Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 May 8;25(7):1389–1428. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1768

Human surrogate models of central sensitization: A critical review and practical guide

Charles Quesada 1,2, Anna Kostenko 3, Idy Ho 4, Caterina Leone 5, Zahra Nochi 6, Alexandre Stouffs 7, Matthias Wittayer 3, Ombretta Caspani 3, Nanna Brix Finnerup 6, André Mouraux 7, Gisèle Pickering 8, Irene Tracey 4, Andrea Truini 5, Rolf‐Detlef Treede 3, Luis Garcia‐Larrea 1,2,
PMCID: PMC8360051  PMID: 33759294

Abstract

Background

As in other fields of medicine, development of new medications for management of neuropathic pain has been difficult since preclinical rodent models do not necessarily translate to the clinics. Aside from ongoing pain with burning or shock‐like qualities, neuropathic pain is often characterized by pain hypersensitivity (hyperalgesia and allodynia), most often towards mechanical stimuli, reflecting sensitization of neural transmission.

Data treatment

We therefore performed a systematic literature review (PubMed‐Medline, Cochrane, WoS, ClinicalTrials) and semi‐quantitative meta‐analysis of human pain models that aim to induce central sensitization, and generate hyperalgesia surrounding a real or simulated injury.

Results

From an initial set of 1569 reports, we identified and analysed 269 studies using more than a dozen human models of sensitization. Five of these models (intradermal or topical capsaicin, low‐ or high‐frequency electrical stimulation, thermode‐induced heat‐injury) were found to reliably induce secondary hyperalgesia to pinprick and have been implemented in multiple laboratories. The ability of these models to induce dynamic mechanical allodynia was however substantially lower. The proportion of subjects who developed hypersensitivity was rarely provided, giving rise to significant reporting bias. In four of these models pharmacological profiles allowed to verify similarity to some clinical conditions, and therefore may inform basic research for new drug development.

Conclusions

While there is no single “optimal” model of central sensitization, the range of validated and easy‐to‐use procedures in humans should be able to inform preclinical researchers on helpful potential biomarkers, thereby narrowing the translation gap between basic and clinical data.

Significance

Being able to mimic aspects of pathological pain directly in humans has a huge potential to understand pathophysiology and provide animal research with translatable biomarkers for drug development. One group of human surrogate models has proven to have excellent predictive validity: they respond to clinically active medications and do not respond to clinically inactive medications, including some that worked in animals but failed in the clinics. They should therefore inform basic research for new drug development.

1. INTRODUCTION

Human experimental pain models play an important role in understanding physiological and pathological aspects of pain. They allow inducing temporarily a painful state in healthy subjects and thus offer an interim stage between animal models and clinical trials. This intermediate stage is critical, as human and rodent nociceptive systems show significant differences at peripheral (Schmelz & Petersen, 2001), thalamic (Barbaresi et al., 1986) and cortical levels (Evrard et al., 2014; van Heukelum et al., 2020), so that the data are often not transferable across species. Despite large preclinical investments, the overall clinical success of drugs for chronic pain remains low. Early phases of drug development routinely generate promises that are not confirmed in subsequent trials (London & Kimmelman, 2020), one prominent explanation being lack of cross‐species translation (Denayer et al., 2014). Being able to mimic aspects of pathological pain directly in humans has a huge potential not only to understand its physiopathology, but also to feed‐back animal research with translatable biomarkers for drug development.

While acute pain models are easy to produce using electric or thermal stimulation (Di Stefano et al., 2012), such acute stimuli have proved of little value to model neuropathic symptoms or signs in healthy humans, or to predict the effect of analgesic procedures (Bradley et al., 2016; Chapman et al., 1965; Weyer‐Menkhoff & Lötsch, 2018). Chronic neuropathic pain is characterized by a continuous or recurrent background component (ongoing or paroxysmal pain), associated in a variable proportion of cases with over‐reactivity to external stimuli reflecting peripheral or central sensitization mechanisms (Jensen & Finnerup, 2014). Reproducing ongoing neuropathic pain in healthy humans is challenging, because the initiating clinical event is damage to neural tissue, and there is no current human surrogate model that is able to produce continuous neuropathic pain without inducing unacceptable harm (Petersen et al., 2014; Schmelz, 2009). An alternative approach uses models that induce facilitation of nociceptive processing, and trigger central hyperexcitable states of which secondary mechanical hyperalgesia and dynamic mechanical allodynia are the measurable effects. The term ‘hyperalgesia’ denotes increased pain to stimuli that are normally painful (hence transmitted peripherally by nociceptive afferents), while ‘allodynia’ refers to pain elicited by a stimulus that normally does not cause pain, hence activating non‐nociceptive fibres. These two abnormal percepts occur in 20%–50% of patients with neuropathic pain (Jensen & Finnerup, 2014; Maier et al., 2010), and up to 70% in particular conditions such as post‐herpetic neuralgia (Johnson et al., 2010). Experimentally induced focal secondary hyperalgesia (2HA) and dynamic mechanical allodynia (DMA) are considered surrogates of neuropathic pain hypersensitivity: they are perceptually similar and share pain descriptors with their neuropathic counterparts, suggesting a commonality of underlying mechanisms (Gottrup et al., 2003; Jensen & Finnerup, 2014; Koltzenburg et al., 1994; Samuelsson et al., 2011), and sensory profiles observed in neuropathic pain can be mimicked by human surrogate models (Baumgärtner et al., 2002; Vollert et al., 2018). 2HA/DMA are defined as hypersensitive areas in non‐injured skin surrounding a real or simulated cutaneous injury, and there is ample consensus that they reflect central sensitization mechanisms (Arendt‐Nielsen et al., 2018; Klede et al., 2003; Koltzenburg et al. 1994; LaMotte et al., 1991; Rossler, 2013; Samuelsson et al., 2011; Schmelz, 2009; Simone et al., 1991; Torebjörk et al., 1992; Treede et al., 1992).

Some aspects of human surrogate models have been reviewed recently (van Amerongen et al., 2016; Vollert et al., 2018). The aims of the present systematic literature review were (1) to verify their ability to induce clinically relevant phenotypes of 2HA/DMA consistent with neuropathic central sensitization; (2) to analyse their success rate, spatial and temporal amplification; (3) to compare practical characteristics, reliability across studies and feasibility of these models for use in clinical trials, and (4) to compare their sensitivity to drugs. Our general objective is therefore to provide pain scientists with an updated analysis of the different possibilities to induce clinically relevant phenomena of central sensitization in humans, according to their experimental needs.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Systematic search, data extraction and analysis

The review is an outcome of IMI‐PainCare (BioPain) initiative. and was conducted according to PRISMA with the following limitations: quantitative meta‐analysis was only possible for success rate and spatial amplification, while temporal amplification admitted semi‐quantitative analysis (see below). The remaining parameters were analysed qualitatively; bias reduction was only analysed for drug‐related RCTs. We searched from PubMed‐Medline, Cochrane, Google Scholar, WoS, Clinicaltrials.gov, using the terms “[hyperalgesia OR allodynia] AND [human] AND [model]”, limited to human models by introducing restricting terms [NOT [rat OR mouse OR rodent OR murine OR nonhuman]”. Complementary searches were performed later in the process to tag electrical models: “[(high frequency) AND (electrical) AND (stimulation) AND (hyperalgesia OR allodynia)]”, as well as wind‐up ([(Wind‐up OR Windup) AND (capsaicin OR HFS OR LFS OR electric* OR freeze OR menthol OR “heat injury” OR “central sensitization” OR “human model”)], always with the above restricting terms. Iteratively, manual searches were conducted for reports not detected by automatic modes but available from the reference lists in literature. Searches were limited to English language, since database inception to March 31, 2020.

Three investigators (C.Q, A.K and L.G‐L) selected for further analysis publications reporting secondary hyperalgesia and/or allodynia in human surrogate pain models, and excluded papers reporting exclusively animal data, or exclusively primary hyperalgesia (i.e. within the area of induction only), or duplicate work. Work‐in‐progress was discussed during IMI PainCare‐BioPain meetings, then consecutive drafts were circulated among all co‐authors until a consensus was obtained on selected references, items to prioritize, conclusions and iconography. From each study, we extracted and summarized data on the characteristics of the sensitizing stimulus, the mode of assessment, details on outcome measures, consistency of responses across subjects, and –in studies on drug effects– whether blinding, randomization and placebo‐control were respected. These data are summarized in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and Supporting Information tables A&B.

TABLE 1.

Summary of studies on intradermal Capsaicin

First author and year N Capsa dose (ug) Pain on application (VAS) 2HA area (cm2) DMA area (cm2) Delay of max effect (min) Pin‐prick force (g) Responders (%) Spatial amplification index* Pharma intervention
Lötsch (2020) 16 100 26 Pregabaline (+)
Larsen (2018) 21 100 19.00 8.00 30.00 76.00 Diclofenac (−) & Methadone (+)
Andersen, Elberling, et al., (2017) 28 100 7.80 45.00 5.40 2.00 0.08–52 100.00 180.00
Diener (2017) 28 15 8.50 209.00 26 836.00 Botox A (+)
Ragavendran (2016) 63 250 26 Clonidine (+) & pentocyfylline (+)
Arout (2016) 18 250 21 Ethanol (−)
Wallace (2016) 19 250 8.00 40.00 20.00 160.00 Pregabalin (+) & T‐Ca2+ blocker (−)
Silberberg (2015) 15 100 54.58 22.70 100 218.32
Van Den Broeke (2015) 19 40 7.50 2.00 1.6–52
Nilsson (2014) 20 100 75.00 57 20 26/60 100 300.00
Wong (2014) 18 250 117.00 36.70 15 78.00 468.00 Pregabalin (+)
Hutchinson (2013) 12 50 5.60 83.80 70.00 15.00 335.20
Iannetti (2013) 11 60/120 9.50 13 90.00
Vuilleumier (2013) 16 100 9.60 44.70 23.30 120.00 52 100.00 178.80 Clobazepam (−) & clonazepam (−)
Kalliomäki (2013) 44 0.3 7.80 Cannabinoid (−)
Sumracki (2012) 18 10 52.70 210.80 Pregabaline (−) Minocycline (−)
Aykanat (2012) 14 10 3.14 26 12.56
Michaux (2012) 10 40 10 Cortisol (+)
Samuelsson (2011) 9 60/120 15.00 88.00
Gustorff (2011) 16 20 48.30 193.20 Lidocaine (+)
Andresen (2011a) 22 100 50.00 39 70.00 200.00 Buprenorphine (−) & Fentanyl (−)
Lam (2011) 13 10 13.00 11 140 52.00 Lidocaine (−)
Gustaffson (2009) 16 100/30/10/1 4.52 26 18.08
Lee (2008) 15 50 64/128/256/512 80.00
Klein, Magerl, et al., (2008) 18 40 0.08–52 88.00 Neramexane (+)
Wang (2008) 20 100 38.00 21 100.00 152.00 Pregabalin (+), Morphine (+), Diphenydramine (+)
Kraft (2008) 18 80.00 150 320.00 Cannabinoid (−)
Wallace (2008) 13 100 7 20.00 100.00 80.00 Gabapentine (−)
Gazerani (2007)
Wallace (2007) 15 100 37.00 16 15.00 93.00 148.00 Cannabinoid (−)
Geber (2007) 10 50 8.30 82.2 20.00 30.00 26.00 100.00 328.80
Pöyhia (2006) 9 250 16.38 164.00 100.00 65.52 Ketamine (+)
Scanlon (2006) 19 10/100 44.00 176.00
Gazerani (2006) 32 100 7.03 60 100 28.12 Botox A (+)
Gazerani (2005) 28 100 6.30 6.00 9 24.00
Pud (2005) 14 50 14.50 60 58.00
Gottrup (2004) 41 100 44.00 23.00 100.00 176.00 Gabapentin (+)
Wallace (2004) 14 100 35.00 20.00 15 140.00 Lamotrigine (–)
Yucel (2004) 12 50 6.51 48.00 39.00 31 100.00 192.00
Hughes, Macleod, et al. (2002) 12 250 63.00 31.00 20 252.00
Wallace, Ridgeway et al. (2002) 11 100 7.80 60.00 25.00 100.00 240.00 Ketamine (+) & Alfentanyl (+)
Eisenach (2002) 25 100 9.00 90.00 65.00 10.00 22 360.00 Adenosine (+)
Wallace, Barger et al. (2002) 13 100 24.00 15 92.00 96.00 Desipramine (−)
Yucel (2001) 10 50 7.80 75.9 60.00 0.00
Huang (2000) 11 50 93.00 20 90.00 372.00
Eisenach (2000) 24 100 70.00 34.00 22 280.00 Clonidine (+)
Witting (2000) 17 10 19.00 9.00 75.86 76.00
Gottrup (2000) 12 100 22.00 26.00 15.00 100.00 88.00 Ketamine (+) & lidocaine (+)
Ando (2000) 12 100 8.20 41.00 9.00 15 100.00 164.00 Mexiletine (+)
Koppert (2000) 12 2 9.20 16.50 6.00 12.00 45 66.00 Lidocaine (+)
Wasner (1999) 10 100 68.00 100.00
Baron (1999) 22 100 4.40 88.00 68.00 30.00 25 352.00
Magerl (1998) 12 40 6.80 26 83.00
Liu (1998) 12 100 59 20.00 236.00
Wallace (1997) 15 10 7.00 17.00 10.00 100.00 68.00 Lidocaïne (−)
Eisenach (1997) 6 100 8.20 47.00 28.00 7.00 188.00 Alfentanyl (+) & Amitriptyline (−) & Midazolam (−)
Kinnman (1997) 10 300 129.00 20 516.00 Morphine (+)
Ali (1996) 16 50 96.20 15 100 384.80
Park (1995) 12 250 19.00 6.00 76.00 Ketamine (+) & Alfentanil (+)
Treede (1993) 1 60 20
Lamotte (1991) 40 100 55.00 37.00 22.5 220.00
Simone (1989) 20 0.01/0.1/1/10/100 0/4/9.5/34/63 8.00 252.00

Some of the studies in this table also dealt with other models and are therefore presented in other tables too. Note the lack of quantitative information in a significant proportion of studies. Pinprick force is given in grams (1g = 9.8 mN). VAS (app)= pain during application on a 0–10 scale. Values of VAS, delay of effects and 2HA/DMA surface areas are given as within‐study averages. *Spatial amplification index refers to the ratio [2HA area/Application area]. Symbols in the last column indicate the efficacy of the drug [+/−] to significantly abate hypersensitivity versus control/placebo in a given study.

TABLE 2.

Summary of studies on topical Capsaicin

First author and year N Mode of appli. Kindling [C] (%) Dur. of application (mins) Appli. Area (cm2) VAS (app) 2HA area (cm2) DMA area (cm2) Delay of max effect (min) Pin‐prick force (g) Resp. (%) Spatial Amplif. index* Pharma intervention
Ditre (2018) 66 solution No 10 30 2.25 4.2 45.07 300 20.03 Nicotine withdrawal
Enax‐ Krumova (2017) 30 No 0.6 15 12 66 100 5.5
Niu, (2017) 28 cream No 10 20.9 Acupuncture
Maracle (2017) 20 cream No 0.075 30 3.30 10 0.08–52 80
Wanigasekera (2016) 24 cream No 1 90 16 1.8 52 100 Gabapentin (+) & Ibuprofen (−)
Doll (2016) 8 patch No 8 60 36
Kalliomäki (2013) 44 cream No 0.075 90 16.5 7.8 16.5 15
Zheng (2009) 40 solution No 30 3.14 40 26 12.73 Lidocaine (+)
Rukwied (2003) 20 solution No 1 15 0.5 3 20 10 30 5.1 40 Cannabinoid (+)
Liu (1998) 12 patch No 1 4 46 15.81 10 11.5
Morris (1997) 12 solution No 0.1 30 0.5 200 45 400 NSAID (−)
Andersen (1996) 17 cream No 1 4.9 25.9 5.28 Ketamine (+)
Kilo (1995) 24 solution No 1 2.25 33.8 Ibuprofen (−)
Kilo (1994) 24 solution 1 1.5 47 24 23 16
Koltzenburg (1992) 20 solution No 1 30 4 71.9 38 23 17.9
Linde (2019) 20 cream Heat 0.1 30 50 30
Smith (2019) 79 cream Heat 0.1 30 9 15 15 1.67
Price (2018) 24 cream Heat 0.075 20 100 50 90
Arendt‐Nielsen (2016) 36 cream Heat 1 30 9 60 Celecoxib (+) & AntiTRPV1 (−)
Rempe (2014) 16 solution Heat 0.6 30 9 1.3 83 17 8.55
Liljencrantz (2014) 40 cream Heat 0.075 9 25
You (2014) 78 solution Heat 0.6 30 2.25 70 26 89 31.11
Cavallone (2013) 15 cream Heat 1 30 9 4 40 26 100 4.44 Gabapentine (+)
Andresen (2011) 40 cream Heat 0.075 30 9 48 30 5.3
Campbell (2011) 32 cream Heat 10 6.25 47 21.5 7.52
Eisenach (2010) 14 cream Heat 0.075 30 1 65 10 65 Ketorolac (−)
Bishop (2009) 12 solution Heat 1 30 10.24 22.7 21.3 2.22
Frymoyer (2007) 23 cream Heat 0.075 30 22.8 140 95 4.38 Dextrometorphan(−) & Morphine (+)
Mathiesen (2006) 27 cream Heat 1 12.5 120.7 26 9.65 Gabapentine (+) & CHF3381 (+)
Jensen (2006) 85 cream Heat 0.075 30 22.8 137 107 26 5.52
Zambreanu (2005) 12 cream Heat 0.075 45 9 170 100
Iannetti (2005) 12 cream Heat 0.075 9 4 100 Gabapentine (+)
Duedahl (2005) 25 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 85 45 6.8 Dextrometorphan (+)
Maihöfner (2004) 11 solution Heat 2.5 30 1.53 3.7 79.7 21.5
Dirks (2003) 20 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 26 100
Petersen (2003) 23 cream Heat 0.075 30 22.8 8 66 23 Morphine (+) Remifentanil (+) Lamotrigine (−)
Hood (2003) 10 cream Heat 0.075 30 4 64 42 21.5 16 Remifentanil (+)
Eisenach (2002) 30 cream Heat 0.075 4 3.2 60 37 15 Adenosine (+)
Dirks, Petersen, et al., (2002) 25 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 97 7.76 Gabapentine (+)
Dirks, Møiniche, et al., (2002) 12 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 200 40 16 Remifentanil (+)
Harding (2001) 13 solution Heat 1 0.64 98 47.9 8.4 153.12
Petersen (2001) 14 cream Heat 0.075 30 22.8 3.7 142 100 21 100 6.23 Remifentanil (+)
Yucel (2001) 10 cream Heat 1 4 4.05 75.9
Dirks (2001) 23 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 94 60 21.5 7.52 Adenosine (−)
Mikkelsen (2001) 25 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 2.2 104 77 21.5 8.32 Magnesium (−)
McCormarck (2000) 10 solution Heat 0.075 12.5 75 NSAID (−)
Dirks (2000) 25 cream Heat 0.075 30 12.5 111 92 21.5 8.88 Lidocaine (+)
Petersen (1999) 10 cream Heat 0.075 30 22.8 1.5 176 118 21 5.26

Studies with and without heat‐kindling are presented separately. Please note that some of the studies in this table also dealt with other models and are therefore presented in other tables too. Pinprick force is given in grams (1g = 9.8 mN). Values of VAS, delay of effects and 2HA/DMA surface areas are given as within‐study averages. *Spatial amplification index refers to the ratio [2HA area/Application area]. Symbols in the last column indicate the efficacy of the drug [+/−] to significantly abate hypersensitivity versus control/placebo in a given study.

TABLE 3.

Summary of studies using heat‐injury (thermode‐based) models

First author and year N Temp (C°) Duration of appli. (min) Thermode area (cm2) Pin‐prick force (g) 2HA area (cm2) DMA area (cm2) Responders (%) Delay of max effect (min) Spatial amplification index* Pharma intervention
Hansen (2018) 121 45° 3–5 (BTS) 12.5 50 448 35.84
Hansen (2017) 121 45° 3–5 (BTS) 12.5 19 448 35.84
Schifftner (2017) 18 40 to 45° (+1°) 15’ step 4 6 28 7 7 Alfentanil (−)
Hansen (2016) 54 45° 3–5 (BTS) 12.5 19 310.1 24.81
Rasmussen (2015) 17 47° 7 12.5 90 55 45 4.4 Hyperbaric oxygen (−)
Andersen, Gögenur, et al., (2015) 29 47° 7 12.5 13 41 60 3.28 Melatonin (−)
Ringsted (2015) 24 47° 7 12.5 51 30 87 2.4
Petersen (2014) 27 45° 3–5 (BTS) 15.7 26 345 100 21.98 Gabapentine (+) & Glutamate‐antagonist (+)
Jürgens (2014) 18 48° 10x 6sec 9 26 80 60 8.89 Acetaminophen (+)
Ravn (2014) 28 47° 7 12.5 90 Morphine (−)
Ravn (2013) 28 47° 7 12.5 90 25 2 Buprenorphine (+)
Bishop (2009) 27 45° 5.5 10.24 10 16 1.56
Petersen (2008) 60 45° 3–5 (BTS) 22.8 26 Morphine (+)
Stubhaug (2007) 12 47° 5 12.5 5.6 35 100 65 2.8 Ketolorac (+) & Methyloprednisolone (+)
Frymoyer (2007) 23 45° 3–5 (BTS) 22.8 26 Morphine (+) & Dextrometorphan (−)
Schulte (2005) 16 46° 7 12.5 45 33.5 2.68 Morphine (+)
Staud (2005) 44 45° Dextrometorphan (+)
Yucel (2004) 12 47° 7 12.5 31 39.4 20.9 3.15
Dirks (2003) 20 45° 3–5 (BTS) 12.5 21.5 85 100 6.8
Werner (2002) 22 47° 7 12.5 45.5 85 41 86 6.8 Dexamethasone (−)
Dirks, Petersen, et al., (2002) 25 Gabapentine (+)
Dirks, Møiniche, et al., (2002) 12 45° 3 (BTS) 12.5 40 19 100 1.52 Remifentanil (+)
Brennum (2001) 25 47° 7 12.5 117 125 190 10 Naloxone (−)
Yucel (2001) 10 47° 7 3.75 75.9 50
Werner (2001) 22 47° 7 12.5 2 25 60 2 Gabapentine (+) & Dexamethasone (−)
Warncke (2000) 12 47° 7 12.5 5.6 65.5 5.24 Morphine (+) & Ketamine (+)
Mikkelsen (2000) 25 47° 7 12.5 117 86 100 60 6.88 Ketamine (+)
Lillesø (2000) 18 47° 7 12.5 43 50 50 4 Morphine (−)
Sjölund (1999) 10 47° 7 12.5 46 30 60 2.4 Adenosine (+)
Mikkelsen (1999) 25 47° 7 12.5 117 115 100 60 9.2 Naloxone (+) & ketamine (+)
Hammer (1999) 20 47° 7 12.5 42 40 60 3.2 Riluzole (−)
Pedersen (1998) 12 47° 7 12.5 34 85 28 100 120 6.8
Pedersen et al. (1998) 15 47° 7 12.5 46 85 100 60 6.8 Ketamine (−)
Petersen (1997) 20 47° 7 12.5 117 67.7 63.7 120 5.42 Ibuprofen (−)
Warncke (1997) 12 47° 6 12.5 50.2 100 30 4.02 Morphine (−) & Ketamine (+)
Ilkjaer (1997) 19 47° 7 12.5 117 80 42 60 6.4 Ketamine (−)
Warncke (1996) 20 47° 5 12.5 62 140 4.96 Ibuprofen (−)
Ilkjaer (1996) 25 47° 7 12.5 117 109 42 60 8.72 Dextrometorphane (+)
Brennum (1994) 10 47° 7 12.5 98 61 100 7.84 Morphine (+)
Pedersen (1994) 12 49° 5 3.75 16 38 100 180 10.13 Clobetasol propionate (−)
Moiniche (1994) 12 49° 5 3.75 16 53 91 180 14.13 Ketolorac (−)
Moiniche (1993) 12 49° 4 3.75 16 68 100 180 18 13 Piroxicam (−)
Dahl (1993) 18 50° 7 3.75 58 94 40 15.47 Lidocaine (+)
Cervero (1993) 10 39 to 42° 30’ step 5 14.2 100
Raja (1984) 8 53° 0.5 0.38 20.1 30 52.89

A number of papers also dealt with other models, and are therefore presented in other tables too. Pinprick force is given in grams (1g = 9.8 mN). VAS (app) = pain during application on a 0–10 scale. BTS, brief thermal stimulation. Values of VAS, delay of effects and 2HA/DMA surface areas are given as within‐study averages. *Spatial amplification index refers to the ratio [2HA area/Application area]. Symbols in the last column indicate the efficacy of the drug [+/−] to significantly abate hypersensitivity versus control/placebo in a given study.

TABLE 4.

Models based on repetitive electrical stimuli

First author N Frequency (Hz) Dur. of application VAS 2HA area (cm2) DMA area (cm2) Delay of max effect (mins) Pin‐prick force (g) Responders (%) Spatial amplification index Pharma intervention
Mauermann (2017) 16 2 120 min 6 27 15 26 Fentanyl (+)
Wehrfritz (2016) 20 2 155 min 6 50 40 30 45 95 166.6666667 Remifentanyl (+)
Nickel (2016) 48 1 45/60 min 5.6 15.6 26 Propofol (+)
Reindl (2016) 19 0.5/20 35 min 3 26 10 Neurostim (−)
Boyle (2014) 30 5 3.12 58 40 30 26 Gabapentine (+)
Tröster (2012) 15 2 150 min 5.5 31.2 2 45 104 Fentanyl (+)
Chu (2012) 10 2 210 min 5 12 Remifentanyl (+)
Nickel (2011) 12 2 35 min 26.6 2 26 88.67 Neurostim (+)
Lenz (2011) 16 2 150 min 6 44.1 30 26 100 147 Ketorolac (+) & remifentanyl (+)
Wehrfritz (2010) 20 2 180 min 6 45 30 26 150 Physostigmine (+) & N2O/O2 (+)
Bandschapp (2010) 14 2 180 min 4.2 64 50 25 26 64 Propofol (+)
Seifert (2010) 10 1 13.4 26 44.67
Seifert (2009) 12 1 50 min 4.92 46.76 26 155.87 Lidocaine (+)
Ayesh (2007) 24 5 20min 84
Geber (2007) 10 1 35 min 3.32 89.6 30 26 100 298.67
Chizh (2007) 32 2 180 min 6 44.69 38.44 40 26 148.97 Pregabalin (+) & Anti substance P (–)
Schulte (2005) 16 5 315 min 5 49.5 45 100 49.5 Morphine (+)
Chizh (2004) 20 1 155 min 5.8 28.1 37.4 30 45 93.67 Adenosine (+)
Pahl (2003) 10 5 120 min 5.9 35 20 30 45 100 Alfentanyl (+)
Klede (2003) 12 1 30 min 5 Lidocaïne (−)
Koppert (2001) 12 5 120 min 43 27.5 40 45 100 Ketamine (+), Alfentanyl (+), Lidocaine (+)
Van Der Broeke (2019) 15 100/42/20/5 45s/2min /4min /17min 50/80/60/35 14 3.98/6.56/ 4.78/2.78
Lenoir (2018) 18 100 45s 14
Cayrol (2018) 16 100 45s 14
Biurrun‐Manresa (2018) 17 100 45s 12 88
Reitz (2016) 10 100 45s 7.7
Van Der Broeke (2016) 14 100 45s 14
Henrich (2015) 20 100 45s 3.72 39 12 8.67
Xia (2016) 15 10/100/200 45s 55 30 13/30
Van Der Broeke (2014) 15 100 45s 15
Van Der Broeke (2014) 17 100 45s 15
Pfau (2011) 55 100 45s 4 83 83
Klein, Stahn, et al., (2008) 24 100 45s 4.9 9.89 79 2.82
Klein (2007) 8 100 45s 6.7 Ketamine (−)
Klein (2006) 13 100 45s 3.6 50 100
Klein (2004) 24 1/100 45s 5 38 45 0.02–51

Upper part of the table: models using low‐frequency stimulation (LFS, 0.5–5Hz); lower part of the table: models using high‐frequency stimulation (HFS, >5Hz, mostly 100 Hz). Pinprick force is given in grams (1g = 9.8 mN Values of VAS, delay of effects and 2HA/DMA surface areas are given as within‐study averages. *Spatial amplification index refers to the ratio [2HA area/Application area]. Symbols in the last column indicate the efficacy of the drug [+/−] to significantly abate hypersensitivity versus control/placebo in a given study.

TABLE 5.

Summary of the principal features of models

Models Number of studies Number of subjects Conditioning stimulus Secondary hyperalgesia (pinprick) Amplification index DMA
Duration (min, studies) Area (cm2, studies) Pain (0–10. studies) Duration (h, studies) Area (cm2, 95% CI, studies) Responder rate (%, studies) Spatial amp mean, 95% CI, studies Temporal ampl. (min,med,max) Area (cm2, studies)
Intradermal capsaicin 61 1,063 ~ 0.1 ~0.25 7.8 22 nr 51.1 [40–62] 47 93.3 28 196.2 [161–231] 49 +++ 28.1 30
Topical capsaicin 47 1,228 33.7 34 12.7 44 3.7 15 nr 70.2 [56–85] 27 95.8 11 17.6 [7–28] 29 +/++ 53.3 19
Burn injury by heat 43 940 6.4 32 11.4 41 3.3 18 3–72 3 81.6 [53–111] 38 94.9 18 9.0 [6–12] 36 +++ 38.2 8
Sunburn by ultraviolet light 28 490 nr 32.2 27 0.0 4 >36 14 44.1 [26–62] 18 49.5 13 5.1 [3–7.5) 13 +++ 11.2 5
Low frequency electrical stimulation 21 378 124.4 18 0.4 15 5.2 16 2.0 18 39.7 [30–49] 18 99.2 6 117.1 [77–157] 13 + 36.19 7
High frequency electrical stimulation 15 281 0.8 14 9.6 11 5.0 7 5.2 3 34.2 [17–51] 4 87.7 4 5.2 [1.7–8.7] 3 +++ 55 1

Data averaged across reports are summarized for models with >10 published studies and reporting data from >100 subjects. Confidence intervals (95%) are provided for areas of secondary hyperalgesia and spatial amplification. “Studies” refer to the number of reports on which data are based. Duration and area of injection of intradermal capsaicin are estimations, as papers do not report these items. Note that in low‐frequency electrical (LFS) models the 2HA duration is the same as that of stimulus application. Due to lack of sufficient data, temporal amplification was semi‐quantitatively stratified on three levels: hypersensitivity duration less than two‐times (+), 2–10 fold (++) or >10 fold the conditioning time (+++). nr=not reported. Note the important reporting bias.

TABLE 6.

Effects of drugs on the four principal human models of secondary hyperalgesia (Capsaicin, heat‐injury, UVB irradiation, LFS electrical stimuli) representing 90% of published data

Models

Drugs

Capsaicin (Intra‐d. & topic.) Heat (thermal injury) Ultraviolet‐B (UV‐B) Electric (LFS)

Gabapentinoids

+

Lötsch (2020) (id) rc‐, n = 16

Wallace (2016) (id) rcb, n = 19

Wong (2014) (id) rcb, n = 13

Wang (2008) (id) rcb, n = 20

Gottrup (2004) (id) rcb, n = 41

Wanigasekera (2016) (t) rc‐, n = 24

Mathiesen (2006) (ht) rcb, n = 27

Iannetti (2005) (t) rcb, n = 12

Dirks (2002a) (ht) rcb, n = 25

+

Petersen (2014) rcb, n = 27

Dirks (2002a) rcb, n = 12

Werner (2001) rcb, n = 22

(p =.06)

+

+

Boyle (2014) rcb, n = 30 Enggaard (2010) rcb, n = 18 Chizh (2007) rcb, n = 32 Segerdahl (2006) rcb, n = 16

_

Cavallone (2013) (ht) rcb, n = 15

Sumracki (2012) (id) rcb*, n = 18

Wallace (2008) (id) rcb, n = 13

_

_

Gustorff, Anzenhofer et al. (2004) rcb, n = 16

_

Opioids

+

Wang (2008) (id) rcb, n = 20

Wallace (2002b) (id) rcb, n = 11

Eisenach (1997) (id) rcb, n = 46

Kinnman (1997) (id) rcb, n = 10

Park (1995) (id) rcb, n = 12

Larsen (2018) (id) rcb, n = 21

Frymoyer (2007) (ht) ‐cb, n = 23

Petersen (2003) (ht) rcb,n = 18

Hood (2003) (ht) ‐‐‐, n = 10

Petersen (2001) (ht) rcb,n = 14

+

Petersen (2008) rcb, n = 60

Frymoyer (2007) rcb, n = 23

Schulte (2005) rcb, n = 16

Dirks (2002b) ‐cb, n = 12

Warncke (2000) rcb, n = 12

Brennum (1994) r‐‐, n = 10

+

Ortner (2012) rcb, n = 32

Gustorff, Anzenhofer et al. (2004) rcb, n = 16

+

Mauermann(2017) rcb, n = 16

Wehrfritz (2016) rcb, n = 20

Tröster (2012) rcb, n = 15

Chu (2012) rcb, n = 10

Lenz (2011) rcb, n = 16

Schulte (2005) rcb, n = 16

Pahl (2003) r‐b, n = 10

Koppert (2001) rcb, n = 12

_

Andresen (2011a ) (id) rcb, n = 22 (transdrm)

_

Schifftner (2017) rcb, n = 18

Ravn (2013) rcb, n = 28

Ravn (2014) rcb, n = 28

Lilleso (2000) rcb, n = 18

Warncke (1997) rcb, n = 12

_

Andresen (2011b) rcb, n = 22

_

Anti‐NMDA (Oral/topical)

+

Klein (2008) (id) rcb, n = 18

Pöyhiä (2006) (id) rcb, n = 9

Mathiesen (2006) (ht) rcb, n = 27

+

Staud (2005), rcb, n = 44

Mikkelsen (1999) rcb, n = 25

Ilkjaer (1997) rcb, n = 19

+

+

_

Frymoyer (2007) (ht) rcb, n = 23

Kauppila (1995) (t) rcb, n = 8

_

Frymoyer (2007) rcb, n = 23

Hughes, Rhodes, et al. (2002) rcb, n = 12

Mikkelsen (2000) rcb, n = 25

Pedersen (1998b) rcb, n = 15

_

_

Anti‐NMDA

(iv, mainly ketamine)

+

Wallace (2002) (id) rcb,n = 11

Gottrup (2000) (id) rcb, n = 12

Park (1995) (id) rcb, n = 12

Duedahl (2005) (ht) rcb,n = 25

Andersen (1996) (t) rcb,n = 17

+

Hughes, Rhodes, et al. (2002) rcb, n = 12

Warncke (2000) rcb, n = 12

Mikkelsen (1999) rcb, n = 25

Warncke (1997) rcb, n = 20

Ilkjaer (1996) rcb, n = 25

+

+

Koppert (2001) rcb, n = 12
_ _ _ _

Lidocaine

+

Gustorff (2011) (id) rcb,n = 16

Gottrup (2000) (id) rcb, n = 12

Koppert (2000) (id) rcb,n = 12

Dirks (2000) (ht) ‐cb, n = 25

Zheng (2009) (t) rc‐, n = 40

+

Dahl (1993) r‐‐, n = 18

+

Gustorff (2011) rcb, n = 16

(topical inside spot)

+

Seifert (2009) rcb, n = 12

Koppert (2001) rcb, n = 12

_

Wallace (1997) (id) rcb,n = 15

Lam (2011) (id) rcb, n = 13

_

_

Rossler (2013) rcb, n = 12

(topical outside spot)

_

Klede (2003) ‐‐‐, n = 12

Other NAch‐blockers

+

Ando (2000) (id) rcb, n = 12

+

+

+

_

Wallace (2004) (id) rcb,n = 14

Petersen(2003) (ht) rcb, n = 23

_

_

_

Cannabinoids

+

Rukwied (2003) (t) rc‐, n = 20

+

+

+

_

Kalliomäki (2013) (id,t) rcb, n = 44

Kraft (2008) (id) rcb, n = 18

Wallace (2007) (id) rcb,n = 15

_

Redmond (2008) rcb, n = 17

_

Kraft (2008) rcb, n = 18

_

NSAID

+

A‐Nielsen (2016) (t) rcb, n = 36

McCormack (2000) (t) rcb, n = 6

+

Stubhaug (2007) rcb, n = 12

+

Maihöfner (2007) rcb, n = 14

Sycha (2005) rcb, n = 42

Eisenach (2010) rcb, n = 14

+

Lenz (2011) rcb, n = 16

_

Larsen (2018) (id) rcb, n = 21

Eisenach (2010) (ht) rcb, n = 14

Wanigasekera (2016) (t) rc‐, n = 24

Morris (1997) (t) ‐cb, n = 12

Kilo (1995) (t) rcb, n = 24

_

Petersen (1997) rcb, n = 20

Moiniche (1994) rcb, n = 12

Moiniche (1993) rcb, n = 12

Warncke (1996) rcb, n = 20

_

Lorenzini (2011) rcb, n = 12

_

Tricyclic

+ + + + Enggaard (2001) rcb, n = 18

_

Wallace (2002a) (id) rcb, n = 13

Eisenach (1997) (id) rc‐, n = 6

_

_

_

TRPV1

antagonist

+ + + +

_

A‐Nielsen (2016) (t) rcb, n = 36

_

_

_

Adenosine

+

Eisenach (2002) (t) rcb, n = 30

+

Sjölund (1999) rcb, n = 10

+

+

Chizh (2004) r‐b, n = 20
_ Dirks (2001) (ht) rcb, n = 23 _ _ _

Adrenergic α−2

+

Ragavendran (2016) (id) rcb, n = 63

Eisenach (2000) (id) b, n = 24

+ + +
_ _ _ _

Botulinum

Tox. A

+

Gazerani (2006) (id) rcb, n = 32

+ + +
_ Diener (2017) (id) ‐‐‐, n = 28 _ _ Sycha (2006) rcb, n = 6 _

Steroids

+ Michaux (2012) (id) rcb, n = 10 + Stubhaug (2007) rcb, n = 12 + +
_ _

Werner (2002) rcb, n = 22

Pedersen (1994) rcb, n = 12

_ _

Paracetamol

+ + Jürgens (2014) ‐cb, n = 18 +

Ortner (2012) rcb, n = 16

+
_ _ _ Lorenzini (2011) rcb, n = 12 _
Glutamate antogonist + +

Petersen (2014) rcb, n = 27

+ +
_ _ Hammer (1999) rcb, n = 20 _ _

Propofol

+ + + + Bandschapp (2010) rcb,n = 14
_ _ _ _

Nickel (2016) rcb, n = 48

20Hz‐neurostim.

+ + + +

Nickel (2011) rc‐‐, n = 12

_ _ _ _

Reindl (2015) rc‐‐, n = 19

The acronym “rcb” in brackets stands for “randomized, controlled, blinded”, when one of these was lacking and is replaced by a hyphen (“‐“). For capsaicin models, “id”: intradermal; “t”: topical; “ht”: heat‐topical. *=tested in patients. We included drugs tested in at least two independent studies with n > 10 subjects each. Studies were classed as “positive” if the drug affected significantly at least one output readout of hyperalgesia (e.g. 2HA area). Capsaicin, heat‐injury and LFS models responded to drugs clinically accepted for neuropathic pain (gabapentinoids, TCAs, iv ketamine, iv lidocaine) in a significantly greater proportion than to drugs with non‐clinical effect in NP (NSAIDs, oral anti‐NMDA, mexiletine, minocycline, steroids, anti‐TRPV1, magnesium, anti‐substance P, melatonin) (82.5% versus. 30.3%, p <.01).

We focused on mechanical hypersensitivity to gentle brushing (DMA: dynamic mechanical allodynia) and to punctate stimuli (pinprick hyperalgesia) since they are prevalent in neuropathic pain (Jensen & Finnerup, 2014; Maier et al., 2010), have long been recognized as hallmark signs of central sensitization (Treede et al., 1992), and were tested in a vast majority of published studies. Other mechanical stimuli (blunt pressure, impact stimuli) were not included because they are not unanimously acknowledged to reflect central sensitization (Göttrup et al., 2000; Kilo et al., 1994). Although more controversial, we included occasional reports describing changes in temporal summation of pain (“wind‐up”), either as signs of central sensitization or as models of central hypersensitivity (Andersen et al., 1996; Enggaard et al., 2001; Hughes et al., 2002).

Except for the rare cases where neurophysiological readouts were available, the methods used to assess “success” or “failure” of models were based on subjective reports of a “change in perception”. While we had to accept assessment as face value from each report, inconsistencies across studies were detected and discussed. As a quantitative measure of sensory amplification, we calculated the ratio between the area of secondary hyperalgesia and the area where the sensitizing stimulus was applied (“spatial amplification index”). When outcomes were measured repeatedly, we chose for statistics the time point showing highest area. A symmetrical measure of “temporal amplification” (duration of hypersensitivity relative to duration of the conditioning stimulus) could only be estimated semi‐quantitatively due to lack of data. Temporal amplification was therefore stratified on 3 levels, depending on whether hypersensitivity duration was less than 2‐times, 2–10 fold or >10 fold the conditioning time.

To avoid ambiguities in definitions, unless stated otherwise the term “allodynia” will be restricted here to dynamic mechanical allodynia (DMA), tested using brush stroke, and the term “secondary hyperalgesia” (2HA) will refer to responses to pinprick, even when pinprick force was not painful.

Hypothesis‐testing was used to compare 2HA induction ratios for different concentrations, thermode surfaces, duration of irradiation, etc. Relations between the areas of secondary hyperalgesia and areas of sensitizing stimuli were tested by linear or polynomial correlation models. Chi‐2 and confidence interval analyses were used to test possible associations between type of drugs tested and anti‐hyperalgesic effects.

Methodological quality of studies dealing with drug effects was assessed using the 5‐point Oxford Quality Scale (Jadad et al., 1996). A minimum of 10 subjects and score of 2B was required for inclusion. Risk of bias (Higgins et al., 2011) including no allocation concealment, lack of blinding (performance or detection bias), lack of control condition and reporting bias, was checked, and unless explicitly stated all included studies on drug effects were randomized and placebo‐controlled (Table 6). Absence of blinding was allowed if considered unavoidable (e.g. because of drug effects) and it did not decrease the level of evidence. Given the extreme heterogeneity in test stimulus, timing of assessment, specific readouts, etc., the evaluation of drug efficacy could not be expressed as VAS changes with confidence intervals but only in binary form (significant success/failure vs. placebo). When essential data were missing, we contacted authors to request additional information, and if these data could not be obtained we excluded those studies from further analysis.

3. RESULTS

Initial electronic search from databases identified n = 1569 publications, of which 719 were considered potentially eligible after a first analysis of title and abstract. Exclusion of duplicates, of studies limited to primary hyperalgesia and/or to animal models constrained the sample to n = 173 papers. Full text analysis of these (C.Q., L.G‐L.) led to exclusion of 81 reports due to (1) lack of enough information on methods or outcomes, (2) lack of adequate control in drug studies, or (3) anecdotal data or single case reports. This list was completed with articles identified from the reference lists in literature and other sources, yielding a final dataset of n = 269 papers. The selection flowchart and the number of papers by type of model are detailed in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Flow Chart illustrating the paper selection procedure, and final number of articles retained according to specific models. Initial electronic search from databases (uppermost box) identified n = 1569 publications, of which 719 were potentially eligible from title/abstract. Exclusion of non‐human studies, studies limited to primary hyperalgesia and duplicates constrained the sample to n = 173 papers. A further 81 reports were excluded due to lack of enough information on methods/outcomes, lack of adequate control group or low sample size. The list was then completed by manual search with articles identified from reference lists and other sources, yielding a final dataset of n = 269 papers

We identified 108 studies reporting on the capsaicin models (61 intradermal, 47 topical); 72 on thermal or inflammatory injury models (44 heat, 28 UVB); 36 on electrically induced models (21 low and 15 high‐frequency stimulation), and 40 reports on less prevalent models, including menthol/freeze (n = 10); nerve growth factor (NGF; n = 8), mustard oil or cinnamaldehyde (n = 6), hypertonic and acidic saline injections (n = 5), incisional models (n = 5), glutamate, endothelin‐1, lauryl sulphate or ciguatoxin (n = 6). In addition, 13 review papers on different aspects of models were also analysed.

3.1. Capsaicin‐based models

Capsaicin (8‐methyl‐N‐vanillyl‐6‐nonenamide) is an alkaloid found naturally in pepper (Nelson, 1919), which induces intense sensations of burn by its agonist effect on the transient receptor potential vanilloid‐1 (TRPV1) ion channel receptors (Bautista & Julius, 2008; Caterina et al., 1997; Gannon et al., 2016; Schmelz et al., 2000). The use of capsaicin as a surrogate model inducing secondary hyperalgesia is very common, with more than 100 original studies reported so far. Capsaicin delivery can be topical (Jancso, 1960) or via intra‐dermal injection (Simone et al., 1989); these modes of application lead to different patterns of effects at the application site and around it.

3.1.1. Intradermal (ID) capsaicin

Secondary hyperalgesia surrounding an intradermal capsaicin injection is the only model where central sensitization has been conclusively shown to be the underlying mechanism in translational studies in humans and animals (Baumann et al., 1991; LaMotte et al., 1991; Simone et al., 1991; Torebjörk et al., 1992) (Table 1). A solution of capsaicin is injected intradermally, generally through a 30‐gauge needle, which is painful and technically more demanding than a simple subcutaneous injection. Previously suggested differences in 2HA area depending on injection site (Gazerani et al., 2007; Liu et al., 1998) could not be confirmed in this review. The quantity of injected capsaicin ranged from 0.01 µg to 300 µg, with preference for 50–100 µg (Table 1). A dose–response relationship was demonstrated repeatedly (Gustafsson et al., 2009; Scanlon et al., 2006; Simone et al., 1989), with 1 µg being the lowest dose producing a measurable hyperalgesic area. Due to capsaicin saturation properties, the gain of injecting more than 50 µg may be minimal, such doses yielding inconsistent results (Gustafsson et al., 2009; Figure 2). Responder rates (reported in 28/61 studies) were in most cases 75%–100% for 2HA, without significant difference between doses of 40–60, 100–120 or 250–300 µg of capsaicin (Table 1). Lower responder rates were reported for DMA at all the doses tested (Liu et al., 1998; Pöyhia & Vainio, 2006; Samuelsson et al., 2011). Geber et al. (2007) reported excellent test–retest reproducibility (r = 0.77) of stimulus‐response functions over two consecutive days.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Dose–response graphs for intradermal capsaicin, as reported in three independent studies. Note the lack of significant effects of doses lower than 1 µg, and the inconsistent results of the 100 µg dosage, especially at 60 min

The latency to develop 2HA/DMA after intradermal capsaicin is very short, from virtually no latency to a few minutes (Nilsson et al., 2014). Duration of effects increase with dose, from a few minutes for 1 µg to about 2 hr for 50–100 µg (Geber et al., 2007; Gottrup et al., 2004; Simone et al., 1989). No gender effects were reported in the limbs (Eisenach et al., 1997), but larger hyperalgesic areas in women relative to men, and in Indians relative to Caucasians were found in the forehead (Gazerani et al., 2005a,2005b, 2007).

Gabapentinoids decreased ID capsaicin hyperalgesic and/or allodynic area and pain intensity in five controlled studies on healthy subjects (Table 6). Subcutaneous or intravenous opioids also abated ID capsaicin hyperalgesia versus placebo in five studies (Table 6), while results were inconsistent for the transdermal route: positive for methadone/diclofenac (Larsen et al., 2018) but not for buprenorphine or fentanyl (Andresen, Staahl, et al., 2011). Intravenous NMDA receptor antagonists, including intravenous ketamine and intravenous ethanol decreased 2HA versus placebo (Arout et al., 2016; Gottrup et al., 2000; Park et al., 1995; Pöyhiä & Vainio, 2006), while oral naramexane decreased DMA but not pinprick hyperalgesia (Klein et al., 2008). Lidocaine inconsistently affected 2HA, both by intravenous route (decreased for Gottrup et al., 2000, Koppert et al., 2000; unchanged for Wallace et al., 1997) and by topical/regional routes (decreased for Zheng et al., 2009, Gustorff et al 2011a; unchanged for Koppert et al., 2000, Lam et al., 2011). Other sodium channel blockers such as lamotrigine, mexiletine and 4030W92 had little or no effect on 2HA (Ando et al., 2000; Wallace et al., 2004). Non‐replicated studies reported positive results with hydrocortisone (Michaux et al., 2012), intrathecal adenosine (Eisenach et al., 2002), and epidural or intrathecal clonidine (Eisenach et al., ,1998, 2000). Inconsistent results were observed with botulinum toxin (Diener et al., 2017; Gazerani et al., 2006). The tricyclic antidepressants amitriptyline and desipramine failed to modify 2HA in two studies (Eisenach et al., 1997; Wallace et al., 2002). No anti‐hyperalgesic effect was reported with oral minocycline (Sumracki et al., 2012), topical ibuprofen (Morris et al., 1997), T‐type calcium channels blockers (Wallace et al., 2016) or cannabinoid receptors (Kalliomäki et al., 2013; Kraft et al., 2008; Wallace et al., 2007). One study reported that unpleasantness, but not intensity, of capsaicin‐induced hyperalgesia was attenuated by THC (Lee et al., 2013).

No serious adverse effects have been reported so far. The main qualities of ID capsaicin are the short latency of 2HA and allodynia allowing rapid testing after injection, the sizeable duration and consistency of the effects, the minimal size of primary hyperalgesia and the high rate of responders. Such advantages may be offset by limitations such as the slightly invasive nature of the technique, the higher discomfort upon injection relative to its topical counterpart (e.g. Kraft et al., 2008), the difficulty to prepare lipophilic capsaicin in aqueous solution and target injections to the dermis layer (similar to tuberculine injection). Changes in blood pressure and heart rate have been documented, which may vary with injection at different depths in the skin (Silberberg et al., 2015).

3.1.2. Topical capsaicin

We identified 47 studies using capsaicin applied topically in form of cream, solution‐soaked gauze or patch, generally during 30 min, on either the upper or the lower limb. Since the effects tend to be briefer than those of intradermal injection, most studies employed a heat sensitization procedure by applying a thermode at 40–45°C on the site of capsaicin application, which improves the stability of secondary hyperalgesia (Dirks & Petersen, 2003; Linde & Srbely, 2019; Petersen & Rowbotham, 1999) (Table 2). The duration of hyperalgesia was rarely indicated, but iterative application of cutaneous heat every 45 min proved useful to sustain the hypersensitivity during several hours in some studies (Dirks, et al., 2002; Modir & Wallace, 2010a; Petersen et al., 2001; Petersen & Rowbotham, 1999), although exceptions exists (e.g. Cavallone et al., 2013). Responder rate was 80%–100%, but information was provided in 11/47 studies only (Table 2).

A 2‐way factorial ANOVA with “mode of application” (solution vs. cream/patch) and “kindling” (yes/no) as factors, showed a significant enhancement of allodynic area for heat‐kindling (F(1,15) = 7.05; p =0.018) but no effect of application mode (F(1,15) = 0.19; p =0.6), and no interaction. Neither mode of application nor kindling influenced significantly the area of 2HA (Table 2).

No correlation was found between the concentration of topical capsaicin and 2HA/DMA areas (R = 0.04; p =0.15; Table 2). Conversely, the application surface, which varied from 1 to 100 cm2, was positively correlated with the area of secondary hyperalgesia (Figure 3a), while the spatial amplification ratio (relation between 2HA area and surface of capsaicin application) declined exponentially with increased application surfaces (Figure 3b).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Relations between area of application and area of hyperalgesia in the topical capsaicin model. Data from the 31 studies providing enough data to compute correlations (see Table 2). Top: Positive correlation between the surface of topical application and the secondary hyperalgesic area. Bottom: Inverse exponential correlation between area of application and spatial amplification ratio of hyperalgesia: the ratio of surface amplification (2HA area / application area) decreases steeply with increasing surface of application

The hyperalgesic effects of topical capsaicin were attenuated by gabapentin 1200–1800 mg, versus placebo in 4 out of 5 studies (Cavallone et al., 2013; Dirks et al., 2002; Iannetti et al., 2005; Mathiesen et al., 2006; Wanigasekera et al., 2016). The opioids morphine, fentanyl and remifentanil decreased 2HA in one open and 3 placebo‐controlled studies (Frymoyer et al., 2007; Hood et al., 2003; Petersen et al., 2001, 2003). While opioids reduced both 2HA and physiological nociception, gabapentin did not affect nociceptive thresholds in normal skin. One study found the N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist ketamine to decrease heat/capsaicin‐induced 2HA, nociceptive reflexes and wind‐up responses versus placebo (Andersen et al., 1996). Other NMDA antagonists such as dextromethorphan reduced 2HA when applied intravenously (0.5 mg/kg) (Duedahl et al., 2005) but not by oral route (30–100 mg; Kawamata, et al., 2002; Frymoyer et al., 2007). Intrathecal adenosine reduced 2HA and DMA areas (Eisenach et al., 2002), but was ineffective by systemic route (Dirks et al., 2001). Topical cannabinoid agonists gave inconsistent results, positive versus placebo for Rukwied et al., (2003) but not for Kalliomäki et al., (2013). Nonsteroidal anti‐inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) were inactive on 2HA/DMA when administered systemically (Kilo et al., 1995; Wanigasekera et al., 2016) or intrathecally (Eisenach et al., 2010), but reduced DMA if administered topically (McCormack et al., 2000). Capsaicin‐induced 2HA was not attenuated beyond placebo by the sodium channel blocker lamotrigine (Petersen et al., 2003), the TRPV1 antagonist V116517 (Arendt‐Nielsen et al., 2016) or intravenous magnesium (Mikkelsen et al., 2001).

Topical capsaicin is a safe and easy‐to‐use model of hyperalgesia. No serious adverse effects have been reported. Main qualities are ease of handling and very moderate pain at induction relative to ID capsaicin. Limitations may be the unfavourable ratio between the duration of application and the duration of hyperalgesia, as well as the relatively low spatial amplification factor. These difficulties can be improved by heat‐kindling, which is the recommended procedure to enhance stability and duration of hypersensitivity.

3.2. Heat injury models

Initial techniques using small skin spots at >50°C on glabrous skin were instrumental in clarifying many aspects of peripheral encoding of heat pain (Meyer & Campbell, 1981; Raja et al., 1984) (Table 3), but produced second degree burns, blisters and visible oedema, and have been replaced by techniques using lower temperatures on hairy skin. Prolonged thermal stimulation at non‐painful levels (40–42°C) can also trigger secondary hyperalgesia; however, the stimulus has to be maintained for long periods, and hyperalgesia is extremely short‐lived (Cervero et al., 1993; Schifftner et al., 2017). Therefore, most studies used thermodes at higher temperatures (~47°C) applied during 5–7 min to a 9–16 cm2 hairy skin contact area (Table 3). This results in a first‐degree burn injury (redness without blistering) for less than one day, primary hyperalgesia on the site of exposure and 2HA/DMA in adjacent tissue. Areas of 2HA were quite variable (95% CI: 53–111), with average surface amplification index ~9 (95% CI 5.59–11.7). Maximum effect is reached at about 75 min [95% CI 54–93] and the duration of hyperalgesia (reported in very few studies) could range from 3 to 72 hr. Most studies reported high 2HA response rates (80%–100%), while the incidence of allodynia was only ~60% (Hammer et al., 1999; Werner et al., 2001). (all calculations from data in Table 3).

The “brief thermal stimuli” variant (Dirks et al., 2002, 2003), uses 1°C/sec temperature increase from 32°C to 45°C, and tests hyperalgesia after 3 min at 45°C. Although this procedure provides large areas of hypersensitivity and high responder rates (200–300 cm2, 100%), the duration of hyperalgesia is very short, and assessment has to be performed with the thermode still in place (Dirks et al., 2003; Hansen et al., 2017). Using a different approach (60 heat‐pulses at 48°C for 6 s) Jurgens et al., (2014) reported large areas of 2HA/DMA (80 cm2) with maximal effect at 60 min and 8‐hr duration, but the procedure has not been replicated.

Thermode‐induced 2HA was decreased by systemic opioids including morphine, buprenorphine and alfentanil in seven studies (six controlled; Table 6), but failed to overpower placebo in four controlled trials (Lillesø et al., 2000; Ravn et al., 2014; Schifftner et al., 2017; Warncke et al., 1997). Gabapentin decreased 2HA in two controlled studies (Dirks et al., 2002; Petersen et al., 2014) and showed a trend in a third one (p =.06; Werner et al., 2001). The NMDA receptor antagonist ketamine had consistent effects on 2HA when administered intravenously (Hughes, Rhodes, et al., 2002; Ilkjaer et al., 1996; Mikkelsen et al., 1999; Warncke et al., 1997, 2000), but not via subcutaneous or oral routes (Mikkelsen et al., 2000; Pedersen et al., 1998). Oral dextromethorphan did not modify brush‐evoked allodynia, barely decreased pinprick hyperalgesia (Ilkjaer et al., 1997), had inconsistent results on temporal summation (wind‐up) (Hugues, Rhodes, et al., 2002; Staud et al., 2005), and added to morphine did not modify results relative to morphine alone (Frymoyer et al., 2007). Non‐replicated reports described efficacy of acetaminophen (Jürgens et al., 2014), intravenous adenosine (Sjölund et al., 1999), glutamate receptor antagonist LY545694 (Petersen et al., 2014) and hyperbaric oxygen (Rasmussen et al., 2015). Pre‐emptive local infiltration of lidocaine postponed but not prevented 2HA (Dahl et al., 1993), and steroids yielded conflicting results, negative for topical clobetasol and iv dexamethasone (Pedersen et al., 1994; Werner et al., 2002) but positive for iv methylprednisolone (Stubhaug et al., 2007). NSAIDs did not reduce heat‐injury 2HA (Moiniche et al., 1993, 1994; Petersen et al., 1997; Warncke et al., 1996) except when administered intravenously (Stubhaug et al., 2007). Intravenous melatonin 10‐100mg and the cannabinoid analogue nabilone (tetrahydrocannabinol‐THC) were ineffective (Andersen, Poulsen, et al., 2015; Redmond et al., 2008).

Merits of classical thermode‐based procedures (47°C, 7 min) are the ease and speed of the technique, long duration of 2HA and relatively good consistency across studies. Disadvantages are the high cost of the thermode, the potential epidermal injuries if applied for long, and the inter‐subject variability of 2HA surface. The ‘brief induction’ techniques might be less prone to induce epidermal lesions. Late hyperpigmentation in the area may occur in 1%–2% of participants, and blistering in up to 25% when using thermodes 12.5 cm2 or larger (Dahl et al., 1993; Pedersen et al., 1998; Sjölund et al., 1999; Dirks et al., 2003), and even with smaller thermodes if temperature is pushed to 50°C (Dahl et al., 1993). Although very rare, severe thermal injury has been described following the use of malfunctioning, overheating contact thermodes (Springborg et al., 2016).

3.3. Ultraviolet‐induced inflammation techniques (Supporting Information Table A)

These models were developed in the mid‐1990s to induce inflammation‐related hyperalgesia in animals, then in humans (Benrath et al., 1995; Bickel et al., 1998). The technique is based on skin irradiation with a source of ultraviolet type B (UVB) at 290‐320nm. All protocols use the notion of “Minimal Erythema Dose” (MED), which is the minimal dose of irradiation to induce a visibly irritated red area (Hoffmann & Schmelz, 1999). MEDs are generally determined 1–7 days before the experiments, using five circular spots of 1.5cm with growing duration of irradiation from a calibrated UVB source (Modir & Wallace, 2010b). On the experimental day a single spot of 1.5–5 cm diameter is irradiated, in general at 3 MED, which provides better reliability than 1–2 MED (Bickel et al., 1998; Siebenga et al., 2019). Skin areas show no alterations immediately after UVB exposure, and neither spontaneous pain nor allodynia are described. An erythema develops at about 6 hr after irradiation and reaches maximum intensity after 12–36 hr. At this point primary hyperalgesia is a constant feature, while 2HA/DMA are inconsistent, with large variations in published reports (0–137 cm2, Supporting Information Table A). When they develop, 2HA/DMA attain their maximum 16–24 hr after irradiation and may last up to 4 days. Spatial amplification index in 13 papers providing quantitative data was 5.1 [95%CI 3–7.5]; however, at least seven studies failed to detect any significant 2HA outside the irradiated spot, and in two it was obtained only after heat rekindling (Eisenach et al., 2010; O’Neill et al., 2015) (Supporting Information Table A). Intra‐ and inter‐individual variation coefficients of 2HA area were reported to be 23% and 46%, respectively (Lorenzini et al., 2012). The rate of 2HA induction, when reported, ranged from 0% to 100% (Supporting Information Table A).

Drugs found effective to decrease UVB‐induced 2HA in controlled studies (Table 6) included NSAIDs (Eisenach et al., 2010; Maihöfner et al., 2007; Sycha et al., 2005), paracetamol/tramadol (Ortner et al., 2012) and systemic opioids (Gustorff, Hoechtl, et al., 2004). Topical lidocaine was effective if applied within the irradiated spot, but not when infused just outside it (Gustorff, et al., 2011; Rössler et al., 2013). Regional/transdermal opioids (Andresen, Staahl, et al., 2011), paracetamol/ketorolac (Lorenzini et al., 2011) and botulinum toxin (Sycha et al., 2006) failed to modify 2HA.

Gabapentin 600 mg (Gustorff, Hoechtl, et al., 2004) and Tetrahydrocannabinol/cannabidiol (THC/CBD) (Kraft et al., 2008), did not modify UVB‐related 2HA. One single study found a significant effect of benzodiazepines clobazam and clonazepam on UVB‐induced secondary hyperalgesia (Besson et al., 2015), but a nonspecific effect on vigilance could not be excluded.

The main qualities of UV‐B sensitization are the long duration of effects and the absence of ongoing pain. Limitations are the need to calibrate the MED at least 24 hr before sensitization, the long time needed for induction of symptoms, the possibility of hyperpigmentation in the irradiated spot in >50% of subjects, and even up to 3 years after exposure (Siebenga et al., 2019), and the inconsistent development of secondary hyperalgesia.

3.4. Models based on electrical stimulation

Nociceptive long‐term potentiation (LTP) leads to amplification of synaptic signals and is thought to be at the basis of central sensitization (Klein et al., 2004; Sandkühler et al., 2012) (Table 4). Spinal LTP can be induced in rodents by repeated high‐frequency bursts of electrical stimulation (~100 Hz: Liu & Sandkühler, 1997; Benrath et al., 2005), but also using lower frequencies at 1–10 Hz (Drdla & Sandkühler, 2008; Ikeda et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2015; Terman et al., 2001). Such intense input induces NMDA‐dependent intracellular Ca2+ increase in second‐order neurons and astrocytes, with release of brain‐derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and activation of purinergic glial receptors (Kim et al., 2015; Retamal et al., 2018; Sandkühler & Gruber‐Schoffnegger, 2012). All these mechanisms can entail signal amplification, but their relation with hyperalgesia remains imperfectly known. Here, we will distinguish studies using low‐frequency stimulation, typically with intradermal electrodes, and studies using high‐frequency stimuli via surface electrodes.

3.4.1. Percutaneous low frequency stimulation (LFS)

Originally described by Koppert et al., (2001), 21 studies used low‐frequency electric stimulation to elicit secondary hyperalgesia. A majority of studies used micro‐dialysis catheters or micro‐neurography needles with 4 mm anode‐cathode distance, stimulating at 0.5–5 Hz. No significant difference was detected when comparing 0.5–1 Hz versus 2Hz versus 5Hz in terms of area, duration or maintenance of 2HA (data from Table 4). LFS intensity is gradually increased during the first 15 min to reach pain intensity reports at 5–6/10, and then kept constant until the end of the experiment. This generally allows maintaining a continuous ongoing pain, upon which secondary hyperalgesia may be assessed. The average hyperalgesic area reported was 39.7 cm2 [95% CI: 30–49]. Higher ongoing pain was associated to larger 2HA areas (Table 4), which were in general similar to those under intradermal capsaicin (Geber et al., 2007). The delay to maximal effect was quite variable (range 2–40 min) and 2HA could be maintained for 1.5–5 hr if the stimulation was continuously applied during this period. Both hyperalgesia and allodynia abate in a few minutes after discontinuation of the inducing stimulus (Klede et al., 2003; Koppert et al., 2001). Six of 21 studies reported success rates in inducing 2HA, always at 95%–100%. Excellent test–retest reproducibility of stimulus–response functions (r = 0.7) was reported by Geber et al., (2007).

LFS‐induced hyperalgesia was significantly abated by systemic opioids including fentanyl, alfentanil, remifentanil and buprenorphine in eight controlled studies (Table 6). Remifentanil lessened 2HA during infusion but generated late hyperalgesia on the post‐infusion period (Lenz et al., 2011; Chu et al., 2012). Gabapentin 1200–2600 mg (Boyle et al., 2014; Segerdahl, 2006) and pregabalin 300 mg (Chizh et al., 2007) also decreased 2HA/DMA areas and intensities in this model, and one further study reported that 600 mg gabapentin increased the threshold for wind‐up to repetitive electrical stimulation (Enggaard et al., 2010). Systemic lidocaine was efficacious in two studies (Koppert et al., 2001; Seifert et al., 2009), as were the NSAIDs ketorolac and parecoxib in one single report (Lenz et al., 2001). Modest but significant effects of propofol, alone or in combination with remifentanil/ketamine, were also observed (Bandschapp et al., 2010; Nickel et al., 2016). Transcutaneous nerve stimulation (TENS‐like) at 20 Hz reduced LFS‐induced hyperalgesia in upper limbs (Nickel et al., 2011), but not at cranial sites (Reindl et al., 2016). LFS‐induced wind‐up was abated by the tricyclic antidepressant imipramine in one single study (Enggaard et al., 2001). Neither cholecystokinin (Pahl et al., 2003) nor the anti‐neurokinin‐1 drug aprepitant (Chizh et al., 2007) modified LFS‐hyperalgesia.

The low‐frequency electric model is easy to implement, well‐controlled and provides a large hyperalgesic area with an excellent rate of induction and without serious adverse events. Since this model entails both ongoing pain and 2HA, it may adequately mimic some clinical neuropathic conditions; however, the continuous background pain might hinder the specific assessment of hypersensitivity. Limitations are the slightly invasive nature of electrode placement, discomfort during induction, rapid fall of 2HA/DMA if the electrical stimulus is discontinued (Klede et al., 2003; Koppert et al., 2001), and the need to continuously adjust the stimulation intensity to maintain stable pain ratings (e.g. Boyle et al., 2014).

3.4.2. Cutaneous high‐frequency electrical stimulation (HFS)

High‐frequency stimuli via surface electrodes mimic injury‐induced high‐frequency discharge in altered axons, and are expected to induce central synaptic changes similar to those of real injury (Table 4). The concept was derived from results in animal recordings in nerve and spinal cord (see above), thus enhancing back‐translability. HFS shares many properties with the ID capsaicin model in terms of afferents activated, pain on induction and duration of effect (Henrich et al., 2015). Stimuli are usually delivered through a circular array of 10–16 pin electrodes with array diameter of 6–45 mm (0.3–16 cm2 area) designed to activate preferentially superficial nociceptive afferents. Possible influences of application surface and other parameters could not be estimated because of lack of data. Most studies used high‐frequency trains (100 Hz) of 1 s duration, repeated five times at 10‐s intervals, with intensity 10–20 times the electrical detection threshold to single pulses (EDT). One study (Klein et al., 2004) reported larger 2HA areas for 20 versus 10 EDT (56 cm2 versus. 38 cm2). Xia et al., (2016) reported similar pain amplification induced by 10, 100 and 200 Hz, but higher pain at induction for the 100 Hz model, while Van den Broeke et al., (2019) reported larger hyperalgesic areas to 42 Hz, relative to 100 Hz (80 cm2 vs. 50 cm2). Hyperalgesia starts rapidly, with maximum effect in 15–60 min, stable for 1–5 hr (Klein et al., 2004, 2006; Pfau et al., 2011; Xia et al., 2016). Two studies using a longer follow‐up reported recovery to baseline in ~24 hr (Klein, et al., 2008; Pfau et al., 2011). When available, responder rates were 80%–100% (Biurrun‐Manresa et al. 2018; ; Pfau et al., 2011). One open label, unblinded study failed to show significant effects of ketamine (0.25 mg/kg) on HFS‐induced 2HA/DMA (Klein et al., 2007).

The main qualities of the HFS model are rapidity and ease of induction and maintenance of 2HA for several hours, the relatively inexpensive equipment and ease of handling. No serious adverse events have been reported. Its main limitation is the unpleasantness of the stimulation (4–8/10) (Magerl et al., 2018; Pfau et al., 2011; Reitz et al., 2016). Because of the relative paucity of information on the behaviour of this model, including to medications, it is likely that our knowledge on its effects will evolve in the next years.

3.5. Less prevalent techniques to induce secondary hyperalgesia

Many other pain models have been described that are able to induce secondary hyperalgesia. They are less prevalent than those described above, for reasons including methodological difficulties, inconsistency of results or recent development. Here, we describe models that have provided sufficient data to allow at least a summary of their characteristics and possible practicality.

3.5.1. Procedures activating cold receptors (Supporting Information Table B)

Although cold hypersensitivity is a frequent symptom in patients with neuropathic pain, validated experimental models in humans are scarce. Two main experimental modalities of hyperalgesia induced by cold stimuli have been described, namely the topical application of menthol and the freeze injury model (the latter sharing also properties with burn‐injury and UVB models). Other substances such as mustard's oil and cinnamaldehyde also activate cold‐related receptors such as transient receptor potential ankyrin 1 (TRPA1) (Bandell et al., 2004).

The topical menthol hyperalgesia model

Menthol (C10 H20 O) is a cyclic terpene alcohol widely used in anti‐pruritic creams and nasal decongesting formulae, and has been employed as topical application to provoke cold hyperalgesia. Low concentrations (5%–10%) do not produce consistent pain changes (Green, 1992; Yosipovitch et al., 1996), but concentrations of 40% produce pain and local thermo‐mechanical primary hyperalgesia when applied topically (Wasner et al., 2004, Förster et al., 2016). Typical protocols use a 3 × 3 cm soaked gaze with a solution of 30%–40% menthol in 90% ethanol, covered by an adhesive film and applied to the skin during 20 min. Eleven reports using menthol were identified, but only six explicitly tested central sensitization via mechanical 2HA/DMA, and only three provided enough quantitative data. The average area of hyperalgesia was 34.91 cm2 [95% CI 18–51] with amplification ratio 3.88 [95% CI 2.04–5.7] (Suppl Table B). Response rates varied from nil to 100%. When 2HA developed, maximum effects were reached immediately and lasted up to 135 min. Sensitization could be prolonged by repeated kindling with cold stimuli (Andersen, Poulsen, et al., 2015).

Menthol‐induced 2HA developed inconsistently: it was systematic in two studies (Andersen, Poulsen, et al., 2015; Binder, et al., 2011); in the others, it developed in a subset of subjects (Namer et al., 2005; Wasner et al., 2004) or could not be measured at all (Hatem et al., 2006; Helfert et al., 2018). Changes in pain thresholds were found reproducible one week apart, but areas of pinprick hyperalgesia were not, and those of DMA could not be determined (Mahn et al., 2014). No controlled studies were identified on the effect of analgesics on menthol‐induced 2HA. Topical menthol was, on the contrary, able to reduce 2HA from cinnamaldehyde (Andersen, et al., 2015).

The main qualities of the menthol model are low discomfort, absence of adverse effects and the fact that cold hypersensitivity may mimic some clinical conditions such as oxaliplatin‐induced neuropathies (Forstenpointner et al. 2018). These advantages may be outweighed by the low success rate and the limited spatio‐temporal amplification.

The freeze injury hyperalgesia model

Freezing as a human pain model was first reported one Century ago (Lewis & Love, 1926), but its detailed assessment developed in the 1990s. Beise and colleagues (1998) used a small thermode frozen at −11°C through temperature separation in a Ranque–Hilsch tube. A simpler technique utilizes a copper cylinder of ~2 cm2 frozen at −28°C, applied on the anterior part of the forearm for 8–10 s (Chassaing et al., 2006; Kilo et al., 1994; Martin et al., 2019). This procedure was reported to produce a mean 2HA area of 26.85 cm2 [95% CI: 13.8–39.9] a surface amplification ratio of 15.23 [95% CI: 7.8–22.7], and a maximum effect 20 hr after application. Secondary hypearalgesia was systematic in three studies; it persisted up to 72 hr in one (Chassaing et al., 2006), and decreased or disappeared after 24h in the other two (Supporting Information Table B). DMA was absent in the two reports that tested it (Kilo et al., 1994; Lötsch & Angst, 2003). Freeze‐induced 2HA was half the size of that obtained with topical capsaicin, in the only study that contrasted both techniques (Kilo et al., 1994).

Three reports examined drug effects on freeze‐induced hyperalgesia. Dextromethorphan 30 mg exerted a significant effect on the change of mechanical thresholds within the 2HA region, without modifying the 2HA surface, pain thresholds or pupillary reactions (Martin et al., 2019). Oral ibuprofen (400 mg) increased pain thresholds within the 2HA area in one study (Chassaing et al., 2006), while acetaminophen (1,000 mg) had no effect on freeze‐induced pinprick hyperalgesia (Chassaing et al., 2006).

The main qualities of the freeze injury are ease of application, low discomfort and long duration of secondary hyperalgesia, making of it a promising technique for drug evaluation needing several testing days. Limitations are the slow development of hyperalgesia, which forces the experimenter to wait one full day between freeze application and testing, as well as the lack of brush‐induced allodynia and the tendency of 2HA to shrink toward the injured area. Serious adverse events have not been described.

Activation of TRPA1 receptors: Mustard oil and Cinnamaldehyde

TRPA1 receptors are activated at low temperatures close to cold pain (<17°C) but also by topical application of natural oils such as cinnamaldehyde and mustard oil (Bandell et al., 2004). The specificity of these receptors for cold stimuli remains debated (Weyer‐Menkhoff & Lötsch, 2018, 2019), as they have been also implicated in the perception of heat (e.g. Moparthi et al., 2016).

Mustard oil (Allyl isothiocyanate or AITC) has been extensively used in preclinical studies but only rarely as human model, and only five reports were identified. Topical application for 4–5 min induces strong pain almost immediately, and subsequent 2HA/DMA development was consistently obtained in the few reports available. Koltzenburg et al., (1992), Koltzenburg et al., (1994) induced DMA with mustard oil in 100% of 29 subjects, and could abolish it by blocking large myelinated afferents. 2HA developed inconstantly and was not systematically investigated. Highly variable areas of hypersensitivity across subjects were reported by Sjölund et al., (1999) who also showed reduction of 2HA area (but not DMA) by intravenous adenosine, and a more pronounced, but shorter lasting, 2HA to mustard oil relative to a ‘classical’ thermal‐induced model (47°C, 7 min). Andersen, Elberling, et al., (2017) reported enhanced 2HA/DMA with increasing concentrations from 10% to 50%, without further change at 90%.

Cinnamaldehyde, another powerful activator of TRPA1 receptors, was reported to induce 2HA in two studies (Andersen, et al., 2015; Namer et al., 2005), the latter with average area of pinprick hyperalgesia threefold the application surface (29.43 cm2 versus. 9 cm2). Co‐application of menthol significantly decreased intensity and 2HA area, which was attributed to a possible combination of segmental spinal inhibition and peripheral receptor‐mediated antagonism between TRPA1 and TRPM8 (Andersen, et al., 2015).

3.5.2. Incisional and pre‐incisional models

Following the description of a plantar incision model in rodents (Brennan et al., 1996), the procedure was translated to humans (Kawamata, Watanabe, et al., 2002). A 4‐mm‐long incision through skin, fascia and muscle in the volar forearm consistently entails 10–12 cm2 of pinprick hyperalgesia after 5–15 min, which becomes maximal at 1–2 hr and disappears over the next 6–72 hr (Fißmer et al., 2011; Kawamata, Takahashi, et al., 2002; Kawamata, Watanabe, et al., 2002). The model did not entail brush allodynia. In women, both the intensity and extent of hyperalgesia were found sensitive to the hormonal phases (Pogatzki‐Zahn et al., 2019). Systemic lidocaine previous to incision prevented 2HA, while neither subcutaneous nor systemic lidocaine reverted hyperalgesia once it was fully developed (Kawamata, Takahashi, et al., 2002; Kawamata, Watanabe, et al., 2002).

The incision model appears as a reliable method to induce central sensitization, but its invasive nature represents an obvious disadvantage towards other procedures. A model of non‐injurious sharp mechanical pain using a blade pressing on, but not entering the skin, was proposed to mimic incision‐induced hyperalgesia (Shabes et al., 2016). Although blade and incision‐induced pain descriptors were similar, the size, reproducibility and duration of 2HA to non‐invasive blade were not prominent compared with invasive models.

3.6. Models with inconsistent 2HA‐inducing properties

A number of other models with potential to induce 2HA/DMA have received less support from published evidence, or have not been reproduced after their initial description.

3.6.1. Nerve growth factor (NGF) injection

Nerve growth factor (NGF) is a neurotrophin with biological role in the development of small sensory neurons, and participates to the cascade of events leading to lesion‐related neuropathic pain (Khan & Smith, 2015). When injected intradermally, NGF evokes a long‐lasting sensitization of nociceptors with initial heat hypersensitivity, and delayed mechanical hyperalgesia peaking around 3 weeks later (Dyck et al., 1997; Petty et al. 1994; Rukwied et al., ,2010, 2013). Although 2HA and DMA have been occasionally described (Andresen, et al., 2011), in most studies hypersensitivity was primary, i.e. restricted to the application area (Dyck et al., 1997; Munkholm & Arendt‐Nielsen, 2017; Petty et al., 1994; Rukwied et al., 2010, 2013). Adding NGF to previous UVB irradiation did not influence the hyperalgesic effects (Vecchio, Finocchietti, et al., 2014; Vecchio, Petersen, et al., 2014), while UVB applied three weeks after NGF enhanced the hypersensitivity. NGF has been essentially used to assess muscle hypersensitivity without inducing inflammation.

3.6.2. Injection of hypertonic or acid saline

Injecting hypertonic saline into musculoskeletal structures induces neural firing in A‐delta and C‐nociceptive afferents (Graven‐Nielsen, 2006; Oda et al., 2018). This model is essentially used to mimic muscular or tendon‐related pain, but it has also been shown to induce superficial allodynia to brush or cold stimuli over skin regions surrounding the injection, and might reflect central sensitization (Nagi & Mahns, 2013; Oda et al., 2018; Samour et al., 2017). However, low consistency and concomitant muscle pain greatly decrease the possible impact of this procedure as a convenient 2HA model. A variant of the above considers muscle injection of acidic saline. The acid‐sensing ion channels (ASICs) activate nociceptors in low pH conditions, and acid infusion entails sustained pain behaviour without significant tissue damage (Sluka et al., 2001). Although repeated injections were reported to induce 2HA in animal models (Sluka et al., 2001, 2003) this has not been reproduced with repeated injections in humans (Ernberg et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2017).

3.6.3. Skin irritants, endotoxemia, ciguatoxin

Topical application of sodium lauryl sulphate, a skin irritant that releases pro‐inflammatory mediators, was reported to induce both primary and secondary hyperalgesia (Petersen et al., 2010). More robust secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical and cold stimuli was obtained through intradermal injection of endothelin‐1 (Hans et al., 2007), but neither of the two models has been consistently replicated. Endotoxemia via i.v. injection of Escherichia coli lipopolysaccharides induce robust visceral and musculoskeletal hyperalgesia (Benson et al., 2012; Wegner et al., 2014), and may have the potentiality of inducing cutaneous 2HA too (de Goeij et al., 2013), but this has not been specifically explored. Ciguatoxins that cause the ‘ciguatera’ condition produce a painful neuropathy characterized by strong cold allodynia (Zimmerman et al., 2013). Experimental injection of ciguatoxin has been used as a surrogate model of cold allodynia in humans (Eisenblätter et al., 2017) but its ability to induce reproducible central sensitization has not been established.

4. DISCUSSION

Although more than 15 different human models of secondary hyperalgesia have been described, four classes accounted for more than 90% of published reports. They were based on (1) capsaicin application or injection (reported in >2000 subjects); (2) thermal heat injury (~850 subjects); (3) ultraviolet‐B irradiation (~500 subjects), and (4) repetitive electrical stimuli (~550 subjects). As summarized in Table 5 and Supporting Information Fig. A, these models have different profiles in regard of timing of effects, pain intensity during induction, spatiotemporal amplification and proportion of responding subjects. This latter point was reported in less than 50% of studies, giving rise to a high reporting bias.

4.1. Differences, strengths and drawbacks of different models

Rather than absolute advantages or disadvantages, each of the principal models appears more or less adapted to different research questions and experimental designs. In what follows, models are discussed in terms of their success rate, timing, spatial and temporal amplification, pain during induction and response to drugs (Table 5).

4.1.1. Success rate

Albeit reported in a minority of studies, success in obtaining hyperalgesia reached 85%–90% for all principal models save the UVB procedure, where half of the accounts either failed to obtain sizeable 2HA areas, or defined them as “barely exceeding the irradiated spot” (Eisenach et al., 2010; Harrison et al., 2004; Morch et al., 2013; Seifert et al., 2008). This model has the particularity of not inducing pain on application. Since nociceptor activity is critical to initiate and maintain central sensitization from peripheral injury (LaMotte et al., 1991; Schmelz et al., 2000, 2009), failure to induce a sustained nociceptive barrage is a likely explanation of the difficulties to obtain 2HA/DMA in the UVB model (Bishop et al., 2009; O’Neill et al., 2015), which remains a robust human model of inflammatory pain, but appears of limited value for the specific study of central sensitization mechanisms (Gustorff et al., 2013).

4.1.2. Spatial and temporal amplification

In order to be useful in pharmacological studies, a model needs (1) to provide an area of 2HA large enough for repeated application of test stimuli, and (2) to induce effects that last long enough to cover peak plasma concentrations. Spatial amplification is obviously highest in models applied to small skin areas (ID‐capsaicin, LFS, HFS). Among the others, amplification ratios were double in topical heat/capsaicin (~18) than thermal injury models (~9), themselves being twice those from UVB models (~4) (Supporting Information Fig. A).

Temporal amplification (ratio between the duration of effect and of conditioning stimulus) is negligible for the “brief thermal” and LFS models, whose effects disappear a few minutes after discontinuation of the inducing stimulus, (Dirks et al., 2003; Hansen et al., 2017; Klede et al., 2003; Koppert et al., 2001). It is also minimal for isolated (non‐kindled) topical capsaicin, but reaches 2–10 fold induction times for ID capsaicin and heat‐injury models (Rasmussen et al., 2015; Simone et al., 1989). Maximal temporal amplification (>10‐fold conditioning times) is attained for intradermal and heat‐kindled capsaicin, UVB, freeze and HFS electric models (Table 5). Temporal amplification, however, cannot be dissociated from the time needed to develop 2HA symptoms, which separates ‘rapid’ from slow‐inducting models. Thus, intradermal capsaicin and HFS provide both a sizeable duration of hyperalgesia and an immediate onset of effects. Conversely, UVB and freeze‐injury models, although able to generate long hyperalgesic periods, only do so after a latent interval that may last one full day.

A brief duration of hypersensitivity may be especially bothersome when testing DMA, which has intrinsically shorter duration than pinprick hyperalgesia in all models (Geber et al., 2007; Gottrup et al., 2000; LaMotte et al., 1991; Magerl et al. 1998; Pfau et al. 2011; Warnacke et al. 1997). Providing new peripheral input by heating iteratively the skin allows obtaining stable 2HA/DMA during extended periods of time (Dirks et al., 2003; Modir & Wallace, 2010a; Petersen & Rowbotham, 1999). LFS electrical models (0.5–5 Hz) can maintain hyperalgesia for as long as the duration of electrical input; however, the duration of experiments is limited by painfulness of the background stimulus. On the other hand, models allowing a protracted hypersensitivity without pain at induction (UVB, freeze) have the lowest prevalence of 2HA/DMA, most probably because of the limited nociceptive activation they entail (see above).

4.1.3. Pain provoked during induction

Pain provoked during induction may influence subjects’ compliance and attrition rate. Intradermal capsaicin and electrical HFS models induce a very unpleasant stinging sensation, which may reach 7–9/10 on VAS but rapidly abates after stimulus application. On the other hand, the low‐frequency (LFS) electrical models use repetitive noxious stimuli during all the duration of the experiment, and necessarily induce a combination of ongoing pain and secondary hyperalgesia during minutes to hours (Bandschapp et al., 2010; Koppert et al., 2001; Nickel et al., 2011; Wehrfritz et al., 2016). Topical capsaicin and contact‐heat models produce secondary hyperalgesia with only moderate pain on application, which makes them attractive on the condition that the experiments do not exceed several hours. Although no pain at all is induced by freeze and UVB models, this advantage is mitigated by the difficulty to evoke hyperalgesia beyond the territory treated (Harrison et al. 2004; Koppert et al., 1999; O’Neill et al. 2015) and its tendency to shrink towards the irradiated or freezed skin (Chassaing et al., 2006; Eisenach et al., 2010).

4.2. Are human models useful surrogates of clinical hyperalgesia?

Human 2HA models can generate an enhanced nociceptive barrage and central sensitization symptoms that are similar to those of neuropathic pain. They cannot mimic the extensive metabolic changes due to neural lesions, nor are they able to model primary central damage responsible for spinal injury or post‐stroke pain. Their translational capacities are indirect, and their value as surrogate models of neuropathic hyperalgesia subject to debate (Van Amerongen et al., 2016; Aykanat et al., 2012; Samuelsson et al. 2011). The quality of models must therefore be validated not only by their capacity to reproduce clinical symptoms, but also by their response to drugs –i.e. they should be responsive to medications active on neuropathic hyperalgesia, and remain insensitive to those without clinical effect (predictive validity). The reverse translation potential of human procedures, i.e. their capacity to inform preclinical models on the adequate endopoints/biomarkers to be used confidently, depends on their ability to respond to pain‐relieving methods that are clinically useful.

4.2.1. Effects of drugs

As summarized in Table 6 and Figure 4, topical and i.d. capsaicin, heat‐injury and LFS electrical models consistently responded to classes of drugs that are clinically valuable for neuropathic pain while remaining largely insensitive to clinically ineffective approaches to central sensitization, including oral NMDA receptor antagonists and NSAIDs. For instance, capsaicin hyperalgesia responded in 73% of reports to drugs clinically accepted for neuropathic pain (gabapentinoids, TCAs, iv ketamine, iv lidocaine) and did not respond in 27% (d = 46; 95% CI 5–85; p < 0.05). Although with lower amount of evidence, the capsaicin model also responded to different formulations of clonidine (Eisenach, et al., 2002; Ragavendran et al., 2016), an alpha‐2 agonist used with success in refractory cases of neuropathic pain (Campbell et al., 2012; Schechtmann et al. 2010; Wrzosek et al., 2015).

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Graphical summary of the models’ responsiveness to major classes of drugs used for neuropathic pain. Studies were classed as “positive” (blue bars) if the drug affected significantly at least one output readout (e.g. 2HA area) versus control/placebo. The number of studies is indicated in abscissae; the number of subjects tested for each drug in negative and positive reports is noted besides each bar. Please note the convergent profile of capsaicin, heat‐injury and electrical models in their response to drugs acting on NP, which contrasts with the UVB profile, mainly reactive to anti‐inflammatory drugs

Drugs may abate secondary hyperalgesia via attenuation of afferent input, central hyperexcitability, or both, and models may help disclosing such mechanisms in humans. Gabapentinoids, intravenous lidocaine and ketamine acted on 2HA but did not affect physiological nociception, as reflected by pain thresholds in normal skin, suggesting a specific reduction of central hyperexcitability. Conversely, opioids and local anaesthetics reduced both secondary hyperalgesia and physiological nociception (Andersen et al., 1996; Ilkjaer et al., 1996; Eckhardt et al., 2000, Gottrup et al., 2000; Koppert et al., 2000; Warncke et al., 2000; Dirks et al., 2000; Dirks, Petersen, et al., 2002; Werner et al., 2001; Mathiesen et al., 2006, Wallace, et al., 2002, 2008; Petersen et al., 2014). The facts that opioids may be insufficient to suppress temporal summation, and that they influence hyperalgesia only when the dose permits also a reduction of acute pain, has been put forward to suggest that their action on 2HA is mainly driven by a reduction of afferent nociceptive input, rather than a specific effect on central mechanisms (Brennum et al., 1994; Eisenach et al., 1997; Warncke et al., 1997).

The pattern of response of UVB‐based hyperalgesia differed from that of capsaicin, thermal‐injury and LFS models. UVB 2HA did not respond to gabapentinoids but was sensitive to NSAIDs, which do not show effects on central sensitization in animals except under direct spinal administration (Malmberg & Yaksh, 1992). The surprising sensitivity to NSAIDs of the UVB model may be linked to its inflammatory nature, and the small afferent nociceptive barrage that it triggers. Insufficient ascending input may be inadequate to generate centrally sustained hyperalgesia, and the hyperalgesic state would subside if peripheral input is further reduced due to the peripheral action of NSAIDs such dissimilarities confirm that the UBV procedure is a good model for peripheral inflammation, but not a translational replica of central neuropathic hyperalgesia.

Both capsaicin and heat‐injury models remained insensitive to a number of drugs that worked in animals, but have no or very little efficacy in human neuropathic pain. These include benzodiazepines (Park et al., 1995; Vuilleumier et al., 2013), anti‐histaminics (Wang et al., 2008), lamotrigine (Petersen et al., 2003), T‐type calcium channel blockers (Wallace, et al., 2002), minocycline (Sumracki et al., 2012), melatonin (Andersen, Poulsen, et al., 2015), the anti‐NK1 aprepitant (Chizh et al., 2007), dextromethorphan as add‐on to morphine (Frymoyer et al., 2007), and still others. Also, no anti‐hyperalgesic response was obtained to cannabinoids in the studies reported so far (review De Vita et al., 2018), although one report described a specific decrease in unpleasantness, without changes in pain intensity (Lee et al., 2013).

Some first‐line drugs for human neuropathic pain such as tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) were only inconsistently active on human 2HA models: TCAs abated wind‐up to repetitive electrical stimuli (Enggaard et al., 2001), but failed to modify 2HA in two capsaicin studies (Eisenach et al. 1997; Wallace, Ridgeway, et al., 2002). The clinical effects of TCAs require a sustained treatment to allow recruitment of downstream mechanisms that cannot be tagged by acute models (Kremer et al., 2016). Adequate testing of the effect of these drugs would need several weeks of continuous treatment, difficult to implement in healthy subjects. These drugs might also target mechanisms that are only activated in chronic conditions and not modelled in volunteers.

Taken together, hypersensitivity from capsaicin, heat‐injury and LFS models responded to drugs clinically accepted for neuropathic pain (gabapentin/pregabalin, iv ketamine, iv lidocaine) in a greater proportion than to drugs with non‐clinical effect in NP, the difference being highly significant (see Table 6).

4.2.2. Methodological issues and controversies

Induction of central sensitization

The duration of inducing stimuli, their intensity/dosage and spatial extension, the bending force of testing filaments, the time of testing and various other parameters vary enormously from one study to another, and the same group of investigators can report on the “same” model using different standards. This reflects the lack of consensus on optimal procedures, and together with the variety of output variables hampers generalization of results. Some overall considerations may however be cautiously pondered, for instance that models inducing only mild effects may induce false positive results by responding to drugs with insufficient clinical activity. Relatively ‘soft’ models using low‐concentration topical capsaicin (0.075%–1%) responded to NMDA‐antagonists without proven clinical efficacy such as dextromethorphan or CHF3381 (Duedahl et al., 2005; Mathiesen et al., 2006), while only intravenous ketamine was able to counteract ‘strong’ surrogate 2HA models such as rekindled 47°C heat‐injury (compare Ilkjaer et al., 1996, 1997). It appears reasonable that pain models eliciting relatively intense effects may be more appropriate to detect drugs potentially useful in the clinics. It remains to be ascertained whether models eliciting too strong hyperalgesic reactions might also mask the effect of clinically useful agents (Scanlon et al. 2006).

Mode of assessment of central sensitization

The ‘optimal’ variable to reproduce clinical data and predict drug effects remains unsettled. Areas of hyperalgesia/allodynia have been the most frequently used readouts, and are often correlated with the evoked pain within the hyperalgesic region. However, these two variables can also be dissociated (Ando et al., 2000; Schifftner et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2009), and pain within the hyperalgesic region has been reported as more reliable than area size to tag clinically useful analgesia (Ando et al., 2000; Lötsch et al., 2020). Quantifying pain intensity is more subjective and prone to bias than measuring the area of hyperalgesia, which is performed without visual control from subjects (Jensen & Petersen, 2006). On the other hand, the area measured is greatly dependent on the subject's attention and the pressure exerted by the filament (Ringsted et al., 2015), and this may have strong consequences, especially in ‘soft’ models.

A further source of incertitude concerns whether static (pinprick) 2HA or dynamic (brush) allodynia are of equivalent value to predict drug efficacy. These two abnormal percepts result from different central and peripheral mechanisms, may not respond similarly to drugs, and are often dissociated in timing and intensity in both healthy subjects (Cervero et al., 1993; Gottrup et al., 2000; Witting et al., 2000) and neuropathic patients (Gottrup et al., 1998). In all models, pinprick 2HA tends to develop more consistently than DMA, which is often restricted to a smaller area, is less stable, lasts a shorter time and has less distinct borders (Dirks et al., 2003; Geber et al., 2007; Gottrup et al., 2000; LaMotte et al., 1991; Magerl et al., 1998; Pfau et al. 2011; Pöyhiä & Vainio, 2006; Wallace, et al., 2002; Warnacke et al. 1997). Yet, DMA interferes extensively with the patients’ common activities and is considered more troublesome in daily life than pinprick hyperalgesia (Yezierski & Hansson, 2018). Patients with neuropathic pain tend to fear moving, rather than static stimuli (Koltzenburg et al., 1992, 1994; Peyron et al., 1998), and brush‐evoked allodynia correlates with ongoing pain in patients with painful neuropathies (Koltzenburg et al., 1994; Rowbotham & Fields, 1996; Samuelsson et al., 2011), while this has not been shown for pinprick hyperalgesia. Failure to consistently induce DMA may therefore hinder the translational capacities of some experimental models: for instance, of 18 studies reporting significant antihyperalgesic effects of gabapentinoids on surrogate models, 10 failed to induce, report or modify dynamic allodynia.

Bias

Many uncontrolled sources of error apply to results of drug trials on hyperalgesia models. At variance with clinical settings, experimental studies often test drug efficacy with a single dose administered before the inducing stimulus. Blinding may not be feasible if subjects experience subjective effects when administered drugs (ketamine, gabapentinoids, lidocaine), and it would not be reasonable to dismiss a trial as of low quality because of the absence of blinding (Higgins et al., 2011). Dose of inducting agents, skin temperature, pre‐induction pain thresholds and timing of testing are sources of study variability that can hardly be controlled for (Hansen et al., 2017; Liu et al., 1998; Scanlon et al., 2006).

5. GENERAL CONCLUSIONS & HINTS FOR FUTURE STUDIES

More than a dozen human surrogate models have been published that mimic aspects of ongoing and evoked neuropathic pain. Despite a significant reporting bias, in particular regarding the percentage of responders and the respective effect on 2HA and DMA, six of these models have been tested in multiple laboratories, and five were found to reliably induce secondary hyperalgesia to pinprick. This may facilitate translation from rodent models (where hypersensitivity to von Frey monofilaments is a frequent readout) to humans using equivalent readouts. More important, crucial benefits should be obtained from reverse translation, whereby preclinical models will take advantage of biomarkers that have proven sensitive in human beings. Failure to consistently elicit dynamic allodynia is a yet unsolved drawback, which may hinder the models’ translational capacities. Whether the areas of hypersensitivity or the pain intensity within these areas should be preferred to model NP symptoms and quantify analgesia remains debatable. For four models, pharmacological profiles have been obtained in sufficient detail to verify similarity to some clinical conditions. Intradermal and high‐dose capsaicin, heat‐injury and LFS models responded in significantly higher proportion to clinically anti‐hyperalgesic drugs than to drugs without proven clinical value, and may be relevant to mimic neuropathic hyperalgesia. The UVB model appears biased towards inflammatory peripheral mechanisms with little contribution of central sensitization. In summary, while it is clearly not possible to model a disease such as neuropathic pain in healthy subjects, there is a sufficient range of validated and easy to use models of key mechanisms and symptoms. Future essays on drug development for neuropathic pain conditions should use them in order to close the translation gap.

Of note, although the initial automatized key‐word based search returned >1,500 papers, this figure became drastically reduced upon multi‐level inspection for relevance. In parallel, automatic search failed to identify a substantial number of reports where terms such as “allodynia”, “hyperalgesia” or “model” were present in the main text but absent from title, abstract or keywords. This may underscore the importance of complementary manual search from bibliographic lists, review papers and grey literature to maximize the number of relevant contributions when dealing with complex topics –and for future researchers looking to update or expand on this review.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest regarding this work, which received support from the European Union Project EU/EFPIA/Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking (IMI‐PAINCARE), grant n° 777,500.

The statements and opinions presented here reflect the author's view and neither IMI nor the European Union, EFPIA, or any Associated Partners are responsible for any use that may be made of the information contained therein. www.imi.europa.eu; www.imi‐paincare.eu. All authors had full access to all of the study data and the corresponding author had the final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Supporting information

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Quesada C, Kostenko A, Ho I, et al. Human surrogate models of central sensitization: A critical review and practical guide. Eur J Pain. 2021;25:1389–1428. 10.1002/ejp.1768

References

  1. Ali, Z., Meyer, R. A., & Campbell, J. N. (1996). Secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical but not heat stimuli following a capsaicin injection in hairy skin. Pain, 68, 401–411. 10.1016/s0304-3959(96)03199-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen, H. H., Elberling, J., Sharma, N., Hauberg, L. E., Gazerani, P., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2017). Histaminergic and non‐histaminergic elicited itch is attenuated in capsaicin‐evoked areas of allodynia and hyperalgesia: A healthy volunteer study. European Journal of Pain, 21, 1098–1109. 10.1002/ejp.1013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersen, H. H., Gazerani, P., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2016). High‐concentration l‐menthol exhibits counter‐irritancy to neurogenic inflammation, thermal and mechanical hyperalgesia caused by trans‐cinnamaldehyde. The Journal of Pain, 17, 919–929. 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersen, H. H., Poulsen, J. N., Uchida, Y., Nikbakht, A., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Gazerani, P. (2015). Cold and L‐menthol‐induced sensitization in healthy volunteers—a cold hypersensitivity analogue to the heat/capsaicin model. Pain, 156, 880–889. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andersen, L. P., Gögenur, I., Fenger, A. Q., Petersen, M. C., Rosenberg, J., & Werner, M. U. (2015). Analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of melatonin in a human inflammatory pain model: A randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, three‐arm crossover study. Pain, 156, 2286–2294. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Andersen, O. K., Felsby, S., Nicolaisen, L., Bjerring, P., Jensen, T. S., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (1996). The effect of Ketamine on stimulation of primary and secondary hyperalgesic areas induced by capsaicin–a double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, human experimental study. Pain, 66, 51–62. 10.1016/0304-3959(96)02995-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ando, K., Wallace, M. S., Braun, J., & Schulteis, G. (2000). Effect of oral mexiletine on capsaicin‐induced allodynia and hyperalgesia: A double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study. Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, 25, 468–474. 10.1097/00115550-200009000-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Andresen, T., Lunden, D., Drewes, A. M., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2011). Pain sensitivity and experimentally induced sensitisation in red haired females. Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 2, 3–6. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2010.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Andresen, T., Nilsson, M., Nielsen, A. K., Lassen, D., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Drewes, A. M. (2013). Intradermal injection with nerve growth factor: A reproducible model to induce experimental allodynia and hyperalgesia. Pain Practice, 16, 12–23. 10.1111/papr.12267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Andresen, T., Staahl, C., Oksche, A., Mansikka, H., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Drewes, A. (2011). Effect of transdermal opioids in experimentally induced superficial, deep and hyperalgesic pain: Tissue differentiated effects of opioids. British Journal of Pharmacology, 164, 934–945. 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01180.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Arendt‐Nielsen, L., Harris, S., Whiteside, G. T., Hummel, M., Knappenberger, T., O'Keefe, S., Kapil, R., & Kyle, D. (2016). A randomized, double‐blind, positive‐controlled, 3‐way cross‐over human experimental pain study of a TRPV1 antagonist (V116517) in healthy volunteers and comparison with preclinical profile. Pain, 157, 2057–2067. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Arendt‐Nielsen, L., Morlion, B., Perrot, S., Dahan, A., Dickenson, A., Kress, H. G., Wells, C., Bouhassira, D., & Mohr Drewes, A. (2018). Assessment and manifestation of central sensitisation across different chronic pain conditions. European Journal of Pain, 22, 216–241. 10.1002/ejp.1140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Arout, C. A., Perrino, A. C., Jr, Ralevski, E., Acampora, G., Koretski, J., Limoncelli, D., Newcomb, J., & Petrakis, I. L. (2016). Effect of intravenous ethanol on capsaicin‐induced hyperalgesia in human subjects. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 40, 1425–1429. 10.1111/acer.13095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Aykanat, V., Gentgall, M., Briggs, N., Williams, D., Yap, S., & Rolan, P. (2012). Intradermal capsaicin as a neuropathic pain model in patients with unilateral sciatica. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 73, 37–45. 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2011.04059.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bandell, M., Story, G. M., Hwang, S. W., Viswanath, V., Eid, S. R., Petrus, M. J., Earley, T. J., & Patapoutian, A. (2004). Noxious cold ion channel TRPA1 is activated by pungent compounds and bradykinin. Neuron, 41, 849–857. 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00150-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bandschapp, O., Filitz, J., Ihmsen, H., Berset, A., Urwyler, A., Koppert, W., & Ruppen, W. (2010). Analgesic and antihyperalgesic properties of propofol in a human pain model. Anesthesiology, 113, 421–428. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181e33ac8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Barbaresi, P., Spreafico, R., Frassoni, C., & Rustioni, A. (1986). GABAergic neurons are present in the dorsal column nuclei but not in the ventroposterior complex of rats. Brain Research, 382, 305–326. 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91340-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Baron, R., Wasner, G., Borgstedt, R., Hastedt, E., Schulte, H., Binder, A., Kopper, F., Rowbotham, M., Levine, J. D., & Fields, H. L. (1999). Effect of sympathetic activity on capsaicin‐evoked pain, hyperalgesia, and vasodilatation. Neurology, 52, 923–932. 10.1212/WNL.52.5.923 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Baumann, T. K., Simone, D. A., Shain, C. N., & LaMotte, R. H. (1991). Neurogenic hyperalgesia: The search for the primary cutaneous afferent fibers that contribute to capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia. Journal of Neurophysiology, 66, 212–227. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Baumgärtner, U., Magerl, W., Klein, T., Hopf, H. C., & Treede, R. D. (2002). Neurogenic hyperalgesia versus painful hypoalgesia: Two distinct mechanisms of neuropathic pain. Pain, 96, 141–151. 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00438-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bautista, D., & Julius, D. (2008). Fire in the hole: Pore dilation of the capsaicin receptor TRPV1. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 528–529. 10.1038/nn0508-528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Beise, R. D., Carstens, E., & Kohllöffel, L. U. (1998). Psychophysical study of stinging pain evoked by brief freezing of superficial skin and ensuing short‐lasting changes in sensations of cool and cold pain. Pain, 74, 275–286. 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00179-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Benrath, J., Brechtel, C., Stark, J., & Sandkühler, J. (2005). Low dose of S (+)‐ketamine prevents long‐term potentiation in pain pathways under strong opioid analgesia in the rat spinal cord in vivo. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 95, 518–523. 10.1093/bja/aei215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Benrath, J., Eschenfelder, C., Zimmermann, M., & Gillardon, F. (1995). Calcitonin gene‐related peptide, substance P and nitric oxide are involved in cutaneous inflammation following ultraviolet irradiation. European Journal of Pharmacology, 293, 87–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Benson, S., Kattoor, J., Wegner, A., Hammes, F., Reidick, D., Grigoleit, J. S., Engler, H., Oberbeck, R., Schedlowski, M., & Elsenbruch, S. (2012). Acute experimental endotoxemia induces visceral hypersensitivity and altered pain evaluation in healthy humans. Pain, 153, 794–799. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Berkley, K. J. (1997). Sex differences in pain. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 371–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Besson, M., Matthey, A., Daali, Y., Poncet, A., Vuilleumier, P., Curatolo, M., Zeilhofer, H. U., & Desmeules, J. (2015). GABAergic modulation in central sensitization in humans: A randomized placebo‐controlled pharmacokinetic‐pharmacodynamic study comparing clobazam with clonazepam in healthy volunteers. Pain, 156, 397–404. 10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460331.33385.e8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Bickel, A., Dorfs, S., Schmelz, M., Forster, C., Uhl, W., & Handwerker, H. O. (1998). Effects of antihyperalgesic drugs on experimentally induced hyperalgesia in man. Pain, 76, 317–325. 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00062-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Binder, A., Stengel, M., Klebe, O., Wasner, G., & Baron, R. (2011). Topical high‐concentration (40%) menthol—somatosensory profile of a human surrogate pain model. The Journal of Pain, 12, 764–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Bishop, T., Ballard, A., Holmes, H., Young, A. R., & McMahon, S. B. (2009). Ultraviolet‐B induced inflammation of human skin: Characterisation and comparison with traditional models of hyperalgesia. European Journal of Pain, 2009(13), 524–532. 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Biurrun, M. J., Kæseler Andersen, O., Mouraux, A., & van den Broeke, E. N. (2018). High frequency electrical stimulation induces a long‐lasting enhancement of event‐related potentials but does not change the perception elicited by intra‐epidermal electrical stimuli delivered to the area of increased mechanical pinprick sensitivity. PLoS One, 13, e0203365. 10.1371/journal.pone.0203365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Boyle, Y., Fernando, D., Kurz, H., Miller, S. R., Zucchetto, M., & Storey, J. (2014). The effect of a combination of gabapentin and donepezil in an experimental pain model in healthy volunteers: Results of a randomized controlled trial. Pain, 155, 2510–2516. 10.1016/j.pain.2014.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Bradley, C., Perchet, C., Lelekov‐Boissard, T., Magnin, M., & Garcia‐Larrea, L. (2016). Not an aspirin: No evidence for acute anti‐nociception to laser‐evoked pain after motor cortex rTMS in healthy humans. Brain Stimulation, 9, 48–57. 10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Brennan, T. J., Vandermeulen, E. P., & Gebhart, G. F. (1996). Characterization of a rat model of incisional pain. Pain, 64, 493–501. 10.1016/0304-3959(95)01441-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Brennum, J., Dahl, J. B., Moiniche, S., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (1994). Quantitative sensory examination of epidural anaesthesia and analgesia in man: Effects of pre‐ and post‐traumatic morphine on hyperalgesia. Pain, 59, 261–271. 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90079-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Brennum, J., Kaiser, F., & Dahl, J. B. (2001). Effect of naloxone on primary and secondary hyperalgesia induced by the human burn injury model. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 45, 954–960. 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2001.450806.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Campbell, C. M., Bounds, S. C., Simango, M. B., Witmer, K. R., Campbell, J. N., Edwards, R. R., Haythornthwaite, J. A., & Smith, M. T. (2011). Self‐reported sleep duration associated with distraction analgesia, hyperemia, and secondary hyperalgesia in the heat‐capsaicin nociceptive model. European Journal of Pain, 15, 561–567. 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.11.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Campbell, C. M., Kipnes, M. S., Stouch, B. C., Brady, K. L., Kelly, M., Schmidt, W. K., Petersen, K. L., Rowbotham, M. C., & Campbell, J. N. (2012). Randomized control trial of topical clonidine for treatment of painful diabetic neuropathy. Pain, 153, 1815–1823. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Caterina, M. J., Schumacher, M. A., Tominaga, M., Rosen, T. A., Levine, J. D., & Julius, D. (1997). The capsaicin receptor: A heat‐activated ion channel in the pain pathway. Nature, 389, 816–824. 10.1038/39807 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cavallone, L., Frey, K., Montana, M., Joyal, J., Regina, K., Petersen, K., & Gereau, R. (2013). Reproducibility of the heat/capsaicin skin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Journal of Pain Research, 6, 771–784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Cayrol, T., Pitance, L., Roussel, N., Mouraux, A., & van den Broeke, E. N. (2018). No evidence of widespread mechanical pressure hyperalgesia after experimentally induced central sensitization through skin nociceptors. Pain reports, 3, e691. 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000691 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Cervero, F., Gilbert, R., Hammond, R. G., & Tanner, J. (1993). Development of secondary hyperalgesia following non‐painful thermal stimulation of the skin: A psychophysical study in man. Pain, 54, 181–189. 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90207-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Chapman, L. F., Dingman, H. F., & Ginzberg, S. P. (1965). Failure of systemic analgesic agents to alter the absolute sensory threshold for the simple detection of pain. Brain, 88, 1011–1022. 10.1093/brain/88.5.1011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Chassaing, C., Schmidt, J., Eschalier, A., Cardot, J. M., & Dubray, C. (2006). Hyperalgesia induced by cutaneous freeze injury for testing analgesics in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 61, 389–397. 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2006.02582.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Chizh, B. A., Dusch, M., Puthawala, M., Schmelz, M., Cookson, L. M., Martina, R., Brown, J., & Koppert, W. (2004). The effect of intravenous infusion of adenosine on electrically evoked hyperalgesia in a healthy volunteer model of central sensitization. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 99, 816–822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Chizh, B. A., Göhring, M., Tröster, A., Quartey, G. K., Schmelz, M., & Koppert, W. (2007). Effects of oral pregabalin and aprepitant on pain and central sensitization in the electrical hyperalgesia model in human volunteers. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 98, 246–254. 10.1093/bja/ael344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Chu, L. F., Cun, T., Ngai, L. K., Kim, J. E., Zamora, A. K., Young, C. A., Angst, M. S., & Clark, D. J. (2012). Modulation of remifentanil‐induced postinfusion hyperalgesia by the β‐blocker propranolol in humans. Pain, 153, 974–981. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Dahl, J. B., Brennum, J., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., Jensen, T. S., & Kehlet, H. (1993). The effect of pre‐ versus postinjury infiltration with lidocaine on thermal and mechanical hyperalgesia after heat injury to the skin. Pain, 53, 43–51. 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90054-S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. de Goeij, M., van Eijk, L. T., Vanelderen, P., Wilder‐Smith, O. H., Vissers, K. C., van der Hoeven, J. G., Kox, M., Scheffer, G. J., & Pickkers, P. (2013). Systemic inflammation decreases pain threshold in humans in vivo. PLoS One, 8, e84159. 10.1371/journal.pone.0084159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Denayer, T., Stöhr, T., & VanRoy, M. (2014). Animal models in translational medicine: Validation and prediction. New Horizons in Translational Medicine, 2, 5–11. 10.1016/j.nhtm.2014.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. De Vita, M. J., Moskal, D., Maisto, S. A., & Ansell, E. B. (2018). Association of cannabinoid administration with experimental pain in healthy adults: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 1118–1127. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Di Stefano, G., La Cesa, S., Biasiotta, A., Leone, C., Pepe, A., Cruccu, G., & Truini, A. (2012). Laboratory tools for assessing neuropathic pain. Neurological Sciences, 33, 5–7. 10.1007/s10072-012-1033-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Diener, S. A., Breimhorst, M., Vogt, T., Krämer, H. H., Drummond, P. D., Geber, C., & Birklein, F. (2017). Differential effect of Incobotulinumtoxin A on pain, neurogenic flare and hyperalgesia in human surrogate models of neurogenic pain. European Journal of Pain, 21, 1326–1335. 10.1002/ejp.1031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Dirks, J., Fabricius, P., Petersen, K. L., Rowbotham, M. C., & Dahl, J. B. (2000). The effect of systemic lidocaine on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 91, 967–972. 10.1097/00000539-200010000-00037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Dirks, J., Møiniche, S., Hilsted, K. L., & Dahl, J. B. (2002). Mechanisms of postoperative pain: Clinical indications for a contribution of central neuronal sensitization. Anesthesiology, 97, 1591–1596. 10.1097/00000542-200212000-00035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Dirks, J., Petersen, K. L., & Dahl, J. B. (2003). The heat/capsaicin sensitization model: A methodologic study. The Journal of Pain, 4, 122–128. 10.1054/jpai.2003.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Dirks, J., Petersen, K. L., Rowbotham, M. C., & Dahl, J. B. (2001). Effect of systemic adenosine on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, 26, 414–419. 10.1097/00115550-200109000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Dirks, J., Petersen, K. L., Rowbotham, M. C., & Dahl, J. B. (2002). Gabapentin suppresses cutaneous hyperalgesia following heat‐capsaicin sensitization. Anesthesiology, 97, 102–107. 10.1097/00000542-200207000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ditre, J. W., Zale, E. L., LaRowe, L. R., Kosiba, J. D., & De Vita, M. J. (2018). Nicotine deprivation increases pain intensity, neurogenic inflammation, and mechanical hyperalgesia among daily tobacco smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 578–589. 10.1037/abn0000353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Doll, R. J., van Amerongen, G., Hay, J. L., Groeneveld, G. J., Veltink, P. H., & Buitenweg, J. R. (2016). Responsiveness of electrical nociceptive detection thresholds to capsaicin (8 %)‐induced changes in nociceptive processing. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2505–2514. 10.1007/s00221-016-4655-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Drdla, R., & Sandkühler, J. (2008). Long‐term potentiation at C‐fibre synapses by low‐level presynaptic activity in vivo. Molecular Pain, 4, 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Duedahl, T. H., Dirks, J., Petersen, K. B., Kamilla, B., Romsing, J., Larsen, N.‐E., & Dahl, J. B. (2005). Intravenous dextromethorphan to human volunteers: Relationship between pharmacokinetics and anti‐hyperalgesic effect. Pain, 113, 360–368. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Dyck, P. J., Peroutka, S., Rask, C., Burton, E., Baker, M. K., Lehman, K. A., Gillen, D. A., Hokanson, J. L., & O’Brien, P. C. (1997). Intradermal recombinant human nerve growth factor induces pressure allodynia and lowered heat‐pain threshold in humans. Neurology, 48, 501–505. 10.1212/WNL.48.2.501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Eckhardt, K., Ammon, S., Hofmann, U., Riebe, A., Gugeler, N., & Mikus, G. (2000). Gabapentin enhances the analgesic effect of morphine in healthy volunteers. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 91, 185–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Eisenach, J. C., Curry, R., & Hood, D. D. (2002). Dose response of intrathecal adenosine in experimental pain and allodynia. Anesthesiology, 97, 938–942. 10.1097/00000542-200210000-00028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Eisenach, J. C., Curry, R., Tong, C., Houle, T. T., & Yaksh, T. L. (2010). Effects of intrathecal ketorolac on human experimental pain. Anesthesiology, 112, 1216–1224. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181d94d8b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Eisenach, J. C., Hood, D. D., & Curry, R. (1998). Intrathecal, but not intravenous, clonidine reduces experimental thermal or capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia in normal volunteers. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 87, 591–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Eisenach, J. C., Hood, D. D., & Curry, R. (2000). Relative potency of epidural to intrathecal clonidine differs between acute thermal pain and capsaicin‐induced allodynia. Pain, 84, 57–64. 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00181-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Eisenach, J. C., Hood, D. D., & Curry, R. (2002). Preliminary efficacy assessment of intrathecal injection of an American formulation of adenosine in humans. Anesthesiology, 96, 29–34. 10.1097/00000542-200201000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Eisenach, J. C., Hood, D. D., Curry, R., & Tong, C. (1997). Alfentanil, but not amitriptyline, reduces pain, hyperalgesia, and allodynia from intradermal injection of capsaicin in humans. Anesthesiology, 86, 1279–1287. 10.1097/00000542-199706000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Eisenblätter, A., Lewis, R., Dörfler, A., Forster, C., & Zimmermann, K. (2017). Brain mechanisms of abnormal temperature perception in cold allodynia induced by ciguatoxin. Annals of Neurology, 81, 104–116. 10.1002/ana.24841 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Enax‐Krumova, E. K., Pohl, S., Westermann, A., & Maier, C. (2017). Ipsilateral and contralateral sensory changes in healthy subjects after experimentally induced concomitant sensitization and hypoesthesia. BMC Neurology, 17(1), 60. 10.1186/s12883-017-0839-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Enggaard, T. P., Mikkelsen, S. S., Zwisler, S. T., Klitgaard, N. A., & Sindrup, S. H. (2010). The effects of gabapentin in human experimental pain models. Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 1, 143–148. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2010.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Enggaard, T. P., Poulsen, L., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., Hansen, S. H., Bjørnsdottir, I., Gram, L. F., & Sindrup, S. H. (2001). The analgesic effect of codeine as compared to imipramine in different human experimental pain models. Pain, 92, 277–282. 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00267-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ernberg, M., Castrillon, E. E., Ghafouri, B., Larsson, B., Gerdle, B., List, T., & Svensson, P. (2013). Experimental myalgia induced by repeated infusion of acidic saline into the human masseter muscle does not cause the release of algesic substances. European Journal of Pain, 17, 539–550. 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00216.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Evrard, H. C., Logothetis, N. K., & Craig, A. D. (2014). Modular architectonic organization of the insula in the macaque monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 522, 64–97. 10.1002/cne.23436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Fißmer, I., Klein, T., Magerl, W., Treede, R. D., Zahn, P. K., & Pogatzki‐Zahn, E. M. (2011). Modality‐specific somatosensory changes in a human surrogate model of postoperative pain. Anesthesiology, 115, 387–397. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318219509e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Forstenpointner, J., Oberlojer, V. C., Naleschinski, D., Höper, J., Helfert, S. M., Binder, A., Gierthmühlen, J., & Baron, R. (2018). A‐fibers mediate cold hyperalgesia in patients with oxaliplatin‐induced neuropathy. Pain Practice, 18, 758–767. 10.1111/papr.12670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Förster, M., Helfert, S., Dierschke, R., Großkopf, M., Hüllemann, P., Keller, T., Baron, R., & Binder, A. (2016). Evaluation of the antihyperalgesic effect of tapentadol in two human evoked pain models – the TapCapMentho pilot trial. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 17, 1717–1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Frymoyer, A. R., Rowbotham, M. C., & Petersen, K. L. (2007). Placebo‐controlled comparison of a morphine/dextromethorphan combination with morphine on experimental pain and hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. Journal of the American Pain Society, 8, 19–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Gannon, N. P., Lambalot, E. L., & Vaughan, R. A. (2016). The effects of capsaicin and capsaicinoid analogs on metabolic molecular targets in highly energetic tissues and cell types. BioFactors, 42, 229–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Gazerani, P., Andersen, O. K., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2005a). A human experimental capsaicin model for trigeminal sensitization. Gender‐specific Differences. Pain, 118, 155–163. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Gazerani, P., Andersen, O. K., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2007). Site‐specific, dose‐dependent, and sex‐related responses to the experimental pain model induced by intradermal injection of capsaicin to the foreheads and forearms of healthy humans. Journal of Oral and Facial Pain, 21, 289–302. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Gazerani, P., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2005b). The impact of ethnic differences in response to capsaicin‐induced trigeminal sensitization. Pain, 117, 223–229. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Gazerani, P., Staahl, C., Drewes, A. M., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2006). The effects of botulinum toxin type A on capsaicin‐evoked pain, flare, and secondary hyperalgesia in an experimental human model of trigeminal sensitization. Pain, 122, 315–325. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Geber, C., Fondel, R., Krämer, H. H., Rolke, R., Treede, R.‐D., Sommer, C., & Birklein, F. (2007). Psychophysics, flare, and neurosecretory function in human pain models: Capsaicin versus electrically evoked pain. The Journal of Pain, 8, 503–514. 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Gottrup, H., Hansen, P. O., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Jensen, T. S. (2000). Differential effects of systemically administered ketamine and lidocaine on dynamic and static hyperalgesia induced by intradermal capsaicin in humans. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 84, 155–162. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bja.a013396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Gottrup, H., Juhl, G., Kristensen, A. D., Lai, R., Chizh, B. A., Brown, J., Bach, F. W., & Jensen, T. S. (2004). Chronic oral gabapentin reduces elements of central sensitization in human experimental hyperalgesia. Anesthesiology, 101, 1400–1408. 10.1097/00000542-200412000-00021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Gottrup, H., Kristensen, A. D., Bach, F. W., & Jensen, T. S. (2003). Aftersensations in experimental and clinical hypersensitivity. Pain, 103, 57–64. 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00415-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Gottrup, H., Nielsen, J., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Jensen, T. S. (1998). The relationship between sensory thresholds and mechanical hyperalgesia in nerve injury. Pain, 75, 321–329. 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00011-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Graven‐Nielsen, T. (2006). Fundamentals of muscle pain, referred pain, and deep tissue hyperalgesia. Scandinavian Journal of Rheumatology. Supplement, 122, 1–43. 10.1080/03009740600865980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Green, B. G. (1992). The sensory effects of l‐menthol on human skin. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 9, 235–244. 10.3109/08990229209144774 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Gustafsson, H., Akesson, J., Lau, C. L., Williams, D., Miller, L., Yap, S., & Rolan, P. (2009). A comparison of two formulations of intradermal capsaicin as models of neuropathic pain in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 68, 511–517. 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2009.03489.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Gustorff, B., Anzenhofer, S., Sycha, T., Lehr, S., & Kress, H. G. (2004). The sunburn pain model: The stability of primary and secondary hyperalgesia over 10 hours in a crossover setting. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 98, 173–177. 10.1213/01.ane.0000093224.77281.a5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Gustorff, B., Hauer, D., Thaler, J., Seis, A., & Draxler, J. (2011). Antihyperalgesic efficacy of 5% lidocaine medicated plaster in capsaicin and sunburn pain models–two randomized, double‐blinded, placebo‐controlled crossover trials in healthy volunteers. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 12, 2781–2790. 10.1517/14656566.2011.601868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Gustorff, B., Hoechtl, K., Sycha, T., Felouzis, E., Lehr, S., & Kress, H. G. (2004). The effects of remifentanil and gabapentin on hyperalgesia in a new extended inflammatory skin pain model in healthy volunteers. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 98, 401–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Gustorff, B., Sycha, T., Lieba‐Samal, D., Rolke, R., Treede, R. D., & Magerl, W. (2013). The pattern and time course of somatosensory changes in the human UVB sunburn model reveal the presence of peripheral and central sensitization. Pain, 154, 586–597. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.12.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Hammer, N. A., Lillesø, J., Pedersen, J. L., & Kehlet, H. (1999). Effect of riluzole on acute pain and hyperalgesia in humans. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 82, 718–722. 10.1093/bja/82.5.718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Hans, G., Deseure, K., Robert, D., & De Hert, S. (2007). Neurosensory changes in a human model of endothelin‐1 induced pain: A behavioral study. Neuroscience Letters, 418, 117–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Hansen, M. S., Asghar, M. S., Wetterslev, J., Pipper, C. B., Mårtensson, J., Becerra, L., Christensen, A., Nybing, J. D., Havsteen, I., Boesen, M., & Dahl, J. B. (2018). The association between areas of secondary hyperalgesia and volumes of the caudate nuclei and other pain relevant brain structures‐A 3‐tesla MRI study of healthy men. PLoS One, 13, e0201642. 10.1371/journal.pone.0201642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Hansen, M. S., Wetterslev, J., Pipper, C. B., Asghar, M. S., & Dahl, J. B. (2017). Heat pain detection threshold is associated with the area of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization: A study of healthy male volunteers. Journal of Pain Research, 10, 265–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Hansen, M. S., Wetterslev, J., Pipper, C. B., Østervig, R., Asghar, M. S., & Dahl, J. B. (2016). The area of secondary hyperalgesia following heat stimulation in healthy male volunteers: Inter‐ and intra‐individual variance and reproducibility. PLoS One, 11, e0155284. 10.1371/journal.pone.0155284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Harding, L. M., Murphy, A., Kinnman, E., & Baranowski, A. P. (2001). Characterization of secondary hyperalgesia produced by topical capsaicin jelly–a new experimental tool for pain research. European Journal of Pain, 5, 363–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Harrison, G. I., Young, A. R., & McMahon, S. B. (2004). Ultraviolet radiation‐induced inflammation as a model for cutaneous hyperalgesia. The Journal of Investigative Dermatology, 122, 183–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Hatem, S., Attal, N., Willer, J.‐C., & Bouhassira, D. (2006). Psychophysical study of the effects of topical application of menthol in healthy volunteers. Pain, 122, 190–196. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.01.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Helfert, S., Reimer, M., Barnscheid, L., Hüllemann, P., Rengelshausen, J., Keller, T., Baron, R., & Binder, A. (2018). Impact of suggestion on the human experimental model of cold hyperalgesia after topical application of high‐concentration menthol [40%]. European Journal of Pain, 22, 1517–1527. 10.1002/ejp.1240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Henrich, F., Magerl, W., Klein, T., Greffrath, W., & Treede, R. D. (2015). Capsaicin‐sensitive C‐ and A‐fibre nociceptors control long‐term potentiation‐like pain amplification in humans. Brain, 138, 2505–2520. 10.1093/brain/awv108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Higgins, J. P., Altman, D. G., Gøtzsche, P. C., Jüni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., Savovic, J., Schulz, K. F., Weeks, L., & Sterne, J. A. (2011). Cochrane Bias Methods Group; Cochrane Statistical Methods Group. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ, 343(d5928), d5928. 10.1136/bmj.d5928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Hoffmann, R. T., & Schmelz, M. (1999). Time course of UVA‐and UVB‐induced inflammation and hyperalgesia in human skin. European Journal of Pain, 3, 131–139. 10.1053/eujp.1998.0106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Hood, D. D., Curry, R., & Eisenach, J. C. (2003). Intravenous remifentanil produces withdrawal hyperalgesia in volunteers with capsaicin‐induced hyperalgesia. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 97, 810–815. 10.1213/01.ANE.0000078811.80093.88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Huang, J. H., Ali, Z., Travison, T. G., Campbell, J. N., & Meyer, R. A. (2000). Spatial mapping of the zone of secondary hyperalgesia reveals a gradual decline of pain with distance but sharp borders. Pain, 86, 33–42. 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00314-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Hughes, A., Macleod, A., Growcott, J., & Thomas, I. (2002). Assessment of the reproducibility of intradermal capsaicin as a model for inducing human pain. Pain, 99, 323–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Hughes, A. M., Rhodes, J., Fisher, G., Sellers, M., & Growcott, J. W. (2002). Assessment of the effect of dextromethorphan and ketamine on the acute nociceptive threshold and wind‐up of the second pain response in healthy male volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 53, 604–612. 10.1046/j.1365-2125.2002.01602.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Hutchinson, M. R., Buijs, M., Tuke, J., Kwok, Y. H., Gentgall, M., Williams, D., & Rolan, P. (2013). Low‐dose endotoxin potentiates capsaicin‐induced pain in man: Evidence for a pain neuroimmune connection. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 30, 3–11. 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Iannetti, G. D., Baumgärtner, U., Tracey, I., Treede, R. D., & Magerl, W. (2013). Pinprick‐evoked brain potentials: A novel tool to assess central sensitization of nociceptive pathways in humans. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 1107–1116. 10.1152/jn.00774.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Iannetti, G. D., Zambreanu, L., Wise, R. G., Buchanan, T. J., Huggins, J. P., Smart, T. S., Vennart, W., & Tracey, I. (2005). Pharmacological modulation of pain‐related brain activity during normal and central sensitization states in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 18195–18200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Ikeda, H., Stark, J., Fischer, H., Wagner, M., Drdla, R., Jäger, T., & Sandkühler, J. (2006). Synaptic amplifier of inflammatory pain in the spinal dorsal horn. Science, 312, 1659–1662. 10.1126/science.1127233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Ilkjaer, S., Dirks, J., Brennum, J., Wernberg, M., & Dahl, J. B. (1997). Effect of systemic N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptor antagonist (dextromethorphan) on primary and secondary hyperalgesia in humans. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 79, 600–605. 10.1093/bja/79.5.600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Ilkjaer, S., Petersen, K. L., Brennum, J., Wernberg, M., & Dahl, J. B. (1996). Effect of systemic N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptor antagonist (ketamine) on primary and secondary hyperalgesia in humans. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 76, 829–834. 10.1093/bja/76.6.829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Jadad, A. R., Moore, R. A., Carroll, D., Jenkinson, C., Reynolds, D. J., Gavaghan, D. J., & McQuay, H. J. (1996). Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: Is blinding necessary? Controlled clinical trials, 17, 1–12. 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Jancso, N. (1960). Role of the nerve terminals in the mechanism of inflammatory reactions. Bull Millard Fillmore Hosp, 7, 53–77. [Google Scholar]
  122. Jensen, M. T., & Petersen, K. L. (2006). Gender differences in pain and secondary hyperalgesia after heat/capsaicin sensitization in healthy volunteers. The Journal of Pain, 7, 211–217. 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Jensen, T. S., & Finnerup, N. B. (2014). Allodynia and hyperalgesia in neuropathic pain: Clinical manifestations and mechanisms. The Lancet Neurology, 13, 924–935. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70102-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Johnson, R. W., Bouhassira, D., Kassianos, G., Leplège, A., Schmader, K. E., & Weinke, T. (2010). The impact of herpes zoster and post‐herpetic neuralgia on quality‐of‐life. BMC Medicine, 8, 37. 10.1186/1741-7015-8-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Jürgens, T. P., Sawatzki, A., Henrich, F., Magerl, W., & May, A. (2014). An improved model of heat‐induced hyperalgesia—repetitive phasic heat pain causing primary hyperalgesia to heat and secondary hyperalgesia to pinprick and light touch. PLoS One, 9, e99507. 10.1371/journal.pone.0099507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Kalliomäki, J., Annas, P., Huizar, K., Clarke, C., Zettergren, A., Karlsten, R., & Segerdahl, M. (2013). Evaluation of the analgesic efficacy and psychoactive effects of AZD1940, a novel peripherally acting cannabinoid agonist, in human capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia. Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology, 40, 212–218. 10.1111/1440-1681.12051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Kauppila, T., Grönroos, M., & Pertovaara, A. (1995). An attempt to attenuate experimental pain in humans by dextromethorphan, an NMDA receptor antagonist. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 52, 641–644. 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00106-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Kawamata, M., Takahashi, T., Kozuka, Y., Nawa, Y., Nishikawa, K., Narimatsu, E., Watanabe, H., & Namiki, A. (2002). Experimental incision‐induced pain in human skin: Effects of systemic lidocaine on flare formation and hyperalgesia. Pain, 100, 77–89. 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00233-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Kawamata, M., Watanabe, H., Nishikawa, K., Takahashi, T., Kozuka, Y., Kawamata, T., Omote, K., & Namiki, A. (2002). Different mechanisms of development and maintenance of experimental incision‐induced hyperalgesia in human skin. Anesthesiology, 97, 550–559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Khan, N., & Smith, M. T. (2015). Neurotrophins and neuropathic pain: role in pathobiology. Molecules, 20, 10657–10688. 10.3390/molecules200610657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Kilo, S., Forster, C., Geisslinger, G., Brune, K., & Handwerker, H. O. (1995). Inflammatory models of cutaneous hyperalgesia are sensitive to effects of ibuprofen in man. Pain, 62, 187–193. 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00265-G [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Kilo, S., Schmelz, M., Koltzenburg, M., & Handwerker, H. O. (1994). Different patterns of hyperalgesia induced by experimental inflammation in human skin. Brain, 117, 385–396. 10.1093/brain/117.2.385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Kim, H. Y., Jun, J., Wang, J., Bittar, A., Chung, K., & Chung, J. M. (2015). Induction of long‐term potentiation and long‐term depression is cell‐type specific in the spinal cord. Pain, 156, 618–625. 10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460354.09622.ec [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Kinnman, E., Nygards, E.‐B., & Hansson, P. (1997). Peripherally administrated morphine attenuates capsaicin‐induced mechanical hypersensitivity in humans. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 84, 595–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Klede, M., Handwerker, H. O., & Schmelz, M. (2003). Central origin of secondary mechanical hyperalgesia. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 353–359. 10.1152/jn.01136.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Klein, T., Magerl, W., Hanschmann, A., Althaus, M., & Treede, R. D. (2008). Antihyperalgesic and analgesic properties of the N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist neramexane in a human surrogate model of neurogenic hyperalgesia. European Journal of Pain, 12, 17–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Klein, T., Magerl, W., Hopf, H.‐C., Sandkühler, J., & Treede, R.‐D. (2004). Perceptual correlates of nociceptive long‐term potentiation and long‐term depression in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 964–971. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1222-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Klein, T., Magerl, W., Nickel, U., Hopf, H. C., Sandkühler, J., & Treede, R. D. (2007). Effects of the NMDA‐receptor antagonist ketamine on perceptual correlates of long‐term potentiation within the nociceptive system. Neuropharmacology, 52, 655–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Klein, T., Magerl, W., & Treede, R. D. (2006). Perceptual correlate of nociceptive long‐term potentiation (LTP) in humans shares the time course of early‐LTP. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 3551–3555. 10.1152/jn.00755.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Klein, T., Stahn, S., Magerl, W., & Treede, R. D. (2008). The role of heterosynaptic facilitation in long‐term potentiation (LTP) of human pain sensation. Pain, 139, 507–519. 10.1016/j.pain.2008.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Koltzenburg, M., Lundberg, L. E., & Torebjörk, H. E. (1992). Dynamic and static components of mechanical hyperalgesia in human hairy skin. Pain, 51, 207–219. 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90262-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Koltzenburg, M., Torebjörk, H. E., & Wahren, L. K. (1994). Nociceptor modulated central sensitization causes mechanical hyperalgesia in acute chemogenic and chronic neuropathic pain. Brain, 117(Pt 3), 579–591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Koppert, W., Dern, S. K., Sittl, R., Albrecht, S., Schüttler, J., & Schmelz, M. (2001). A new model of electrically evoked pain and hyperalgesia in human skin: The effects of intravenous alfentanil, S(+)‐ketamine, and lidocaine. Anesthesiology, 95, 395–402. 10.1097/00000542-200108000-00022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Koppert, W., Likar, R., Geisslinger, G., Zeck, S., Schmelz, M., & Sittl, R. (1999). Peripheral antihyperalgesic effect of morphine to heat, but not mechanical, stimulation in healthy volunteers after ultraviolet‐B irradiation. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 88, 117–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Koppert, W., Ostermeier, N., Sittl, R., Weidner, C., & Schmelz, M. (2000). Low‐dose lidocaine reduces secondary hyperalgesia by a central mode of action. Pain, 85, 217–224. 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00268-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Kraft, B., Frickey, N., Kauffmann, R., Reif, M., Frey, R., Gustorff, B., & Kress, H. (2008). Lack of analgesia by oral standardized cannabis extract on acute inflammatory pain and hyperalgesia in volunteers. Anesthesiology, 109, 101–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Kremer, M., Salvat, E., Muller, A., Yalcin, I., & Barrot, M. (2016). Antidepressants and gabapentinoids in neuropathic pain: Mechanistic insights. Neuroscience, 338, 183–206. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.06.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Lam, V. Y., Wallace, M., & Schulteis, G. (2011). Effects of lidocaine patch on intradermal capsaicin‐induced pain: A double‐blind, controlled trial. The Journal of Pain, 12, 323–330. 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. LaMotte, R. H., Shain, C. N., Simone, D. A., & Tsai, E. F. (1991). Neurogenic hyperalgesia: Psychophysical studies of underlying mechanisms. Journal of Neurophysiology, 66, 190–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Larsen, I. M., Drewes, A. M., & Olesen, A. E. (2018). The effect of a combination of diclofenac and methadone applied as gel in a human experimental pain model ‐ a randomized, placebo‐controlled trial. Basic and Clinical Pharmacology and Toxicology, 123, 188–194. 10.1111/bcpt.12993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Lee, M. C., Ploner, M., Wiech, K., Bingel, U., Wanigasekera, V., Brooks, J., Menon, D. K., & Tracey, I. (2013). Amygdala activity contributes to the dissociative effect of cannabis on pain perception. Pain, 154, 124–134. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Lee, M. C., Zambreanu, L., Menon, D. K., & Tracey, I. (2008). Identifying brain activity specifically related to the maintenance and perceptual consequence of central sensitization in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 11642–11649. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2638-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Lenoir, C., Plaghki, L., Mouraux, A., & van den Broeke, E. N. (2018). Quickly responding C‐fibre nociceptors contribute to heat hypersensitivity in the area of secondary hyperalgesia. Journal of Physiology, 596, 4443–4455. 10.1113/JP275977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Lenz, H., Raeder, J., Draegni, T., Heyerdahl, F., Schmelz, M., & Stubhaug, A. (2011). Effects of COX inhibition on experimental pain and hyperalgesia during and after remifentanil infusion in humans. Pain, 152, 1289–1297. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Lewis, T., & Love, W. S. (1926). Vascular reactions of the skin to injury. Part III. Some effects of freezing, of cooling and of warming. Heart, 13, 27. [Google Scholar]
  156. Liljencrantz, J., Marshall, A., Ackerley, R., & Olausson, H. (2014). Discriminative and affective touch in human experimental tactile allodynia. Neuroscience Letters, 563, 75–79. 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.01.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Lillesø, J., Hammer, N. A., Pedersen, J. L., & Kehlet, H. (2000). Effect of peripheral morphine in a human model of acute inflammatory pain. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 85, 228–232. 10.1093/bja/85.2.228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Linde, L. D., & Srbely, J. Z. (2019). The acute effect of skin preheating on capsaicin‐induced central sensitization in humans. Pain Practice, 19, 811–820. 10.1111/papr.12811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Liu, M., Max, M. B., Robinovitz, E., Gracely, R. H., & Bennett, G. J. (1998). The human capsaicin model of allodynia and hyperalgesia: Sources of variability and methods for reduction. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 16, 10–20. 10.1016/S0885-3924(98)00026-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Liu, X.‐G., & Sandkühler, J. (1997). Characterization of long‐term potentiation of C‐fiber–evoked potentials in spinal dorsal horn of adult rat: Essential role of NK1 and NK2 receptors. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1973–1982. 10.1152/jn.1997.78.4.1973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. London, A. J., & Kimmelman, J. (2020). Against pandemic research exceptionalism. Science, 368, 476–477. 10.1126/science.abc1731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Lorenzini, K. I., Besson, M., Daali, Y., Salomon, D., Dayer, P., & Desmeules, J. (2011). A randomized, controlled trial validates a peripheral supra‐additive antihyperalgesic effect of a paracetamol‐ketorolac combination. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology, 109, 357–564. 10.1111/j.1742-7843.2011.00733.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Lorenzini, K. I., Besson, M., Daali, Y., Salomon, D., Dayer, P., & Desmeules, J. (2012). Validation of the simplified UVB model to assess the pharmacodynamics of analgesics in healthy human volunteers. CHIMIA International Journal for Chemistry, 66, 296–299. 10.2533/chimia.2012.296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Lötsch, J., & Angst, M. S. (2003). The mu‐opioid agonist remifentanil attenuates hyperalgesia evoked by blunt and punctuated stimuli with different potency: A pharmacological evaluation of the freeze lesion in humans. Pain, 102, 151–161. 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00349-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Lötsch, J., Walter, C., Zunftmeister, M., Zinn, S., Wolters, M., Ferreiros, N., Rossmanith, T., Oertel, B. G., & Geisslinger, G. (2020). A data science approach to the selection of most informative readouts of the human intradermal capsaicin pain model to assess pregabalin effects. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology, 126, 318–331. 10.1111/bcpt.13337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Magerl, W., Hansen, N., Treede, R. D., & Klein, T. (2018). The human pain system exhibits higher‐order plasticity (metaplasticity). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 154, 112–120. 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Magerl, W., Wilk, S. H., & Treede, R. D. (1998). Secondary hyperalgesia and perceptual wind‐up following intradermal injection of capsaicin in humans. Pain, 74, 257–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Mahn, F., Hüllemann, P., Wasner, G., Baron, R., & Binder, A. (2014). Topical high‐concentration menthol: Reproducibility of a human surrogate pain model. European Journal of Pain, 18, 1248–1258. 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.484.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Maier, C., Baron, R., Tölle, T. R., Binder, A., Birbaumer, N., Birklein, F., Gierthmühlen, J., Flor, H., Geber, C., Huge, V., Krumova, E. K., Landwehrmeyer, G. B., Magerl, W., Maihöfner, C., Richter, H., Rolke, R., Scherens, A., Schwarz, A., Sommer, C., … Treede, D.‐R. (2010). Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): Somatosensory abnormalities in 1236 patients with different neuropathic pain syndromes. Pain, 150, 439–450. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Maihöfner, C., Ringler, R., Herrndobler, F., & Koppert, W. (2007). Brain imaging of analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of cyclooxygenase inhibition in an experimental human pain model: A functional MRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1344–1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Maihöfner, C., Schmelz, M., Forster, C., Neundörfer, B., & Handwerker, H. O. (2004). Neural activation during experimental allodynia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 3211–3218. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03437.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Malmberg, A. B., & Yaksh, T. L. Antinociceptive actions of spinal nonsteroidal anti‐inflammatory agents on the formalin test in the rat. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 263, 136‐146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Maracle, E. C., Hung, L. Y., Fell, S. I., Osmond, M. R., Brown, S. H., & Srbely, J. Z. (2017). A comparison of the sensitivity of brush allodynia and semmes‐weinstein monofilament testing in the detection of allodynia within regions of secondary hyperalgesia in humans. Pain Practice, 17, 16–24. 10.1111/papr.12418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Martin, E., Narjoz, C., Decleves, X., Labat, L., Lambert, C., Loriot, M.‐A., Ducheix, G., Dualé, C., Pereira, B., & Pickering, G. (2019). Dextromethorphan analgesia in a human experimental model of hyperalgesia. Anesthesiology, 131, 356–368. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Mathiesen, O., Imbimbo, B. P., Hilsted, K. L., Fabbri, L., & Dahl, J. B. (2006). CHF3381, a N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptor antagonist and monoamine oxidase–A inhibitor, attenuates secondary hyperalgesia in a human pain model. The Journal of Pain, 7, 565–574. 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Mauermann, E., Blum, C. A., Lurati Buse, G., Bandschapp, O., & Ruppen, W. (2017). Time course of copeptin during a model of experimental pain and hyperalgesia: A randomised volunteer crossover trial. European Journal of Anaesthesiology, 34, 306–314. 10.1097/EJA.0000000000000592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. McCormack, K., Kidd, B. L., & Morris, V. (2000). Assay of topically administered ibuprofen using a model of post‐injury hypersensitivity. A randomised, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 56, 459–462. 10.1007/s002280000132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Meyer, R. A., & Campbell, J. N. (1981). Myelinated nociceptive afferents account for the hyperalgesia that follows a burn to the hand. Science, 213, 1527–1529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Michaux, G. P., Magerl, W., Anton, F., & Treede, R. D. (2012). Experimental characterization of the effects of acute stresslike doses of hydrocortisone in human neurogenic hyperalgesia models. Pain, 153, 420–428. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.10.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Mikkelsen, S., Dirks, J., Fabricius, P., Petersen, K. L., Rowbotham, M. C., & Dahl, J. B. (2001). Effect of intravenous magnesium on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 86, 871–873. 10.1093/bja/86.6.871 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Mikkelsen, S., Ilkjaer, S., Brennum, J., Borgbjerg, F. M., & Dahl, J. B. (1999). The effect of naloxone on ketamine‐induced effects on hyperalgesia and ketamine‐induced side effects in humans. Anesthesiology, 90, 1539–1545. 10.1097/00000542-199906000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Mikkelsen, S., Jørgensen, H., Larsen, P. S., Brennum, J., & Dahl, J. B. (2000). Effect of oral ketamine on secondary hyperalgesia, thermal and mechanical pain thresholds, and sedation in humans. Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, 25, 452–458. 10.1053/rapm.2000.8456 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Modir, J. G., & Wallace, M. S. (2010a). Human experimental pain models 3: Heat/capsaicin sensitization and intradermal capsaicin models. Methods in Molecular Biology, 617, 169–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Modir, J. G., & Wallace, M. S. (2010b). Human experimental pain models 1: The ultraviolet light UV‐B pain model. Methods in Molecular Biology, 617, 159–164. 10.1007/978-1-60327-323-7_12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Moiniche, S., Dahl, J. B., & Kehlet, H. (1993). Short‐term topical Piroxicam has no anti‐inflammatory or Antinociceptive effects after burn injury. Current Therapeutic Research, 53, 466–472. 10.1016/S0011-393X(05)80654-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  186. Moiniche, S., Pedersen, J. L., & Kehlet, H. (1994). Topical ketorolac has no antinociceptive or anti‐inflammatory effect in thermal injury. Burns, 20, 483–486. 10.1016/0305-4179(94)90001-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Moparthi, L., Kichko, T. I., Eberhardt, M., Högestätt, E. D., Kjellbom, P., Johanson, U., Reeh, P. W., Leffler, A., Filipovic, M. R., & Zygmunt, P. M. (2016). Human TRPA1 is a heat sensor displaying intrinsic U‐shaped thermosensitivity. Scientific Reports, 6, 28763. 10.1038/srep28763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Morch, C. D., Gazerani, P., Nielsen, T. A., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2013). The UVB cutaneous inflammatory pain model: A reproducibility study in healthy volunteers. International Journal of Physiology, Pathophysiology and Pharmacology, 5, 203–215. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Morris, V. H., Cruwys, S. C., & Kidd, B. L. (1997). Characterisation of capsaicin‐induced mechanical hyperalgesia as a marker for altered nociceptive processing in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Pain, 71, 179–186. 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)03361-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Munkholmn, T. K., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2017). The interaction between NGF‐induced hyperalgesia and acid‐provoked pain in the infrapatellar fat pad and tibialis anterior muscle of healthy volunteers. European Journal of Pain, 21, 474–485. 10.1002/ejp.941 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Nagi, S. S., & Mahns, D. A. (2013). Mechanical allodynia in human glabrous skin mediated by low‐threshold cutaneous mechanoreceptors with unmyelinated fibres. Experimental Brain Research, 231, 139–151. 10.1007/s00221-013-3677-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Namer, B., Seifert, F., Handwerker, H. O., & Maihöfner, C. (2005). TRPA1 and TRPM8 activation in humans: Effects of cinnamaldehyde and menthol. NeuroReport, 16, 955–959. 10.1097/00001756-200506210-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Nelson, E. (1919). The constitution of capsaicin, the pungent principle of capsicum. Journal of the American Chemical Society, 41, 1115–1121. [Google Scholar]
  194. Nickel, F. T., DeCol, R., Jud, S., Fasching, P. A., Seifert, F., & Maihöfner, C. (2011). Inhibition of hyperalgesia by conditioning electrical stimulation in a human pain model. Pain, 152, 1298–1303. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Nickel, F. T., Ott, S., Möhringer, S., Münster, T., Rieß, S., Filitz, J., Koppert, W., & Maihöfner, C. (2016). Effects of different anesthetics on pain processing in an experimental human pain model. Pain Practice, 16(7), 820–830. 10.1111/papr.12326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Nilsson, M., Lassen, D., Andresen, T., Nielsen, A., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Drewes, A. (2014). Intradermal glutamate and capsaicin injections: Intra‐ and interindividual variability of provoked hyperalgesia and allodynia. Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology, 41, 423–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Niu, X., Zhang, M., Liu, Z., Bai, L., Sun, C., Wang, S., Wang, X., Chen, Z., Chen, H., & Tian, J. (2017). Interaction of acupuncture treatment and manipulation laterality modulated by the default mode network. Molecular Pain, 13, 1–14. 10.1177/1744806916683684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Oda, S., Izumi, M., Aso, K., & Ikeuchi, M. (2018). Impact of medial versus lateral knee pain on deep tissue hyperalgesia and muscle strength. European Journal of Pain, 22(10), 1757–1766.10.1002/ejp.1271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. O'Neill, J., Sikandar, S., McMahon, S. B., & Dickenson, A. H. (2015). Human psychophysics and rodent spinal neurones exhibit peripheral and central mechanisms of inflammatory pain in the UVB and UVB heat rekindling models. Journal of Physiology, 593, 4029–4042. 10.1113/JP270294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Ortner, C. M., Steiner, I., Margeta, K., Schulz, M., & Gustorff, B. (2012). Dose response of tramadol and its combination with paracetamol in UVB induced hyperalgesia. European Journal of Pain, 16, 562–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Pahl, I. R., Koppert, W., Enk, C., Sittl, R., Mühldorfer, S., Hahn, E. G., Schmelz, M., & Schwab, D. (2003). Different lipid profiles as constituencies of liquid formula diets do not influence pain perception and the efficacy of opioids in a human model of acute pain and hyperalgesia. Pain, 104, 519–527. 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00094-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Park, K. M., Max, M. B., Robinovitz, E., Gracely, R. H., & Bennett, G. J. (1995). Effects of intravenous ketamine, alfentanil, or placebo on pain, pinprick hyperalgesia, and allodynia produced by intradermal capsaicin in human subjects. Pain, 63, 163–172. 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00029-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Pedersen, J. L., Galle, T. S., & Kehlet, H. (1998). Peripheral analgesic effects of ketamine in acute inflammatory pain. Anesthesiology, 89, 58–66. 10.1097/00000542-199807000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Pedersen, J. L., & Kehlet, H. (1998). Hyperalgesia in a human model of acute inflammatory pain: A methodological study. Pain, 74, 139–151. 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00160-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Pedersen, J. L., Moiniche, S., & Kehlet, H. (1994). Topical glucocorticoid has no antinociceptive or anti‐inflammatory effect in thermal injury. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 72, 379–382. 10.1093/bja/72.4.379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Petersen, K. L., Brennum, J., & Dahl, J. B. (1997). Experimental evaluation of the analgesic effect of ibuprofen on primary and secondary hyperalgesia. Pain, 70, 167–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Petersen, K. L., Iyengar, S., Chappell, A. S., Lobo, E. D., Reda, H., Prucka, W. R., & Verfaille, S. J. (2014). Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and effects on human experimental pain of the selective ionotropic glutamate receptor 5 (iGluR5) antagonist LY545694 in healthy volunteers. Pain, 155, 929–936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Petersen, K. L., Jones, B., Segredo, V., Dahl, J. B., & Rowbotham, M. C. (2001). Effect of remifentanil on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat–capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology, 94, 15–20. 10.1097/00000542-200101000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Petersen, K. L., Maloney, A., Hoke, F., Dahl, J. B., & Rowbotham, M. C. (2003). A randomized study of the effect of oral lamotrigine and hydromorphone on pain and hyperalgesia following heat/capsaicin sensitization. The Journal of Pain, 4, 400–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Petersen, K. L., Meadoff, T., Press, S., Peters, M. M., LeComte, M. D., & Rowbotham, M. C. (2008). Changes in morphine analgesia and side effects during daily subcutaneous administration in healthy volunteers. Pain, 137, 395–404. 10.1016/j.pain.2007.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Petersen, K. L., & Rowbotham, M. C. (1999). A new human experimental pain model: The heat/capsaicin sensitization model. NeuroReport, 10, 1511–1516. 10.1097/00001756-199905140-00022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Petersen, L. J., Lyngholm, A. M., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2010). A novel model of inflammatory pain in human skin involving topical application of sodium lauryl sulfate. Inflammation Research, 59, 775–781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Petty, B. G., Cornblath, D. R., Adornato, B. T., Chaudhry, V., Flexner, C., Wachsman, M., Sinicropi, D., Burton, L. E., & Peroutka, S. J. (1994). The effect of systemically administered recombinant human nerve growth factor in healthy human subjects. Annals of Neurology, 36, 244–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Peyron, R., García‐Larrea, L., Grégoire, M. C., Convers, P., Lavenne, F., Veyre, L., Froment, J. C., Mauguière, F., Michel, D., & Laurent, B. (1998). Allodynia after lateral‐medullary (Wallenberg) infarct. A PET study. Brain, 121(2), 345–356. 10.1093/brain/121.2.345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Pfau, D. B., Klein, T., Putzer, D., Pogatzki‐Zahn, E. M., Treede, R.‐D., & Magerl, W. (2011). Analysis of hyperalgesia time courses in humans after painful electrical high‐frequency stimulation identifies a possible transition from early to late LTP‐like pain plasticity. Pain, 152, 1532–1539. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.02.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Pogatzki‐Zahn, E. M., Drescher, C., Englbrecht, J. S., Klein, T., Magerl, W., & Zahn, P. K. (2019). Progesterone relates to enhanced incisional acute pain and pinprick hyperalgesia in the luteal phase of female volunteers. Pain, 160, 1781–1793. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Pöyhiä, R., & Vainio, A. (2006). Topically administered ketamine reduces capsaicin‐evoked mechanical hyperalgesia. Clinical Journal of Pain, 22, 32–36. 10.1097/01.ajp.0000149800.39240.95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Price, R., Gandhi, W., Nadeau, C., Tarnavskiy, R., Qu, A., Fahey, E., Stone, L., & Schweinhardt, P. (2018). Characterization of a novel capsaicin/heat ongoing pain model. European Journal of Pain, 22, 370–384. 10.1002/ejp.1126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Pud, D., Andersen, O. K., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., Eisenberg, E., & Yarnitsky, D. (2005). Vasomotor response to cold stimulation in human capsaicin‐induced hyperalgesic area. Experimental Brain Research, 164, 334–340. 10.1007/s00221-005-2254-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Ragavendran, J. V., Laferrière, A., Bennett, G. J., Ware, M. A., Gandhi, W., Bley, K., Schweinhardt, P., & Coderre, T. J. (2016). Effects of topical combinations of clonidine and pentoxifylline on capsaicin‐induced allodynia and postcapsaicin tourniquet‐induced pain in healthy volunteers: A double‐blind, randomized, controlled study. Pain, 2016(157), 2366–2374. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Raja, S. N., Campbell, J. N., & Meyer, R. A. (1984). Evidence for different mechanisms of primary and secondary hyperalgesia following heat injury to the glabrous skin. Brain, 107, 1179–1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Rasmussen, V. M., Borgen, A. E., Jansen, E. C., Rotbøll Nielsen, P. H., & Werner, M. U. (2015). Hyperbaric oxygen therapy attenuates central sensitization induced by a thermal injury in humans. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 59, 749–762. 10.1111/aas.12492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Ravn, P., Foster, D. J., Kreilgaard, M., Christrup, L., Werner, M. U., Secher, E. L., Skram, U., & Upton, R. (2014). Pharmacokinetic‐pharmacodynamic modelling of the analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of morphine after intravenous infusion in human volunteers. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology, 115, 257–267. 10.1111/bcpt.12213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Ravn, P., Secher, E. L., Skram, U., Therkildsen, T., Christrup, L. L., & Werner, M. U. (2013). Morphine‐ and buprenorphine‐induced analgesia and antihyperalgesia in a human inflammatory pain model: A double‐blind, randomized, placebo‐controlled, five‐arm crossover study. Journal of Pain Research, 6, 23–38. 10.2147/JPR.S36827 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Redmond, W. J., Goffaux, P., Potvin, S., & Marchand, S. (2008). Analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of nabilone on experimental heat pain. Current Medical Research and Opinion, 24(4), 1017–1024. 10.1185/030079908X280635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Reindl, C., Seifert, F., Nickel, F., & Maihöfner, C. (2016). Neuromodulation of electrically induced hyperalgesia in the trigeminocervical system. Pain Practice, 16, 712–719. 10.1111/papr.12320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Reitz, M.‐C., Hrncic, D., Treede, R.‐D., & Caspani, O. (2016). A comparative behavioural study of mechanical hypersensitivity in 2 pain models in rats and humans. Pain, 157, 1248–1258. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Rempe, T., Wolff, S., Riedel, C., Baron, R., Stroman, P. W., Jansen, O., & Gierthmühlen, J. (2014). Spinal fMRI reveals decreased descending inhibition during secondary mechanical hyperalgesia. PLoS One, 9, e112325. 10.1371/journal.pone.0112325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Retamal, J., Reyes, A., Ramirez, P., Bravo, D., Hernandez, A., Pelissier, T., Villanueva, L., & Constandil, L. (2018). Burst‐like subcutaneous electrical stimulation induces BDNF‐mediated, cyclotraxin B‐sensitive central sensitization in rat spinal cord. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 1143. 10.3389/fphar.2018.01143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Ringsted, T. K., Enghuus, C., Petersen, M. A., & Werner, M. U. (2015). Demarcation of secondary hyperalgesia zones: Punctate stimulation pressure matters. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 256, 74–81. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Rossler, B. (2013). Sunburn—A human inflammatory pain model for primary and secondary hyperalgesia. Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 4, 38–39. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2012.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Rowbotham, M. C., & Fields, H. L. (1996). The relationship of pain, allodynia and thermal sensation in post‐herpetic neuralgia. Brain, 119, 347–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Rukwied, R., Mayer, A., Kluschina, O., Obreja, O., Schley, M., & Schmelz, M. (2010). NGF induces non‐inflammatory localized and lasting mechanical and thermal hypersensitivity in human skin. Pain, 148, 407–413. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Rukwied, R., Watkinson, A., McGlone, F., & Dvorak, M. (2003). Cannabinoid agonists attenuate capsaicin‐induced responses in human skin. Pain, 102, 283–288. 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00401-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. Rukwied, R., Weinkauf, B., Main, M., Obreja, O., & Schmelz, M. (2013). Inflammation meets sensitization– an explanation for spontaneous nociceptor activity? Pain, 154, 2707–2714. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Samour, M. S., Nagi, S. S., Shortland, P. J., & Mahns, D. A. (2017). Minocycline prevents muscular pain hypersensitivity and cutaneous allodynia produced by repeated intramuscular injections of hypertonic saline in healthy human participants. The Journal of Pain, 18, 994–1005. 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Samuelsson, M., Leffler, A.‐S., & Hansson, P. (2011). Dynamic mechanical allodynia in the secondary hyperalgesic area in the capsaicin model—perceptually similar phenomena as in painful neuropathy? Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 2, 85–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Sandkühler, J., & Gruber‐Schoffnegger, D. (2012). Hyperalgesia by synaptic long‐term potentiation (LTP): An update. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 12, 18–27. 10.1016/j.coph.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Scanlon, G. C., Wallace, M. S., Ispirescu, S. J., & Schulteis, G. (2006). Intradermal capsaicin causes dose‐dependent pain, allodynia, and hyperalgesia in humans. Journal of Investigative Medicine, 54, 238–244. 10.2310/6650.2006.05046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Schechtmann, G., Lind, G., Winter, J., Meyerson, B. A., & Linderoth, B. (2010). Intrathecal clonidine and baclofen enhance the pain‐relieving effect of spinal cord stimulation: A comparative placebo‐controlled, randomized trial. Neurosurgery, 67, 173–181. 10.1227/01.NEU.0000370249.41634.4F [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Schifftner, C., Schulteis, G., & Wallace, M. S. (2017). Effect of intravenous alfentanil on nonpainful thermally induced hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 57, 1207–1214. 10.1002/jcph.911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Schmelz, M. (2009). Translating nociceptive processing into human pain models. Experimental Brain Research, 196, 173–178. 10.1007/s00221-009-1809-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Schmelz, M., & Petersen, L. J. (2001). Neurogenic inflammation in human and rodent skin. News in Physiological Sciences, 16, 33–37. 10.1152/physiologyonline.2001.16.1.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Schmelz, M., Schmid, R., Handwerker, H. O., & Torebjörk, H. E. (2000). Encoding of burning pain from capsaicin‐treated human skin in two categories of unmyelinated nerve fibres. Brain, 123(Pt 3), 560–571. 10.1093/brain/123.3.560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Schulte, H., Sollevi, A., & Segerdahl, M. (2005). Dose‐dependent effects of morphine on experimentally induced cutaneous pain in healthy volunteers. Pain, 116, 366–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Segerdahl, M. (2006). Multiple dose gabapentin attenuates cutaneous pain and central sensitisation but not muscle pain in healthy volunteers. Pain, 125, 158–164. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Seifert, F., Bschorer, K., De Col, R., Filitz, J., Peltz, E., Koppert, W., & Maihöfner, C. (2009). Medial prefrontal cortex activity is predictive for hyperalgesia and pharmacological antihyperalgesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 6167–6175. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4654-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Seifert, F., Fuchs, O., Nickel, F. T., Garcia, M., Dörfler, A., Schaller, G., Kornhuber, J., Sperling, W., & Maihöfner, C. (2010). A functional magnetic resonance imaging navigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study of the posterior parietal cortex in normal pain and hyperalgesia. Neuroscience, 170, 670–677. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.07.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Seifert, F., Jungfer, I., Schmelz, M., & Maihöfner, C. (2008). Representation of UV‐B‐induced thermal and mechanical hyperalgesia in the human brain: A functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 1327–1342. 10.1002/hbm.20470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  250. Shabes, P., Schloss, N., Magerl, W., Schmahl, C., Treede, R. D., & Baumgärtner, U. (2016). A novel human surrogate model of noninjurious sharp mechanical pain. Pain, 157, 214–224. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Siebenga, P. S., van Amerongen, G., Klaassen, E. S., de Kam, M. L., Rissmann, R., & Groeneveld, G. J. (2019). The ultraviolet B inflammation model: Postinflammatory hyperpigmentation and validation of a reduced UVB exposure paradigm for inducing hyperalgesia in healthy subjects. European Journal of Pain, 23, 874–883. 10.1002/ejp.1353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. Silberberg, A., Moeller‐Bertram, T., & Wallace, M. S. (2015). A randomized, double‐blind, crossover study to evaluate the depth response relationship of intradermal capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia in healthy adult volunteers. Pain Medicine, 16, 745–752. 10.1111/pme.12639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Simone, D. A., Baumann, T. K., & LaMotte, R. H. (1989). Dose‐dependent pain and mechanical hyperalgesia in humans after intradermal injection of capsaicin. Pain, 38, 99–107. 10.1016/0304-3959(89)90079-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  254. Simone, D. A., Sorkin, L. S., Oh, U., Chung, J. M., Owens, C., LaMotte, R. H., & Willis, W. D. (1991). Neurogenic hyperalgesia: Central neural correlates in responses of spinothalamic tract neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 66, 228–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  255. Sjölund, K. F., Segerdahl, M., & Sollevi, A. (1999). Adenosine reduces secondary hyperalgesia in two human models of cutaneous inflammatory pain. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 88, 605–610. 10.1213/00000539-199903000-00027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  256. Sluka, K. A., Kalra, A., & Moore, S. A. (2001). Unilateral intramuscular injections of acidic saline produce a bilateral, long‐lasting hyperalgesia. Muscle and Nerve, 24, 37–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  257. Sluka, K. A., Price, M. P., Breese, N. M., Stucky, C. L., Wemmie, J. A., & Welsh, M. J. (2003). Chronic hyperalgesia induced by repeated acid injections in muscle is abolished by the loss of ASIC3, but not ASIC1. Pain, 106, 229–239. 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00269-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  258. Smith, M. T., Remeniuk, B., Finan, P. H., Speed, T. J., Tompkins, D. A., Robinson, M., Gonzalez, K., Bjurstrom, M. F., & Irwin, M. R. (2019). Sex differences in measures of central sensitization and pain sensitivity to experimental sleep disruption: Implications for sex differences in chronic pain. Sleep, 42, zsy209. 10.1093/sleep/zsy209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  259. Springborg, A. D., Jensen, E. K., Taylor, B. K., & Werner, M. U. (2016). Effects of target‐controlled infusion of high‐dose naloxone on pain and hyperalgesia in a human thermal injury model: A study protocol: A randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover trial with an enriched design. Medicine (Baltimore), 95, e5336. 10.1097/MD.0000000000005336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  260. Staud, R., Vierck, C. J., Robinson, M. E., & Price, D. D. (2005). Effects of the N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptor antagonist dextromethorphan on temporal summation of pain are similar in fibromyalgia patients and normal control subjects. The Journal of Pain, 6, 323–332. 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.01.357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  261. Stubhaug, A., Romundstad, L., Kaasa, T., & Breivik, H. (2007). Methylprednisolone and ketorolac rapidly reduce hyperalgesia around a skin burn injury and increase pressure pain thresholds. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 51, 1138–1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  262. Sumracki, N. M., Hutchinson, M. R., Gentgall, M., Briggs, N., Williams, D. B., & Rolan, P. (2012). The effects of pregabalin and the glial attenuator minocycline on the response to intradermal capsaicin in patients with unilateral sciatica. PLoS One, 7, e38525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  263. Sycha, T., Anzenhofer, S., Lehr, S., Schmetterer, L., Chizh, B., Eichler, H. G., & Gustorff, B. (2005). Rofecoxib attenuates both primary and secondary inflammatory hyperalgesia: A randomized, double blinded, placebo controlled crossover trial in the UV‐B pain model. Pain, 113, 316–322. 10.1016/j.pain.2004.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  264. Sycha, T., Samal, D., Chizh, B., Lehr, S., Gustorff, B., Schnider, P., & Auff, E. (2006). A lack of antinociceptive or antiinflammatory effect of botulinum toxin A in an inflammatory human pain model. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 102, 509–516. 10.1213/01.ane.0000194447.46763.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  265. Terman, G. W., Eastman, C. L., & Chavkin, C. (2001). Mu opiates inhibit long‐term potentiation induction in the spinal cord slice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85, 485–494. 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  266. Torebjörk, H. E., Lundberg, L. E., & LaMotte, R. H. (1992). Central changes in processing of mechanoreceptive input in capsaicin‐induced secondary hyperalgesia in humans. Journal of Physiology, 448, 765–780. 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp019069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  267. Treede, R. D., & Cole, J. D. (1993). Dissociated secondary hyperalgesia in a subject with a large‐fibre sensory neuropathy. Pain, 53, 169–174. 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90077-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  268. Treede, R. D., Meyer, R. A., Raja, S. N., & Campbell, J. N. (1992). Peripheral and central mechanisms of cutaneous hyperalgesia. Progress in Neurobiology, 38, 397–421. 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90027-c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  269. Tröster, A., Ihmsen, H., Singler, B., Filitz, J., & Koppert, W. (2012). Interaction of fentanyl and buprenorphine in an experimental model of pain and central sensitization in human volunteers. Clinical Journal of Pain, 28, 705–711. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318241d948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  270. van Amerongen, G., de Boer, M. W., Groeneveld, G. J., & Hay, J. L. (2016). A literature review on the pharmacological sensitivity of human evoked hyperalgesia pain models: Human evoked hyperalgesia pain models. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 82, 903–922. 10.1111/bcp.13018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  271. van den Broeke, E. N., Mouraux, A., Groneberg, A. H., Pfau, D. B., Treede, R. D., & Klein, T. (2015). Characterizing pinprick‐evoked brain potentials before and after experimentally induced secondary hyperalgesia. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114, 2672–2681. 10.1152/jn.00444.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  272. van Heukelum, S., Mars, R. B., Guthrie, M., Buitelaar, J. K., Beckmann, C. F., Tiesinga, P. H. E., Vogt, B. A., Glennon, J. C., & Havenith, M. N. (2020). Where is cingulate cortex? A cross‐species view. Trends in Neurosciences, 43, 285–299. 10.1016/j.tins.2020.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  273. Vecchio, S. L., Finocchietti, S., Gazerani, P., Petersen, L. J., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Graven‐Nielsen, T. (2014). Heat‐rekindling in UVB‐irradiated skin above NGF‐sensitized muscle: Experimental models of prolonged mechanical hypersensitivity. International Journal of Physiology, Pathophysiology and Pharmacology, 6, 143–152. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  274. Vecchio, S. L., Petersen, L. J., Finocchietti, S., Gazerani, P., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Graven‐Nielsen, T. (2014). Hyperalgesia and allodynia to superficial and deep‐tissue mechanical stimulation within and outside of the UVB irradiated area in human skin. Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 5, 258–267. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2014.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  275. Vollert, J., Magerl, W., Baron, R., Binder, A., Enax‐Krumova, E., Geisslinger, G., Gierthmuhlen, J., Henrich, F., Hüllemann, P., Klein, T., Lötsch, J., Maier, C., Oertel, B., Schuh‐Hofer, S., Tölle, T. R., & Treede, R. D. (2018). Pathophysiological mechanisms of neuropathic pain: Comparison of sensory phenotypes in patients and human surrogate pain models. Pain, 159, 1090–1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  276. Vuilleumier, P. H., Besson, M., Desmeules, J., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Curatolo, M. (2013). Evaluation of anti‐hyperalgesic and analgesic effects of two benzodiazepines in human experimental pain: A randomized placebo‐controlled study. PLosOne, 8, e43896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  277. Wallace, M. S., Barger, D., & Schulteis, G. (2002). The effect of chronic oral desipramine on capsaicin‐induced allodynia and hyperalgesia: A double‐blinded, placebo‐controlled, crossover study. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 95, 973–978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  278. Wallace, M., Duan, R., Liu, W., Locke, C., & Nothaft, W. (2016). A randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of the T‐type calcium channel blocker ABT‐639 in an intradermal capsaicin experimental pain model in healthy adults. Pain Med., 17, 551–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  279. Wallace, M. S., Laitin, S., Licht, D., & Yaksh, T. L. (1997). Concentration‐effect relations for intravenous lidocaine infusions in human volunteers: Effects on acute sensory thresholds and capsaicin‐evoked hyperpathia. Anesthesiology, 86, 1262–1272. 10.1097/00000542-199706000-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  280. Wallace, M. S., Quessy, S., & Schulteis, G. (2004). Lack of effect of two oral sodium channel antagonists, lamotrigine and 4030W92, on intradermal capsaicin‐induced hyperalgesia model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 78, 349–355. 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  281. Wallace, M. S., Ridgeway, B., 3rd, Leung, A., Schulteis, G., & Yaksh, T. L. (2002). Concentration‐effect relationships for intravenous alfentanil and ketamine infusions in human volunteers: Effects on acute thresholds and capsaicin‐evoked hyperpathia. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 42(1), 70–80. 10.1177/0091270002042001008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  282. Wallace, M. S., & Schulteis, G. (2008). Effect of chronic oral gabapentin on capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia: A double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study. Clinical Journal of Pain, 24, 544–549. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181673b93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  283. Wallace, M., Schulteis, G., Atkinson, J. H., Wolfson, T., Lazzaretto, D., Bentley, H., Gouaux, B., & Abramson, I. (2007). Dose‐dependent effects of smoked cannabis on capsaicin‐induced pain and hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology, 107, 785–796. 10.1097/01.anes.0000286986.92475.b7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  284. Wang, H., Bolognese, J., Calder, N., Baxendale, J., Kehler, A., Cummings, C., Connell, J., & Herman, G. (2008). Effect of morphine and pregabalin compared with diphenhydramine hydrochloride and placebo on hyperalgesia and allodynia induced by intradermal capsaicin in healthy male subjects. The Journal of Pain, 9, 1088–1095. 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  285. Wang, K., Luo, Y., Asaki, T., Graven‐Nielsen, T., Cairns, B. E., Arendt‐Nielsen, T., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2017). Acid‐induced experimental muscle pain and hyperalgesia with single and repeated infusion in human forearm. Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 17, 260–266. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  286. Wanigasekera, V., Mezue, M., Andersson, J., Kong, Y., & Tracey, I. (2016). Disambiguating pharmacodynamic efficacy from behavior with neuroimaging: Implications for analgesic drug development. Anesthesiology, 124, 159–168. 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000924 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  287. Warncke, T., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Branebjerg, P. E. (1996). Effects of local and systemic ibuprofen on primary and secondary hyperalgesia in man. Current Therapeutic Research, 57, 937–949. [Google Scholar]
  288. Warncke, T., Stubhaug, A., & Jørum, E. (1997). Ketamine, an NMDA receptor antagonist, suppresses spatial and temporal properties of burn‐induced secondary hyperalgesia in man: A double‐blind, cross‐over comparison with morphine and placebo. Pain, 72, 99–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  289. Warncke, T., Stubhaug, A., & Jørum, E. (2000). Preinjury treatment with morphine or ketamine inhibits the development of experimentally induced secondary hyperalgesia in man. Pain, 86, 293–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  290. Wasner, G., Baron, R., & Jänig, W. (1999). Dynamic mechanical allodynia in humans is not mediated by a central presynaptic interaction of A beta‐mechanoreceptive and nociceptive C‐afferents. Pain, 79, 113–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  291. Wasner, G., Schattschneider, J., Binder, A., & Baron, R. (2004). Topical menthol–a human model for cold pain by activation and sensitization of C nociceptors. Brain, 127, 1159–1171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  292. Wegner, A., Elsenbruch, S., Maluck, J., Grigoleit, J. S., Engler, H., Jäger, M., Spreitzer, I., Schedlowski, M., & Benson, S. (2014). Inflammation‐induced hyperalgesia: Effects of timing, dosage, and negative affect on somatic pain sensitivity in human experimental endotoxemia. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 41, 46–54. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  293. Wehrfritz, A. P., Ihmsen, H., Schmidt, S., Müller, C., Filitz, J., Schüttler, J., & Koppert, W. (2010). Interaction of physostigmine and alfentanil in a human pain model. British journal of Anaesthesia, 104, 359–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  294. Wehrfritz, A., Schaefer, S., Troester, A., Noel, N., Bessiere, B., Apiou‐Sbirlea, G., Simonnet, G., Schuettler, J., & Richebé, P. (2016). A randomized phase I trial evaluating the effects of inhaled 50–50% N2 O‐O2 on remifentanil‐induced hyperalgesia and allodynia in human volunteers. European Journal of Pain, 20, 1467–1477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  295. Werner, M. U., Lassen, B., & Kehlet, H. (2002). Analgesic effects of dexamethasone in burn injury. Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, 27, 254–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  296. Werner, M. U., Perkins, F. M., Holte, K., Pedersen, J. L., & Kehlet, H. (2001). Effects of gabapentin in acute inflammatory pain in humans. Regional Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, 26, 322–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  297. Weyer‐Menkhoff, I., & Lötsch, J. (2018). Human pharmacological approaches to TRP‐ion‐channel‐based analgesic drug development. Drug Discov Today., 23, 2003–2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  298. Weyer‐Menkhoff, I., & Lötsch, J. (2019). Sensitization produces hyperalgesia to heat but not to cold stimuli in human volunteers. Clinical Journal of Pain, 35, 321–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  299. Witting, N., Svensson, P., Arendt‐Nielsen, L., & Jensen, T. S. (2000). Repetitive intradermal capsaicin: Differential effect on pain and areas of allodynia and punctate hyperalgesia. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 17, 5–12. 10.1080/08990220070256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  300. Wong, W., & Wallace, M. S. (2014). Determination of the effective dose of pregabalin on human experimental pain using the sequential up‐down method. The Journal of Pain, 15, 25–31. 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  301. Wrzosek, A., Woron, J., Dobrogowski, J., Jakowicka‐Wordliczek, J., & Wordliczek, J. (2015). Topical clonidine for neuropathic pain. Cochrane Database Systematic Review, 8, CD010967. 10.1002/14651858.CD010967.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  302. Xia, W., Mørch, C. D., & Andersen, O. K. (2016). Exploration of the conditioning electrical stimulation frequencies for induction of long‐term potentiation‐like pain amplification in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 2479–2489. 10.1007/s00221-016-4653-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  303. Yezierski, R. P., & Hansson, P. (2018). Inflammatory and neuropathic pain from bench to bedside: What went wrong? The Journal of Pain, 19, 571–588. 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.12.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  304. Yosipovitch, G., Szolar, C., Hui, X. Y., & Maibach, H. (1996). Effect of topically applied menthol on thermal, pain and itch sensations and biophysical properties of the skin. Archives of Dermatological Research, 288, 245–248. 10.1007/BF02530092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  305. You, D. S., Creech, S. K., Vichaya, E. G., Young, E. E., Smith, J. S., & Meagher, M. W. (2014). Effect of written emotional disclosure on secondary hyperalgesia in women with trauma history. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76(5), 337–346. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  306. Yucel, A., Miyazawa, A., Andersen, O. K., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2001). The effect of heat conditioning of the primary area before and after induction of hyperalgesia by topical/intradermal capsaicin or by controlled heat injury. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 18, 295–302. 10.1080/01421590120089677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  307. Yucel, A., Miyazawa, A., Andersen, O. K., & Arendt‐Nielsen, L. (2004). Comparison of hyperalgesia induced by capsaicin injection and controlled heat injury: Effect on temporal summation. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 21, 15–24. 10.1080/0899022042000201263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  308. Zambreanu, L., Wise, R. G., Brooks, J. C., Iannetti, G. D., & Tracey, I. (2015). A role for the brainstem in central sensitisation in humans. Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Pain, 114, 397–407. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  309. Zheng, Z., Gibson, S. J., Helme, R. D., & McMeeken, J. M. (2009). The effect of local anaesthetic on age‐related capsaicin‐induced mechanical hyperalgesia – A randomised, controlled study. Pain, 144, 101–109. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  310. Zimmermann, K., Deuis, J. R., Inserra, M. C., Collins, L. S., Namer, B., Cabot, P. J., Reeh, P. W., Lewis, R. J., & Vetter, I. (2013). Analgesic treatment of ciguatoxin‐induced cold allodynia. Pain, 154, 1999–2006. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material


Articles from European Journal of Pain (London, England) are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES