Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Aug 13;16(8):e0256229. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256229

Laughter influences social bonding but not prosocial generosity to friends and strangers

R I M Dunbar 1,*, Anna Frangou 2, Felix Grainger 1, Eiluned Pearce 1,3
Editor: Valerio Capraro4
PMCID: PMC8362988  PMID: 34388212

Abstract

Humans deploy a number of specific behaviours for forming social bonds, one of which is laughter. However, two questions have not yet been investigated with respect to laughter: (1) Does laughter increase the sense of bonding to those with whom we laugh? and (2) Does laughter facilitate prosocial generosity? Using changes in pain threshold as a proxy for endorphin upregulation in the brain and a standard economic game (the Dictator Game) as an assay of prosociality, we show that laughter does trigger the endorphin system and, through that, seems to enhance social bonding, but it does not reliably influence donations to others. This suggests that social bonding and prosociality may operate via different mechanisms, or on different time scales, and relate to different functional objectives.

Introduction

Human societies are characterised by high levels of prosociality, both in the sense of acting generously (altruistically) towards others and in the sense of cooperating with others on some communal task. Such behaviour is often viewed as being a generalised predisposition, with a prominent role for oxytocin [16]. In fact, altruism and cooperation are not distributed indiscriminately, but are directed disproportionately towards members of one’s own community and, especially, to close friends and family [712], suggesting that the prior existence of close social bonds may play an important role.

In primates in general, social bonds are underpinned by social grooming, and are mediated by β-endorphin upregulation in the brain [1315] via the highly specialised afferent C-tactile peripheral neuron system that responds only to light slow stroking [1618]. It is the endorphin system that provides the psychopharmacological underpinnings for social bonding [1924] in a way that creates stable groups out of dyadic ‘friendships’ [2527]. These bonded relationships are what allow primates to cooperate with, and behave altruistically towards, each other. This reflects a crucial contrast in the way the oxytocin and endorphin systems function: the one is endogenous (my oxytocin levels predispose me to behave prosocially towards you, but I cannot influence your behaviour) while the other acts exogenously (I can influence your β-endorphin levels directly and so make you be more prosocial towards me). In addition, endorphins have a serum half-life that is measured in hours [28], whereas that for oxytocin is measured in minutes [2932], with half-life in the CNS being longer but proportionately similar [28, 33].

Social grooming continues to function in humans just as it does in nonhuman primates [15, 34, 35]. However, increased demand for bonding larger numbers of individuals when time to do so is severely limited has resulted in grooming being augmented by a variety of other behaviours that also trigger the endorphin system without need for physical contact, thereby allowing more individuals to be ‘groomed’ simultaneously [36, 37]. These behaviours include laughter [38, 39], singing [40], dancing [41, 42], emotional storytelling [43]), feasting (social eating and alcohol consumption [4446] and the rituals of religion [47], all of which elevate pain thresholds and/or endorphin uptake. Of these, all but laughter have also been shown to increase the sense of social bonding with specific individuals [40, 43, 4750]. None, however, have been directly tested to determine whether they also influence the likelihood of prosociality (generosity to others).

The key issue here is whether social bonding behaviours like laughter trigger cooperation/generosity directly or whether generosity is the outcome of an intervening mechanism (e.g. a longstanding sense of obligation to another individual in well-bonded relationships). In the latter case, bonding is a state built up over an extended period of time, with the level of bonding reflecting both the frequency of interaction and the duration of the relationship [51]. We test between these two possibilities in a set of two experiments that seek to establish three key points: (1) whether laughter enhances the sense of social bonding, (2) whether laughter increases the level of generosity towards others, and (3) whether an increase in generosity towards strangers, in particular, is mediated via the endorphin system or a sense of social bonding, or both. We use the Dictator Game as our index of prosociality (generosity) because we are explicitly interested in altruism, not trust (which underpins most of the public good games used in micro-economic experiments). We follow common practice [40, 41] in using changes in pain threshold as a proxy for endorphin activation [52]. Because the genders differ strikingly in both sensitivity to pain [53] and the psychological mechanisms that underpin friendship [5456], we analyse the data separately for the two genders.

Experiment 1

Experiment 1 aimed to determine whether laughter influenced generosity via the endorphin system. We test three hypotheses: that, relative to those in a control condition, individuals who watch a comedy video (1) laugh more often, and therefore (2) exhibit higher pain thresholds, in consequence of which (3) they are more generous in their donations to other group members. To be certain that any such effects are due to the endorphin system and not simply to changes in emotional state [57, 58], we also tested for correlated changes in positive and negative affect.

Methods

Subjects were solicited via University lists: those who responded were invited to attend an experimental session in the Department with a friend (of either gender–all by one came with a friend of the same gender, however). In total, 66 subjects took part in the experiment. However, one pair who opted not to declare their gender and one pair who failed to complete all the tasks were excluded from the analyses, leaving 21 men and 41 women (mean age 24.6±9.0 years). Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: watching a comedy video alone or in a group of four (comedy conditions) or watching a golfing instruction video alone (control condition). The videos lasted ~20 mins in each case. While subjects were assigned individually to both of the ‘Alone’ conditions, they were assigned as a pair to the ‘Group’ condition. The requirement to bring a friend proved challenging, since the whole group session was lost if one person came to their appointed session alone. All subjects were paid a flat-rate fee of £5 for attending. Subjects were not aware of the purpose of the experiment.

Laughter while watching the video was recorded on a dictaphone hung from each subject’s neck; whether or not they laughed was subsequently sampled from the audio record at 15-sec intervals by AF. To determine pain threshold before and after watching the video, we used a standard sphygmomanometer blood pressure cuff on the dominant arm, recording how long the subject could stand the pain when the cuff was inflated to 300 Hg [38, 40]. The pretext was to measure physiological state. Pain thresholds were determined with subjects isolated in separate rooms while completing pre- and post-experimental paperwork and rating tasks. The index of interest was not absolute pain threshold but change in threshold after watching the video. After completing these tasks, subjects were invited to take part in two successive Dictator Games in which they could notionally donate some or all of their fee in units of £1 to (1) themselves versus the friend they came with or (2) themselves versus an unnamed stranger, for a maximum combined outlay of £10. Subjects did this task in isolation, and the order of the two donation tasks was randomised.

To determine whether any of the behavioural responses were due to affect rather than the endorphin-related effects of laughter (the two are not the same [43]), subjects completed the PANAS affect scale [59] before and after watching the video. The PANAS scale indexes positive and negative affect using 10 positive and 10 negative questions concerned with current emotional feelings, each rated on a 5-point scale. Scores for each scale were summed. Subjects were also asked to rate how emotionally close they felt to the friend they had come with on a 1–10 analogue scale.

Since, inevitably, laughter never occurred in the control (neutral) condition (it was explicitly designed not to elicit laughter), we use χ2 to compare whether laughter occurs or not (as a binary choice) for each subject across conditions. For all other dependent variables, we first tested for homogeneity of error variance using Levene’s test. Since in every case except negative affect (PANAS, p = 0.038), the null hypothesis was upheld, we use parametric analysis of variance to test for main effects due to condition and gender and, more importantly, interaction effects. In each case, we treat condition as a fixed factor and gender as a random variable. Note that, of the parametric tests, ANOVA is considered the most robust to departures from parametric assumptions, and in particular departures from the assumption of normality. All correlation tests are Spearman correlations. Whenever specific directional hypotheses are under consideration in the correlation tests, 1-tailed tests are appropriate because a significant negative correlation would be as much evidence against the hypothesis as a non-significant correlation. In all other cases, 2-tailed tests are used.

Ethical approval was provided under Departmental arrangements. All subjects were over 18 and provided informed consent on signature.

The data are given in online S1 Data”.

Results

Fig 1A plots the frequency of laughter for the three conditions. For both genders, the frequency of laughter increases from zero in the control condition to around 25% of samples in the group comedy condition. To test for a difference, we rescaled laughter frequencies as a simple binary choice (0 = no laughter; 1 = at least one scan sample with laughter). The differences across conditions, as a whole and for each gender separately, were significant (χ2 = 40.5, df = 2, p<0.0001; for each gender: χ2Women = 26.4, df = 2, p<0.0001; χ2Men = 14.1, df = 2, p = 0.001). Note that the data in Fig 1A suggest that laughter is approximately four times more likely in groups of four than when viewing the same comedy video alone.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Mean (±se) (a) laughter rate (number of 15-sec scans on which subject was audibly laughing), (b) change in positive affect score and (c) change in negative affect score, for men (unfilled symbols) and women (filled symbols) in Experiment 1.

Fig 1B and 1C plot the change in positive and negative affect (PANAS) across the three conditions. Positive affect increased across the conditions for both genders, and negative affect declined slightly in the two comedy conditions. However, only the increase in positive affect is significant (positive affect: condition, F2,56 = 91.5, p = 0.011, partial η2 = 0.989; gender, F1,56 = 1.2, p = 0.194; condition*gender interaction, F2,56 = 0.03, p = 0.975; negative affect: condition, F2,56 = 7.1, p = 0.124; gender, F1,56 = 3.6, p = 0.177; condition*gender interaction, F2,52 = 0.4, p = 0.661). On an individual subject basis, the frequency of laughter was significantly positively correlated with the change in positive affect (Spearman rS = 0.434, N = 62, p = 0.004) and negatively correlated with the change in negative affect (rS = -0.356, N = 62, p = 0.003) (1-tailed tests in both cases since opposite-sign results would not be expected).

Fig 2A plots the change in pain threshold. The data suggest a more muted response in women, but a steep linear increase in men across the three conditions. Analysis of variance yields non-significant main effects for both condition (F2,56 = 1.04, p = 0.491) and gender (F1,56 = 0.19, p = 0.706), but a significant condition*gender interaction (F2,56 = 5.33, p = 0.008, partial η2 = 0.160). This can be explained by the fact that, on the individual subject level, the frequency of laughter correlates significantly with the change in pain threshold in men (rS = 0.50, N = 21, p = 0.010 1-tailed positive) but not in women (rS = -0.140, N = 37, p = 0.705 1-tailed in a positive direction). Fig 2B plots the total amount donated in the two Dictator Games. There were no effects due to condition (F2,54 = 0.61, p = 0.621) or gender (F1,54 = 0.34, p = 0.612), and no interaction effect (F2,54 = 0.39, p = 0.677).

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Mean±se (a) Change in pain threshold and (b) total donations, for men (unfilled symbols) and women (filled symbols) in Experiment 1.

There was no correlation between total donation rate and pain threshold change in either gender (men: rS = 0.06, N = 19, p = 0.411; women: rS = -0.002, N = 41, p = 0.504; 1-tailed tests in both cases). Nor does donation rate correlate with laughter rate (women: rS = 0.17, N = 37, p = 0.165; men: rS = -0.05, N = 19, p = 0.582; all tests 1-tailed). Neither of the PANAS affect indices correlates significantly with changes in pain threshold (positive: rS = 0.05, p = 0.687; negative: rS = -0.12, p = 0.349) or with the amount of money donated (positive: rS = -0.03, p = 0.832; negative: rS = 0.01, p = 0.933) (N = 60, all tests 2-tailed, since no predictions are made). There was no correlation between subjects’ rating of how emotionally close they were to their friend and the money they donated to the friend (women: rS = 0.10, N = 41, p = 0.277; men: rS = 0.02, N = 19, p = 0.467; 1-tailed tests) or their total donation to the group as a whole (women: rS = 0.10, N = 41, p = 0.259; men: rS = 0.12, N = 19, p = 0.309; 1-tailed tests). More importantly, perhaps, there was no correlation in the Comedy/Group condition (the only condition in which friends were together) in the donation scores of pairs of friends (rS = -0.012, N = 8, p = 0.998 1-tailed).

Fig 3 plots the ratio of donation to Friend vs Self and to Stranger vs Friend as a function of experimental condition. The ratios do not vary significantly across condition or gender, and there is no interaction effect (Friend/Self: condition: F2,38 = 1.38, p = 0.421; gender: F1,38 = 1.81, p = 0.454; condition*gender interaction: F2,38 = 0.27, p = 0.762; Stranger/Friend: condition: F2,49 = 0.03, p = 0.973; 0; gender: F1,49 = 0.49, p = 0.778; condition*gender interaction: F2,49 = 0.25, p = 0.778). Note that, while subjects split their earnings more or less 50:50 with the friend, they were much less generous to the stranger (one-sample t-tests of H0[ratio = 1] in each case: Friend/Self: t43 = 0.64, p = 0.524; Stranger/Friend: t54 = -7.97, p<0.0001).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Mean±se ratio of (a) donation to friend divided by donation to self and (b) donation to stranger divided by donation to friend, for men (unfilled symbols) and women (filled symbols) in Experiment 1.

Discussion

Experiment 1 confirms previous studies that laughter triggered by comedy elevates pain thresholds [38], indicating activation of the endorphin system [39]. The results also confirm Provine’s [60] claim that people are much more likely to laugh at something when in a group than when they are alone. Provine simply noted whether or not someone laughed, whereas we estimated the proportion of time they spent laughing over a period of time, which not surprisingly yields a lower figure. Although there was an increase in positive affect and a reduction in negative affect following laughter, the magnitude of these effects were, at best, modest (and not significant), suggesting that affect cannot explain the results [38, 43, 61]. Donations did not increase with laughter or pain threshold. Subjects were more generous to friends than to strangers while not differentiating between themselves and a close friend (as has been found in previous experimental tests of Hamilton’s Rule [8]). In some cases, there were significant differences between the genders in their responses, as would be expected given the gender differences in both sensitivity to pain [53] and the psychological mechanisms that underpin friendship [55, 56].

The main result of interest is the lack of any robust correlation between donations and either laughter or change in pain threshold. There are three possible explanations for this. One is that subjects were asked to make notional donations to both friends and strangers in the same game: generosity to friends might have influenced attitudes to strangers and levelled up donations, or vice versa. A second possibility is that donations depend not on laughter (and the endorphin system) per se but on the perceived degree of bonding to other group individuals, and that bonding is a consequence of a long term relationship rather than an immediate response to some behaviour or physiological response. Although there was no correlation between money donated to the friend and the self-rated emotional closeness to the friend, our experimental manipulation may not have been sufficient to trigger the required level of bonding on the day to affect donation rates to strangers. The third possibility is that altruism (donations) and sociality are two separate processes, and only one of them depends on the endorphin system. We know from other experiments that triggering the endorphin system increases the sense of bonding to the group [40, 41, 43, 48, 62]. This would, therefore, imply that altruism depends on other motivations (e.g. a sense of fairness, socially imposed rules) that are not influenced by laughter. Experiment 2 sought to test between these alternatives.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 aimed (1) to confirm that laughter results in elevated pain thresholds in groups when these consist only of strangers, (2) to test the new hypothesis that laughter increases the sense of bonding to strangers and (3) to determine whether donation rates were influenced by laughter, endorphin upregulation (indexed by a change in pain threshold) or bonding. Of the three possible explanations for the results in Experiment 1, we test the first by ensuring that all participants were strangers. If generosity to friends influenced attitudes towards strangers involved in the same experiment, then a stranger-only condition should remove that influence and allow a natural response to emerge. We tested the second explanation by including a psychological bonding measure so as to test directly whether laughter caused a change in bonding across the experimental manipulation. Finally, we tested the third possibility by asking whether donations were predicted by laughter, pain or bonding.

Methods

Subjects were solicited from the Departmental subject panel, and were invited to attend an experimental session at a University location in preassigned groups of three or four. There was no overlap in the subject pool with Experiment 1. Experiment 2 followed the same design as Experiment 1 except that there were only two conditions: an experimental comedy condition using the same comedy video and a control condition (a TV wildlife documentary). The videos were 9m13s long in each case. 52 subjects (22 men; mean age 21.3 years) were randomly assigned to the two conditions. Care was taken to ensure that individuals did not know other group members, so that groups consisted of strangers. One subject did not complete all the tasks, and two subjects asked not to carry out the pain threshold task. Subjects received a £5 appearance fee. As before, subjects were not aware of the purpose of the experiment.

Since audio recordings made it difficult to pick up ‘silent’ laughs, subjects were videoed while watching the video and the number of laughter bouts, including broad smiles not accompanied by vocalisations, was subsequently coded by FG. The mean frequency of laughter so defined in the comedy condition was 4.2±0.23 bouts per minute (range 2.8–6.9). As in Experiment 1, there was no laughter at all in the control condition. Pain thresholds were determined before and after watching the video using the wall-sit (or Roman Chair) task [8, 41, 63], which is less susceptible to subjective bias than the pressure cuff test. For this task, subjects adopt a seated position with their back against a wall and hips and knees bent at right angles, and are asked to hold the position for as long as possible until they collapse. The task is a standard skiing exercise. It is initially very comfortable, but becomes exponentially painful after about 30 secs; few people can maintain the position for more than about a minute. Subjects were tested in their groups on this task, since past experience with this task indicates that the presence of others makes little difference to how long the position is held. As with the blood pressure task, we are concerned only with the magnitude of the change in performance either side of an activity.

As a measure of bonding, subjects completed the Inclusion-of-Other-in-Self (IOS) task [64] with respect to the other members of their group before and after watching the video [40, 48]. The IOS task presents subjects with a set of seven pairs of circles that vary between almost complete overlap and no overlap at all in what is effectively a 7-point Likert scale; subjects are asked to specify which pair of circles best represented their emotional closeness to the other members of their experimental group. After completing these tasks, subjects were invited to decide what percentage of their £5 participation fee they would be willing to donate to the person on their immediate left in their group (subjects completed this part of the experiment seated in a circle, and were instructed not to discuss their answers).

Ethical approval was provided by the University of Oxford Combined University Research Ethics Committee (CUREC) under departmental provisions. All subjects were over 18 and provided written informed consent.

The data are given in online S2 Data”.

Results

Fig 4A plots the frequency of laughter in the two conditions. As in Experiment 1, subjects were not expected to laugh in the control condition, so we rescaled rates of laughter to 0 (no laughter at all) or 1 (some laughter), and used a χ2 test to test for differences in laughter between conditions. There was significantly more laughter in the comedy condition (χ2 = 25.0, df = 1, p<0.0001), and this was true for both genders separately, though less strongly so for men (χ2Women = 25.2, df = 1, p<0.0001; χ2Men = 4.1, df = 1, p = 0.044). Fig 4B plots the equivalent values for change in pain threshold. There is a significant effect of condition (F1,45 = 1584.0, p = 0.016, partial η2 = 0.999), with a marginal effect due to gender (F1,45 = 125.4, p = 0.057), but no condition*gender interaction (F1,45 = 0.01, p = 0.945).

Fig 4.

Fig 4

Mean±se (a) frequency of laughter (number of laughs given) and (b) change in pain threshold, for men (unfilled symbols) and women (filled symbols) in Experiment 2.

Fig 5A plots change in IOS, or bonding index, across the experiment (rating after minus rating before). There are no main effects due to condition (F1,47 = 0.7, p = 0.549) or gender (F1,47 = 0.01, p = 0.932), but there is a significant condition*gender interaction effect (F1,47 = 13.28, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.220) with women exhibiting an increase between conditions and men showing a much smaller change. The value of donations (Fig 5B) yielded a significant difference across conditions (F1,47 = 187.0, p = 0.046, partial η2 = 0.995), but no main effect for gender (F1,47 = 0.5, p = 0.611) and no condition*gender interaction (F1,47 = 0.03, p = 0.866).

Fig 5.

Fig 5

Mean±se (a) change IOS score (with respect to rest of experimental group) and (b) donation, for men (unfilled symbols) and women (filled symbols) in Experiment 2.

There are significant correlations between laughter rate and both changes in pain threshold (Spearman rS = 0.267, p = 0.032) and the IOS bonding index (rS = 0.452, p = 0.001), but only IOS correlates with the level of donation (laughter: rS = 0.141, p = 0.163; pain threshold: rS = 0.053, p = 0.358; IOS: rS = 0.425, p = 0.036) (all tests 1-tailed for positive correlations, N = 49 subjects that completed all tasks). While laughter correlated positively (but weakly) with pain threshold change in both sexes (men, rS = 0.302, N = 22, p = 0.086; women, rS = 0.303, N = 27, p = 0.062), only women exhibited a significant positive correlation between laughter rate and IOS (men, rS = -0.021, N = 22, p = 0.537; women, rS = 0.623, N = 29, p<0.001). For women, but not men, there was a weak (but only marginally significant) correlation between laughter rate and donations (men, rS = -0.163, N = 22, p = 0.766; women, rS = 0.288, N = 29, p = 0.065), and between IOS and donations (men, rS = 0.229, N = 22, p = 0.153; women, rS = 0.282, N = 29, p = 0.069). For neither gender, did pain threshold change correlate with donations (men, rS = 0.001, N = 22, p = 0.234; women, rS = 0.106, N = 27, p = 0.299). However, only the relationship for laughter with pain threshold and (for women) with IOS would survive correction for multiple testing. Indeed, a multiple regression with donation as the outcome variable and laughter rate, change in pain threshold and change in IOS as predictor variables was not significant, either overall (F3,45 = 1.30, p = 0.287) or for the two genders individually (men: F3,18 = 0.50, p = 0.690; women: F3,23 = 1.39, p = 0.272), with no individual variables significant.

Discussion

Experiment 2 confirmed the main finding from Experiment 1 that laughter induces a change in pain threshold (implying activation of the brain’s endorphin system). More importantly, it also confirmed that there is an associated change in the sense of bonding to the group as a consequence of laughter. However, neither change in pain threshold nor change in perceived bonding correlated at all strongly with level of generosity to strangers. The fact that men did not exhibit as strong a bonding response as women (Fig 5A) might have been due to their much lower rates of laughter when watching the comedy video (Fig 4A); alternatively, it may simply reflect the fact that men’s friendships are more casual and less intense than those of women [54, 65].

Rates of laughter were higher in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1 for women but not men, but it is not possible to say whether this was due to an increase in audible or inaudible laughter. However, the difference between the experiments in the laughter rates of the two genders would not explain the patterns for either pain threshold change or donation rate between the two experiments.

Of the three alternative explanations for the Experiment 1 results, the first is excluded by the experimental design (friends are excluded) and the second (that it is bonding, not laughter, that determines donation level) does not yield a significant effect. The third possibility (that bonding and prosociality are separate processes underpinned by different (neuro-)psychological mechanisms) is supported by the lack of correlation between donation and any of the other three variables, making this the most likely explanation.

General discussion

Taken together, these results confirm that laughter elevates pain thresholds, a proxy for activation of the brain’s endorphin system [39], and that laughter is also associated with an increased sense of bonding to strangers (at least in women). Although laughter resulted in greater generosity to strangers in Experiment 2, the significance level was on the margin (p≈0.05), there was no effect in Experiment 1; moreover, in neither experiment was there was a quantitative relationship between the amount of laughter and the size of donation. More importantly, change in pain threshold (indicating endorphin upregulation) did not correlate with size of donation in either experiment. The data in Fig 3 confirm that generosity to friends is much greater than generosity to strangers [8], suggesting that longstanding bonds trump any instantaneous bonds created by laughter. Experiment 1 allowed us to rule out affect as a likely explanation for these results. Given that laughter has been shown to upregulate the brain’s endorphin system [39] and that both endorphin receptor density [21] and baseline pain threshold [66] correlate with social engagement (indexed by attachment style and number of friends, respectively), Experiment 2 allows us to conclude that the relationship between laughter and enhanced bonding reported here is mediated by the brain’s endorphin system, whereas generosity (at least to strangers) is not, suggesting that bonding and prosocial generosity may be underpinned by two different psychological mechanisms.

The suggestion that two mechanisms may be involved is reinforced by studies that have used behavioural synchrony to induce a sense of bonding. Sullivan et al. [67] showed that behavioural synchrony in a physical exercise (a phenomenon known to enhance sense of bonding [41, 48] directly influenced willingness to cooperate with other group members on a public good game, but that this was not mediated by a change in pain threshold. Using a similar experimental design, Lang et al. [68] found that engaging in a highly synchronised physical exercise increased both the perceived likeability of the confederate with whom they interacted and willingness to invest in a Trust game with them; however, endorphin upregulation (indexed as pain threshold change) mediated only performance on the investment game. Note that to assess generosity, both these studies used a standard economic Public Good Game, which has the form of a trust task (subjects invest in a central pool in the expectation that everyone else will do so, otherwise they risk losing their capital and would have done better to hold onto their money). When played repeatedly against strangers, such tasks invariably exhibit a decline of investments into the common pool with successive trials [69, 70] as individuals become increasingly aware that they can benefit by behaving selfishly.

Thus, despite using a variety of different ways of triggering the endorphin system and several different kinds of prosociality tasks, these studies and ours collectively seem to suggest that bondedness and prosociality may be underpinned by different mechanisms. We suggest that this is because cooperative tasks, or tasks that are essentially forms of economic trading, are not central to the dynamics of core human social groups (i.e. personal social networks), even though they may provide the principal function for very large scale groupings (e.g. tribes in traditional societies) [71]. Personal social networks, like all small scale communities, exist to provide mutual support and ensure coordination rather than trading arrangements, and are based on bonding and a sense of obligation rather than economic exchange [36, 72]. At the larger social scale beyond this, most individuals are to all intents and purposes strangers [72], and the rules of trading are those of commerce and not those of community. Such arrangements will be more open to the establishment of cultural norms [10].

Endorphin-based social bonding may require repeated interactions to build into a fully bonded relationship that leads to unconditional prosociality (Fig 3). In contrast, instantaneous bonding to a group of strangers may not, of itself, make a transactional relationship with a stranger more likely. In other words, it is only when the bond reaches a specific strength that unconditional prosociality becomes the norm in a dyadic relationship [8, 51]. This may make the use of conventional economic games inappropriate as tools for exploring human social (as opposed to economic) behavior [10, 70]. In other words, there is an important distinction between bonded relationships (as an intermediate step to support unconditional cooperation) and purely transactional (or trading) relationships. In short, human (and anthropoid primate [26, 27]) sociality involves personalised relationships of obligation and trust, but not necessarily one-off transactional relationships where different rules of trade and fairness may apply.

Although the effects we found appear to be robust (and are confirmed by studies using different designs), we should note that they depend on using changes in pain threshold as a proxy for endorphin activation. While there is extensive evidence from pain research to justify this [52, 53, 73, 74], a more direct test for endorphin activation would obviously provide stronger confirmation. Such tests are not, however, straightforward because endorphins do not cross the blood-brain barrier [75, 76]. CNS endorphin assays require either lumbar puncture or PET scanning, neither of which are pleasant to experience or free of side effects. Pain threshold assays are generally considered the better option, although the use of endorphin antagonists (e.g. naltrexone) may provide indirect evidence for the role of endorphins [41]. A second issue is that are all our participants were students. A replication with older subjects, a wider ethnic range, and larger samples would be desirable. None of these, of course, are novel limitations, but they do remind us that experiments are always subject to practical constraints that may limit their generalizability until they have been replicated. Our limited range of videos might also seem a weakness of experimental design. However, we are not here testing the causes of laughter; rather, we are testing the consequences of laughter. So long as our subjects laugh, we have no interest in why they laugh (i.e. whether some videos are funnier than others). In any case, we should note that other studies of laughter [38] have used a wider variety of comedy (and control) videos, and all comedy videos stimulate laughter.

A more serious weakness of our design is the fact that subjects made purely notional decisions about how to allocate their endowment. Their decisions might have been very different if they actually had to give their money away to a stranger. In mitigation, we merely note that, in previous studies where subjects have been asked to earn money for others [8], the results have been broadly similar to those reported here. More importantly, given that people invariably claim to be more generous than they actually are [77], this could only weaken the relationship between laughter and generosity and thus reinforce our claim that prosociality is underpinned by a different mechanism.

In sum, we show that, like a number other specifically social human behaviours (including singing, dancing and feasting [40, 48, 49]), laughter promotes social bonding in a group, and can do so even among complete strangers (although this may be true mainly for women). Unlike these other behaviours, laughter is something we share with the great apes [78], and is likely to have deep evolutionary and biological roots; in contrast, the other behaviours have more recent historical origins. Perhaps because of this, generosity (prosociality) towards strangers is unrelated either to laughter and the endorphin flux it generates or to the momentary psychological sense of bonding that these produce. Rather, altruistic acts of this kind seem to involve different psychological or cultural mechanisms from those designed to create bonded social relationships and bonded groups.

Supporting information

S1 Data. Study 1.

(XLSX)

S2 Data. Study 2.

(XLSX)

Data Availability

Data included as ESM files.

Funding Statement

(1) RIMD: School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography [no grant numbers] (2) RIMD: Department of Experimental Psychology The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005;435: 673–676. doi: 10.1038/nature03701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carter CS, Grippo AJ, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Ruscio MG, Porges SW. Oxytocin, vasopressin and sociality. Prog Brain Res. 2008;170: 331–336. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00427-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008;322: 900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mikolajczak M, Gross JJ, Lane A, Corneille O, de Timary P, Luminet O. Oxytocin makes people trusting, not gullible. Psych Sci. 2010;21: 1072–1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Daughters K, Manstead AS, ten Velden FS, De Dreu CK. Oxytocin modulates third-party sanctioning of selfish and generous behavior within and between groups. Psychoneuroendocrin. 2017;77: 18–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hurlemann R, Marsh N. Deciphering the modulatory role of oxytocin in human altruism. Rev Neurosci. 2017;28: 335–342. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2016-0061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dunbar RIM, Clark A, Hurst NL. Conflict and cooperation among the Vikings: contingent behavioural decisions. Ethol Sociobiol. 1995;16: 233–246. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Madsen E, Tunney R, Fieldman G, Plotkin H, Dunbar RIM., Richardson J, et al. Kinship and altruism: a cross-cultural experimental study. Brit J Psychol. 2007;98: 339–359. doi: 10.1348/000712606X129213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Goeree JK, McConnell MA, Mitchell T, Tromp T, Yariv L. The 1/d law of giving. Amer Econ J Microecon. 2010;2: 183–203. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, et al. “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28: 795–815. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Engel C. Dictator games: a meta study. Exp Econ. 2011;14: 583–610. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Candelo N, Eckel C, Johnson C. Social distance matters in Dictator Games: evidence from 11 Mexican villages. Games. 2018;9: 77 [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B. Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrin. 1989;14: 155–161. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(89)90065-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Martel F, Nevison CM, Simpson MJ, Keverne EB. Effects of opioid receptor blockade on the social behavior of rhesus monkeys living in large family groups. Develop Psychobiol. 1995;28: 71–84 doi: 10.1002/dev.420280202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nummenmaa L, Tuominen L, Dunbar RIM, Hirvonen J, Manninen S, Arponen E, et al. Reinforcing social bonds by touching modulates endogenous μ-opioid system activity in humans. NeuroImage 2016;138: 242–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Vallbo Å, Olausson H, Wessberg J, Norrsell U. A system of unmyelinated afferents for innocuous mechanoreception in the human skin. Brain Res. 1993;628: 301–304. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90968-s [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Löken LS,Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F, Olausson H. Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nature Neurosci. 2009;12: 547–548. doi: 10.1038/nn.2312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Olausson H, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F, Vallbo A. The neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34: 185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dunbar RIM. The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuros Biobehav Rev. 2010;34: 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Machin A, Dunbar R. The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence. Behaviour. 2011;148: 985–1025. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Nummenmaa L, Manninen S, Tuominen, Hirvonen J, Kalliokoski K., Nuutila P, et al. Adult attachment style is associated with cerebral μ-opioid receptor availability in humans. Human Brain Mapp. 2015;36: 3621–3628. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Loseth GE, Ellingsen DM., Leknes S. State-dependent μ-opioid modulation of social motivation–a model. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8: 430. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pearce E, Wlodarski R, Machin A, Dunbar RIM. Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. PNAS. 2017;114: 5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pearce E., Wlodarski R, Machin A, Dunbar RIM. The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: a replication study. Adapt Human Behav Physiol. 2018;4, 400–422. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0101-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Silk J. Using the ’F’-word in primatology. Behaviour. 2002;139: 421–446. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour. 2010;147: 775–803. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Massen JJM, Sterck EHM, de Vos H. Close social associations in animals and humans: functions and mechanisms of friendship. Behaviour. 2010;147: 1379–1412. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Foley KM, Kourides IA, Inturrisi CE, Kaiko RF, Zaroulis CG, Posner JB,.et al. β-Endorphin: Analgesic and hormonal effects in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1979;76: 5377–5381. doi: 10.1073/pnas.76.10.5377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rydén G, Sjöholm I. Half-life of oxytocin in blood of pregnant and non-pregnant women. Eur J Endocrin. 1969;61: 425–431. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Homeida AM, Cooke RG. Biological half-life of oxytocin in the goat. Res Vet Sci. 1984;37: 364–365. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wachs EA, Gorewit RC, Currie WB. Half-life, clearance and production rate for oxytocin in cattle during lactation and mammary involution I. Domest Anim Endocrin. 1984;1: 121–140. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Paccamonti DL, Pycock JF, Taverne MAM, Bevers M, van der Weijden GC, et al. PGFM response to exogenous oxytocin and determination of the half‐life of oxytocin in nonpregnant mares. Equine Vet J. 1999;31: 285–288. doi: 10.1111/j.2042-3306.1999.tb03818.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev. 2001;8: 629–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Suvilehto J, Glerean E, Dunbar RIM, Hari R, Nummenmaa L. Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. PNAS. 2015;112: 13811–16. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1519231112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Suvilehto J, Nummenmaa L, Harada T, Dunbar RIM, Hari R, Turner R, et al. Cross-cultural similarity in relationship-specific social touching. Proc R Soc Lond. 2019; 286B: 20190467. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0467 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Dunbar RIM. Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans. Phil Trans R Soc Lond. 2012;367B: 1837–1846. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dunbar RIM. Virtual touch and the human social world. Curr Opin Behav Sci (in press). [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dunbar RIM., Baron R, Frangou A, Pearce E, van Leeuwen EJC, Stow J, et al. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc R Soc Lond. 2012;279B: 1161–1167. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Manninen S, Tuominen L, Dunbar RIM, Karjalainen T, Hirvonen J, Arponen E, et al. Social laughter triggers endogenous opioid release in humans. J Neurosci. 2017;37: 6125–6131. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0688-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pearce E, Launay J, Dunbar RIM. The ice-breaker effect: singing mediates fast social bonding. R Soc Open Sci. 2015; 2: 150221. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tarr B, Launay J, Cohen E, Dunbar RIM. Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol Letts. 2015;11: 20150767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tarr B, Launay J, Dunbar RIM. Naltrexone blocks endorphins released when dancing in synchrony. Adapt Human Behav Physiol. 2017; 3: 241–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dunbar RIM., Teasdale B, Thompson J, Budelmann F, Duncan S, van Emde Boas E, et al. Emotional arousal when watching drama increases pain threshold and social bonding. R Soc Open Sci. 2016;3: 160288. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tuulari JJ, Tuominen L, de Boer FE, Hirvonen J, Helin S, Nuutila P, et al. Feeding releases endogenous opioids in humans. J Neurosci. 2017;37: 8284–8291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0976-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ree JM. Endorphins and experimental addiction. Alcohol. 1996;13: 25–30. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(95)02036-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Modesto-Lowe V, Fritz EM. The opioidergic-alcohol link. CNS Drugs. 2005;19: 693–707. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200519080-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Tarr B, Launay J, Dunbar RIM. Silent disco: dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evol Human Behav. 2016;37: 343–349. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Dunbar RIM. Breaking bread: the functions of social eating. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. 2017;3: 198–211. doi: 10.1007/s40750-017-0061-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dunbar RIM, Launay J, Wlodarski R, Robertson C, Pearce E, Carney J, et al. Functional benefits of (modest) alcohol consumption. Adapt Human Behav Physiol. 2017;3: 118–133. doi: 10.1007/s40750-016-0058-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Charles SJ, Farias M, van Mulukom V, Saraswati A, Dein S, Watts F, et al. Blocking mu-opioid receptors inhibits social bonding in rituals. Biol Letts. 2020;16: 20200485. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2020.0485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sutcliffe AJ, Dunbar RIM, Binder J, Arrow H. Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Brit J Psychol. 2012;103: 149–168. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02061.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zubieta J-K, Smith YR, Bueller JA, Xu K, Kilbourn MR, Jewett DM, et al. Regional mu opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. Science. 2001;293: 311–315. doi: 10.1126/science.1060952 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Zubieta J-K, Smith Y, Bueller JA, Xu Y, Kilbourn MR, Jewett DM, et al. Mu-opioid receptor-mediated antinociception differs in men and women. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 5100–5107. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-12-05100.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dunbar RIM. The anatomy of friendship. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22: 32–51. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Pearce E., Wlodarski R., Machin A, Dunbar RIM. Exploring the links between dispositins, romantic relationships, support networks and community inclusion in men and women. PLoS One. 2019. a;14: e0216210. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Pearce E, Wlodarski R, Machin A, Dunbar RIM. Genetic influences on social relationships: sex differences in the mediating role of personality and social cognition. Adapt Human Behav Physiol. 2019. b;5: 331–351. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.J A, Owren MJ. Not all laughs are alike: voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psych Sci. 2001;12: 252–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Fredrickson BL. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Phil Trans R Soc Lond. 2004;359B:1367–1377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54: 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Provine RR. Laughter: A Scientific Investigation. London: Penguin, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.B, Jetten J, Fasoli F. Cleansing the soul by hurting the flesh: The guilt-reducing effect of pain. Psych Sci. 2011;22: 334–335. doi: 10.1177/0956797610397058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Weinstein D, Launay J, Pearce E, Dunbar R, Stewart L. Singing and social bonding: changes in connectivity and pain threshold as a function of group size. Evol Human Behav. 2016; 37: 152–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Harrison F, Sciberras J, James R. Strength of social tie predicts cooperative investment in a human social network. PLoS One. 2011;6: e18338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Aron A, Aron EN, Smollan D. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J Pers Soc Psych. 1992;63: 596–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dàvid-Barrett T, Rotkirch A, Carney J, Behncke Izquierdo I, Krems J, Townley D, et al. Women favour dyadic relationships, but men prefer clubs. PLoS One. 2015;10: e0118329. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Johnson K, Dunbar RIM. Pain tolerance predicts human social network size. Scient Repts. 2016;6: 25267. doi: 10.1038/srep25267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sullivan P, Gagnon M, Gammage K, Peters S. Is the effect of behavioral synchrony on cooperative behavior mediated by pain threshold? J Soc Psychol. 2015;155: 650–660. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2015.1071766 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Lang M, Bahna V, Shaver JH, Reddish P, Xygalatas D. Sync to link: Endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation. Biol Psychol. 2017;127: 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Ostrom E, Gardner R, Walker J. Rules, Games and Common-Pool Resources. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Burton-Chellew MN, West SA. Prosocial preferences do not explain human cooperation in public-goods games. PNAS. 2013;110: 216–221. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210960110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lehmann J, Lee PC, Dunbar RIM. Unravelling the evolutionary function of communities. In: Dunbar RIM, Gambl C, Gowlett JAJ(eds) Lucy to Language: the Benchmark Papers, pp. 245–276. Oxford: Oxford University Press; (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dunbar RIM. Structure and function in human and primate social networks: Implications for diffusion, network stability and health. Proc R Soc Lond. 2020;476A: 20200446. doi: 10.1098/rspa.2020.0446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Huang CJ, Liu HF, Su NY, Hsu YW, Yang CH, Chen CC, et al. Association between human opioid receptor genes polymorphisms and pressure pain sensitivity in females. Anaesthesia. 2008;63: 1288–1295. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2008.05760.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Hagelberg N, Aalto S, Tuominen L, Pesonen U, Någren K, Hietala J, et al. Striatal μ-opioid receptor availability predicts cold pressor pain threshold in healthy human subjects. Neurosci Letts. 2012;521: 11–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bloom E. The endorphins: a growing family of pharmacologically pertinent peptides. Ann Rev Pharm Toxi. 1983:23: 151–170. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pa.23.040183.001055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kalin NH, Loevinger BL. The central and peripheral opioid peptides: their relationships and functions. Psych Clinics N Amer. 1983;6: 415–428. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Epley N, Dunning D. Feeling "holier than thou": are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self-or social prediction? J Pers Soc Psych. 2001;79: 861–875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Davila Ross M, Owren J, Zimmermann E. Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Curr Biol. 2009;19: 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Valerio Capraro

10 Jun 2021

PONE-D-21-08705

Laughter, Social Bonding, and the Generosity of Friends and Strangers

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Dunbar,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please find below the reviewers' comments, as well as those of mine.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 25 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please change "female” or "male" to "woman” or "man" as appropriate, when used as a noun (see for instance https://apastyle.apa.org/style-grammar-guidelines/bias-free-language/gender).

3. Please modify the title to ensure that it is meeting PLOS’ guidelines (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title). In particular, the title should be "specific, descriptive, concise, and comprehensible to readers outside the field" and in this case it is not sufficiently informative and specific about your study's scope and methodology.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

Additional Editor Comments:

I have now collected two reviews from two experts in the field, whom I thank for their punctual, detailed, and constructive feedback. Both reviewers think that the topic of the paper is interesting, but their final judgment is very split, with one recommending minor revision and one recommending rejection. Looking at the negative review, I find that the main criticisms regard a lack of connection of this work with previous related work, a lack of clarity in the experimental design of Study 1 and probably an overstatement of the results of the second experiment. I think that these points can potentially be addressed with a suitable revision. Therefore, I have decided to invite you to revise your work for Plos One. Needless to say that all reviewers' comments, from both reviewers, should be addressed.

I am looking forward for the revision.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This study tries to answer two questions: Does laughter increases social bonding, and does social bonding in return increase pro-sociality? (see abstract). Both questions are very interesting. However, the authors do not link their research sufficiently to the already existing evidence. For instance, on page 4 they write: “While it may seem obvious that a sense of social bonding and altruism ought to be associated, this key relationship has never been directly tested. However, most studies of cooperation and altruism have used economic games between strangers, ignoring the fact that cooperation and altruism occurs mainly between individuals who have explicit relationships with each other or belong to the same small community.” This is an inadequate description of the literature in experimental game theory. There are numerous studies investigating the effect of social distance on cooperation and prosocial behavior. I provide just a few examples:

1) Goeree et al. (2010): The 1/d Law of Giving. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics 2: 183–203.

2) Engel (2011): Dictator games: a meta study. Experimental Economics 14: 583–610.

3) Candelo et al. (2018) Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages. Games 9,77.

I am also confused about the design of the first experiment. On page 5 the authors write “We use an experimental design to test (1) whether laughter increases the level of generosity towards others, (2) whether laughter enhances the sense of social bonding, and (3) whether an increase in generosity towards strangers, in particular, is mediated via the endorphin system or a sense of social bonding, or both.” But experiment 1 does not include a measure of social bonding, but looks only at the relation of laughter on generosity. However, laughing should only increase social bonding in the group condition, to those one is laughing with. How this was taken into account in the matching of the recipients in the dictator game remains unclear. Overall, my impression is that experiment 1 does help to answer the questions raised by the authors. It introduces confusion and makes it harder to follow the line of reasoning in the manuscript.

The second experiment addresses many problems of experiment 1 and is much better linked to the research questions. However, the results are inconclusive. For instance, the finding that laughter elevates bonding is only found for females. This fact is downplayed in the general discussion when the authors write “Taken together, these results confirm that laughter elevates pain thresholds, a proxy for activation of the brain’s endorphin system (Manninen et al. 2017), and suggest that laughter is also associated with an increased sense of bonding to strangers (albeit more strongly in females than males).” Similarly, on page 18: “In sum, we show that, like a number other specifically social human behaviours (…), laughter promotes social bonding in a group, and can do so even among complete strangers.” In fact, it is only found in females in experiment 2. Peculiarly, this important caveat is not discussed by the authors, and their general conclusion is only partly supported by the results of experiment 2.

In sum, I am not convinced that the manuscript makes an important contribution to the literature on bonding and giving in dictator games.

Reviewer #2: This paper studies the relations between laughter, tolerance to pain, bonding, and generosity. In two experimental studies, the authors manipulate participants’ exposure to a comedy video (which successfully induced laughter) and then measure laughter (using audio and video recording), tolerance to pain (via a pressure-cuff task in Exp. 1 and a wall-sit task in Exp. 2), emotional closeness and bonding, as well as generosity in hypothetical decision-making tasks (using dictator games). They find that laughter is associated with higher pain thresholds (which they interpret as consistent with the activation of the endorphin system), and with a stronger sense of bonding to strangers (especially among women, see Exp. 2), but that induced laughter is not associated with increased generosity (i.e., donations in the dictator game).

Overall, this is an interesting and clearly written manuscript. I agree with the authors that it is important to study prosocial behaviors in a relational context, looking into interactions between different relationship partners (here, both friends and strangers). I think the reported experiments can provide insights into social bonding and generosity—and the role of laughter in promoting one versus the other. That said, I have some questions and recommendations regarding the methods and analyses used, as well as regarding the interpretation of results.

1. As the authors note in the discussion, the sample sizes of the two experiments are relatively small. It could be then that the studies are underpowered to detect some effects of interest. I would recommend that the authors report how they determined the targeted sample size for the studies. Further, the authors can consider conducting sensitivity analyses to give readers some idea of the smallest effect size that their studies would be able to detect, given their sample sizes.

2. It is not clear to me why gender is included as a predictor in all main analyses. Are there a priori reasons to expect that men and women will differ in the DVs and, perhaps more importantly, that gender would moderate the effects of interest? Presumably, presenting the main analyses without the gender main effects and interactions would not change results by much, but could make for a clearer presentation of main findings.

3. One reason that the authors observe no effects of laughter on generosity could be that they use hypothetical dictator games. Given that decisions are hypothetical, there is no cost associated with being generous. It seems that indeed participants generally follow equality norms, donating about half of their endowment across conditions. The authors acknowledge this issue in the discussion and suggest that “asking people to actually give away their money might be expected to produce a less generous outcome than the one we observed.” But that might be exactly the situation in which laughter, endorphin activation, and bonding could make a difference. If decisions are costly, having experienced something funny and having bonded with others could make one more willing to pay the cost and be generous (than if no common bonding experience has occurred).

4. In the second experiment, the authors use videos, based on which they can code more nuanced expressions of laughter (e.g., smiles that are not audible). Have they perhaps tried to also use a similar coding of audible laughter as in Experiment 1? If so, are the occurrences of audible laughter similarly frequent across the experiments?

5. In the results of Experiment 2, the authors note that “none of these [the other variables of interest] correlate with the level of donation.” However, the results in parentheses suggest that there is a statistically significant and moderately strong association between bonding (IOS) and generosity.

6. In the discussion, the authors note that “it seems likely that the relationship between laughter and enhanced bonding is mediated by the brain’s endorphin system, whereas generosity to strangers is underpinned by a different mechanism.” My impression is that the experiments in the paper do not directly test this—i.e., a mediating role of endorphin activation and pain tolerance. Perhaps the authors could clarify whether this is speculative.

7. Finally, a related point: In the discussion, p. 15, the authors cite studies by Sullivan et al. (2015) and Lang et al. (2017) which have found evidence that mechanisms other than endorphin system activation might motivate generosity. Could the authors provide a few more details in text about what these alternative potential mechanisms might be?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Aug 13;16(8):e0256229. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256229.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


22 Jul 2021

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

RESPONSE: this has now been done.

2. Please change "female” or "male" to "woman” or "man" as appropriate, when used as a noun (see for instance https://apastyle.apa.org/style-grammar-guidelines/bias-free-language/gender).

RESPONSE: this has now been done.

3. Please modify the title to ensure that it is meeting PLOS’ guidelines (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title). In particular, the title should be "specific, descriptive, concise, and comprehensible to readers outside the field" and in this case it is not sufficiently informative and specific about your study's scope and methodology.

RESPONSE: this has now been done.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

RESPONSE: this has now been done.

Additional Editor Comments:

I have now collected two reviews from two experts in the field, whom I thank for their punctual, detailed, and constructive feedback. Both reviewers think that the topic of the paper is interesting, but their final judgment is very split, with one recommending minor revision and one recommending rejection. Looking at the negative review, I find that the main criticisms regard a lack of connection of this work with previous related work, a lack of clarity in the experimental design of Study 1 and probably an overstatement of the results of the second experiment. I think that these points can potentially be addressed with a suitable revision. Therefore, I have decided to invite you to revise your work for Plos One. Needless to say that all reviewers' comments, from both reviewers, should be addressed.

I am looking forward for the revision.

Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1:

This study tries to answer two questions: Does laughter increases social bonding, and does social bonding in return increase pro-sociality? (see abstract). Both questions are very interesting. However, the authors do not link their research sufficiently to the already existing evidence. For instance, on page 4 they write: “While it may seem obvious that a sense of social bonding and altruism ought to be associated, this key relationship has never been directly tested. However, most studies of cooperation and altruism have used economic games between strangers, ignoring the fact that cooperation and altruism occurs mainly between individuals who have explicit relationships with each other or belong to the same small community.” This is an inadequate description of the literature in experimental game theory. There are numerous studies investigating the effect of social distance on cooperation and prosocial behavior. I provide just a few examples:

1) Goeree et al. (2010): The 1/d Law of Giving. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics 2: 183–203.

2) Engel (2011): Dictator games: a meta study. Experimental Economics 14: 583–610.

3) Candelo et al. (2018) Social Distance Matters in Dictator Games: Evidence from 11 Mexican Villages. Games 9,77.

RESPONSE: We thank the reviewer for drawing our attention to these papers, which we now cite. We should note, however, that our paper is not concerned with testing whether social distance is a factor in giving but with whether laughter, acting via the endorphin system, facilitates giving. We raise the issue of social distance simply to point to something we need to consider as a confound. Our point (which remains true) is (a) that people discriminate between degrees of social closeness (as the papers we cite also show) and, in passing, (b) that this isn’t often taken into account in microeconomic experiments (as the Engel meta-analysis of the field itself confirms: only 1% of the subjects in his total sample took part in experiments where social distance was a measured variable). The text has been amended to reflect this more clearly.

I am also confused about the design of the first experiment. On page 5 the authors write “We use an experimental design to test (1) whether laughter increases the level of generosity towards others, (2) whether laughter enhances the sense of social bonding, and (3) whether an increase in generosity towards strangers, in particular, is mediated via the endorphin system or a sense of social bonding, or both.” But experiment 1 does not include a measure of social bonding, but looks only at the relation of laughter on generosity. However, laughing should only increase social bonding in the group condition, to those one is laughing with. How this was taken into account in the matching of the recipients in the dictator game remains unclear. Overall, my impression is that experiment 1 does help to answer the questions raised by the authors. It introduces confusion and makes it harder to follow the line of reasoning in the manuscript.

RESPONSE: The aims cited by the referee are not the aims of Experiment 1; they refer to the combined overall aims of the study. The aims of Experiment 1 are stated in the text as: “[those] who watch a comedy video (1) laugh more often, and therefore (2) exhibit higher pain thresholds, in consequence of which (3) they are more generous in their donations than those that watch a control video.” To avoid confusion, the aims of the study as a whole on p. 5 have been restructured to reflect the sequence of experiments.

The second experiment addresses many problems of experiment 1 and is much better linked to the research questions. However, the results are inconclusive. For instance, the finding that laughter elevates bonding is only found for females. This fact is downplayed in the general discussion when the authors write “Taken together, these results confirm that laughter elevates pain thresholds, a proxy for activation of the brain’s endorphin system (Manninen et al. 2017), and suggest that laughter is also associated with an increased sense of bonding to strangers (albeit more strongly in females than males).”

RESPONSE: The results are not, in fact, inconclusive. They show that laughter does not trigger generosity via the endorphin system, but rather generosity has more to do with history of the relationship. In other words, that bonding and prosociality are two different things, acting via different routes. That is a very significant finding in the context of understanding social processes (even if it is disappointing from a purely economic point of view). We have revised the text to clarify this.

Similarly, on page 18: “In sum, we show that, like a number other specifically social human behaviours (…), laughter promotes social bonding in a group, and can do so even among complete strangers.” In fact, it is only found in females in experiment 2. Peculiarly, this important caveat is not discussed by the authors, and their general conclusion is only partly supported by the results of experiment 2.

RESPONSE: This has now been rectified.

In sum, I am not convinced that the manuscript makes an important contribution to the literature on bonding and giving in dictator games.

RESPONSE: We fear that this observation misses the point of the study: this study is not about micro-economic experiments, the Dictator Game or altruistic prosociality, but about whether laughter facilitates generosity. We know of no experiment that actually tests that claim – but would, of course, be glad to know of any that we have not come across in the economics literature.

Reviewer #2:

This paper studies the relations between laughter, tolerance to pain, bonding, and generosity. In two experimental studies, the authors manipulate participants’ exposure to a comedy video (which successfully induced laughter) and then measure laughter (using audio and video recording), tolerance to pain (via a pressure-cuff task in Exp. 1 and a wall-sit task in Exp. 2), emotional closeness and bonding, as well as generosity in hypothetical decision-making tasks (using dictator games). They find that laughter is associated with higher pain thresholds (which they interpret as consistent with the activation of the endorphin system), and with a stronger sense of bonding to strangers (especially among women, see Exp. 2), but that induced laughter is not associated with increased generosity (i.e., donations in the dictator game).

Overall, this is an interesting and clearly written manuscript. I agree with the authors that it is important to study prosocial behaviors in a relational context, looking into interactions between different relationship partners (here, both friends and strangers). I think the reported experiments can provide insights into social bonding and generosity—and the role of laughter in promoting one versus the other. That said, I have some questions and recommendations regarding the methods and analyses used, as well as regarding the interpretation of results.

1. As the authors note in the discussion, the sample sizes of the two experiments are relatively small. It could be then that the studies are underpowered to detect some effects of interest. I would recommend that the authors report how they determined the targeted sample size for the studies. Further, the authors can consider conducting sensitivity analyses to give readers some idea of the smallest effect size that their studies would be able to detect, given their sample sizes.

RESPONSE: Part of the reason for the small sample size was given in the Methods: subjects had to come in pairs, and if one member failed to turn up the whole session had to be abandoned. In addition, it just proved difficult to recruit people, mainly due to the out-of-the-way location where we ran the experiment. For this reason, partial eta’s were given to indicate the magnitude of the effects in each case.

2. It is not clear to me why gender is included as a predictor in all main analyses. Are there a priori reasons to expect that men and women will differ in the DVs and, perhaps more importantly, that gender would moderate the effects of interest? Presumably, presenting the main analyses without the gender main effects and interactions would not change results by much, but could make for a clearer presentation of main findings.

RESPONSE: Gender was included because there are well-attested differences in (a) basal pain threshold and (b) sociality and prosocial demeanour. We have added detail on this to the Introduction.

3. One reason that the authors observe no effects of laughter on generosity could be that they use hypothetical dictator games. Given that decisions are hypothetical, there is no cost associated with being generous. It seems that indeed participants generally follow equality norms, donating about half of their endowment across conditions. The authors acknowledge this issue in the discussion and suggest that “asking people to actually give away their money might be expected to produce a less generous outcome than the one we observed.” But that might be exactly the situation in which laughter, endorphin activation, and bonding could make a difference. If decisions are costly, having experienced something funny and having bonded with others could make one more willing to pay the cost and be generous (than if no common bonding experience has occurred).

RESPONSE: The usual expectation (and finding) is that people are more generous in virtual/hypothetical situations than when they have to hand over actual money. This should mean that the differences between laughter and no-laughter conditions will be smaller than they already are. We add a comment to this effect to the Discussion.

4. In the second experiment, the authors use videos, based on which they can code more nuanced expressions of laughter (e.g., smiles that are not audible). Have they perhaps tried to also use a similar coding of audible laughter as in Experiment 1? If so, are the occurrences of audible laughter similarly frequent across the experiments?

RESPONSE: Rates of laughter in Expt 2 were similar to those in Expt 1 for males, but higher for females. We cannot now determine whether the latter was due to increased audible or inaudible laughter (the video recordings had to be destroyed after the initial data were extracted). This difference did not, however, relate to patterns of donation or pain difference. A note on this has been added to the text.

5. In the results of Experiment 2, the authors note that “none of these [the other variables of interest] correlate with the level of donation.” However, the results in parentheses suggest that there is a statistically significant and moderately strong association between bonding (IOS) and generosity.

RESPONSE: But, as stated immediately afterwards, this does not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple tests. We applied a precautionary principle here.

6. In the discussion, the authors note that “it seems likely that the relationship between laughter and enhanced bonding is mediated by the brain’s endorphin system, whereas generosity to strangers is underpinned by a different mechanism.” My impression is that the experiments in the paper do not directly test this—i.e., a mediating role of endorphin activation and pain tolerance. Perhaps the authors could clarify whether this is speculative.

RESPONSE: The point is a good one. However, while it is correct that we did not test directly whether endorphins mediate the relationship in this paper, we have established elsewhere both that laughter triggers the endorphin system [Manninen et al. 2017, J Neurosience] and that endorphin upregulation correlates with more intense bonding [Nummenmaa et al. 2015, Human Brain Mapping; Tarr et al. 2015, Evol Human Behav]. The Discussion has been adjusted to reflect this point.

7. Finally, a related point: In the discussion, p. 15, the authors cite studies by Sullivan et al. (2015) and Lang et al. (2017) which have found evidence that mechanisms other than endorphin system activation might motivate generosity. Could the authors provide a few more details in text about what these alternative potential mechanisms might be?

RESPONSE: Details now added.

Decision Letter 1

Valerio Capraro

3 Aug 2021

Laughter Influences Social Bonding But Not Prosocial Generosity to Friends and Strangers

PONE-D-21-08705R1

Dear Dr. Dunbar,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Valerio Capraro

5 Aug 2021

PONE-D-21-08705R1

Laughter Influences Social Bonding But Not Prosocial Generosity to Friends and Strangers

Dear Dr. Dunbar:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data. Study 1.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Data. Study 2.

    (XLSX)

    Data Availability Statement

    Data included as ESM files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES