-time scale in the cortex (reviewed in ref. 73). However, these measurements currently do not permit an analysis at shorter time scales that are relevant for theta oscillations. In a recent study, optogenetic stimulation of cholinergic MS cells boosted atropine-sensitive theta oscillations indirectly via local MS connectivity. It also directly increased the precision of pyramidal cell discharge via the cholinergic MS – hippocampal pathway48. Here we focused on cholinergic modulation of burst discharge in behaving mice. The loss of theta entrainment of bursts in Kcnq3−/− mice was not due to a higher overall excitability or an increased probability of bursts in the mutant, which were rather reduced or unaffected, respectively. The observed impaired rhythmic responses of pyramidal cells might be caused, for instance, by a failure to effectively follow theta-rhythmic GABAergic inputs from MS. This scenario would be consistent with the contribution of M-currents to the subthreshold theta-band resonance in pyramidal cells22,74. Yet we found that intact resonant properties of pyramidal cells, in wild type mice, may not be sufficient for the entrainment of bursts by GABAergic inputs alone. A more consistent explanation for the imprecise burst discharge of Kcnq3−/− cells is their reduced capability to receive relevant timing signals in the absence of Kcnq3-containing M-channels. The possibility that the cholinergic inputs contribute to these entraining signals is supported, firstly, by the entrainment of bursts via the phase locked stimulation of GABAergic and non-GABAergic (mostly cholinergic) MS afferents to CA1 (Fig. 4g), secondly, by the failure of GABAergic inputs in the absence of simultaneous stimulation of non-GABAergic inputs to entrain bursts (Fig. 4c) and, thirdly, by the theta-rhythmic firing of a subpopulation of the putative ChAT+ MS cells (Fig. 4l).
During theta oscillations, somatic membrane potentials in CA1 pyramidal cells oscillate out-of-phase with the LFP rhythm in stratum pyramidale and with the membrane potential in apical dendrites75,76. A key mechanism of this rhythmicity is the periodic inhibition of basket interneurons by GABAergic inputs from the medial septum72. While the dendritic excitation that drives burst discharge68 is maximal during phases of somatic hyperpolarization75, bursts are typically fired during subsequent sufficiently depolarized phases which are closer to troughs. Due to their voltage dependence, M-currents are probably most active at depolarized theta phases (possibly in anti-phase with Ih-currents), during which bursts are mostly emitted. The present results suggest that the cholinergic inhibition of the M-current can therefore be essential for the high excitability and bursting of pyramidal cells at these phases. During these time windows, the depolarization of pyramidal cells is also mediated by the perisomatic disinhibition, timing of which is determined by GABA-ergic MS inputs.
Spatial navigation and alert immobility are the principal awake states associated with theta oscillations16,45,77,78. The release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus is higher during active than during quiet wakefulness79. During alert immobility, cholinergic inputs account for the sensitivity of theta oscillations to atropine, which, in turn, at least in part depends on hippocampal parvalbumin (PV)—positive interneurons13. The unchanged LFP theta oscillations after ablation of Kcnq3 preferentially in pyramidal cells indicate that the cholinergic input to interneurons, which are spared by the mutation, can be essential for the theta rhythm during awake states. Apart from PV-positive basket cells, another possible target for acetylcholine are oriens-lacunosum/moleculare (O-LM) cells. Their interspike intervals are known to be regulated by Kcnq channels23. Overall, these results extend our view of how Kcnq3-containing M-type K+-channels regulate excitability in the hippocampus during presumably high levels of cholinergic activity. It is distinct from mechanisms involved in the generation of network oscillations, and potentially complements actions of acetylcholine during memory formation38,79. Elucidating a differential impact of clinically relevant Kcnq3-gene variants on cortical excitability and representations will likely have to rely on novel knock-in mouse models. Our work reports an important action of Kcnq3-containing M-channels in pyramidal cells. Synergistically with MS GABA-ergic inputs, these channels coordinate complex spike bursts during theta oscillations, a coordination that is required for spatial coding by single spikes. The disruption of this mechanism may be one of the functional deficits that underlie effects of cholinergic dysfunction on cognition across neuropsychiatric disorders, including, but not limited to, those associated with KCNQ3 dysfunction.
Methods
Animals
Twenty-seven male mice, older than 12 weeks, were used in chronic experiments. Mice were housed under standard conditions in the animal facility and kept on a 12 h light/dark cycle, with access to food and water ad libitum. Mouse housing and experiments were in accordance with national and international guidelines and were approved by the local health authority (Landesamt für Gesundheit und Soziales, Berlin and Regierung von Unterfranken).
Kcnq3 constitutive knock-out (Kcnq3−/−)39 and Kcnq3 conditional knock-out mice were generated using the Cre/loxP system. The targeting vector to generate Kcnq3lox/lox mice was derived by cloning C57BI/6 J Kcnq3 genomic DNA containing exon 2–6 from a BAC-clone into the pKO901 vector containing a diphtheria toxin A cassette (DTA) for negative selection (Lexicon Genetics). A neomycin resistance (NEOR) cassette flanked by FRT/loxP sites was inserted into intron 4. An additional loxP site was introduced into intron 3 for recombinant deletion of exon 4 by Cre enzyme activity. The final targeting vector containing a Kcnq3 DNA fragment of 4.4 kb (short arm) and 6.1 kb (long arm), a NEOR cassette flanked by FRT sites and exon 4 flanked by two loxP sites is shown in Supplementary Fig. 1. The targeting vector, which was verified by sequencing, was electroporated into R1 (129/SvJ) embryonic stem cells (ESCs). ESC clones were selected by neomycin resistance and screened for correct genomic recombination by Southern blot analysis. Selected ESCs were injected into C57BI/6 J blastocysts that were implanted into foster mothers (carried out by the Transgenic Core Facility of MDC, Berlin). Chimeric offspring was crossed with FLPe recombinase expressing mice80 to excise the NEOR cassette. Successful recombination in F1 animals was assessed by Southern blot and PCR. Mice carrying the floxed allele (Kcnq3+/lox) were crossed with Cre-recombinase expressing mice81 to excise exon 4 from the Kcnq3 locus resulting in a frame-shift and early stop codon insertion (Kcnq3+/−). Heterozygous mice were crossed to obtain homozygous Kcnq3lox/lox and Kcnq3−/− mice, respectively. Kcnq3lox/lox mice were crossed with the Emx1-ires-cre-recombinase mice82, also in a C57BL/6 background (The Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, Maine, USA), to obtain cerebral cortex-specific deletion of Kcnq3 channels. Kcnq3lox/lox; Emx1cre/+ and Kcnq3lox/lox; Emx1cre/cre were considered conditional knock-out mice. The knock-out of Kcnq3 was confirmed by Southern blot, reverse transcriptase-PCR (RT-PCR, see list of primers in Supplementary Table 1) and Western blot analysis.
For optogenetic stimulation experiments PV-Cre knock-in mice83 (The Jackson Laboratory), ChAT-Cre knock-in mice84 (The Jackson Laboratory) and C57BL/6 male mice, 10–25 weeks old, were used. A part of the optogenetic stimulation dataset (from present PV-Cre mice) and of spontaneous neuronal activity recordings (in a wild-type mouse) have been used in previous reports27,53.
Whole cell electrophysiology
Transverse brain (current clamp experiments) or coronal (voltage clamp experiments) slices were taken from 8 Kcnq3 null mice85 and 7 wild-type controls ages P15-P20 in an ice-cold solution consisting of the following: 25 mm NaHCO3, 200 mm sucrose, 10 mm glucose, 2.5 mm KCl, 1.3 mm NaH2PO4, 0.5 mm CaCl2, and 7 mm MgCl2. The cerebellum, prefrontal lobe, and temporal lobe were removed and 300 μm hippocampal slices, were cut using a Leica vibratome (Leica VT1200S). Slices were then transferred to a holding chamber containing artificial CSF (ACSF) consisting of the following: 125 mm NaCl, 26 mmNaHCO3, 2.5 mm KCl, 1 mm NaH2PO4, 1.3 mm MgCl2, 2.5 mm CaCl2, and 12 mm glucose. Slices were first equilibrated at 35 °C for 30 min and maintained at room temperature (~22 °C) for ≥1 h before any electrophysiological recordings. The cutting and holding solutions (ACSF) were saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. The experiments were performed at 30–32 °C, with the exception of M-current measurements, which were carried out at room temperature.
Conventional whole cell patch clamp recordings were carried out on pyramidal neurons using electrodes pulled from thin-walled borosilicate glass capillaries having resistances between 2.5 and 4.5 MΩ when filled with recording solution as described below (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL, USA). Pyramidal cells from the CA1 area of the hippocampus were visually identified with infrared differential interference contrast optics using a 40× water-immersion objective lens on an upright microscope (Olympus BX51, Olympus). The internal recording solution for whole-cell recording consisted of the following: 130 mM potassium methylsulfate, 10 mM KCl, 10 mM HEPES, 20 mM inositol, 4 mM NaCl, 4 mM Mg2ATP, and 0.4 mM Na4GTP (osmolarity, 300–305 mOsm). The pH was adjusted to 7.25–7.3 with KOH. CNQX (4 μM), APV (10 μM), and picrotoxin (100 μM) (Abcam) were present in experiments to block AMPA-mediated, NMDA-mediated, and GABA-mediated synaptic transmission. For M-current measurements 500 nM TTX, 1 mM Cs, 2 mM 4-AP, and 100 μM Cadmium we also added to the extracellular solution. All recordings were performed using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), low-pass-filtered at 2 kHz or 10 kHz (spectral power difference, n.s., Wilcoxon signed rank test), and sampled at 10 kHz. Data were analyzed offline using Clampfit 10.7 (Molecular Devices) and MS Excel 16.
Immunohistochemistry
Mice were anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine and perfused through the heart with 1% (w/v) paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. The brain was dissected, post-fixed for 1 h in 1% PFA at 4 °C and incubated overnight in 30% (w/v) sucrose at 4 °C and embedded in Tissue-Tek O.C.T. (Sakura). Cryosections were cut at 8 µm and blocked in 2% (w/v) BSA and 0.5% (v/v) Nonidet P-40 in PBS for 2 h. Antibodies were diluted in PBS containing 1% BSA and 0.25% Nonidet P-40 and incubated for 1 h. Nuclei were stained with DAPI, alternatively neurons were visualized with a fluorescent Nissl stain. All sections were imaged using a Zeiss LSM 510 confocal microscope. Image analysis was performed off-line with the ZEN 2009 light edition software (Zeiss) and Adobe Photoshop CS6. The following primary Kcnq3 antibody was used: rabbit anti-Kcnq3 (named Q3Crb128, 1:200). The secondary goat anti-rabbit Alexa 555 antibody (1:1000) was from Invitrogen.
Virus injections
Animals were anaesthetized with isoflurane and placed in a head frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, USA). Stereotactic virus injections were performed at a rate of 100 nl min−1 using a 34-gauge beveled metal needle or a glass micropipette connected via a tube to a microsyringe pump (PHD Ultra, Harvard Apparatus, Holliston, MA, USA)53. PV-Cre mice were injected in the MS (AP 0.98, L 0.0, V −5.0 and −4.5 mm) with a total of 1000 nl of Cre-dependent ChR2 (AAV2/1.CAGGS.flex.ChR2.tdTomato.WPRE.SV40, Penn Vector Core). ChAT-Cre mice were injected in the MS (same coordinates) with a total of 900 nl of Cre-dependent eNPAC2.0 (AAVdj-nEF-DIO-NpHR-TS-p2A-hChR2(H134R)-eYFP, a gift from Karl Deisseroth). C57BL/6 mice were injected in the MS (same coordinates) with a total of 700 nl of ChR2 (AAVdj-hSyn-NpHR-TS-p2A-hChR2(H134R)-eYFP, a gift from Karl Deisseroth, or AAV2/5.CAG.hChR2(H134R)-mCherry.WPRE.SV40, Penn Vector Core). After infusion, the injector was left at the injection site for 10 min, then slowly withdrawn and the incision was sutured.
Implantations of optic fiber and electrodes
Arrays of single tungsten wires (40 μm, angular cut, California Fine Wire Company, Grover Beach, CA, USA), linear silicon probes (CM32, NeuroNexus Technologies, Ann Arbor, MI, USA), octrode probes (B32, NeuroNexus Technologies) mounted on a custom-made microdrive and independently movable 8 tetrodes (fabricated from 12 μm tungsten wire, California Fine Wire Company, Grover Beach, CA, USA) loaded on a microdrive (Minidrive-8, BioSignal Group, New York, USA) were stereotactically implanted under isoflurane anesthesia either in the str. oriens, pyramidale and radiatum of the hippocampal CA1 area (AP -1.94, L 1.4, V 1.4 mm, wire arrays, CM32 probes), or in the str. oriens with subsequent positioning in the pyramidal layer (B32 probes, tetrodes), using LFP and unitary activity as a reference to record units across the pyramidal layer. A silicon probe (B32) was implanted in the MS at AP 0.98, L + 0.6, V 3.4, 8° and subsequently advanced. Reference and ground electrodes were miniature stainless-steel screws in the skull above the cerebellum. The implants were secured on the skull with dental acrylic.
Optic fibers were fabricated from 100 μm diameter fiber (0.22 numerical aperture, Thorlabs, Bergkirchen, Germany) and zirconia ferrules (Precision Fiber Products, Chula Vista, CA, USA). Optic fibers were implanted above the hippocampal pyramidal layer (AP -1.94, L 1.4, V 1.4 mm) for optogenetic entrainment of MS to hippocampus projections or in the MS (AP 0.98, L -0.6, V 3.4, 8°) for optogenetic identification of ChAT cells.
Data acquisition and pre-processing
Electrodes were connected to operational amplifiers (Neuralynx, Bozeman, MT, USA or Noted B.T., Pecs, Hungary) to eliminate cable movement artifacts. Electrophysiological signals were differentially amplified, band-pass filtered (1 Hz–10 kHz, Digital Lynx, Neuralynx) and acquired continuously at 32 kHz. Recordings were performed as animals explored a gray wooden rectangular arena (50×30×18 cm), a circular track (diameter 60 cm) or rested in home cage. A light emitting diode was attached to the headset to track the animal’s position at 25 frames/s.
Optogenetic stimulation
Implanted optic fibers were connected to a 473-nm diode-pumped solid-state laser (Laserglow Technologies, Toronto, ON, Canada) via a fiberoptic patch cord (Thorlabs) and a zirconia sleeve (Precision Fiber Products). The laser was controlled by a stimulus generator (Multichannel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany). For the stimulation of the MS to hippocampus pathway 30 ms pulses of blue light (473 nm) were applied in the hippocampus at frequencies of 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, or 12 Hz. For optogenetic stimulation of MS ChAT cells 5 ms pulses of blue light were applied in the MS at frequencies of 2 or 5 Hz. Light power at the tip of the patch cord during light on-phase of pulses patterned according to the above protocols was 5–10 mW (measured with PM100D power meter, Thorlabs).
Histology
After completion of the experiments, mice were deeply anaesthetized and electrolytic lesions at selected recording sites were performed. Then the animals were perfused intracardially with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS and decapitated. Brains were fixed overnight in 4% paraformaldehyde, equilibrated in 1% PBS for an additional night, sectioned in 40 µm slices using an oscillating tissue slicer (EMS 4500, Electron Microscopy Science, Hatfield, PA, USA) and stained with cresyl violet to confirm recording sites.
Data analysis
Electrophysiological data were preprocessed using NeuroSuite (https://sourceforge.net/projects/neurosuite) and analyzed using custom-written MATLAB algorithms (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). LFP was obtained off-line by down-sampling of the wide-band signal to 1250 Hz using Neurophysiological Data Manager. Action potentials were detected in a high-pass filtered signal. Spike waveforms were extracted and represented by the first three principal components and by amplitudes of action potentials. Spike sorting was performed automatically using KlustaKwik followed by manual clusters adjustment using Klusters86. Putative pyramidal cells and interneurons were identified based on their auto-correlograms and firing rate (<3 Hz for pyramidal cells, >7 Hz for putative basket cells). Single pyramidal cells with a clear refractory period (<3 ms) as well as single and multiunit interneurons were used in further analysis. Isolation distance was computed for sorted units (putative pyramidal cells, Kcnq3+/+: 48.9 ± 1.2, n = 309; Kcnq3−/−: 67.2 ± 2.1, n = 413). Bursts were defined similar to earlier reports51 as a series of spikes separated by intervals of no more than 15 ms. Time stamp of the first spike in a burst was considered as a time stamp of the burst.
Firing maps of pyramidal cells were computed by dividing the number of spikes in a given spatial pixel (2×2 cm) by the time spent in this pixel. Periods of immobility were excluded from the analysis. Peak firing rate was defined as the maximum firing rate over all pixels in the environment. Place fields were detected as spatially continuous areas where the firing rate exceeded 1 Hz. Sparsity, a measure of firing field compactness was computed as in13.
Theta oscillations were detected automatically in the recordings during running based on theta/delta power ratio. Theta oscillation phase was extracted by the Hilbert transform of 5–10 Hz filtered signal53. Phase histograms of individual spike trains were computed and normalized, first, by the deviation (if any) of the underlying phase distribution from uniformity in respective phase bins and, second, by the total number of events. Circular uniformity (Rayleigh) test, mean phase and the resultant vector length were estimated for each histogram. Histograms of preferred discharge phases were computed by grouping mean phases of individual cells. Individual unit discharge probability histograms were convolved with the Gaussian kernel of size 2 SD. Burst probability – theta phase histograms during immobility and movement were computed for theta episodes during running (>3 cm/s) and immobility (<2 cm/s). Continuous Morlet wavelet transform (<40 Hz) was used to detect theta-oscillatory components in unitary discharge cross-correlograms. For spectral analysis of co-firing, the probability at zero time was computed as the most likely −200 to 200 ms average lag between bursts in pairs of spike trains. Power spectral density (PSD) of LFP was computed using the multitaper method (NW = 3, window size 1024).
In optogenetic stimulation experiments, for each stimulation epoch entrainment fidelity was computed within 10 sec intervals as the ratio of cumulative PSD around the optogenetic stimulation frequency (± 0.5 Hz) to the cumulative PSD in the theta band53. Stimulation epochs with mean theta-entrainment fidelity > 0.3 were defined as epochs with high entrainment. Bursts were detected during theta episodes in baseline (when stimulation was not applied) and during stimulation epochs with high entrainment.
For the identification of ChaT+-cells stimulation with 2 Hz and 5 Hz (epoch duration is 4 min each, pulse duration is 5 ms) was applied in the ChaT-Cre mouse transduced with a Cre-dependent opsin construct in the MS. Cells with a clear refractory period (<3 ms) were used in further analysis. To estimate responses of the cells to the stimulation, a cross-correlation with the times of light stimulation on the interval [−10 ms, 10 ms] around the light stimulus was computed for each cell (size of the bin is 1 ms). Maximum number of spikes within the bin before () and after () the light stimulus was identified for real data. Then the train of light stimuli was shifted to the baseline for a random time interval from the beginning of stimulation times. Maximum number of spikes within the bin before and after the light stimulus were identified for each shift and surrogate distributions and were obtained. For each cell p-values were defined as and . A cell was classified as a putative ChaT+- cell if at least for one stimulation epoch it was light-responsive after light stimulus () and non-light-responsive before the pulse ().
Statistics and reproducibility
The statistical significance of single comparisons was determined by the Mann-Whitney-U-test or with t-test. The normality of distributions was determined by the Lilliefors test. P-values <0.05 were considered to indicate significance, statistical tests were two-sided except for the one-sided Silverman’s bootstrap test of bimodality in Fig. 4l. The center line in box and whiskers plots indicates the median, the top and bottom edges indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively, and the whiskers extend to the maximum and minimum data points. In analyses that involved two or more factors, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied. For representative images and recordings, similar data were collected from at least 3 different animals. A detailed description of statistical comparisons is provided in Supplementary Information, Statistical Analysis.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Dr. R. Sahdev and Dr. M. Larkum for providing ChAT-Cre mice, Dr. K. Deisseroth for providing AAVdj-eNPAC and M. Zeller for the valuable input during preparation of the manuscript.
Source data
Author contributions
X.G., F.B., C.C., M.C.-C. M.A. and T.K. performed in vivo electrophysiological experiments, M.H. developed genetic mouse models, M.H. and S.S. generated genetically modified mice, S.S. performed IH, X.G., M.A., M.G., F.B., M.C.-C., M.-A.C. and A.P. analysed in vivo electrophysiological data; H.S. performed ex vivo electrophysiological experiments, A.V.T. designed and supervised ex vivo electrophysiological experiments, A.P. and T.K. supervised in vivo electrophysiological part, T.J.J. supervised molecular biological and cellular imaging experiments, A.P. and T.J.J. wrote the paper with input from all authors, all authors contributed to the study design, T.J.J. initiated the study.
Funding
This work was supported by a SAW grant of the Leibniz Gemeinschaft (TJJ) and Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; Exc 257 NeuroCure, TJJ, TK and AP; SPP1665, 1799/1-2, Heisenberg Programme, 1799/3-1, AP). Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
Source data underlying Figs. 1–4, Tables 1,2 and Supplementary Figs. 1–6 are provided with the paper (Source Data file). Spike trains recorded in Kcnq3-/- and Kcnq3+/+ mice were made available via Figshare (10.6084/m9.figshare.14785434). Further datasets generated during the current study are available from corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Code availability
All the codes used in the current study are available from corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review informationNature Communications thanks Michael Hasselmo, Lorna Role, and Vsevolod Telezhkin for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Xiaojie Gao, Franziska Bender.
Contributor Information
Thomas J. Jentsch, Email: jentsch@fmp-berlin.de
Alexey Ponomarenko, Email: alexey.ponomarenko@fau.de.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-021-24805-2.
References
- 1.Ranck JB. Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats: Part I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Exp. Neurol. 1973;41:462–531. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(73)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stevens CF, Wang Y. Facilitation and depression at single central synapses. Neuron. 1995;14:795–802. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90223-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Xu W, et al. Distinct neuronal coding schemes in memory revealed by selective erasure of fast synchronous synaptic transmission. Neuron. 2012;73:990–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tanaka KZ, et al. The hippocampal engram maps experience but not place. Science. 2018;361:392–397. doi: 10.1126/science.aat5397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.O’Keefe, J. & Dostrovsky, J. The hippocampus as a spatial map: preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res.34, 171–175 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 6.McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser M-B. Path integration and the neural basis of the’cognitive map’. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:663–678. doi: 10.1038/nrn1932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Solstad T, Boccara CN, Kropff E, Moser MB, Moser EI. Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science. 2008;322:1865–1868. doi: 10.1126/science.1166466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Treves A, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Science. 2004;305:1295–1298. doi: 10.1126/science.1100265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ito HT, Zhang S-J, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser M-B. A prefrontal–thalamo–hippocampal circuit for goal-directed spatial navigation. Nature. 2015;522:50–55. doi: 10.1038/nature14396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kaifosh P, Lovett-Barron M, Turi GF, Reardon TR, Losonczy A. Septo-hippocampal GABAergic signaling across multiple modalities in awake mice. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1182–1184. doi: 10.1038/nn.3482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Takeuchi T, et al. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature. 2016;537:357–362. doi: 10.1038/nature19325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Korotkova T, Fuchs EC, Ponomarenko A, von Engelhardt J, Monyer H. NMDA receptor ablation on parvalbumin-positive interneurons impairs hippocampal synchrony, spatial representations, and working memory. Neuron. 2010;68:557–569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schoenenberger P, O’Neill J, Csicsvari J. Activity-dependent plasticity of hippocampal place maps. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11824. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.McHugh TJ, Blum KI, Tsien JZ, Tonegawa S, Wilson MA. Impaired hippocampal representation of space in CA1-specific NMDAR1 knockout mice. Cell. 1996;87:1339–1349. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81828-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Buzsáki G, Moser EI. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:130–138. doi: 10.1038/nn.3304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shah MM, Migliore M, Valencia I, Cooper EC, Brown DA. Functional significance of axonal Kv7 channels in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:7869–7874. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0802805105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Madison D, Nicoll R. Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. J. Physiol. 1984;354:319–331. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wang H-S, et al. KCNQ2 and KCNQ3 potassium channel subunits: molecular correlates of the M-channel. Science. 1998;282:1890–1893. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5395.1890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Golomb D, Yue C, Yaari Y. Contribution of persistent Na+ current and M-type K+ current to somatic bursting in CA1 pyramidal cells: combined experimental and modeling study. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1912–1926. doi: 10.1152/jn.00205.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Battefeld A, Tran BT, Gavrilis J, Cooper EC, Kole MH. Heteromeric Kv7. 2/7.3 channels differentially regulate action potential initiation and conduction in neocortical myelinated axons. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:3719–3732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4206-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Peters HC, Hu H, Pongs O, Storm JF, Isbrandt D. Conditional transgenic suppression of M channels in mouse brain reveals functions in neuronal excitability, resonance and behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:51–60. doi: 10.1038/nn1375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lawrence JJ, et al. Somatodendritic Kv7/KCNQ/M channels control interspike interval in hippocampal interneurons. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12325–12338. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3521-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tzingounis AV, Nicoll RA. Contribution of KCNQ2 and KCNQ3 to the medium and slow afterhyperpolarization currents. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2008;105:19974–19979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810535105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shah MM, Migliore M, Brown DA. Differential effects of Kv7 (M‐) channels on synaptic integration in distinct subcellular compartments of rat hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Physiol. 2011;589:6029–6038. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.220913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Soh H, Pant R, LoTurco JJ, Tzingounis AV. Conditional deletions of epilepsy-associated KCNQ2 and KCNQ3 channels from cerebral cortex cause differential effects on neuronal excitability. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:5311–5321. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3919-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fidzinski P, et al. KCNQ5 K+ channels control hippocampal synaptic inhibition and fast network oscillations. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:1–13. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tzingounis AV, et al. The KCNQ5 potassium channel mediates a component of the afterhyperpolarization current in mouse hippocampus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2010;107:10232–10237. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1004644107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Biervert C, et al. A potassium channel mutation in neonatal human epilepsy. Science. 1998;279:403–406. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5349.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Charlier C, et al. A pore mutation in a novel KQT-like potassium channel gene in an idiopathic epilepsy family. Nat. Genet. 1998;18:53–55. doi: 10.1038/ng0198-53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Singh NA, et al. A novel potassium channel gene, KCNQ2, is mutated in an inherited epilepsy of newborns. Nat. Genet. 1998;18:25–29. doi: 10.1038/ng0198-25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hou, B., Varghese, N., Soh, H., Santaniello, S. & Tzingounis, A. V. Loss of KCNQ2 or KCNQ3 leads to multifocal time-varying activity in the neonatal forebrain ex vivo. eNeuro8, ENEURO.0024-21.2021 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Gilling M, et al. Dysfunction of the heteromeric KV7. 3/KV7. 5 potassium channel is associated with autism spectrum disorders. Front. Genet. 2013;4:54. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2013.00054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kaminsky Z, et al. DNA methylation and expression of KCNQ 3 in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2015;17:150–159. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Miceli F, et al. A novel KCNQ3 mutation in familial epilepsy with focal seizures and intellectual disability. Epilepsia. 2015;56:e15–e20. doi: 10.1111/epi.12887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lauritano A, et al. A novel homozygous KCNQ3 loss‐of‐function variant causes non‐syndromic intellectual disability and neonatal‐onset pharmacodependent epilepsy. Epilepsia Open. 2019;4:464–475. doi: 10.1002/epi4.12353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Herrero MJ, et al. Identification of amygdala-expressed genes associated with autism spectrum disorder. Mol. Autism. 2020;11:39. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00346-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hasselmo ME, Sarter M. Modes and models of forebrain cholinergic neuromodulation of cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:52–73. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Schutze S, Orozco IJ, Jentsch TJ. KCNQ Potassium Channels Modulate Sensitivity of Skin Down-hair (D-hair) Mechanoreceptors. J. Biol. Chem. 2016;291:5566–5575. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M115.681098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Choveau FS, Hernandez CC, Bierbower SM, Shapiro MS. Pore determinants of KCNQ3 K+ current expression. Biophysical J. 2012;102:2489–2498. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2012.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schroeder BC, Kubisch C, Stein V, Jentsch TJ. Moderate loss of function of cyclic-AMP-modulated KCNQ2/KCNQ3 K+ channels causes epilepsy. Nature. 1998;396:687–690. doi: 10.1038/25367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Li Y, Gamper N, Hilgemann DW, Shapiro MS. Regulation of Kv7 (KCNQ) K+ channel open probability by phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:9825–9835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2597-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Telezhkin V, Reilly JM, Thomas AM, Tinker A, Brown DA. Structural requirements of membrane phospholipids for M-type potassium channel activation and binding. J. Biol. Chem. 2012;287:10001–10012. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M111.322552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mizuseki K, Royer S, Diba K, Buzsaki G. Activity dynamics and behavioral correlates of CA3 and CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus. 2012;22:1659–1680. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sainsbury R, Montoya C. The relationship between type 2 theta and behavior. Physiol. Behav. 1984;33:621–626. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90381-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kramis R, Vanderwolf CH, Bland BH. Two types of hippocampal rhythmical slow activity in both the rabbit and the rat: relations to behavior and effects of atropine, diethyl ether, urethane, and pentobarbital. Exp. Neurol. 1975;49:58–85. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(75)90195-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lawson VH, Bland BH. The role of the septohippocampal pathway in the regulation of hippocampal field activity and behavior: analysis by the intraseptal microinfusion of carbachol, atropine, and procaine. Exp. Neurol. 1993;120:132–144. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1993.1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Dannenberg H, et al. Synergy of direct and indirect cholinergic septo-hippocampal pathways coordinates firing in hippocampal networks. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:8394–8410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4460-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mizuno T, Endo Y, Arita J, Kimura F. Acetylcholine release in the rat hippocampus as measured by the microdialysis method correlates with motor activity and exhibits a diurnal variation. Neuroscience. 1991;44:607–612. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90081-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Acquas E, Wilson C, Fibiger HC. Conditioned and unconditioned stimuli increase frontal cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release: effects of novelty, habituation, and fear. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:3089–3096. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03089.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Harris KD, Hirase H, Leinekugel X, Henze DA, Buzsáki G. Temporal interaction between single spikes and complex spike bursts in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron. 2001;32:141–149. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00447-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tóth K, Freund T, Miles R. Disinhibition of rat hippocampal pyramidal cells by GABAergic afferents from the septum. J. Physiol. 1997;500:463–474. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bender F, et al. Theta oscillations regulate the speed of locomotion via a hippocampus to lateral septum pathway. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:1–11. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Robinson J, et al. Optogenetic activation of septal glutamatergic neurons drive hippocampal theta rhythms. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:3016–3023. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2141-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Frotscher M, Leranth C. Cholinergic innervation of the rat hippocampus as revealed by choline acetyltransferase immunocytochemistry: a combined light and electron microscopic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1985;239:237–246. doi: 10.1002/cne.902390210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brown D, Adams P. Muscarinic suppression of a novel voltage-sensitive K+ current in a vertebrate neurone. Nature. 1980;283:673–676. doi: 10.1038/283673a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Stewart M, Luo Y, Fox SE. Effects of atropine on hippocampal theta cells and complex-spike cells. Brain Res. 1992;591:122–128. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90986-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Marguet SL, et al. Treatment during a vulnerable developmental period rescues a genetic epilepsy. Nat. Med. 2015;21:1436. doi: 10.1038/nm.3987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hasselmo ME, Bower JM. Cholinergic suppression specific to intrinsic not afferent fiber synapses in rat piriform (olfactory) cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1222–1229. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.5.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Linster C, Maloney M, Patil M, Hasselmo ME. Enhanced cholinergic suppression of previously strengthened synapses enables the formation of self-organized representations in olfactory cortex. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 2003;80:302–314. doi: 10.1016/S1074-7427(03)00078-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hasselmo ME, Cekic M. Suppression of synaptic transmission may allow combination of associative feedback and self-organizing feedforward connections in the neocortex. Behav. Brain Res. 1996;79:153–161. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00010-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gil Z, Connors BW, Amitai Y. Differential regulation of neocortical synapses by neuromodulators and activity. Neuron. 1997;19:679–686. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80380-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Giocomo LM, Hasselmo ME. Nicotinic modulation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission in region CA3 of the hippocampus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;22:1349–1356. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04316.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Huerta PT, Lisman JE. Heightened synaptic plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neurons during a cholinergically induced rhythmic state. Nature. 1993;364:723–725. doi: 10.1038/364723a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Hasselmo ME, Barkai E. Cholinergic modulation of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in the piriform cortex and associative memory function in a network biophysical simulation. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:6592–6604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06592.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hasselmo ME. The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006;16:710–715. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Pissadaki EK, Sidiropoulou K, Reczko M, Poirazi P. Encoding of spatio-temporal input characteristics by a CA1 pyramidal neuron model. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6:e1001038. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bittner KC, et al. Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1133–1142. doi: 10.1038/nn.4062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Storm JF. An after‐hyperpolarization of medium duration in rat hippocampal pyramidal cells. J. Physiol. 1989;409:171–190. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Cho KK, et al. Gamma rhythms link prefrontal interneuron dysfunction with cognitive inflexibility in Dlx5/6(+/-) mice. Neuron. 2015;85:1332–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Buchanan KA, Petrovic MM, Chamberlain SE, Marrion NV, Mellor JR. Facilitation of long-term potentiation by muscarinic M1 receptors is mediated by inhibition of SK channels. Neuron. 2010;68:948–963. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Freund TF, Antal M. GABA-containing neurons in the septum control inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus. Nature. 1988;336:170–173. doi: 10.1038/336170a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sarter M, Parikh V, Howe WM. Phasic acetylcholine release and the volume transmission hypothesis: time to move on. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:383–390. doi: 10.1038/nrn2635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hu H, Vervaeke K, Storm JF. Two forms of electrical resonance at theta frequencies, generated by M-current, h-current and persistent Na+ current in rat hippocampal pyramidal cells. J. Physiol. 2002;545:783–805. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.029249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Kamondi A, Acsady L, Wang XJ, Buzsaki G. Theta oscillations in somata and dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal cells in vivo: activity-dependent phase-precession of action potentials. Hippocampus. 1998;8:244–261. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<244::AID-HIPO7>3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW. Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature. 2009;461:941–946. doi: 10.1038/nature08499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hasselmo ME, Hay J, Ilyn M, Gorchetchnikov A. Neuromodulation, theta rhythm and rat spatial navigation. Neural Netw. 2002;15:689–707. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00057-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Seidenbecher T, Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape H-C. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science. 2003;301:846–850. doi: 10.1126/science.1085818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ruivo LMT-G, et al. Coordinated acetylcholine release in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus is associated with arousal and reward on distinct timescales. Cell Rep. 2017;18:905–917. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.12.085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Farley FW, Soriano P, Steffen LS, Dymecki SM. Widespread recombinase expression using FLPeR (flipper) mice. genesis. 2000;28:106–110. doi: 10.1002/1526-968X(200011/12)28:3/4<106::AID-GENE30>3.0.CO;2-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Schwenk F, Baron U, Rajewsky K. A cre-transgenic mouse strain for the ubiquitous deletion of loxP-flanked gene segments including deletion in germ cells. Nucleic Acids Res. 1995;23:5080–5081. doi: 10.1093/nar/23.24.5080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gorski JA, Zeiler SR, Tamowski S, Jones KR. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is required for the maintenance of cortical dendrites. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:6856–6865. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-17-06856.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Hippenmeyer S, et al. A developmental switch in the response of DRG neurons to ETS transcription factor signaling. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Rossi J, et al. Melanocortin-4 receptors expressed by cholinergic neurons regulate energy balance and glucose homeostasis. Cell Metab. 2011;13:195–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2011.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Kim KS, Duignan KM, Hawryluk JM, Soh H, Tzingounis AV. The voltage activation of cortical KCNQ channels depends on global PIP2 levels. Biophysical J. 2016;110:1089–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2016.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hazan L, Zugaro M, Buzsaki G. Klusters, NeuroScope, NDManager: a free software suite for neurophysiological data processing and visualization. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2006;155:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Source data underlying Figs. 1–4, Tables 1,2 and Supplementary Figs. 1–6 are provided with the paper (Source Data file). Spike trains recorded in Kcnq3-/- and Kcnq3+/+ mice were made available via Figshare (10.6084/m9.figshare.14785434). Further datasets generated during the current study are available from corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
All the codes used in the current study are available from corresponding authors upon reasonable request.