Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Aug 11;16(8):e0255588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255588

Muslim undergraduate biology students’ evolution acceptance in the United States

M Elizabeth Barnes 1,*, Julie A Roberts 2, Samantha A Maas 2, Sara E Brownell 2
Editor: Jamie L Jensen3
PMCID: PMC8357111  PMID: 34379670

Abstract

Evolution is a prominent component of biology education and remains controversial among college biology students in the United States who are mostly Christian, but science education researchers have not explored the attitudes of Muslim biology students in the United States. To explore perceptions of evolution among Muslim students in the United States, we surveyed 7,909 college students in 52 biology classes in 13 states about their acceptance of evolution, interest in evolution, and understanding of evolution. Muslim students in our sample, on average, did not agree with items that measured acceptance of macroevolution and human evolution. Further, on average, Muslim students agreed, but did not strongly agree with items measuring microevolution acceptance. Controlling for gender, major, race/ethnicity, and international status, we found that the evolution acceptance and interest levels of Muslim students were slightly higher than Protestant students and students who are members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints. However, Muslim student evolution acceptance levels were significantly lower than Catholic, Jewish, Buddhist, and Hindu students as well as students who did not identify with a religion (agnostic and atheists). Muslim student understanding of evolution was similar to students from other affiliations, but was lower than agnostic and atheist students. We also examined which variables are associated with Muslim student acceptance of evolution and found that higher understanding of evolution and lower religiosity are positive predictors of evolution acceptance among Muslim students, which is similar to the broader population of biology students. These data are the first to document that Muslim students have lower acceptance of evolution compared to students from other affiliations in undergraduate biology classrooms in the United States.

Introduction

Evolution is a foundation of biology that should be taught at every level of biology education [13], yet it remains a controversial scientific theory among the public [4] and college biology students [58]. Religious identity and beliefs are a major source of rejecting evolution [911], but the evolution acceptance literature in the context of the United States is dominated by the study of Christian students because the vast majority of religious students in biology classes are Christian [5]. However, we currently know very little about evolution acceptance, evolution understanding, and interest in evolution among biology students from other religious affiliations, including Muslim students, in the United States. Further, we do not know if the same variables associated with evolution acceptance among the broader population of students (for example, religiosity and understanding of evolution) are the same for students of other religious affiliations as they are for Christians. In this current study, we extend beyond focusing on Christian biology student evolution perceptions in the United States and examine evolution acceptance, evolution understanding, and interest in evolution among Hindu, Buddhist, and Muslim college biology students across the United States, as well as the variables associated with their evolution acceptance levels. Based on prior data collected outside of the United States, we would predict that Muslim students in the United States may be less receptive to evolution, so we focus the manuscript on this population of students.

Studies of acceptance of evolution around the globe suggest Muslim student acceptance of evolution is low

In Muslim majority countries, studies have demonstrated low rates of evolution acceptance among students and the public. In Turkey, researchers have found that college students training to be biology teachers scored low on the Measure of Acceptance of the Theory of Evolution (MATE) [12, 13]. Among Lebanese, Egyptian Sunni, and Shiite Muslims in high school, 23% of students agreed that “evolution is scientifically wrong” and 26% were undecided about the scientific validity of evolution [14]. Among Muslim medical students in Pakistan, 68% rejected evolution based on their religious beliefs, and these students also showed a low understanding of evolution [15]. Among 18 Pakistani high school teachers who were interviewed about their views on evolution, researchers reported that almost all of the teachers rejected human evolution due to a perceived conflict with their religious beliefs [16]. The conclusion that Muslim evolution acceptance is low has also been found in studies with large sample sizes across many nations. In 2015, researchers surveyed over 10,000 teachers across 30 countries about their perceptions of evolution and found that among all majority Muslim countries they surveyed (Algeria, Morocco, Senegal, Lebanon, and Tunisia), 70% or more of their teachers subscribed to special creationism as opposed to evolution [17]. In Indonesia, where the largest Muslim population in the world resides, Rachmatullah and colleagues found that the MATE scores of pre-service biology teachers were lower than those of pre-service teachers in non-Muslim dominant countries [18]. These studies collectively demonstrate that outside of the United States, levels of evolution acceptance for Muslim individuals are low and that a perceived conflict between religion and evolution is a factor.

Muslim student acceptance of evolution has been shown to be low compared to the evolution acceptance of those with other religious identities. When comparing Muslim and Christian biology students’ views on evolution at a Lebanese university, Muslim students were much less likely than Christian students to support evolution [19]. When comparing Greek and Turkish students’ evolution acceptance scores on the MATE, researchers found that Greek students who were mostly Christian scored approximately two standard deviations higher on acceptance of evolution than Turkish students who were mostly Muslim [20]. Comparisons of Muslim and Christian secondary teachers in Malaysia and South Africa revealed that Muslim teachers were much more likely to subscribe to special creationism than Christian teachers [21, 22]. In England, almost all Muslim 14–16-year-old students believed humans were created in their current form whereas only half of Christian students reported the same belief [23] and Muslim residents were least accepting of the evolution of plants, animals, and humans compared to Christians and non-religious individuals [24]. In the United States, the evolution acceptance of Christian students is low compared to non-religious students [4, 25], but no one has explored perceptions of evolution among Muslim students in the United States. This gap in the literature may make it challenging for evolution instructors to consider the needs of Muslim students at U.S. colleges and universities.

What is known about evolution acceptance among Muslim individuals in the United States?

Muslim students in the United States may have similarly low levels of evolution acceptance as individuals from Muslim majority countries because they are affiliated with the same religion. In Islam, the Quran depicts a special creationist origin of humans similar to that of the Christian Bible [26]. A literal interpretation of the Bible in Christianity has been identified as one of the major sources of rejection of evolution in the United States [27] so we might expect that Muslim individuals who hold a strict literal interpretation of their religious text, regardless of their country of residence, will also have low acceptance of evolution. However, there are some reasons that Muslim individuals might have higher evolution acceptance in the United States.

Sociological public polls show that American Muslims are less likely than Muslims in other countries to believe that the Quran should be read literally and more likely to believe that the Quran can be interpreted multiple ways [28, 29]. Thus, American Muslim students may be more likely to interpret depictions of human creation in the Quran as a symbolic story and thus be able to accommodate an acceptance of evolution. Further, Muslim individuals in the United States have an average of eight more years of formal education compared to Muslims globally [30]. Since higher education levels are associated with higher evolution acceptance [25], we may expect that Muslim students in the United States will be more accepting of evolution, particularly among college biology students who have likely had more formal education than many Muslim individuals in other countries. One study in Canada, which is more reflective of the United States population than majority Muslim countries, found that Muslim high school science teachers largely accepted evolution of organisms except for humans [16]. Among students in the United States, acceptance of human evolution has been shown to be a separate psychological construct from acceptance of microevolution and human evolution [31], so it may be the case that acceptance of evolution among Muslims in the United States depends on the context of evolution and whether it includes humans.

Is understanding of evolution and religiosity related to evolution acceptance levels of Muslim students?

The extent to which religiosity and understanding of evolution are related to acceptance of evolution has been shown to be variable across studies [10, 11, 25, 3236], across scales of evolution (microevolution, macroevolution, human evolution, and common ancestry of life) [25], and may be different across different populations of students [37]. Knowing what variables influence student acceptance of evolution has implications for the extent to which instructors may want to account for student religious beliefs when teaching evolution. For instance, if religiosity is related to Muslim student evolution acceptance to a greater extent than understanding of evolution, then that may indicate that in addition to traditional instruction aimed at increasing understanding of evolution, these students would benefit from opportunities to learn about the relationship between religion and evolution and where religion and evolution can be compatible versus where they are in conflict. The evolution education literature thus far has not explored whether the common variables associated with evolution acceptance of the broader population of American students, who are predominantly Christian, are also associated with evolution acceptance among Muslim students. If we do not explore Muslim students independently from other students, we might make assumptions about factors related to their evolution acceptance that are not true.

The current study and research questions

In this study, we aimed to explore levels of evolution acceptance, interest in evolution, and understanding of evolution among Muslim biology students in the United States. We document the comparisons between these variables among Muslim students and students from other religious affiliations, including Christians, Jewish individuals, Buddhists, Hindus, and individuals who are not religious. Finally, we explore the extent to which variables associated with evolution acceptance in the broader American population are also associated with acceptance of evolution among these students, including Muslims. Our specific research questions are:

  1. Controlling for major, gender, race/ethnicity, and international status, do the average levels of evolution understanding, interest in evolution, and evolution acceptance of Muslim students in the United States differ from students who are Protestant, Catholic, Latter-day Saints (LDS), Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, agnostic, or atheist?

  2. To what extent does understanding of evolution and religiosity contribute to students’ evolution acceptance levels for each religious affiliation?

Methods and analyses

We surveyed students in 52 college biology classes at 22 institutions across 13 U.S. states (Alabama, Arizona, California, Florida, Hawaii, Minnesota, North Carolina, New York, Oklahoma, South Carolina, Texas, Utah, Wisconsin) in fall 2018, spring 2020, and fall 2020. Students were recruited through their instructors, who agreed to forward the survey to students before any evolution instruction occurred in the class and offer a small amount of extra credit to students who took the survey. This study was approved by Arizona State University’s Institutional Review Board, protocol #8191. Students indicated their consent to participate by clicking a box on the online survey.

Survey measures

The survey was conducted as part of a larger study on the impact of evolution education on undergraduate biology students. For this study, we included the variables of religious denomination, religiosity, acceptance of evolution, interest in evolution, understanding of evolution, major, gender, race/ethnicity, and international status. All questions used in the analyses can be found in the S1 File.

Religious denomination

Students were asked to choose a religious affiliation with which they most closely identified and were then grouped into the following categories based on their responses: Muslim, Christian–Catholic, Christian–Protestant/nondenominational, Christian–The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (LDS), Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, agnostic, and atheist. Students who chose a religion that was not part of a large enough group for statistical analyses were not included in the results of this manuscript (for example, Sikh, Pagan, Satanist, spiritualist, Taoist, Christian–orthodox, etc.). Non-denominational Christians were grouped with Protestant Christians because these groups were closely aligned in our analyses. In all analyses, Muslim students are the reference group because they are the focus of this study.

Religiosity

We measured student religiosity using a scale previously validated with college students [38]. The measure consisted of four items with Likert response options that measure two important components of religiosity: the intrinsic strength of one’s religious identity and participation in religious activities. This measure is similar to other common measures used in both studies of religion [39, 40] and studies of evolution acceptance [10, 41]. Further, it was designed to be valid for students across many different religious denominations [38]. Items were aggregated and then divided by four to represent students’ average agreement on a Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree)– 5 (strongly agree) (α = .90).

Acceptance of evolution

Acceptance of evolution refers to the extent to which students personally think evolution is valid and can include acceptance of microevolution, acceptance of macroevolution, acceptance of human evolution, and acceptance of the common ancestry of life on Earth [5, 31]. To measure acceptance of microevolution, macroevolution, and human evolution, we used The Inventory of Student Evolution Acceptance (I-SEA) [31], which has been validated with college biology students [42]. Each scale consists of eight items and items from each scale were aggregated and then divided by eight to represent students’ average agreement on a Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree)– 5 (strongly agree) (α (micro) = .84; α (macro) = .85; α (human) = .91).

To determine whether students accepted the common ancestry of life, we used a previously published survey that asks students to choose from nine different views on the relationship between religion and evolution, some of which reflect an acceptance of the common ancestry of life and some of which indicate a belief that a God/god(s) created species separately from one another [5, 43]. Students were categorized as either accepting or not accepting the common ancestry of life and those who chose options that reflected a special creationist view were categorized as not accepting the common ancestry of life.

Interest in evolution

We developed four items to measure students’ interest in evolution because interest can be a strong indicator of motivation to learn a topic [44, 45] and no prior survey existed to measure this variable when we did this study. We measured students’ interest in (1) taking a course on evolution, (2) doing undergraduate research on evolution, (3) studying evolution as part of their career, and (4) becoming an evolutionary biologist. Students answered each question on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 10 (very much). We conducted cognitive interviews [46, 47] with 25 undergraduate biology students and revised the questions so that they were being interpreted correctly and so that the wording was not confusing for students. This measure was only included in fall and spring 2020 collections. (α = .88).

Understanding of evolution

Understanding of evolution is different from acceptance of evolution and refers to the extent a student has a good conceptual grasp of current evolutionary theory. A student can have a good understanding of evolution, and yet still choose to not accept evolution [48]. To measure students’ evolution understanding, we used two subscales on the previously published Evolutionary Attitudes and Literacy instrument (EALS) [49]. We only used the two subscales (13 items) from the instrument that measure “Evolutionary Knowledge” (e.g., “In most populations, more offspring are born than can survive”) and “Evolutionary Misconceptions” (e.g., “Evolution is a linear progression from primitive to advanced species”) because these were the subscales related to understanding of evolution. Students were asked to decide whether each item was true or false based on their evolution understanding and were also given an option to indicate “I don’t know enough to answer” to avoid correct answers by guessing. We calculated student scores by determining the proportion of correct answers. The EALS has been used in other evolution education studies [10, 50], has shown evidence of reliability and validity among college students [49], and importantly, the items do not appear to conflate evolution acceptance with evolution understanding [25] (α = .58, which is acceptable for a test that measures content knowledge of a domain (see for example, [51], pg. 135–138)).

Demographics

To control for potential confounding variables in our analyses we included race/ethnicity, gender, major, and international status in our survey and our analyses. Students were asked to identify their gender as woman, man, or non-binary. In all analyses, man is the reference group. Students were asked to identify their race/ethnicity and were categorized as Asian, Black, Latinx, White, other race, or multiracial. White is the reference group in all analyses. Students were also asked to identify if they were a biology major or a major other than biology. Finally, to determine international status, we asked students if they were born in the United States.

Analyses

Only complete student responses were included in the analyses. Less than 5% of data were missing. All analyses were done in SPSS version 26. All data and syntax for analyses are included in the S1 File.

We provide tables of the means and standard deviations for outcome variables to illustrate the central tendencies of the raw data. We provide violin plots to illustrate the variability and distribution of the raw data.

To determine if levels of evolution understanding, evolution acceptance, and evolution interest differ between Muslim students and students from other religious denominations, we used multiple linear regressions and controlled for the potentially confounding variables of race/ethnicity, major, gender, and international status in our analyses. We report the standardized coefficients and p-values for comparisons made between Muslim students and students from other religious denominations. In the case of acceptance of common ancestry, which is a binary rather than continuous outcome variable, we used binary logistic regression and report the odds ratios (OR) and p-values for comparisons between Muslim students and students from other religious affiliations. To determine the extent to which evolution understanding and religiosity predict evolution acceptance among students, we selected only students of a particular religious affiliation for analysis and then ran three multiple linear regressions (human, macro, or micro) and one binary logistic regression (acceptance of common ancestry) with the various evolution acceptance measures as the dependent variables and religiosity and understanding of evolution as the predictor variables. To account for potentially confounding variables, we also controlled for gender, major, and international status. There was not enough variation in race/ethnicity to control for this variable in regressions with these analyses. Full regression tables with omnibus statistics, coefficients for all variables, and standard errors for all coefficients in all analyses can be found in the S1 File.

Results

Undergraduate biology instructors sent the survey to approximately 13,100 potential participants and a total of 7,909 college biology students completed the survey (response rate = ~ 60.4%). Of these students, 16.7% identified as Asian, 5.8% as Black, 16.1% as Latinx, 0.4% as Native Islander, 0.5% as Native American, 49.9% as White, 0.1% as another race/ethnicity, and 10.5% as multiracial. Women were 67.2% of the sample, 32.2% were men, and 0.6% were non-binary, which is similar to the broader population of undergraduate biology students [52]. Biology majors were 53.6% of the sample. Muslim students comprised 2.8% of the sample; Muslim populations in the United States is approximately 1.1% of the population so our sample is similar in percentage [53]. For a breakdown of the religious affiliations of students see Table 1 and for the demographics of Muslim students specifically, see Table 2.

Table 1. The religious affiliations of undergraduate biology students in this study.

Religious Affiliation Study Participants n = 7,909% (n)
Muslim 2.8 (219)
Christian–Protestanta 24.7 (1952)
Christian–LDSb 9.9 (780)
Christian—Catholic 23.7 (1877)
Jewish 1.9 (153)
Hindu 2.1 (165)
Buddhist 2.2 (173)
Agnostic 25.1 (1984)
Atheist 7.7 (606)

aIncludes Protestant and nondenominational Christians.

bThis group represents those affiliated with the Church of Jesus Christian of Latter-day Saints who prefer to be named as such as opposed to the term “Mormon.” We acknowledge this preference and use LDS as an acronym to shorten the name to fit in tables and figures.

Table 2. The demographics of college biology Muslim student participants in this study.

Student Demographic Muslim Study Participants n = 219% (n)
Major
 Biology 64.8 (142)
 Other Major 35.2 (77)
Gender
 Female 58.0 (127)
 Male 42.0 (92)
 Non-binary 0.0 (0)
Race/ethnicity
 Asian 57.5 (126)
 Black 14.2 (31)
 Latinx 0.9 (2)
 Native Islander 0.9 (2)
 Multiracial 8.7 (19)
 White 17.8 (39)
Place of Birth
 United States 69.4 (152)
 Other 30.6 (67)

Finding 1: Muslim student understanding of evolution is similar to students from other religious affiliations, but lower than atheist and agnostic students

Muslim student understanding of evolution was lower than agnostic (β = .089, p = .003) and atheist students (β = .108 p < .001). There were no significant differences between Muslim student understanding of evolution and that of students from any other religious affiliations (p > .11). See Table 3 for raw means and standard deviations of understanding levels of evolution broken down by religious affiliation. See Fig 1 for distribution of understanding of evolution levels broken down by religious affiliation.

Table 3. The mean and standard deviation of evolution understanding scores disaggregated by religious affiliation.

Affiliation Mean Standard Deviation
Muslim .67 .16
Christian–Protestant .69 .16
Christian–LDS .71 .16
Christian—Catholic .67 .16
Jewish .71 .14
Hindu .68 .17
Buddhist .69 .17
Agnostic .72 .16
Atheist .75 .17

Fig 1. Evolution understanding.

Fig 1

Violin plots of proportion of correct answers on a test of evolution understanding disaggregated by religious affiliation. The violin shapes are the densities of the data at each point on the y-axis. The solid black lines are the medians, and the top and bottom broken lines are the 75th and 25th percentiles of the data.

Finding 2: Muslim student interest in evolution is higher than Protestant students and students from the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints, but lower than that of students from other religious affiliations

Muslim student interest in evolution was higher than Protestant students (β = -.143, p < .001) and students who are members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (β = -.079, p < .001), but lower than Jewish students (β = .043, p = .003), Buddhist students (β = .078, p < .001), agnostic students (β = .153, p < .001) and atheist students (β = .123, p < .001). See Table 4 for mean and standard deviation of interest scores disaggregated by affiliation. See Fig 2 for distribution of interest in evolution levels broken down by religious affiliation.

Table 4. The mean and standard deviation of students’ aggregated interest in taking an elective course on evolution, interest in conducting research on evolution as an undergraduate, interest in a career involving research on evolution, and interest in becoming an evolutionary biologist.

Affiliation Mean Standard Deviation
Muslim 3.73 2.43
Christian—Protestant 2.69 2.26
Christian—LDS 2.58 2.28
Christian—Catholic 3.83 2.31
Jewish 4.07 2.25
Hindu 4.19 2.28
Buddhist 4.93 2.19
Agnostic 4.43 2.34
Atheist 4.74 2.56

Scores disaggregated by religious affiliation.

Fig 2. Interest in evolution.

Fig 2

Violin plots of students’ average interest in taking an elective course on evolution, doing undergraduate research on evolution, studying evolution as part of their career, and becoming an evolutionary biologist. The violin shapes are the densities of the data at each point on the y-axis. The solid black lines are the medians, and the top and bottom broken lines are the 75th and 25th percentiles of the data.

Finding 3: Muslim student evolution acceptance is higher than Protestant students and students from the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints, but lower than that of students from other religious affiliations

The common ancestry of life on Earth

Thirty-seven percent of Muslim students chose an option that indicated they accepted the common ancestry of life on Earth. Muslim students were significantly less likely to accept the common ancestry of life on Earth compared to Catholic students (OR = 1.96, p < .001), Jewish students (OR = 6.05, p < .001), Hindu students (OR = 6.26, p < .001), Buddhist students (OR = 14.56, p < .001), agnostic students (OR = 32.62, p < .001) and atheist students (OR = 514.63, p < .001). Muslim students were slightly more likely to accept the common ancestry of life compared to students who were members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (OR = .68, p = .026). There was no statistically significant difference between Muslim student and Protestant student acceptance of the common ancestry of life (p = .93). Proportions of students that accepted the common ancestry of life disaggregated by religious affiliation can be found in Table 5.

Table 5. Proportion of students that believe life on Earth shares a common ancestor disaggregated by religious affiliation.
Affiliation Accepts common ancestry of life
Muslim 36.5%
Christian—Protestant 33.3%
Christian—LDS 29.9%
Christian—Catholic 48.5%
Jewish 77.8%
Hindu 78.2%
Buddhist 89.0%
Agnostic 94.7%
Atheist 99.7%

Human evolution

Muslim students were less accepting of human evolution compared to Catholic (β = .190, p < .001), Jewish (β = .110, p < .001), Hindu (β = .107, p < .001), Buddhist (β = .126, p < .001), agnostic (β = .371, p < .001), and atheist (β = .301, p < .001) students. Muslim students were more accepting of human evolution than Protestant students (β = -.079, p = .004) and students who were members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (β = -.094, p < .001).

Macroevolution

Muslim students were less accepting of macroevolution than Catholic students (β = .123, p < .001), Jewish students (β = .075, p < .001), Hindu students (β = .081, p < .001), Buddhist students (β = .092, p < .001), agnostic students (β = .289, p < .001), and atheist students (β = .268, p < .001). Muslim students were more accepting of macroevolution than Protestant students (β = -.098, p = .001) and students who were members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (β = -.071, p = .001).

Microevolution

Muslim students were less accepting of microevolution than Catholic students (β = .094, p = .002), Jewish students (β = .043, p = .002), Hindu students (β = .051, p < .001), Buddhist students (β = .075, p < .001), agnostic students (β = .242, p < .001), and atheist students (β = .205, p < .001). Muslim student acceptance of microevolution was not statistically different from Protestant students (p = .889) or students who were members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints (p = .166).

Means and standard deviations of human evolution acceptance, macroevolution acceptance, and microevolution acceptance disaggregated by religious affiliation can be found in Table 6. Distributions of responses, medians, and 1st and 3rd quartile of responses can be found in Fig 3.

Table 6. The mean and standard deviation of human evolution acceptance, macroevolution acceptance, and microevolution acceptance scores disaggregated by religious affiliation.
Human Evolution Acceptance Macroevolution Acceptance Microevolution Acceptance
Affiliation Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Muslim 3.35 .81 3.60 .62 4.07 .60
Christian—Protestant 3.24 .89 3.48 .74 4.14 .60
Christian—LDS 3.16 .87 3.47 .69 4.20 .54
Christian—Catholic 3.74 .65 3.82 .53 4.24 .52
Jewish 4.07 .58 3.99 .50 4.34 .52
Hindu 3.94 .56 3.97 .51 4.25 .55
Buddhist 4.04 .55 4.01 .48 4.35 .55
Agnostic 4.09 .58 4.07 .53 4.44 .48
Atheist 4.33 .58 4.30 .52 4.55 .48
Fig 3. Acceptance of evolution.

Fig 3

Violin plots of students’ (A) human evolution acceptance scores, (B) macroevolution acceptance scores and (C) microevolution acceptance scores disaggregated by religious affiliation. The violin shapes are the densities of the data at each point on the y-axis. The solid black lines are the medians, and the top and bottom broken lines are the 75th and 25th percentiles of the data. When there is not line for the median or 25th percentile this means those values overlap with the minimum value of the scale.

Overall, these results indicate that Muslim students are less accepting of evolution compared to Catholic, Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, agnostic, and atheist students, but are slightly more accepting of evolution compared to Protestant students and students who are members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints.

Next, we report results on the evolution acceptance of students and ask which variables are related to their evolution acceptance levels.

Finding 4: Among Muslim students, a higher understanding of evolution and lower religiosity are positive predictors of evolution acceptance

The common ancestry of life on Earth

The variables explained approximately 10% of the variation in whether Muslim students accepted the common ancestry of life (chi-square = 21.847, df = 5, p < .001). A higher understanding of evolution was not a significant predictor of whether a Muslim student would accept common ancestry (p = .310), but higher religiosity was a negative predictor of whether a student would accept common ancestry (OR = .41, p < .001).

Human evolution

The variables explained approximately 17% of the variation in human evolution acceptance scores among Muslim students (F(5, 213) = 10.13, p < .001). A higher understanding of evolution was a significant positive predictor of human evolution acceptance scores (β = .200, p = .002), but higher religiosity was a stronger negative predictor of human evolution acceptance scores (β = -.423, p < .001).

Macroevolution

The variables explained approximately 9% of the variation in macroevolution acceptance scores for Muslim students (F(5, 213) = 5.35, p < .001). A higher understanding of evolution was a significant positive predictor of macroevolution acceptance scores (β = .267, p < .001) and higher religiosity was a negative predictor of macroevolution acceptance scores (β = -.223, p = .001).

Microevolution

The variables explained approximately 10% of the variation in microevolution scores for Muslim students (F(5, 213) = 5.80, p < .001). A higher understanding of evolution was a significant positive predictor of microevolution acceptance scores (β = .300, p < .001) and higher religiosity was a weaker negative predictor of microevolution acceptance scores (β = -.202, p = .003).

Since we had an adequate sample size for students with other religious affiliations to do these analyses, we also ran the same regressions for Hindu, Buddhist, Jewish, Protestant, LDS, and Catholic students (S1 File). We found that evolution understanding was related to acceptance of common ancestry, human evolution, macroevolution, and microevolution for almost all religions (with the exception of Jewish student acceptance of common ancestry). We found that religiosity was not related to acceptance of common ancestry among Buddhist students, was not related to acceptance of human evolution, macroevolution or microevolution among Jewish, Hindu, and Buddhist students, and was not related to microevolution acceptance among Catholic students. However, since the focus of this manuscript is on Muslim students, we only report full results for Muslim students in the main body of the manuscript. Full regression tables from the analyses of the broader population of students and for students from each religious affiliation can be found in the S1 File.

Taken together these results indicate that among Muslim students, controlling for major, gender, and international status, a higher understanding of evolution and lower religiosity are significant positive predictors of evolution acceptance. Further, across different measures of evolution acceptance, higher religiosity is a stronger negative predictor of human evolution acceptance and acceptance of the common ancestry of life on Earth than acceptance of microevolution.

Discussion

In the first study that we know of that has examined Muslim students in undergraduate biology classes in the United States, we found that Muslim students’ evolution acceptance and interest levels are lower than Catholic, Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, agnostic, and atheist students and were only slightly higher than Protestant students and students who are members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints. Muslim student acceptance of evolution was particularly low when considering their acceptance of human evolution and the common ancestry of life on Earth; Muslim students, on average, did not agree with items indicating acceptance of human evolution and only 36.5% of Muslim students chose items that indicated acceptance of the common ancestry of life. While past research on student acceptance of evolution in the United States has been focused on Christian students, largely because Christian students are the most prevalent in undergraduate biology classes in the United States, this study implies a need to consider how we can improve evolution acceptance for Muslim students in the United States since their acceptance levels are similarly as low as Christians.

Despite low levels of acceptance of evolution among Muslims students in this study, we did find that compared to Christian students, Muslim student evolution acceptance was slightly higher than Protestant students and lower than Catholic students. In prior studies in which Muslim and Christian evolution acceptance was examined outside of the United States, Christian students were often characterized as much more accepting of evolution than Muslim students. However, the Christians in prior studies were majority Catholic or Eastern Orthodox, both of which have official stances in favor of evolution [19, 20]. In the United States, however, there is a larger population of Protestant Christians, who tend to accept evolution less than those from other denominations [54]. Thus, Muslim students’ evolution acceptance may be comparatively higher in the United States because Christians in the United States are more often affiliated with Protestant denominations of Christianity.

We also looked at variables associated with Muslim student evolution acceptance and found that similar to other biology students, a higher understanding of evolution and lower religiosity is associated with more acceptance of evolution. Understanding of evolution was most strongly related to microevolution acceptance compared to human evolution acceptance and acceptance of the common ancestry of life on Earth. Acceptance of microevolution was relatively high among all students, including Muslim students. However, acceptance of common ancestry of life and human evolution acceptance was low among Muslim students and religiosity was a stronger predictor of human evolution acceptance and common ancestry than understanding of evolution.

Since we had data for students from other religious affiliations, we also looked at relationships between understanding of evolution and religiosity among those groups. While we found that evolution understanding was related to acceptance of evolution consistently across religions, we found that how religious a student is (their religiosity) was not consistently related to evolution acceptance for students from all religious affiliations. Specifically, the strength of students’ religiosity was not related to how much they accepted evolution among Jewish, Hindu, and Buddhist students. This is in line with research that shows perceived conflict between religion and evolution varies across religions, which affects the relationship between religiosity and acceptance of evolution [55].

Acceptance of microevolution and implications for teaching

Microevolution acceptance was consistently high across students from different religions, including Muslim students, and it may be the case that instructors can emphasize the widespread acceptance of microevolution as a gateway to help students accept macroevolution and human evolution. Although novice students may see microevolution, macroevolution, and human evolution as separate phenomena, biologists often see the patterns of macroevolution as a result of accumulation of microevolutionary changes between two populations experiencing reproductive isolation from one another [56]. When two populations of organisms become isolated from one another, either through geographic barriers (allopatric) or behavioral barriers (sympatric), microevolutionary changes accumulate differentially between these two populations, eventually leading to speciation of the two populations (macroevolution). Thus, if students already accept microevolution, then instructors may be able to more effectively persuade students to accept macroevolution and human evolution if they are able to logically articulate how microevolution leads to macroevolution and human evolution. However, this may only be effective if instructors can reduce perceived conflict with evolution and religious beliefs among Christian and Muslim students who likely see microevolution as more compatible with their religious worldviews than macroevolution or human evolution.

Religious Cultural Competence in Evolution Education (ReCCEE) for Muslim students?

Religiosity was a greater predictor of macroevolution and human evolution acceptance than understanding of evolution among Muslim students, which was similar to patterns seen among Protestant students. Prior research with Christian students shows that using Religious Cultural Competence in Evolution Education (ReCCEE) when teaching evolution can help reduce students’ perceived conflict between their religious beliefs and evolution [5759]. Cultural competence is the ability of one culture to effectively communicate to another culture and was born from healthcare studies to take into account racial/ethnic differences between physicians and their patients [60]. In prior research, we adapted this framework to consider cultural differences between secular instructors and Christian students, but this framework could also be useful for non-Muslim instructors who are addressing Muslim student religious beliefs while teaching evolution [9]. The following is a list of practices outlined in ReCCEE that have been adapted to be potentially useful when teaching evolution to Muslim students. While we do not know of studies that have explicitly tested the efficacy of these practices for Muslim student acceptance of evolution, we propose that these are ripe areas for future research to increase Muslim student interest in evolution and their acceptance of evolution. Below we list instructional strategies from the ReCCEE framework that could be explored with Muslim students learning evolution. For a more in-depth overview of the ReCCEE framework, how it was created, and the instructional strategies included in the framework, see Barnes & Brownell, 2017 [9].

Provide examples of Muslim scientists that accept evolution

Prior research shows that when religious students are provided with role models who reflect their identity when learning evolution, it can help them to accept evolution [57, 61, 62]. Instructors can provide Muslim students with examples of scientists who study evolution who are also Muslim. For instance, Fatimah Jackson is a Muslim biologist and anthropologist who won the Charles R. Darwin Lifetime Achievement Award from the American Association of Physical Anthropologists (https://www.physanth.org/news/aapa-announces-2020-darwin-lasker-and-communicationoutreach-awardees/charles-r-darwin-lifetime-achievement-award-2020-fatimah-jackson/) and Rana Dajani is a molecular biologist who has written about the compatibility between Islam and evolution (https://evokeproject.org/1269-2/) [63]. Highlighting these scholars and their role in evolutionary thinking may help Muslim students to see that their religious beliefs do not have to necessarily conflict with evolution.

Teach the scientific process and evolutionary biology as agnostic rather than atheistic

Many students come into the college biology classroom perceiving that in order to fully accept evolution, one would have to be an atheist [5, 57] and this perception is prevalent among both religious and non-religious college biology students [5]. Further, among highly religious students, this perception of evolution as “atheistic” is related to lower levels of evolution acceptance [5]. However, science, including the science of biology, can be accurately described as agnostic. Thomas Henry Huxley, also known as “Darwin’s bulldog” in the nineteenth century coined the term agnostic to describe the most scientific stance on supernatural claims [6466]:

Agnosticism is of the essence of science … It simply means that [we] shall not say [we] know or believe that which [we] have no scientific grounds for professing to know or believe … Consequently, agnosticism puts aside not only the greater part of popular theology, but also the greater part of anti-theology … Agnosticism simply says that we know nothing of what may be beyond phenomena. (Huxley, 1884)

To help students overcome this misperception that evolution makes claims about the existence of a God/god(s), instructors can teach the limits of scientific knowledge as explaining the natural world and explicitly describe evolution as agnostic with respect to a God/god(s) rather than atheistic [5, 67] so that it does not have to conflict with their belief in God/god(s).

Discuss potential compatibility between religion and evolution

The practices outlined in the ReCCEE framework, wholistically, aim to highlight areas of potential compatibility between religion and evolution. For instance, although a literal interpretation of some religious texts is not compatible with evolution (e.g., the special creation of humans separate from other animals), often it is possible for students to reconcile their religious beliefs with evolution if they interpret creation stories in religious texts as symbolic. Overall, the evolution education literature suggests that discussing these areas of potential compatibility between religion and evolution will be effective at increasing student acceptance of evolution [58, 62]. However, students and instructors report that evolution instructors often either ignore religion when teaching evolution or they only discuss where evolution and religion are in conflict [68, 69]. When instructors ignore religion, students may assume that religion and evolution have to be in conflict [68] and when instructors highlight only the conflict between religion and evolution, students cite this as a barrier for their learning of evolution [68]. But when instructors present religion and evolution as reconcilable, student acceptance of evolution increases [70]. Thus, if instructors are interested in increasing Muslim students’ acceptance of evolution, they may need to discuss the relationship between science and religion and highlight areas in which there is potential compatibility between religion and evolution [71].

Future research

We give recommendations for how to improve the experiences of Muslim students using the Religious Cultural Competence in Evolution Education (ReCCEE) framework, but this framework was built from studies that are largely composed of Christian participants. Future research should explore the use of cultural competence specifically for Muslim students who have a distinct religious background and culture from Christian students. We can only know the impact of these practices on Muslim students if researchers continue to explore the unique experiences of Muslim students in evolution education.

Limitations

We gathered data from a large number of courses and states in different geographic regions to try and create a representative sample of introductory college biology students. However, similar to most education research studies, we had to use a convenience sampling procedure and thus the results may not be generalizable to the broader population of introductory biology students. Of note, we recruited students from only 13 states, so while this was a national approach, the experiences of Muslims in the other 37 states could be different. However, given how few research papers have been published on Muslim students in the United States, these data from 13 states are a valuable starting point for understanding U.S.-based Muslim student evolution perceptions.

This study is limited by the quantitative nature of the study. Although we are able to look at averages of variables related to Muslim students’ evolution education experiences, we were not able to get a more detailed understanding of (1) how these students developed their views on evolution, (2) who or what specific experiences were influential for determining these students’ views on evolution, and (3) how Muslim students who have high acceptance of evolution came to their current conceptions. All of these insights would be helpful for making concrete recommendations for instructors when teaching evolution to Muslim students. Future interview studies could help illuminate specific experiences that are influential for Muslim students and help determine how to best make evolution instruction most inclusive for these students.

Conclusion

We found that Muslim students tend to accept evolution less and are interested in evolution less than Catholic, Jewish, Hindu, Buddhist, agnostic, and atheist students. Muslim students had particularly low human evolution acceptance levels and only 36.5% of Muslim students thought life on earth shared a common ancestor. Religiosity and understanding of evolution were important predictors of evolution acceptance among Muslim students (as well as students in the broader population) and higher religiosity was a particularly strong negative predictor of human evolution acceptance and acceptance of the common ancestry of life. These findings indicate that if instructors are interested in creating more inclusive environments for Muslim students or if they are interested in increasing these students’ acceptance of evolution, they may need to consider the religious beliefs and cultures of Muslim students in their classes while teaching evolution.

Supporting information

S1 File

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the instructors of the courses included in this study who were willing to send our survey to their students and the students for completing the survey. We would also like to thank Rachel Scott for her editing of the manuscript.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its S1 File.

Funding Statement

MEB and SEB, National Science Foundation (nsv.gov) IUSE #1818659. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.American Association for the Advancement in Science. Vision and change in undergraduate biology education: a call to action. American Association for the Advancement of Science, Washington, DC.; 2011.
  • 2.Brownell SE, Freeman S, Wenderoth MP, Crowe AJ. BioCore Guide: A Tool for Interpreting the Core Concepts of Vision and Change for Biology Majors. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2014Jun20;13(2):200–11. doi: 10.1187/cbe.13-12-0233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dobzhansky T. Nothing in Biology Makes Sense except in the Light of Evolution. The American Biology Teacher. 1973Mar1;35(3):125–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gallup. 40% of Americans Believe in Creationism [Internet]. Gallup.com. 2019 [cited 2019 Aug 31]. Available from: https://news.gallup.com/poll/261680/americans-believe-creationism.aspx
  • 5.Barnes ME, Dunlop HM, Sinatra GM, Hendrix TM, Zheng Y, Brownell SE. “Accepting Evolution Means You Can’t Believe in God”: Atheistic Perceptions of Evolution among College Biology Students. LSE. 2020May26;19(2):ar21. doi: 10.1187/cbe.19-05-0106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brem SK, Ranney M, Schindel J. Perceived consequences of evolution: College students perceive negative personal and social impact in evolutionary theory. Sci Ed. 2003Mar1;87(2):181–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ingram EL, Nelson CE. Relationship between achievement and students’ acceptance of evolution or creation in an upper-level evolution course. J Res Sci Teach. 2006Jan1;43(1):7–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Walter EM, Halverson KM, Boyce CJ. Investigating the relationship between college students’ acceptance of evolution and tree thinking understanding. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2013Sep9;6(1):26. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Barnes ME, Brownell SE. A Call to Use Cultural Competence When Teaching Evolution to Religious College Students: Introducing Religious Cultural Competence in Evolution Education (ReCCEE). CBE Life Sci Educ. 2017Dec21;16(4):es4. doi: 10.1187/cbe.17-04-0062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dunk RDP, Petto AJ, Wiles JR, Campbell BC. A multifactorial analysis of acceptance of evolution. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2017Jul17;10:4. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Glaze AL, Goldston MJ, Dantzler J. Evolution in the Sotheastern USA: Factors Influencing Acceptance and Rejection in Pre-service Science Teachers. Int J of Sci and Math Educ. 2014May1;13(6):1189–209. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Deniz H, Donnelly LA, Yilmaz I. Exploring the factors related to acceptance of evolutionary theory among Turkish preservice biology teachers: Toward a more informative conceptual ecology for biological evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 2008Apr1;45(4):420–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rutledge ML, Warden MA. The Development and Validation of the Measure of Acceptance of the Theory of Evolution Instrument. School Science and Mathematics. 1999Jan1;99(1):13–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.BouJaoude S, Wiles JR, Asghar A, Alters B. Muslim Egyptian and Lebanese students’ conceptions of biological evolution. Science & Education. 2011;20(9):895–915. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Yousuf A, Daud MA bin, Nadeem A. Awareness and Acceptance of Evolution and Evolutionary Medicine Among Medical Students in Pakistan. Evo Edu Outreach. 2011Dec17;4(4):580–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Asghar A. Canadian and Pakistani Muslim teachers’ perceptions of evolutionary science and evolution education. Evo Edu Outreach. 2013Mar26;6(1):10. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Clément P. Creationism, science and religion: A survey of teachers’ conceptions in 30 countries. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2015;167:279–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rachmatullah A, Nehm RH, Roshayanti F, Ha M. Evolution education in Indonesia: pre-service biology teachers’ knowledge, reasoning models, and acceptance of evolution. In: Evolution education around the globe. Springer; 2018. p. 335–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dagher ZR, BouJaoude S. Scientific views and religious beliefs of college students: The case of biological evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 1997May1;34(5):429–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Athanasiou K, Papadopoulou P. Evolution Theory Teaching and Learning: What Conclusions Can We Get From Comparisons of Teachers’ and Students’ Conceptual Ecologies in Greece and Turkey? Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education. 2015;11(4):841–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stears M, School of Education, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa, Clément P, Learning, Teaching, Assessment and Training (ADEF) Laboratory, Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France, James A, School of Education, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa, et al. Creationist and evolutionist views of South African teachers with different religious affiliations. South African Journal of Science [Internet]. 2016. May 30 [cited 2016 Jul 5];Volume 112(Number 5/6). Available from: http://www.sajs.co.za/creationist-and-evolutionist-views-south-african-teachers-different-religious-affiliations/mich%C3%A8le-stears-pierre-cl%C3%A9ment-angela-james-edith-dempster [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Yok MCK, Clément P, Leong LK, Shing CL, Ragem PA. Preliminary results on Malaysian teachers’ conception of evolution. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2015;167:250–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hanley P, Bennett J, Ratcliffe M. The Inter-relationship of Science and Religion: A typology of engagement. International Journal of Science Education. 2014May3;36(7):1210–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Unsworth A, Voas D. Attitudes to evolution among Christians, Muslims and the Non-Religious in Britain: Differential effects of religious and educational factors. Public Understanding of Science. 2018;27(1):76–93. doi: 10.1177/0963662517735430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Barnes ME, Dunlop HM, Holt EA, Zheng Y, Brownell SE. Different evolution acceptance instruments lead to different research findings. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2019Jan31;12(1):4. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chipman LN. Mythic Aspects of the Process of Adam’s Creation in Judaism and Islam. Studia Islamica. 2001;5–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Numbers RL. The Creationists: From Scientific Creationism to Intelligent Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2006. 636 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Heimlich R. Beliefs and Practices of U.S. Muslims Differ from Muslims Abroad [Internet]. Pew Research Center. [cited 2021 May 13]. Available from: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2012/08/14/beliefs-and-practices-of-u-s-muslims-differ-from-muslims-abroad/
  • 29.Bell J, Lugo L, Cooper A. The World’s Muslims: Unity and Diversity [Internet]. 2012 Aug [cited 2021 May 13]. Available from: https://www.pewforum.org/2012/08/09/the-worlds-muslims-unity-and-diversity-executive-summary/
  • 30.Pew Research Center. Muslim educational attainment around the world [Internet]. Pew Research Center’s Religion & Public Life Project. 2016 [cited 2021 May 13]. Available from: https://www.pewforum.org/2016/12/13/muslim-educational-attainment/
  • 31.Nadelson LS, Southerland S. A More Fine-Grained Measure of Students’ Acceptance of Evolution: Development of the Inventory of Student Evolution Acceptance—I-SEA. International Journal of Science Education. 2012Jul1;34(11):1637–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bishop BA, Anderson CW. Student conceptions of natural selection and its role in evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 1990May1;27(5):415–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mead LS, Kohn C, Warwick A, Schwartz K. Applying measurement standards to evolution education assessment instruments. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2019Feb8;12(1):5. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nadelson LS, Sinatra GM. Shifting acceptance of evolution: promising evidence of the influence of the Understanding Evolution website. The Researcher. 2010;23(1):13–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sinatra GM, Southerland SA, McConaughy F, Demastes JW. Intentions and beliefs in students’ understanding and acceptance of biological evolution. J Res Sci Teach. 2003May1;40(5):510–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Weisberg DS, Landrum AR, Metz SE, Weisberg M. No missing link: Knowledge predicts acceptance of evolution in the United States. BioScience. 2018;68(3):212–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dunk RDP, Barnes ME, Reiss MJ, Alters B, Asghar A, Carter BE, et al. Evolution education is a complex landscape. Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2019Mar;3(3):327. doi: 10.1038/s41559-019-0802-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cohen AB, Shariff AF, Hill PC. The accessibility of religious beliefs. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008Dec;42(6):1408–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dingemans E, Van Ingen E. Does Religion Breed Trust? A Cross-National Study of the Effects of Religious Involvement, Religious Faith, and Religious Context on Social Trust. JOURNAL FOR THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF RELIGION. 2015Nov1;1–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ecklund EH, Scheitle CP, Peifer J. The Religiosity of Academic Scientists in the United Kingdom: Assessing the Role of Discipline and Department Status. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 2018;57(4):743–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rissler LJ, Duncan SI, Caruso NM. The relative importance of religion and education on university students’ views of evolution in the Deep South and state science standards across the United States. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2014Oct15;7(1):24. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sbeglia GC, Nehm RH. Do you see what I-SEA? A Rasch analysis of the psychometric properties of the Inventory of Student Evolution Acceptance. Science Education. 2019;103(2):287–316. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Yasri P, Mancy R. Student positions on the relationship between evolution and creation: What kinds of changes occur and for what reasons? J Res Sci Teach. 2016Mar1;53(3):384–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Harackiewicz JM, Smith JL, Priniski SJ. Interest Matters: The Importance of Promoting Interest in Education. Policy Insights Behav Brain Sci. 2016Oct;3(2):220–7. doi: 10.1177/2372732216655542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hidi S, Harackiewicz JM. Motivating the Academically Unmotivated: A Critical Issue for the 21st Century. Review of Educational Research. 2000Jun1;70(2):151–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.García AA. Cognitive Interviews to Test and Refine Questionnaires. Public Health Nursing. 2011Sep1;28(5):444–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1446.2010.00938.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Willis GB. Cognitive interviewing: A tool for improving questionnaire design. Sage Publications; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hermann RS. Cognitive Apartheid: On the Manner in Which High School Students Understand Evolution without Believing in Evolution. Evo Edu Outreach. 2012Sep9;5(4):619–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hawley PH, Short SD, McCune LA, Osman MR, Little TD. What’s the Matter with Kansas?: The Development and Confirmation of the Evolutionary Attitudes and Literacy Survey (EALS). Evo Edu Outreach. 2010Dec3;4(1):117–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Short SD, Hawley PH. The effects of evolution education: examining attitudes toward and knowledge of evolution in college courses. Evolutionary Psychology [Internet]. 2015;13(1). Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/147470491501300105 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Carlson M, Oehrtman M, Engelke N. The Precalculus Concept Assessment: A Tool for Assessing Students’ Reasoning Abilities and Understandings. Cognition and Instruction. 2010Apr14;28(2):113–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Eddy SL, Brownell SE. Beneath the numbers: A review of gender disparities in undergraduate education across science, technology, engineering, and math disciplines. Physical Review Physics Education Research. 2016;12(2):020106. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lipka M. Muslims and Islam: Key findings in the U.S. and around the world [Internet]. Pew Research Center. [cited 2021 May 13]. Available from: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/08/09/muslims-and-islam-key-findings-in-the-u-s-and-around-the-world/ [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Masci D. 5 facts about evolution and religion [Internet]. 5 facts about evolution and religion. 2014. [cited 2020 Oct 13]. Available from: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/10/30/5-facts-about-evolution-and-religion/ [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Barnes ME, Supriya K, Zheng Y, Roberts JA, Brownell SE. A New Measure of Students’ Perceived Conflict between Evolution and Religion (PCoRE) is a Stronger Predictor of Evolution Acceptance than Understanding or Religiosity. CBE LSE. under review after minor revisions; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 56.Reznick DN, Ricklefs RE. Darwin’s bridge between microevolution and macroevolution. Nature. 2009Feb;457(7231):837–42. doi: 10.1038/nature07894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Barnes ME, Elser J, Brownell SE. Impact of a Short Evolution Module on Students’ Perceived Conflict between Religion and Evolution. The American Biology Teacher. 2017Feb1;79(2):104–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Truong JM, Barnes ME, Brownell SE. Can Six Minutes of Culturally Competent Evolution Education Reduce Students’ Level of Perceived Conflict Between Evolution and Religion? The American Biology Teacher. 2018Feb1;80(2):106–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Barnes ME, Werner R, Brownell SE. Differential Impacts of Religious Cultural Competence on Students’ Perceived Conflict with Evolution at an Evangelical University. The American Biology Teacher. 2020Feb1;82(2):93–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Betancourt JR, Green AR, Carrillo JE, Ananeh-Firempong O. Defining cultural competence: a practical framework for addressing racial/ethnic disparities in health and health care. Public Health Rep. 2003;118(4):293–302. doi: 10.1093/phr/118.4.293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Holt EA, Ogden TH, Durham SL. The positive effect of role models in evolution instruction. Evolution: Education and Outreach. 2018Aug24;11(1):11. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Winslow MW, Staver JR, Scharmann LC. Evolution and personal religious belief: Christian university biology-related majors’ search for reconciliation. J Res Sci Teach. 2011Nov1;48(9):1026–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dajani R. Why I teach evolution to Muslim students. Nature. 2015;520(7548):409–409. doi: 10.1038/520409a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Huxley TH. Agnosticism: A Symposium (1884) [Internet]. 1884 [cited 2019 May 26]. Available from: https://mathcs.clarku.edu/huxley/UnColl/Rdetc/AgnAnn.html
  • 65.Huxley TH, Huxley L. Life and Letters of Thomas Henry Huxley. Vol. 1. London: Macmillan; 1900. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lyons SL. Evolution and Education: Lessons from Thomas Huxley. Science & Education. 2010May;19:445–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gould SJ. Rocks of Ages: Science and Religion in the Fullness of Life. Random House Publishing Group; 2011. 105 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Barnes ME, Truong JM, Brownell SE. Experiences of Judeo-Christian Students in Undergraduate Biology. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2017;16(1):ar15. doi: 10.1187/cbe.16-04-0153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Barnes ME, Brownell SE. Practices and Perspectives of College Instructors on Addressing Religious Beliefs When Teaching Evolution. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2016;15(2):1–19. doi: 10.1187/cbe.15-11-0243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lindsay John, Arok Adhieu, Bybee Seth M., Cho Walter, April Maskiewicz Cordero Daniel G. Ferguson, et al. 2019. “Using a Reconciliation Module Leads to Large Gains in Evolution Acceptance.” CBE—Life Sciences Education 18 (4): ar58. 10.1187/cbe.19-04-0080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Olsen Joseph A., Phillips Andrea J., and Bybee Seth M.. 2019. “Religious Affiliation and Religiosity and Their Impact on Scientific Beliefs in the United States.” BioScience 69 (4): 292–304. 10.1093/biosci/biz014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Jamie L Jensen

26 Apr 2021

PONE-D-21-01450

Muslim Undergraduate Biology Students’ Evolution Acceptance in the United States

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Barnes,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I apologize for the delay in reviews!  It was rather difficult to find reviewers.  I suspect COVID has somewhat to do with this.  This manuscript shows great promise and I thoroughly enjoyed it.  I believe the reviews are fair and thorough and warrant your attention.  I don't think they will be too burdensome.  I recommend that you address each comment from both reviewers.  Of particular note is Reviewer 2's suggestion that you expand the manuscript to include the other non-Christian religions surveyed.  I think the data very much warrants this and makes this work even more applicable. Given the very similar sample sizes, it makes sense to combine results into one manuscript targeting at least three non-Judeo-Christian religions.  In addition, both reviewers pointed out some points of bias or speculation so make sure to address those, as well.  I look forward to reading your revision!

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 10 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Jamie L. Jensen, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

  1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: * When considering relevant prior research on other countries, it might be helpful to chase down some of the research done in McGill, Canada, often associated with Brian Alters. They examined responses to evolution and evolution in education among some Canadian Muslim populations. These might be particularly relevant because I think Canadian Muslim populations are more similar to the US Muslim population than the European and Muslim-majority country research referred to in this paper.

* There is some repetition in the text that needs to be cleaned up. I started getting annoyed at reading multiple variations on "Muslims have low acceptance of evolution" and "not much is known about Muslim students in the US."

* Lines 127-132, about de-emphasis of Muslim identity after 9/11. This is speculation; an opposite reaction—putting more emphasis on distinctly Muslim aspects of identity—is just as plausible. A more important consideration is that the social profile of Muslims in North America is known to be quite different than European immigrant populations and Muslim-majority countries. North American Muslims are comparatively wealthier, more educated, and in professional occupations compared to the country averages. This might result in bringing the level of evolution acceptance up compared to what might be expected otherwise. I didn't notice that the study was able to control for socioeconomic and parental occupational status; that might not have been feasible, but the fact should be noted.

* Someone who knows more about survey techniques and analysis should also review the technical aspects of this paper. I see nothing wrong offhand, but this type of research is far from my expertise.

* Line 227: I'm slightly confused—previously items were scored on a 1-5 Likert scale, but suddenly this becomes a 1-10 scale for Interest in Evolution. This might need some clarification.

* I'd like to see some more explicit acknowledgment of the limitations due to n=219 Muslim students as a sliver of a much larger data set. (There is some of this in the limitations section; it needs more emphasis.)

* Lines 462-468: The authors argue that exploring religion and evolution may increase acceptance of evolution. Why? Is there any solid evidence for such a proposition? This seems to be speculation.

* Line 501: I'd be more careful about reference (62). There is a small literature about alleged Muslim anticipations of evolution. I have invariably found such claims to rely on dubious interpretations of the history of science and a questionably broad understanding of "evolution" that erases substantial differences between premodern ideas about continuities in nature and the modern biological sense of evolution.

* Section starting at line 504. This notion of the "bounded nature of science" may have become dogma within science education circles, but it is intellectually quite dubious. Do the authors really want to commit to such a philosophically naive view? I wince to see that Gould's NOMA is a major reference in this section.

* I would question the competence of biology instructors to wade into Muslim religious beliefs and suggest a cheap compatibility between traditional beliefs and evolution. I doubt it would work well, especially with more theologically conservative Muslim students who are apt to notice religious ignorance on part of the instructor.

Reviewer #2: Overall the manuscript is well written and does not overstate any findings. It is a push forward in US evolution education and will be useful to those that educate Muslim students. The authors should be congratulated on focusing on more than Christian religions in the US. I did list the manuscript in need of "major revision." In reality it is in in between major and minor revision. It needs to have the scope addressed and some biased ideas removed.

Major criticisms:

My main criticism comes in the form of a question. Why only focus on Muslim students? While I agree there is a need to understand Muslim student views on evolution in the US I am not sure why there wasn't an attempt to also focus on Hindu and Buddhist students for this research. The sample sizes were similar and the findings were actually quite similar. A manuscript focusing on all three non-Christian religions is most appropriate and would make the work more interesting and impactful. Certainly by focusing only on Muslims the work is more focused but it does not seem that the paper would be hurt in any way by making it extend to other non-Christian religions as the analyses are done, the data a clearly there and all the discussion and conclusions could very easily be expanded to include Hindu and Buddhist students. Very little that is stated in the manuscript is specific to Muslim students. It would simply be appropriate to include all three in one single publication.

Line 457: I take the most issue with this sentence in the entire manuscript. It comes off as very non-educational and completely misses the mark. Why would an educator not use the common acceptance of microevolution across all religions as the place to start to build bridges and drive deeper acceptance across all aspects of evolutionary biology for all students? Especially, if the concern is that some students might de-emphasize who they are due to a sense of conflict in their beliefs with other students (their peers). My mind is completely blown that this sentence makes it into an educational piece, especially one that is focused on driving acceptance of evolution. As an educator we must always build on what is accepted and build bridges to what is not. Please fix this.

Lines 466-468: Not if the instructor hasn't identified and built upon what they do accept (see Line 457).

Minor comments:

Abstract:

Line 35 & 36: The first sentence should be qualified as being among US students, North American students, or Western students, but not as it is.

Line 52 & 53: Low or Lower? This also needs to be put in the context of the US as similar results have been shown outside the US prior to this publication.

Lines 115-118: Are these lines needed? The first sentence reviews what was directly reviewed only a few lines above and the last sentence has been repeated several times at this point in the document and is becoming highly repetitive.

Lines 128-129: This would be more interesting in the context of multiple non-Christian religions.

Lines 130-132: Why would they de-emphasize it? If Muslim students are surrounded by Protestant and LDS then why de-emphasize it? It may actually be a reason not to de-emphasize it.

Lines 134: Who are their non-Muslim peers? Certainly this varies between institutions but it goes to my previous point that if they are surrounded by peers that are also questioning and/or non-accepting of evolution, regardless of religion, then why de-emphasize their Muslim identity. Seems it would likely do the opposite depending on the composition of peers. Being non-accepting of evolution as an Muslim among a group non-accepting Christians should make Muslims more average and thus more accepted.

Lines 139 and 140: This section is the most novel portion of this research and should be the major focus.

Line 153-156: Or other non-Christian groups and a reason to treat them all in this manuscript.

Line 255: There is an assumption here that if students were born in the US they were raised here. Additional questions about what their current age and when they arrived in the US would be helpful. Another question that would seem to be very important is if their parents were immigrants as this would allow the authors to tease apart acceptance of Muslim students over generations. Not central to the current manuscripts scope but important to future research.

Line 437: in the US or elsewhere?

Lines 451-468: It seems some discussion has seeped into this section of the results.

Lines 471-482: This seems like introduction

Lines 482-483: This line seems awkward and is restated in parts in the rest of the language in the paragraph.

Lines 537-555: Could these be moved to the materials and methods? It would make for a cleaner read and a more positive ending to the manuscript.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Taner Edis

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Aug 11;16(8):e0255588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255588.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


11 Jun 2021

Reviewer #1: * When considering relevant prior research on other countries, it might be helpful to chase down some of the research done in McGill, Canada, often associated with Brian Alters. They examined responses to evolution and evolution in education among some Canadian Muslim populations. These might be particularly relevant because I think Canadian Muslim populations are more similar to the US Muslim population than the European and Muslim-majority country research referred to in this paper.

Thank you for this suggestion. We were able to find papers conducted on Muslim individuals by Brian Alters (one of which was previously included in the manuscript), but they were all outside of Canada. We did find one paper, not by Brian Alters, but by Anila Asghar, which included some data on Canadian Muslim High School teachers, and we have now included that in the manuscript.

* There is some repetition in the text that needs to be cleaned up. I started getting annoyed at reading multiple variations on "Muslims have low acceptance of evolution" and "not much is known about Muslim students in the US."

Thank you for noticing this! We tried to highlight the novelty of the study to make it apparent, but we agree it became repetitive and we have removed several instances of this repetition.

* Lines 127-132, about de-emphasis of Muslim identity after 9/11. This is speculation; an opposite reaction—putting more emphasis on distinctly Muslim aspects of identity—is just as plausible. A more important consideration is that the social profile of Muslims in North America is known to be quite different than European immigrant populations and Muslim-majority countries. North American Muslims are comparatively wealthier, more educated, and in professional occupations compared to the country averages. This might result in bringing the level of evolution acceptance up compared to what might be expected otherwise. I didn't notice that the study was able to control for socioeconomic and parental occupational status; that might not have been feasible, but the fact should be noted.

Thank you for this comment and suggestion. We agree that there are more substantial factors that may contribute to higher evolution acceptance among Muslim students in the US compared to students in other countries. We have removed the reference to potential de-emphasizing of the Muslim identity and added references that discuss how Muslims in the United States tend to (1) have a higher education levels and (2) are less likely to think the Quran should be taken literally.

* Someone who knows more about survey techniques and analysis should also review the technical aspects of this paper. I see nothing wrong offhand, but this type of research is far from my expertise.

* Line 227: I'm slightly confused—previously items were scored on a 1-5 Likert scale, but suddenly this becomes a 1-10 scale for Interest in Evolution. This might need some clarification.

Thank you for the question! The measures that are scored on a 1-5 Likert scale were not created by the authors, but previously published surveys with validity evidence. So, we did not design the response scale for these measures. However, a 5pt Likert scale is a generally accepted way to measure underlying latent constructs in survey research. There were not previous measures published for interest in evolution and we created this measure for a larger study. We used a 10 pt scale because it allowed for finer grained data, but this would not have any bearing on validity of the analyses, except to provide a finer grain description of participants’ interest.

* I'd like to see some more explicit acknowledgment of the limitations due to n=219 Muslim students as a sliver of a much larger data set. (There is some of this in the limitations section; it needs more emphasis.)

We agree that at first glance this might seem low, but the national population of Muslims in the United States is only about 1.1% of the total population. In our sample, students comprised about 2.8% of the total sample. So, in this sense, these students were overrepresented in our data set compared to the national composition. Further, statistically, 219 students are a large enough population to run all of the statistics we ran without running into any power issues. This is why we had to collect data from over 7,900 students in total so that we could get this substantial number of Muslim students for our analyses. Usually, for regressions, one needs at least 10 students per group, per variable in the analysis, which we had in our data set. So, we don’t think we are under sampled or under powered in our analyses. We added information about the percent of Muslims in the United States compared to our sample in the manuscript so that readers can compare.

* Lines 462-468: The authors argue that exploring religion and evolution may increase acceptance of evolution. Why? Is there any solid evidence for such a proposition? This seems to be speculation.

Thank you for encouraging us to be clearer on this matter. Indeed, there is published evidence that discussing religion and evolution while teaching can increase acceptance of evolution and reduce perceived conflict with evolution and religion. We have clarified this in the discussion by specifically saying prior research has shown this and citing these papers.

* Line 501: I'd be more careful about reference (62). There is a small literature about alleged Muslim anticipations of evolution. I have invariably found such claims to rely on dubious interpretations of the history of science and a questionably broad understanding of "evolution" that erases substantial differences between premodern ideas about continuities in nature and the modern biological sense of evolution.

Thank you for your expertise on this! We removed this reference.

* Section starting at line 504. This notion of the "bounded nature of science" may have become dogma within science education circles, but it is intellectually quite dubious. Do the authors really want to commit to such a philosophically naive view? I wince to see that Gould's NOMA is a major reference in this section.

We appreciate this suggestion and agree that Gould’s NOMA is often perceived as intellectually dubious, and it is outside the scope of this manuscript to clarify Gould’s view and how others have misinterpreted it. Thus, we have rewritten this section to focus purely on the limit of scientific knowledge to the natural world and encourage readers to teach science and evolution as agnostic, rather than atheistic, as originally proposed by TH Huxley.

* I would question the competence of biology instructors to wade into Muslim religious beliefs and suggest a cheap compatibility between traditional beliefs and evolution. I doubt it would work well, especially with more theologically conservative Muslim students who are apt to notice religious ignorance on part of the instructor.

We agree that biology instructors would likely be wholly unprepared to discuss Muslim belief systems in depth, so we are not advocating for wading into any religious beliefs. We have revised this section to be clearer that we are suggesting instructors might highlight areas of potential compatibility. For instance, when religious texts are interpreted as symbolic rather than literal interpretations. This may not work for conservative Muslim students who do read their religious text as literal, but previous research suggests this is less common among American Muslims who are more likely to agree the Quran can be interpreted multiple ways. We tried to make this clearer.

Reviewer #2: Overall the manuscript is well written and does not overstate any findings. It is a push forward in US evolution education and will be useful to those that educate Muslim students. The authors should be congratulated on focusing on more than Christian religions in the US. I did list the manuscript in need of "major revision." In reality it is in in between major and minor revision. It needs to have the scope addressed and some biased ideas removed.

Major criticisms:

My main criticism comes in the form of a question. Why only focus on Muslim students? While I agree there is a need to understand Muslim student views on evolution in the US I am not sure why there wasn't an attempt to also focus on Hindu and Buddhist students for this research. The sample sizes were similar, and the findings were actually quite similar. A manuscript focusing on all three non-Christian religions is most appropriate and would make the work more interesting and impactful. Certainly, by focusing only on Muslims the work is more focused, but it does not seem that the paper would be hurt in any way by making it extend to other non-Christian religions as the analyses are done, the data a clearly there and all the discussion and conclusions could very easily be expanded to include Hindu and Buddhist students. Very little that is stated in the manuscript is specific to Muslim students. It would simply be appropriate to include all three in one single publication.

We chose to make the focus is on Muslim students because of the documented perceived conflict between religion and evolution in the Muslim community coupled with the lack of data on US based Muslim students. However, we agree that the data from other religions is valuable and should be explored. Thus, we now ran our analyses for each religious affiliation, reported these results briefly in the results section, and added a paragraph in the discussion about these results. We did want to keep the main focus on Muslim students, so we put the detailed quantitative analyses of these students in the supplement.

Line 457: I take the most issue with this sentence in the entire manuscript. It comes off as very non-educational and completely misses the mark. Why would an educator not use the common acceptance of microevolution across all religions as the place to start to build bridges and drive deeper acceptance across all aspects of evolutionary biology for all students? Especially, if the concern is that some students might de-emphasize who they are due to a sense of conflict in their beliefs with other students (their peers). My mind is completely blown that this sentence makes it into an educational piece, especially one that is focused on driving acceptance of evolution. As an educator we must always build on what is accepted and build bridges to what is not. Please fix this.

We very much appreciate this comment, which helped us reconsider how we can use acceptance of microevolution as a place for students to start with and agree on their acceptance. We have taken care to rewrite this entire section with this valuable perspective in mind. We didn’t mean to imply that acceptance of microevolution is not important in the original writing, but that since it is already highly accepted it might not be a good target to increase acceptance, since it is already high. However, this idea of using shared microevolution acceptance as a starting point we believe is valuable and have now incorporated into the discussion. We believe this has strengthened the discussion.

Lines 466-468: Not if the instructor hasn't identified and built upon what they do accept (see Line 457).

We have removed this section of the text.

Minor comments:

Abstract:

Line 35 & 36: The first sentence should be qualified as being among US students, North American students, or Western students, but not as it is.

We agree and have added this qualifier to the first sentence.

Line 52 & 53: Low or Lower? This also needs to be put in the context of the US as similar results have been shown outside the US prior to this publication.

We again agree and have added this qualifier to the abstract.

Lines 115-118: Are these lines needed? The first sentence reviews what was directly reviewed only a few lines above and the last sentence has been repeated several times at this point in the document and is becoming highly repetitive.

We agree! Thank you for helping us make the manuscript more concise. We have removed these lines.

Lines 128-129: This would be more interesting in the context of multiple non-Christian religions.

Lines 130-132: Why would they de-emphasize it? If Muslim students are surrounded by Protestant and LDS then why de-emphasize it? It may actually be a reason not to de-emphasize it.

Lines 134: Who are their non-Muslim peers? Certainly this varies between institutions but it goes to my previous point that if they are surrounded by peers that are also questioning and/or non-accepting of evolution, regardless of religion, then why de-emphasize their Muslim identity. Seems it would likely do the opposite depending on the composition of peers. Being non-accepting of evolution as an Muslim among a group non-accepting Christians should make Muslims more average and thus more accepted.

For previous three comments on lines 128 – 134: Following concerns from both reviewers, we have removed the references related the previous three comments from the reviewer (lines 128 – 134) and instead now discuss more relevant considerations about differences between American Muslims and Muslims in other countries that may lead to higher evolution acceptance such as a willingness to interpret the Quran in multiple ways and their higher average education levels.

Lines 139 and 140: This section is the most novel portion of this research and should be the major focus.

We agree that this section is important, and we have emphasized this in the introduction and more in the discussion as well as expanded our analyses to students from each religious affiliation and not just Muslim students.

Line 153-156: Or other non-Christian groups and a reason to treat them all in this manuscript.

We agree and have added analyses of students from other religions to the manuscript.

Line 255: There is an assumption here that if students were born in the US they were raised here. Additional questions about what their current age and when they arrived in the US would be helpful. Another question that would seem to be very important is if their parents were immigrants as this would allow the authors to tease apart acceptance of Muslim students over generations. Not central to the current manuscripts scope but important to future research.

Great point! We controlled for whether the student was born in the United States in our analyses. However, your point is well taken that future research could explore how age arriving in the United States and if their parents were immigrants impacts their levels of acceptance of evolution.

Line 437: in the US or elsewhere?

We have clarified that we mean Muslim students in the United States.

Lines 451-468: It seems some discussion has seeped into this section of the results.

We reviewed lines 451 – 468 and they are part of the discussion section and not the results section.

Lines 471-482: This seems like introduction

Thank you for this comment. We have saved this section for the discussion because we felt like it makes more sense to introduce strategies for increasing acceptance after we have established that Muslim students have low acceptance – because if they didn’t, then the instructors would not need these strategies.

Lines 482-483: This line seems awkward and is restated in parts in the rest of the language in the paragraph.

Thanks for catching that! We agree and have revised this section to be more concise.

Lines 537-555: Could these be moved to the materials and methods? It would make for a cleaner read and a more positive ending to the manuscript.

We appreciate this comment but after trying to move this section to the methods, it seemed out of place and so we have decided to keep this portion of the manuscript towards the end.

________________________________________

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Taner Edis

Reviewer #2: No

Decision Letter 1

Jamie L Jensen

21 Jul 2021

Muslim Undergraduate Biology Students’ Evolution Acceptance in the United States

PONE-D-21-01450R1

Dear Dr. Barnes,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Jamie L. Jensen, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Lines 135-137: You don’t mean “human evolution” in both instances in the same sentence, do you? Please fix this; it’s confusing.

Reviewer #2: I have no further comments. Thank you for taking the time to reshape the manuscript and address all comments.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Taner Edis

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Jamie L Jensen

30 Jul 2021

PONE-D-21-01450R1

Muslim Undergraduate Biology Students’ Evolution Acceptance in the United States

Dear Dr. Barnes:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Jamie L. Jensen

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its S1 File.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES