Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychology. 2021 Mar;35(3):265–275. doi: 10.1037/neu0000720

Socioeconomic and psychosocial mechanisms underlying racial/ethnic disparities in cognition among older adults

Laura B Zahodne 1, Neika Sharifian 2, A Zarina Kraal 3, Afsara B Zaheed 4, Ketlyne Sol 5, Emily P Morris 6, Nicole Schupf 7, Jennifer J Manly 8, Adam M Brickman 9
PMCID: PMC8363204  NIHMSID: NIHMS1727716  PMID: 33970660

Abstract

Objective:

Racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging are only partly attributable to socioeconomic indicators. Psychosocial factors, such as discrimination and perceived control, also differ across racial/ethnic groups, and emerging literature highlights their potential role in contributing to cognitive disparities in addition to socioeconomic status.

Method:

1,463 older adults (51% Hispanic, 27% non-Hispanic Black, 22% non-Hispanic White) in the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project completed cognitive and psychosocial measures, including a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, Everyday and Major Experiences of Lifetime Discrimination scales, and the Perceived Control scale. Mediation models quantified separate indirect effects of Black race and Hispanic ethnicity on global cognitive composite scores through education, income, discrimination, and external perceived control.

Results:

Educational attainment, income, and perceived control each mediated racial/ethnic disparities in global cognition. Socioeconomic indicators (i.e., lower education, lower income) explained approximately 50% of the Black-White and Hispanic-White disparities in global cognition, and more external perceived control explained an additional 5–8%. Hispanics reported the lowest levels of discrimination, while non-Hispanic Blacks reported the highest levels. However, neither everyday nor major lifetime discrimination was associated with global cognition. Significant racial/ethnic disparities in global cognition remained after accounting for the included socioeconomic and psychosocial factors.

Conclusions:

This study suggests that psychosocial factors may explain racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging above and beyond socioeconomic indicators. More external perceived control, which could reflect chronic exposure to interpersonal and institutional marginalization, may be a particularly salient psychosocial risk factor for poorer cognitive aging among non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic older adults.

Keywords: Locus of Control, Discrimination, Cognitive Aging, African American, Hispanic

Introduction

Racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging are well-documented (Manly & Mungas, 2015). Upon formal neuropsychological testing, both non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic older adults exhibit lower scores than non-Hispanic Whites across cognitive domains, including episodic memory, language, executive functioning, working memory, processing speed, vocabulary, and visuospatial functioning (Boone et al., 2007; Brewster et al., 2014; Díaz-Venegas et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2015; Sisco et al., 2015; Sol et al., 2020; Wilson et al., 2015; Zahodne et al., 2016). Racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive performance are multiply determined (Glymour & Manly, 2008), and additional research is needed to better understand the variety of causes across different racial/ethnic groups.

Much of the literature on racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging has focused on socioeconomic mediators, including educational attainment (e.g., Weuve et al., 2018) and income (e.g., Haq & Penning, 2019). On average, both Hispanic (Duncan et al., 2006) and non-Hispanic Black (Bloome, 2014; Donovan, 1984; Keith & Benson, 1992; Reynolds, 1989) older adults report lower educational attainment and income than non-Hispanic Whites. In turn, both lower educational attainment and lower income each predict lower cognitive performance in late life when considered together (Koster et al., 2005; Zahodne et al., 2015). However, these objective indicators of socioeconomic status do not fully explain racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging.

Emerging research highlights the role of psychosocial factors, such as racially-patterned social stress, in explaining cognitive inequalities. For example, everyday discrimination mediated Black-White disparities in episodic memory level and change in the Health and Retirement Study (Zahodne, Kraal, Sharifian, et al., 2019; Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). However, findings regarding associations between everyday discrimination and cognitive outcomes have been mixed. While some studies found negative associations (Barnes et al., 2012; Zahodne et al., 2020), some found positive associations (Pugh et al., 2020), and some found no associations (Zahodne et al., 2017). Because these studies focused on everyday forms of discrimination experienced through interpersonal interactions (e.g., microaggressions), even less is known about potential links between major experiences of lifetime discrimination and cognitive aging. Thus, additional research is needed to clarify the relationship between different types of discrimination and cognitive aging and to identify other measures of psychosocial stress that may be relevant to racial/ethnic disparities in cognition.

Above and beyond discrimination, lower perceived control mediated Black-White disparities in episodic memory and executive functioning in the National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (Zahodne et al., 2017). Lower perceived control also contributed to Black-White and Hispanic-White disparities in episodic memory in the Health and Retirement Study (Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). The construct of perceived control originated in social learning theory (Rotter, 1966), and it refers to the extent to which individuals feel like they have control over important life outcomes (Lefcourt, 2014). Perceived control reflects a learned view of the self and the environment, and it can change over time and in response to experience (Eizenman et al., 1997). While early work on the control construct considered internal and external control beliefs as two ends of a single continuum, more recent work emphasizes internal and external perceived control as separate dimensions, with more external perceived control characterized by beliefs about environmental or interpersonal constraints that limit instrumentality (Lefcourt, 2014; Lachman, Neupert & Agrigoroaei, 2011). Compared with non-Hispanic Whites, both Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks report more external perceived control (Mirowsky & Ross, 1983, 1990). In turn, more external perceived control is linked to worse subsequent cognitive aging (Caplan & Schooler, 2003; Seeman et al., 1996). Drawing on cognitive behavioral theory (Bandura, 1997), the relationship between perceived control and cognitive outcomes has been theorized to be reciprocal, and external perceived control may lead to lower cognitive performance via behavioral, motivational, and affective mechanisms (Lachman, 2006).

Together, these studies suggest that the subjective psychosocial experiences of non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic adults may contribute to racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging in addition to objective socioeconomic resources. The biopsychosocial model proposed by Clark and colleagues (1999) provides a theoretical foundation for how racially-patterned social stress may affect health. These authors not only consider the structural and institutional forces that shape racial inequalities in health, but also outline how subjective social experiences can contribute to health disparities via psychological and physiological stress responses. However, this seminal paper focused exclusively on African Americans. Indeed, the vast majority of the extant literature on psychosocial contributors to racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging focuses on Black-White differences, with very few studies including Hispanic older adults (c.f., Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019).

In the United States (U.S.), the broad health impacts of racially-patterned social stress can be seen not only for non-Hispanic Blacks (Beatty Moody et al., 2018; Chae et al., 2010; Guyll et al., 2001; Lewis et al., 2010; Zahodne, Kraal, Zaheed, et al., 2019), but also for Hispanics (Beatty Moody et al., 2018; Cuevas et al., 2016; Molina et al., 2019), who represent the largest and fastest-growing minority group in the U.S. (Colby & Ortman, 2015). For example, greater racial/ethnic discrimination was associated with psychological distress, unhealthy body mass index, and sleep disturbances in a sample of 1,332 Hispanic adults in the Texas City Stress and Health Study (Garcini et al., 2018). Similarly, additional studies have linked greater discrimination to worse overall health (Brondolo et al., 2011), mental health (Howarter & Bennett, 2013), and physical health-related quality of life (Molina et al., 2019) in Hispanic samples. Hispanic immigrants may also face psychosocial stressors related to acculturation, anti-immigrant policies and attitudes, and disruption of social networks (Cervantes, Padilla, & Salgado de Snyder, 1990). Additional research is needed to understand how socioeconomic and psychosocial disadvantages contribute to cognitive disparities across Hispanics, non-Hispanic Blacks, and non-Hispanic Whites.

The current study sought to extend the literature on racial/ethnic inequalities in cognitive aging by examining the separate contributions of multiple socioeconomic and psychosocial factors in a sample of 1,463 Hispanic, non-Hispanic Black, and non-Hispanic White older adults living in northern Manhattan. Specifically, we quantified the proportions of Black-White and Hispanic-White disparities in performance on a comprehensive neuropsychological battery that were explained by educational attainment, income, discrimination, and perceived control. Based on prior research, we predicted that non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic older adults would demonstrate lower cognitive scores compared to non-Hispanic White older adults, and these racial and ethnic differences would be partly attributable to lower socioeconomic status, greater discrimination, and more external perceived control.

Method

Participants and Procedures

The 1,463 individuals in this sample participated in the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP; Manly et al., 2005; Tang et al., 2001), a longitudinal, community-based study of aging and dementia in northern Manhattan. In brief, adults aged 65 and older living in the study catchment area were identified from Medicare records or a commercial marketing company in two initial waves (1992, 1999), and ongoing since 2009. WHICAP visits take place approximately every 18–24 months in participants’ homes or at the Columbia University Medical Center. At each visit, bilingual research staff administer a battery of cognitive, functional, and health measures in the participant’s preferred language (English or Spanish).

The current sample included only participants who received a set of psychosocial measures that was added to the core WHICAP battery in 2017. Thus, while WHICAP is a longitudinal study, only cross-sectional data were available for the current analyses. Additional inclusion criteria for the current study were: (1) availability of at least one cognitive outcome; (2) availability of data on basic sociodemographics (i.e., age, sex/gender, and race/ethnicity). Characteristics of the analytic sample (N = 1,463) are shown in Table 1. This study complied with the ethical rules for human experimentation stated in the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local institutional review board. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the sample by race/ethnicity

Hispanic (N = 740) Non-Hispanic Black (N = 395) Non-Hispanic White (N = 328) Group Differencesa

Age 80.26 (7.53) 77.06 (7.15) 77.50 (6.37) B=W<H
Sex/gender (% female) 73.11 68.35 56.10 W<B<H
Education (0–20) 7.76 (4.37) 13.50 (3.02) 16.26 (2.92) H<B<W
Income (1–12) 6.02 (2.44) 8.50 (2.61) 10.49 (2.27) H<B<W
Depressive symptoms (0–10) 1.71 (1.91) 1.36 (1.73) 1.24 (1.55) W=B<H
Vascular/cardiometabolic diseases (0–3) 1.37 (0.85) 1.24 (0.82) 0.89 (0.82) W<B<H
History of stroke (% yes) 8.97 5.10 2.48 W=B<H
Everyday Discrimination (1–6) 1.10 (0.22) 1.46 (0.59) 1.22 (0.38) H<W<B
Lifetime Discrimination (0–9) 0.36 (0.73) 1.28 (1.74) 0.87 (1.20) H<W<B
Perceived Constraints (0–8) 2.34 (2.13) 1.34 (1.65) 1.01 (1.36) W<B<H
Episodic Memory (z-score composite) −0.14 (0.84) 0.35 (0.91) 0.82 (0.75) H<B<W
Language (z-score composite) −0.18 (0.68) 0.55 (0.56) 1.06 (0.45) H<B<W
Speed (z-score composite) −0.02 (1.02) 0.69 (0.76) 1.07 (0.62) H<B<W
Visuospatial Functioning (z-score composite) −0.03 (0.72) 0.52 (0.47) 0.84 (0.36) H<B<W
Global Cognition (z-score composite) −0.09 (0.66) 0.52 (0.57) 0.95 (0.43) H<B<W
a

Significant group differences were determined using chi-square tests for categorical variables and analyses of variance with post-hoc Tukey’s Honest Significant Difference tests for continuous variables.

Cognitive Outcomes

Cognition in WHICAP is assessed with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery (Stern et al., 1992). English measures were translated into Spanish, and back translation and reconciliation ensured comparability of English and Spanish versions. In the current study, 93% of Hispanics were tested in Spanish. Factor analysis previously revealed that the WHICAP neuropsychological battery assesses the following four cognitive domains: episodic memory, language, speed, and visuospatial functioning, and this factor structure is invariant across English and Spanish speakers (Siedlecki et al., 2010).

Based on this factor analysis, cognitive composites in WHICAP are derived by converting all cognitive test scores to z-scores using means and standard deviations from the overall sample at baseline and averaging them within each domain. Episodic memory composite scores include immediate, delayed, and recognition trials from the Selective Reminding Test (Buschke & Fuld, 1974). Language scores include a modified 15-item Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983), letter fluency (i.e., C, F, L), category fluency (i.e., animals), the Similarities subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (Wechsler, 1981), and the Repetition and Comprehension subtests of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass, 1983). Speed scores include Color Trails I and II (D’Elia et al., 1994). Visuospatial scores include recognition and matching trials from the Benton Visual Retention Test (Benton, 1955), the Rosen Drawing Test (Rosen, 1981), and the Identities and Oddities subtest of the Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis, 1976). In the current study, episodic memory, language, speed, and visuospatial composites were averaged to obtain a comprehensive indicator of global cognitive functioning, which was used as the outcome variable in all models.

Socioeconomic and Psychosocial Mediators

Based on previous literature (Zahodne et al., 2017; Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019), the current study considered multiple potential mediators of associations between race/ethnicity and cognitive performance, including socioeconomic status, discrimination, and perceived control. Correlations among these predictors of interest are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.

Correlations among the predictors of interest

1 2 3 4

All participants

1. Education -
2. Income .584* -
3. Everyday Discrimination .177* .057 -
4. Lifetime Discrimination .250* .111* .471* -
5. Perceived Constraints −.374* −.337* −.002 −.047

Hispanic

1. Education -
2. Income .324* -
3. Everyday Discrimination .076 .032 -
4. Lifetime Discrimination .079* .020 .365* -
5. Perceived Constraints −.248* −.214* .181* .094*

Non-Hispanic Black

1. Education -
2. Income .327* -
3. Everyday Discrimination .162* −.056 -
4. Lifetime Discrimination .243* −.011 .430* -
5. Perceived Constraints −.280* −.195* −.033 −.065

Non-Hispanic White

1. Education -
2. Income .184* -
3. Everyday Discrimination .027 −.035 -
4. Lifetime Discrimination .029 −.174* .405* -
5. Perceived Constraints −.118* −.233* .220* .194*
*

p < .05

Socioeconomic status was operationalized with two variables: educational attainment and monthly household income. Educational attainment was self-reported and ranged from 0 to 20. Monthly self-reported household income was operationalized as a 12-category variable ranging from $450 or less to more than $4,000 and was treated as a continuous variable.

Discrimination was operationalized with the Everyday Discrimination (Williams et al., 1997) and Major Experiences of Lifetime Discrimination (Williams et al., 1997) scales, modified to be administered orally due to the wide range of literacy levels in WHICAP. Everyday discrimination comprised 10 items assessing how often participants experience unfair treatment in their day-to-day life (e.g., “You are treated with less respect than other people”). Responses are on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from Never (6) to Almost Every Day (1). Items were reverse-scored and averaged so that higher scores correspond to greater everyday discrimination. Major Experiences of Lifetime Discrimination comprised 9 dichotomous items assessing the occurrence of unfair treatment in relation to a variety of major life events (e.g., “At any time in your life, have you ever been unfairly fired from a job?”). Items were summed so that higher scores correspond to greater lifetime discrimination.

Perceived control was operationalized using the Constraints subscale of the Perceived Control scale (Lachman & Weaver, 1998), which was modified to be administered orally. This subscale corresponds to external perceived control and comprises 8 items querying whether participants agree with statements such as “What happens in my life is often beyond my control” and “I sometimes feel I am being pushed around in my life.” Items were summed so that higher scores correspond to more perceived constraints (i.e., more external perceived control). Of note, the Mastery subscale of the Perceived Control scale, which corresponds to internal perceived control, was not used in the current study given prior empirical evidence that it does not predict cognitive performance in older adults (Sharpe et al., 2014). Furthermore, previous studies indicate that internal perceived control does not vary by race and does not mediate Black-White disparities in cognitive outcomes in older adults (Zahodne et al., 2015).

Race/Ethnicity

Race and ethnicity were determined using the format of the 2000 U.S. Census. Blacks and Whites were all non-Hispanic, and Hispanics could have identified as any race. To estimate Black-White and Hispanic-White differences, race/ethnicity was operationalized as dummy-coded variables with non-Hispanic Whites as the reference group.

Covariates

All models controlled for age and sex/gender. A sensitivity analysis additionally controlled for depressive symptoms, vascular/cardiometabolic disease burden, and stroke, each of which show reliable associations with cognitive aging outcomes (Diniz, Butters, Albert, Dew, & Reynolds, 2013; Marden, Mayeda, Tchetgen Tchetgen, Kawachi & Glymour, 2017; Savva et al., 2010; Verhaeghen, Borchelt & Smith, 2003). We conceptualize these mental and physical health constructs as potential mediators of the effects of socioeconomic and psychosocial disadvantage on cognition, in line with previous theoretical work (Clark et al., 1999; Zahodne et al., 2019). Therefore, including them as covariates in the primary model could lead to an underestimate of the cognitive effects of the socioeconomic and psychosocial predictors of interest. However, we acknowledge that depressive symptoms, vascular/cardiometabolic diseases, and stroke could also potentially influence socioeconomic conditions and/or psychosocial functioning. Therefore, they could represent confounders of certain associations, which is why we included them as covariates in a sensitivity analysis. Depressive symptoms were quantified with a 10-item version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Irwin, Artin, & Oxman, 1999). Vascular/cardiometabolic disease burden was quantified as the sum of the self-reported presence/absence of hypertension, diabetes, and heart disease. Stroke was operationalized as the self-reported presence/absence of a history of stroke.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were computed using SPSS version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Groups were compared using chi square tests for categorical variables and analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with post-hoc Tukey’s Honest Significant Difference tests for continuous variables. Mediation models were conducted in Mplus version 8 (Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, 2011) using maximum likelihood estimation. All mediators were modeled simultaneously and allowed to covary. Missing data were managed with full information maximum likelihood, which can handle non-random missingness due to variables included in the models (e.g., age, race/ethnicity). Direct effects were defined as the association between race/ethnicity and cognition, adjusted for all mediators and covariates. Indirect effects were defined as the product of the association between race/ethnicity and a mediator (e.g., education) and the association between that mediator and cognition, adjusted for covariates. Total effects were defined as the sum of direct and indirect effects and reflect the association between race/ethnicity and cognition adjusted only for covariates. Because covariates were included to control for both exposure-mediator and mediator-outcome confounding, mediation models were fully saturated, resulting in perfect model fit.

Results

Racial/Ethnic Differences

As shown in Table 1, there were significant racial/ethnic differences in all variables of interest. With regard to socioeconomic status, Hispanics reported the lowest levels of education and income, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks, then non-Hispanic Whites. Hispanics also reported the lowest levels of everyday and lifetime discrimination, followed by non-Hispanic Whites, then non-Hispanic Blacks. Hispanics reported the most external perceived control, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks, then non-Hispanic Whites. Hispanics obtained the lowest cognitive scores across all domains, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks, then non-Hispanic Whites. Hispanics reported more depressive symptoms than non-Hispanic Blacks and Whites, who did not differ from each other. Hispanics reported the most vascular/cardiometabolic diseases, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks, then non-Hispanic Whites. More Hispanics reported a history of stroke compared with both non-Hispanic Blacks and Whites, who did not differ from each other.

As shown in Table 2, patterns of inter-correlation among the predictors of interest were not identical across the three racial/ethnic groups. While eduation and income were positively correlated with one another and negatively correlated with external perceived control among all groups, these variables exhibited different patterns of association with the discrimination measures across groups. Specifically, more frequent everyday discrimination was associated with more education among non-Hispanic Blacks, but not among Hispanics or non-Hispanic Whites. Similarly, more instances of lifetime discrimination were associated with more education among Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks, but not among non-Hispanic Whites. Finally, more discrimination (both everyday and lifetime) was associated with more external perceived control among Hispanics and non-Hispanic Whites, but not among non-Hispanic Blacks.

Mediation Model

Results of the mediation model are shown in Table 3 and Figure 1. As shown, there were total effects of race/ethnicity on global cognition. Controlling for age and sex/gender, both non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic participants obtained lower scores than non-Hispanic Whites. These associations were partially mediated by education, income, and perceived control, but not by everyday discrimination or lifetime discrimination. Compared with non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks and Hispanics reported lower education and income, as well as more external perceived control. In turn, lower education, lower income, and more external perceived control were each associated with worse global cognition. Education explained 29% of the Black-White disparity and 39% of the Hispanic-White disparity. Income explained an additional 16% of the Black-White disparity and 16% of the Hispanic-White disparity. External perceived control explained an additional 5% of the Black-White disparity and 8% of the Hispanic-White disparity. Significant direct effects indicated that racial/ethnic differences in global cognition remained even after considering all of the included mediators and covariates.

Table 3.

Standardized estimates (95% confidence intervals) from the mediation model

Black-White Disparity Hispanic-White Disparity

Total effect −0.276 (−0.315 – −0.237)* −0.664 (−0.700 – −0.628)*
Indirect effects through:
 Education −0.079 (−0.096 – −0.063)* −0.260 (−0.294 – −0.225)*
 Income −0.044 (−0.058 – −0.030)* −0.107 (−0.136 – −0.076)*
 Everyday Discrimination 0.008 (−0.001 – 0.018) −0.003 (−0.008 – 0.001)
 Lifetime Discrimination 0.003 (−0.002 – 0.008) −0.003 (−0.009 – 0.002)
 Perceived Constraints −0.014 (−0.023 – −0.004)* −0.054 (−0.068 – −0.041)*
Direct effect −0.150 (−0.187 – −0.113)* −0.236 (−0.286 – −0.186)*
*

p < .05

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Mediation model. Significant paths are shown as black lines, with corresponding standardized parameter estimates and standard errors. Non-significant paths are depicted with gray, dotted lines. For simplicity, covariates (i.e., age, sex/gender) and covariances between mediators are not shown.

Sensitivity Analyses

Results were unchanged when participants with a consensus diagnosis of dementia (n=172) were excluded from the analysis. In WHICAP, dementia diagnoses are made by a consensus group of neurologists and neuropsychologists based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) using all available neuropsychological, functional, and medical data. A separate sensitivity analysis considered depressive symptoms, vascular/cardiometabolic diseases, and stroke as additional covariates, and the pattern of results was unchanged.

Finally, a separate series of sensitivity analyses was conducted to determine whether effects of discrimination and perceived control had been underestimated due to the inclusion of the other mediators. In separate models that included only a discrimination measure, age, and sex/gender, neither everyday (β=0.029; 95% CI: −0.015 – 0.072) nor lifetime (β=0.031; 95% CI: −0.004 – 0.007) discrimination was associated with global cognition. Similarly, neither everyday (β=0.031; 95% CI: −0.007 – 0.070) nor lifetime (β=0.021; 95% CI: −0.010 – 0.053) discrimination was associated with global cognition in models that included only one discrimination measure, age, sex/gender, education, and income. In a separate model that included only the perceived control measure, age, and sex/gender, external perceived control showed a stronger association with global cognition (β=−0.245; 95% CI: −0.279 – −0.211) and explained a greater proportion of the Hispanic-White (12%) and Black-White (7%) disparities in global cognition compared with the primary model. Specifically, external perceived control explained an additional 4% of the Hispanic-White disparity, which is 50% more than the 8% explained by this variable in the primary model. For the Black-White disparity, external perceived control explained an additional 2%, which is 40% more than the 5% explained by this variable in the primary model.

Discussion

This study of a large, racially/ethnically diverse sample of older adults living in northern Manhattan provides evidence that both socioeconomic and psychosocial experiences contribute to late-life cognitive disparities. Lower educational attainment was the largest mediator of both Black-White and Hispanic-White disparities in global cognition, followed by lower income and more external perceived control (i.e., the perception of environmental or other constraints that limit control over important life outcomes).

Socioeconomic Mediators of Cognitive Disparities

The finding that racial/ethnic differences in educational attainment represented the strongest contributor to cognitive disparities is highly consistent with previous studies (Aiken Morgan et al., 2010; Sisco et al., 2015; Yaffe et al., 2013; Zahodne et al., 2017). Lower educational attainment among Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks is largely attributable to structural inequities, such as racial disparities in wealth and racially-patterned residential segregation, which result in differential educational opportunities for many minority children and young adults (Donovan, 1984; Keith & Benson, 1992; Reynolds, 1989). Because the majority of Hispanic participants in the current study were born and educated in the Carribbean, Hispanic-White disparities in educational attainment also reflect cross-national differences in compulsory school laws, educational quality, achievement, and educational attainment (Schleicher, 2018). Of note, the importance of educational experiences is likely to have been underestimated in the current study, as it did not include measures of educational quality. Single-word reading ability has been used as a proxy for educational quality and helps to explain Black-White cognitive disparities above and beyond years of education (Manly et al., 2002). However, measures of single-word reading ability were not included in the current multiethnic study because different reading tests were necessarily used in English versus Spanish speaking participants.

In addition to education, lower income among Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks compared with non-Hispanic Whites also explained a substantial proportion of cognitive disparities, which is in line with previous research (Mehta et al., 2004; Schwartz et al., 2004). In the U.S., the median income of Black families is approximately 60% that of White families, and this income disparity has remained consistent since the 1960s (Bloome, 2014). While income levels vary widely across Hispanic subgroups in the U.S., national data suggest that Hispanic families’ incomes are approximately 73% that of non-Hispanic White families (Mora & Dávila, 2018). Of note, the current study may have underestimated the importance of economic resources due to the lack of measures of wealth (e.g., financial assets). Wealth may be a better indicator of economic resources among older adults in the U.S. due to leveling effects of retirement and social security (Holtz-Eakin & Smeeding, 1994).

Psychosocial Mediators of Cognitive Disparities

Above and beyond socioeconomic indicators, more external perceived control contributed to both Hispanic-White and Black-White disparities in cognition, which is in line with previous studies (Zahodne et al., 2017; Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). The current study extends these prior results from national data sets to a regionally-representative sample of diverse older adults living in the same neighborhood (roughly the same five square miles) who were tested in-person with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery.

Both Hispanic and non-Hispanic Black older adults in this study reported more external perceived control than non-Hispanic Whites. These racial/ethnic differences are in line with previous work suggesting that repeated experiences of inequity and racism can lead to social and economic constraints, as well as demoralization and fatalism among historically marginalized groups (Mirowsky & Ross, 1983, 1990). In turn, perceiving more external control has been theorized to directly interfere with cognitive performance by heightening anxiety, cognitive rumination, and/or self-doubt (Bandura, 1989; Bandura & Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura, 1989).

Perceiving more external control may also lead to worse cognitive aging indirectly. For example, perceiving more external control has been associated with lower participation in healthy behaviors that have been linked to better cognitive aging (Mueller et al., 2013; Scarmeas et al., 2009), such as cognitively-stimulating activities, refraining from smoking, and eating a balanced diet (Bandura, 1981, 1986, 1988; Lachman et al., 2011). Individuals who feel less in control of their lives may not perceive the utility of personal health behaviors, particularly in the context of other, greater threats to health. They may also engage in behaviors that compromise physical health as a means of coping with psychological distress (Mezuk et al., 2013). Perceived control also appears to have implications for stress responding. Perceiving less external control predicts more adaptive coping (Gourounti et al., 2012) and buffers physiological stress responses related to both acute and lifetime stressors (Bollini et al., 2004; Elliot et al., 2017). In turn, a substantial literature implicates stress processes in poorer cognitive aging (Aggarwal et al., 2014; Machado et al., 2014; Peavy et al., 2009). The current study provides additional support for the relevance of perceived control to the study of cognitive aging and also highlights its particular role in cognitive disparities, which has received relatively less attention in the literature to date.

The finding that non-Hispanic Black older adults reported more discrimination than non-Hispanic Whites is highly consistent with some previous studies (Barnes et al., 2004; Zahodne et al., 2017; Zahodne et al., 2019). However, the extant literature contains mixed findings regarding associations between discrimination and cognitive outcomes. In the current study, neither everyday nor lifetime discrimination was associated with global cognition. This finding is consistent with a previous study of non-Hispanic Black and White adults in the National Survey of Midlife in the United States, which found that everyday discrimination was not associated with measures of episodic memory or executive functioning (Zahodne et al., 2017). However, the lack of association between discrimination and cognition contrasts somewhat with a previous study of diverse older adults in the Health and Retirement Study (Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). While more frequent everyday discrimination was not associated with worse episodic memory level independent of external perceived control in that study, it was associated with faster subsequent episodic memory decline (Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). Among Black older adults, early cross-sectional analyses in the Minority Aging Research Study indicated that more frequent discrimination was associated with worse episodic memory and perceptual speed (Barnes et al., 2012). However, more recent longitudinal analyses in that study indicate that more frequent discrimination is associated with better initial working memory, as well as slower subsequent semantic memory decline (Pugh et al., 2020). Given these mixed results, additional research is needed to clarify associations between discrimination and cognitive aging outcomes, particularly in different racial/ethnic groups.

In line with Clark and colleagues’ (1999) biopsychosocial model, discrimination has the potential to reduce health through stress pathways involving physiological dysregulation. However, the experience of stress includes not only the occurrence of stressors, but also an individual’s interpretations of and reactions to these stressors (Cohen et al., 1983). Importantly, Hispanic and non-Hispanic Black adults in the U.S. report being less upset by chronic stressors (e.g., health, social, or financial problems) than non-Hispanic Whites, despite having greater exposure (Brown et al., 2020). Indeed, the same stressor may exert differential impacts on individuals as a function of their personal characteristics (Kessler, 1979). It is also notable that older adults report less discrimination than middle-aged adults, which could reflect habituation, age differences in social roles, age differences in coping, and/or birth cohort differences in the interpretation of racist events (Clark, 2004). Thus, it is possible that commonly-used discrimination measures do not fully capture individuals’ experiences or, perhaps even more importantly, the potential impact of these experiences.

In the current study, Hispanic participants did not report more frequent experiences of discrimination than non-Hispanic Whites, which is in line with previous findings (Lewis et al., 2012; Zahodne et al., 2017; Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019). Acculturation (e.g., English proficiency), age of immigration, and ethnic identity are each inversely associated with reports of discrimination among Hispanics in the U.S. (Finch et al., 2000; Pérez et al., 2008). These findings may indicate that Hispanics are less likely to interpret interpersonal experiences of discrimination as such when they are less familiar with the racism that is embedded within U.S. culture and institutions. Additional work is needed to understand the extent to which these findings reflect differences in the occurrence and/or the perception of discrimination.

The current finding that Hispanics reported significantly lower rates of discrimination than non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks may also reflect the unique context of ethnic enclaves such as northern Manhattan, which is characterized by a high density of Spanish-speaking businesses and service providers, as well as co-ethnics from the Caribbean. While Hispanics living in such an ethnic enclave may not frequently experience day-to-day instances of interpersonal discrimination or perceive events as such, it is important to note that they still experience significant structural barriers to their well-being, as well as that of their friends and family, which are not likely to be fully captured by the commonly-used measures of discrimination that were developed based on qualitative work with Black adults. Indeed, these structural barriers (e.g., lack of neighborhood resources, anti-immigrant policies) may partly explain why Hispanics in the current study reported the highest levels of external perceived control.

Together, the current pattern of results regarding psychosocial mediators of cognitive disparities may suggest that measures of external perceived control capture the psychosocial experiences of non-Hispanic Blacks and Hispanics that are most relevant to cognitive disparities better than commonly-used measures of discrimination. External perceived control may capture the impacts of not only interpersonal discrimination, but also vicarious discrimination, anticipatory discrimination, institutional racism, and other stressful life events that are more commonly experienced by Hispanic and Black individuals in the U.S. While the proportions of the racial/ethnic disparities explained by external perceived control were relatively low (i.e., 5–8%), it is notable that these proportions were 40–50% larger in sensitivity analyses that excluded SES measures. This pattern of results may suggest that in addition to contributing to cognitive disparities over and above SES (i.e., additive effects), external perceived control may also mediate some of the negative cognitive effects of low SES. Thus, external perceived control may also represent a more proximal intervention target to interrupt risk pathways involving SES.

The current study may have important implications for reducing racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging. Policies to dismantle structural racism (e.g., investing in minority communities, criminal justice reform) may reduce cognitive disparities, in part, by reducing environmental constraints that lead to more external perceived control among non-Hispanic Back and Hispanic adults. In addition, we recommend that individual-level interventions with non-Hispanic Black and Hispanic adults explicitly address external control beliefs, and the environmental constraints that promote them, to maximize cognitive and other health benefits (Zahodne et al., 2015). For example, health promotion interventions could help participants identify strategies to overcome specific structural barriers to behavior change, and cognitive training interventions could include psychoeducation about individuals’ ability to improve their own cognitive performance.

Limitations and Strengths

A primary limitation of this study is its cross-sectional design due to the recent addition of the psychosocial measures to the WHICAP battery. Follow-up of these participants is ongoing, so future work will examine socioeconomic and psychosocial mediators of racial/ethnic differences in cognitive trajectories. Of note, racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging are far more consistent and pronounced for cognitive level than for subsequent rates of cognitive change (Manly & Mungas, 2015). Therefore, eliminating racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive level is likely to have the greatest impact on dementia inequalities, underscoring the significance of the current study. While the simultaneous mediation framework used in the current study allowed for an evaluation of the relative contributions of socioeconomic and psychosocial variables to cognitive disparities, it did not model potential sequential mediation. Specifically, lower socioeconomic economic status can contribute to discrimination and/or perceived control. However, the direction of associations among the included mediators is not clear-cut. For example, the lifetime discrimination measure queries whether participants were ever unfairly discouraged from pursuing higher education. Future research should endeavor to disentangle causal pathways, which may be bi-directional.

Strengths of this study include the racially and ethnically diverse sample, which allowed for an examination of mediators of both Black-White and Hispanic-White cognitive disparities within the same sample. The focus on individuals living in the same geographic region reduces the potential for confounding due to major geographic differences. Another strength is the inclusion of multiple potential psychosocial mediators of disparities, which allowed for preliminary recommendations about psychososcial constructs that should be prioritized in future studies of racial/ethnic disparities (i.e., external perceived control). Finally, the use of a comprehensive, in-person neuropsychological battery allowed for a better characterization of cognition than in previous national studies (e.g., Zahodne et al., 2017; Zahodne, Sol, et al., 2019)

Conclusions

This study of Hispanic, non-Hispanic Black, and non-Hispanic White older adults living in northern Manhattan confirms that commonly-measured socioeconomic indicators only partly explain racial/ethnic disparities in cognition. This study also highlights the additive contribution of external perceived control to both Hispanic-White and Black-White cognitive disparities. External perceived control likely reflects chronic exposure to interpersonal and institutional marginalization that culminates in differential patterns of cognitive health in late life. Future models of racial/ethnic disparities in cognitive aging should explicitly incorporate psychosocial factors such as perceived control.

Key Points:

What is the key question this paper addresses?

What factors explain racial/ethnic inequalities in cognitive functioning among older adults?

What are the primary findings?

Lower socioeconomic status and lower perceived control each contribute independently to racial/ethnic inequalities in cognitive functioning.

What are the key scientific and practical implications of the findings?

Targeting both economic and psychosocial factors may help to reduce racial/ethnc inequalities in cognitive aging.

What directions should be explored in future research?

Future research should disentangle causal pathways to inequality involving socioeconomic and psychosocial factors, which may be interactive and/or bi-directional.

Acknowledgments

Data collection and sharing for this project was supported by the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP, P01AG07232, R01AG037212, RF1AG054023) funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) and through NIA grant numbers R00AG047963 and R01AG054520. This manuscript has been reviewed by WHICAP investigators for scientific content and consistency of data interpretation with previous WHICAP Study publications. We acknowledge the WHICAP study participants and the WHICAP research and support staff for their contributions to this study. This publication was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant Number UL1TR001873. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Contributor Information

Laura B. Zahodne, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan.

Neika Sharifian, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan..

A. Zarina Kraal, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan..

Afsara B. Zaheed, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan

Ketlyne Sol, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan..

Emily P. Morris, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan.

Nicole Schupf, Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center and Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University..

Jennifer J. Manly, Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center and Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University.

Adam M. Brickman, Department of Neurology, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center and Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University.

References

  1. Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, Rajan KB, Mendes De Leon CF, Evans DA, & Everson-Rose SA (2014). Perceived stress and change in cognitive function among adults 65 years and older. Psychosomatic Medicine. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiken Morgan AT, Sims RC, & Whitfield KE (2010). Cardiovascular health and education as sources of individual variability in cognitive aging among African Americans. Journal of Aging and Health, 22(4), 477–503. 10.1177/0898264310361627 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). DSM III-R. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders––Third Revised Version. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bandura A (1981). Self-referent thought: A developmental analysis of self-efficacy. In Flavell & Ross L (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 200–239). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bandura A (1986). Social foundations of thought and action : a social cognitive theory. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandura A (1988). Self-Regulation of Motivation and Action Through Goal Systems. In Hamilton V, Bower GH, & Frijda NH (Eds.), Cognitive Perspectives on Emotion and Motivation (pp. 37–61). Springer; Netherlands. 10.1007/978-94-009-2792-6_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bandura A (1989). Regulation of Cognitive Processes Through Perceived Self-Efficacy. Developmental Psychology. 10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.729 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bandura A (1997) Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. W.H. Freeman and Company, New York. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bandura A, & Wood R (1989). Effect of Perceived Controllability and Performance Standards on Self-Regulation of Complex Decision Making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barnes LL, Lewis TT, Begeny CT, Yu L, Bennett DA, & Wilson RS (2012). Perceived discrimination and cognition in older African Americans. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 856–865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barnes LL, Mendes De Leon CF, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Bennett DA, & Evans DA (2004). Racial differences in perceived discrimination in a community population of older blacks and whites. Journal of Aging and Health, 16, 315–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beatty Moody DL, Chang Y, Brown C, Bromberger JT, & Matthews KA (2018). Everyday Discrimination and Metabolic Syndrome Incidence in a Racially/Ethnically Diverse Sample: Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(1), 114–121. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Benton AL (1955). The Revised Visual Retention Test: Clinical and Experimental Applications. The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bloome D (2014). Racial Inequality Trends and the Intergenerational Persistence of Income and Family Structure. American Sociological Review. 10.1177/0003122414554947 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bollini AM, Walker EF, Hamann S, & Kestler L (2004). The influence of perceived control and locus of control on the cortisol and subjective responses to stress. Biological Psychology. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Boone KB, Victor TL, Wen J, Razani J, & Pontón M (2007). The association between neuropsychological scores and ethnicity, language, and acculturation variables in a large patient population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brewster PWH, Melrose RJ, Marquine MJ, Johnson JK, Napoles A, MacKay-Brandt A, Farias S, Reed B, & Mungas D (2014). Life experience and demographic influences on cognitive function in older adults. Neuropsychology. 10.1037/neu0000098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brondolo E, Hausmann LRM, Jhalani J, Pencille M, Atencio-Bacayon J, Kumar A, Kwok J, Ullah J, Roth A, Chen D, Crupi R, & Schwartz J (2011). Dimensions of perceived racism and self-reported health: Examination of racial/ethnic differences and potential mediators. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 42(1), 14–28. 10.1007/s12160-011-9265-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brown LL, Mitchell UA, & Ailshire JA (2020). Disentangling the Stress Process: Race/Ethnic Differences in the Exposure and Appraisal of Chronic Stressors Among Older Adults. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(3), 650–660. 10.1093/geronb/gby072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Buschke H, & Fuld PA (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 1919–1025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caplan LJ, & Schooler C (2003). The roles of fatalism, self-confidence, and intellectual resources in the disablement process in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 18, 551–561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cervantes RC, Padilla AM, & Salgado de Snyder N (1990). Reliability and Validity of the HIspanic Stress Inventory. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 12, 76–82. [Google Scholar]
  23. Chae DH, Lincoln KD, Adler NE, & Syme SL (2010). Do experiences of racial discrimination predict cardiovascular disease among African American men? The moderating role of internalized negative racial group attitudes. Social Science and Medicine. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Clark R (2004). Significance of Perceived Racism: Toward Understanding Ethnic Group Disparities in Health, the Later Years. In Anderson NB, Bulatao RA, & Cohen B (Eds.), National Research Council (US) Panel on Race, Ethnicity, and Health in Later Life. National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, & Williams DR (1999). Racism as a Stressor for African Americans: A Biopsychosocial Model. American Psychologist. 10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cohen S, Kamarck T, & Mermelstein R (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396. 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Colby S, & Ortman J (2015). Projections of the size and composition of the US population: 2014 to 2060. Current Population Reports. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cuevas AG, Dawson BA, & Williams DR (2016). Race and skin color in latino health: An analytic review. In American Journal of Public Health. 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. D’Elia LF, Satz P, Uchiyama CL, & White T (1994). Color Trails Test (CTT). Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc,. [Google Scholar]
  30. Diniz BS, Butters MA, Albert SM, Dew MA, & Reynolds CF (2013). Late-life depression and risk of vascular dementia and alzheimer’s diseas: Systematic review and meta-analysis of community-based cohort studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202, 329–335. 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.118307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Díaz-Venegas C, Downer B, Langa KM, & Wong R (2016). Racial and ethnic differences in cognitive function among older adults in the USA. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 10.1002/gps.4410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Donovan R (1984). Path analysis of a theoretical model of persistence in higher education among low-income black youth. Research in Higher Education. 10.1007/BF00974861 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Duncan B, Joseph Hotz V, & Trejo SJ (2006). Hispanics in the U.S. labor market. In Hispanics and the Future of America. 10.17226/11539 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Eizenman DR, Nesselroade JR, Featherman DL, & Rowe JW (1997). Intraindividual variability in perceived control in a older sample: The MacArthur successful aging studies. Psychology and aging, 12(3), 489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Elliot AJ, Mooney CJ, Infurna FJ, & Chapman BP (2017). Associations of Lifetime Trauma and Chronic Stress with C-reactive Protein in Adults Ages 50 Years and Older: Examining the Moderating Role of Perceived Control. Psychosomatic Medicine. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Finch BK, Kolody B, & Vega WA (2000). Perceived discrimination and depression among Mexican-origin adults in California. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 10.2307/2676322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Garcini LM, Chirinos DA, Murdock KW, Seiler A, LeRoy AS, Peek K, Cutchin MP, & Fagundes C (2018). Pathways linking racial/ethnic discrimination and sleep among U.S.-born and foreign-born Latinxs. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 41(3), 364–373. 10.1007/s10865-017-9907-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Glymour MM, & Manly JJ (2008). Lifecourse social conditions and racial and ethnic patterns of cognitive aging. Neuropsychology Review, 18(3 SPEC. ISS.), 223–254. 10.1007/s11065-008-9064-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Goodglass H (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.), Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. [Google Scholar]
  40. Gourounti K, Anagnostopoulos F, Potamianos G, Lykeridou K, Schmidt L, & Vaslamatzis G (2012). Perception of control, coping and psychological stress of infertile women undergoing IVF. Reproductive BioMedicine Online. 10.1016/j.rbmo.2012.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Guyll M, Matthews KA, & Bromberger JT (2001). Discrimination and unfair treatment: Relationship to cardiovascular reactivity among African American and European American women. Health Psychology. 10.1037/0278-6133.20.5.315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Haq KS, & Penning MJ (2019). Social Determinants of Racial Disparities in Cognitive Functioning in Later Life in Canada. Journal of Aging and Health. 10.1177/0898264319853137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Holtz-Eakin D, & Smeeding TM (1994). Income, wealth, and intergenerational economic relations of the aged. In Demography of Aging. National Research Council, Division of Behavioral and Social Science and Education. [Google Scholar]
  44. Howarter AD, & Bennett KK (2013). Perceived discrimination and health-related quality of life: Testing the reserve capacity model in Hispanic Americans. Journal of Social Psychology. 10.1080/00224545.2012.703973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Irwin M, Artin KH, & Oxman MN (1999). Screening for Depression in the Older Adult. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159(15), 1701. 10.1001/archinte.159.15.1701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, & Weintraub S (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. [Google Scholar]
  47. Keith TZ, & Benson MJ (1992). Effects of Manipulable Influences on High School Grades Across Five Ethnic Groups. Journal of Educational Research. 10.1080/00220671.1992.9941144 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kessler RC (1979). Stress, social status, and psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 10.2307/2136450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Koster A, Penninx BWJH, Bosma H, Kempen GIJM, Newman AB, Rubin SM, Satterfield S, Atkinson HH, Ayonayon HN, Rosano C, Yaffe K, Harris TB, Rooks RN, Van Eijk JT, & Kritchevsky SB (2005). Socioeconomic differences in cognitive decline and the role of biomedical factors. Annals of Epidemiology. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lachman ME (2006). Perceived control over aging-related declines: Adaptive beliefs and behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 282–286. [Google Scholar]
  51. Lachman ME, Neupert SD, & Agrigoroaei S (2011). The Relevance of Control Beliefs for Health and Aging. In Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 10.1016/B978-0-12-380882-0.00011-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lachman ME, & Weaver SL (1998). The Sense of Control as a Moderator of Social Class Differences in Health and Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lefcourt HM (2014). Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research. Hove: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  54. Lewis TT, Aiello AE, Leurgans S, Kelly J, & Barnes LL (2010). Self-reported experiences of everyday discrimination are associated with elevated C-reactive protein levels in older African-American adults. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.11.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lewis TT, Yang FM, Jacobs EA, & Fitchett G (2012). Racial/ethnic differences in responses to the everyday discrimination scale: a differential item functioning analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 175(5), 391–401. 10.1093/aje/kwr287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Liu SY, Glymour MM, Zahodne LB, Weiss C, & Manly JJ (2015). Role of place in explaining racial heterogeneity in cognitive outcomes among older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 10.1017/S1355617715000806 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Machado A, Herrera AJ, De Pablos RM, Espinosa-Oliva AM, Sarmiento M, Ayala A, Venero JL, Santiago M, Villarán RF, Delgado-Cortés MJ, Argüelles S, & Cano J (2014). Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. In Reviews in the Neurosciences. 10.1515/revneuro-2014-0035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Manly JJ, Bell-McGinty S, Tang M-X, Schupf N, Stern Y, & Mayeux R (2005). Implementing Diagnostic Criteria and Estimating Frequency of Mild Cognitive Impairment in an Urban Community. Archives of Neurology, 62(11), 1739. 10.1001/archneur.62.11.1739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, Small SA, & Stern Y (2002). Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 8(3), 341–348. 10.1017/s1355617702813157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Manly JJ, & Mungas D (2015). JGPS special series on race, ethnicity, life experiences, and cognitive aging. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70, 509–511. 10.1093/geronb/gbv030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Marden JR, Mayeda ER, Tchetgen Tchetgen E,J, Kawachi I, & Glymour MM (2017). High hemoglobin A1c and diabetes predict memory decline in the health and retirement study. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 31, 48–54. 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mattis S (1976). Mental Status Examination for Organic Mental Syndrome in the Elderly Patient. In Bellack L & Karusus TB (Eds.), Geriatric Psychiatry (pp. 77–121). Grune & Stratton. [Google Scholar]
  63. Mehta KM, Simonsick EM, Rooks R, Newman AB, Pope SK, Rubin SM, & Yaffe K (2004). Black and white differences in cognitive function test scores: What explains the difference? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52575.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mezuk B, Abdou CM, Hudson D, Kershaw K, Rafferty JA, Lee H, & Jackson JS (2013). “White box” epidemiology and the social neuroscience of health behaviors: the environmental affordances model. Society and Mental Health, 3, 10.1177/2156869313480892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mirowsky J, & Ross CE (1983). Paranoia and the structure of powerlessness. American Sociological Review. 10.2307/2095107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Mirowsky J, & Ross CE (1990). The Consolation-Prize Theory of Alienation. American Journal of Sociology. 10.1086/229462 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Molina KM, Estrella ML, Durazo-Arvizu R, Malcarne VL, Llabre MM, Isasi CR, Ornelas IJ, Perreira KM, Penedo FJ, Brondolo E, Gallo L, & Daviglus ML (2019). Perceived discrimination and physical health-related quality of life: The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) Sociocultural Ancillary Study. Social Science and Medicine, 222, 91–100. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.12.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mora MT, & Dávila A (2018). The Hispanic-White Wage Gap Has Remained Wide and Relatively Steady: Examining Hispanic-White Gaps in Wages, Unemployment, Labor Force Participation, and Education by Gender, Immigrant Status, and Other Subpopulations. Economic Policy Institute. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mueller AE, Raymond N, & Yochim BP (2013). Cognitive Activity Engagement Predicts Future Memory and Executive Functioning in Older Adults. Activities, Adaptation and Aging. 10.1080/01924788.2013.816833 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (2011). Mplus User’s Guide. Sixth Edition. In Los Angeles: Author. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01711.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Peavy GM, Salmon DP, Jacobson MW, Hervey A, Gamst AC, Wolfson T, Patterson TL, Goldman S, Mills PJ, Khandrika S, & Galasko D (2009). Effects of chronic stress on memory decline in cognitively normal and mildly impaired older adults. American Journal of Psychiatry. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pérez DJ, Fortuna L, & Alegría M (2008). Prevalence and correlates of everyday discrimination among U.S. latinos. Journal of Community Psychology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Pugh E, De Vito A, Divers R, Robinson A, Weitzner DS, & Calamia M (2020). Social factors that predict cognitive decline in older African American adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. doi: 10.1002/gps.5435. Online ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Reynolds AJ (1989). A Structural Model of First-Grade Outcomes for an Urban, Low Socioeconomic Status, Minority Population. Journal of Educational Psychology. 10.1037/0022-0663.81.4.594 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rosen W (1981). The Rosen Drawing Test. Veterans Administration Medical Center. [Google Scholar]
  76. Rotter JB (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological monographs: General and applied, 80(1), 1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Savva GM, Stephan BCM, & the Alzheimer’s Society Vascular Dementia Systematic Review Group (2010). Epidemiological studies of the effects of stroke on incident dementia: A systematic review. Stroke, 41, e41–e46. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.559880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, Brickman AM, Cosentino S, Tang MX, & Stern Y (2009). Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA - Journal of the American Medical Association, 302(6), 627–637. 10.1001/jama.2009.1144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Schleicher A (2018). PISA 2018 Insights and Interpretations. https://www.oecd.org/pisa/PISA 2018 Insights and Interpretations FINAL PDF.pdf
  80. Schwartz BS, Glass TA, Bolla KI, Stewart WF, Glass G, Rasmussen M, Bressler J, Shi W, & Bandeen-Roche K (2004). Disparities in cognitive functioning by race/ethnicity in the Baltimore Memory Study. Environmental Health Perspectives, 112(3), 314–320. 10.1289/ehp.6727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Seeman T, McAvay G, Albert M, Merrill S, & Rodin J (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychology and Aging. 10.1037/0882-7974.11.3.538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sharpe C, Holup AA, Hansen KE, & Edwards JD (2014). Does self-efficacy affect responsiveness to cognitive speed of processing training? Journal of Aging and Health. 10.1177/0898264314531615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Siedlecki KL, Manly JJ, Brickman AM, Schupf N, Tang M-X, & Stern Y (2010). Do neuropsychological tests have the same meaning in Spanish speakers as they do in English speakers? Neuropsychology, 24(3), 402–411. 10.1037/a0017515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sisco S, Gross AL, Shih RA, Sachs BC, Glymour MM, Bangen KJ, Benitez A, Skinner J, Schneider BC, & Manly JJ (2015). The role of early-life educational quality and literacy in explaining racial disparities in cognition in late life. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 10.1093/geronb/gbt133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sol K, Zaheed AB, Kraal AZ, Sharifian N, Arce Rentería M, & Zahodne LB (2020). Psychological predictors of memory decline in a racially and ethnically diverse longitudinal sample of older adults in the United States. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 10.1002/gps.5236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Stern Y, Andrews H, Pittman J, Sano M, Tatemichi T, Lantigua R, & Mayeux R (1992). Diagnosis of Dementia in a Heterogeneous Population. Archives of Neurology, 49(5), 453. 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290035009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, Jacobs DM, Small S, Bell K, Merchant C, Lantigua R, Costa R, Stern Y, & Mayeux R (2001). Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology, 56(1), 49–56. 10.1212/WNL.56.1.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Verhaeghen P, Borchelt M, & Smith J (2003). Relation between cardiovascular and metabolic disease and cognition in very old age: Cross-sectional and longitudinal findings from the berlin aging study. Health Psychology, 22, 559–569. 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wechsler D (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  90. Weuve J, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Rajan KB, Beck T, Aggarwal NT, Hebert LE, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, & Evans DA (2018). Cognitive Aging in Black and White Americans. Epidemiology. 10.1097/ede.0000000000000747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, & Anderson NB (1997). Racial differences in physical and mental health. Socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology. 10.1177/135910539700200305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Sytsma J, Bennett DA, & Barnes LL (2015). Cognitive aging in older black and white persons. Psychology and Aging. 10.1037/pag0000024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Wood R, & Bandura A (1989). Impact of Conceptions of Ability on Self-Regulatory Mechanisms and Complex Decision Making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.3.407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Yaffe K, Falvey C, Harris TB, Newman A, Satterfield S, Koster A, Ayonayon H, & Simonsick E (2013). Effect of socioeconomic disparities on incidence of dementia among biracial older adults: Prospective study. BMJ (Online). 10.1136/bmj.f7051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zahodne LB, Kraal AZ, Sharifian N, Zaheed AB, & Sol K (2019). Inflammatory mechanisms underlying the effects of everyday discrimination on age-related memory decline. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 75, 149–154. 10.1016/j.bbi.2018.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Zahodne LB, Kraal AZ, Zaheed A, Farris P, & Sol K (2019). Longitudinal effects of race, ethnicity, and psychosocial disadvantage on systemic inflammation. SSM - Population Health, 7. 10.1016/j.ssmph.2019.100391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Zahodne LB, Manly JJ, Azar M, Brickman AM, & Glymour MM (2016). Racial Disparities in Cognitive Performance in Mid- and Late Adulthood: Analyses of Two Cohort Studies. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 10.1111/jgs.14113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Zahodne LB, Manly JJ, Smith J, Seeman T, & Lachman ME (2017). Socioeconomic, health, and psychosocial mediators of racial disparities in cognition in early, middle, and late adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 32(2), 118–130. 10.1037/pag0000154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Zahodne LB, Meyer OL, Choi E, Thomas ML, Willis SL, Marsiske M, Gross AL, Rebok GW, & Parisi JM (2015). External locus of control contributes to racial disparities in memory and reasoning training gains in ACTIVE. Psychology & Aging, 30 (3), 561–572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Zahodne LB, Morris EP, Sharifian N, Zaheed AB, Kraal AZ, & Sol K (2020). Everyday discrimination and subsequent cognitive abilities across five domains. Neuropsychology, doi: 10.1037/neu0000693. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 32744838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zahodne LB, Sol K, & Kraal Z (2019). Psychosocial Pathways to Racial/Ethnic Inequalities in Late-Life Memory Trajectories. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(3), 409–418. 10.1093/geronb/gbx113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zahodne LB, Stern Y, & Manly JJ (2015). Differing effects of education on cognitive decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment. Neuropsychology. 10.1037/neu0000141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES