Abstract
Objectives:
Emotional awareness is the ability to recognize, describe, and attend to emotions. A known correlate is emotional processing, the ability to orient to and use inner experiences for information. The goal was to examine emotional awareness during therapy among gynecologic cancer patients, identify baseline predictors, and explore the relationship between in-session emotional awareness and processing.
Methods:
Psychotherapy and baseline data from a randomized controlled trial comparing a supportive counselling (SC) intervention and a cognitive-behavioral coping and communication (CCI) intervention were utilized. The sample was patients with gynecologic cancers randomized to either therapy (N = 246). Emotion episode transcripts from the first, middle, and sixth of seven in-person sessions were coded for emotional awareness using the Program for Open-Ended Scoring and emotional processing using the Experiencing Scale. Descriptive and regression analyses were conducted.
Results:
Participants had moderate in-session emotional awareness. SC participants exhibited higher levels of awareness in the first (p < .001) and sixth (p = .002) session than CCI participants. Awareness was positively correlated with emotional processing in the first and sixth SC sessions (r = .25, .24, respectively) and all CCI sessions (r =.29-.31). Baseline negative emotion expression was associated with awareness during the sixth SC session. Baseline cancer-specific distress was associated with awareness during the sixth CCI session.
Significance of Results:
Supportive counseling may facilitate emotional awareness. Greater emotional awareness in therapy may facilitate emotional processing, which is an important component of most psychotherapies. Patients who are psychologically distressed may exhibit more awareness than others. Similarly, greater emotional awareness may signal greater patient distress.
Keywords: cancer, emotional awareness, gynecological, oncology, psychotherapy
Introduction
Facilitating processing of emotions is an important goal of experiential and cognitive-behavioral treatments (Greenberg et al., 1993; Castonguay et al., 1996). Emotional processing is defined as the degree to which clients orient to their emotions and thoughts and use them as information to solve their problems (Klein Mathieu, & Gendlin 1969; Greenberg & Safran, 1984; Greenberg, 2002; Greenberg & Pascuale Leone, 2006; Watson & Bedard, 2006). In therapy, individuals can improve emotional processing by becoming aware of emotions, acknowledging the information they contain, becoming aware of thoughts and beliefs associated with them, verbalizing them, and ultimately understanding their meaning. In the highest stages of emotional processing, new meanings emerge, and feelings or experiences are used solve problems (Greenberg & Safran, 1984). Research shows a relationship between levels of emotional processing across therapy sessions and better therapy outcomes (Orlinsky & Howard, 1978; Klein et al., 1986; Watson & Bedard, 2006). Greater increases in emotional processing over the course of both experiential and cognitive-behavior therapy are associated with greater post-treatment reductions in symptomatology (Hunt, 1998; Pos Greenberg, Goldman, & Korman 2003; Feldman et al., 2009).
As noted above, emotional awareness is considered the first step in emotional processing. According to Lane and Schwartz (1987), awareness develops through five levels. At Level 1, awareness is focused on physical sensations (e.g., “My head hurts.”). At Level 2, emotions are described as physical sensations and action tendencies (e.g., “I wanted to scream.”). At Level 3, emotions are characterized as one-dimensional (e.g., “I am angry.”). At Level 4, descriptions include feelings that are opposed to and/or different from each other (e.g., “I feel both angry at him and sad because I understand his pain.”). At Level 5, feelings of varying qualities and intensities are blended into new patterns and subtle distinctions are made between emotional nuances (e.g., “I’d feel disappointed that I didn’t win but glad that if someone else did, that person was my friend…My friend would feel happy but slightly worried that my feelings might be hurt.”).
Because of its important role in emotional processing, facilitating emotional awareness is a common goal across therapy modalities. Cognitive-behavioral therapy, emotion-focused therapy, process experiential therapy, mindfulness and acceptance-based treatments (Greenberg & Korman, 1993; Gendlin, 1996; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999; Greenberg, 2002; Roemer & Orsillo, 2005; Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, 2006) consider attending to and recognizing emotions as cornerstones of these approaches. Despite its proposed importance in successful therapy outcomes, there has been little research examining emotional awareness during psychotherapy. It has been studied in various clinical populations, including somatoform disorders (Subic-Wrana et al., 2010), generalized anxiety disorder (Novick-Klein, Turk, Mennin, Hoyt, & Gallagher 2005), and posttraumatic stress disorder (Frewen et al., 2008). Studies showed lower awareness among individuals with somatoform disorders and posttraumatic stress disorders but higher levels among individuals with anxiety disorders (Novick-Klein et al., 2005; Frewen et al., 2008; Subic-Wrana, Beutel, Knebel, & Lane 2010). A link between emotional awareness and therapy outcomes has been found in studies, indicating that greater awareness is associated with greater distress reduction in both psychodynamic and cognitive behavioral therapy (Beutel et al., 2013).
In our prior work, we examined components of emotional processing during psychotherapy sessions of women newly-diagnosed with gynecological cancer. We studied the construct using therapy session tapes from a study evaluating the efficacy of two psychotherapy interventions: a coping and communication intervention (CCI) and a supportive counseling intervention (SC). Gynecological cancer is challenging because of its poor prognosis, difficult treatment regimens, and adverse treatment side effects (Hwang, Cho, & Yoo, 2016). Between 19% and 52% of women reported moderate to severe levels of anxiety during and after treatment (Watts, Prescott, Mason, McLeod, & Lewith, 2015). The diagnosis and treatment of cancer is typically linked with both negative (Kulpa et al., 2017) and positive (Ciere et al., 2017) emotional responses. Whether and how patients manage and utilize their emotions has been linked to psychological adaptation (Classen, Koopman, Angell, & Spiegel, 1996; Stanton et al., 2000; Manne et al., 2008).
Our two prior studies used data from a previously published randomized clinical trial (RCT) (Manne, et al., 2017). In the first study, we characterized emotion episodes (EEs), which are indicators of emotional expression in psychotherapy (Myers Virtue et al., 2015) and the methods used to code and analyze in-session emotional processing (Greenberg & Korman, 1993; Korman, 1998). An EE is a segment of psychotherapy when patients express any emotions in response to a situation. Identifying EEs provides information about the frequency, types, and content of emotions. We examined EEs during early and late psychotherapy sessions and differences between CCI and SC. We had four key findings: 1) Patients experienced at least one EE in the majority of early and late sessions, 2) most EEs focused on negative emotions and cancer-related topics, 3) there was a decrease in total EEs across sessions and an increase in positive EEs, and 4) there were more total EEs in SC than CCI.
In the second study (Manne et al., 2017), we characterized levels of emotional processing during EEs at the beginning, middle and end of SC and CCI sessions using the experiencing scale (Klein et al., 1969). Emotional processing was rated from EEs. Findings indicated that patients discussed events, emotional reactions, and private experiences during sessions. A small proportion had high levels of processing, indicating deeper exploration of the meaning of their feelings. Women in SC achieved higher levels of emotional processing during the middle and later sessions. Emotional processing was not significantly associated with patient’s perceived therapy progress in SC. In CCI, higher levels of emotional processing were associated with greater session progress, suggesting that it may play an important role in patient-rated treatment outcomes.
The current study builds upon this work by measuring emotional awareness, the proposed first step to emotional processing, during therapy sessions. Transcripts of EEs were coded for emotional awareness using a computerized system called the Program for Open-Ended Scoring (POES) (Barchard, Bajgar, Leaf, & Lane, 2010). The POES was developed to score a paper-and-pencil measure of emotional awareness, the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS) (Lane & Schwartz, 1987). The LEAS consists of emotionally-provoking vignettes, and participants are asked to describe their own and others’ feelings. The LEAS uses hypothetical situations that are not cancer-specific and does not code emotional episodes during therapy sessions. The POES assigns numerical values to the emotion words used in the responses based upon complexity and diversity of words used. We adapted the POES to score transcribed EEs from therapy sessions, rather than administering the LEAS. To our knowledge, this is the first study to utilize the POES from actual therapy session tapes.
The current study had three aims. The first aim was to describe levels of emotional awareness across sessions and to examine differences between two different approaches to therapy. Because SC utilized techniques to facilitate emotional exploration and CCI did not target emotional exploration, we hypothesized that emotional awareness would be higher and increase at a higher rate in SC. The second aim was to examine the correspondence between emotional awareness and emotional processing. Consistent with the model (Greenberg & Safran, 1984), we hypothesized that higher in-session emotional awareness would be associated with higher in-session emotional processing. The third aim was to examine baseline psychosocial factors associated with in-session emotional awareness. Since promoting emotional awareness is often a goal in therapy (Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, 2006), identifying factors that influence awareness can provide guidance for clinicians. Prior work found that younger age was associated with higher emotional processing (Manne et al., 2017). As emotional awareness is considered a necessary part of successful emotional processing, we hypothesized that younger women would exhibit greater awareness. Since low levels of emotional awareness focus on bodily sensations, we hypothesized that those with lower physical well-being may focus more on physical sensations rather than feelings, thus receiving lower awareness scores. We hypothesized greater dispositional emotional expressivity would be related to greater emotional awareness. Emotional arousal is an important step in emotional awareness, but there is a tendency to avoid expressing emotions, which can interfere with productively using emotions to create meaning (Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, 2006). Individuals who are more likely to express those emotions have more opportunities to develop emotional awareness. Finally, we hypothesized that greater cancer-specific distress would be associated with greater in-session emotional awareness. Researchers suggest that greater rumination and intensity of emotion may prompt more awareness (Novick-Kline et al., 2005). Intrusive cancer-related thoughts may prompt intense experiences that facilitate greater emotional awareness.
Methods
Participants and procedures
This study is a secondary data analysis of baseline and psychotherapy data from a published, multi-site RCT that evaluated the efficacy of two individual psychotherapy interventions: CCI and SC (Clinicaltrials.gov # NCT01951807) (Manne et al., 2017). The control condition was a Usual Care (UC) arm. Both interventions consisted of seven 1-hour in-person psychotherapy sessions and a telephone booster session. Inclusion criteria were the following: 1) ≥ 18 years, 2) diagnosed with gynecological cancer within the past six months at the time of recruitment, 3) a Karnofsky Performance Status of ≥80 or an Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group score of 0 or 1, 4) lived within a two-hour commuting distance from recruitment center, 5) English speaking, and 6) no hearing impairment. Eligible women were mailed a letter describing the study and contacted by research assistants in-person or by phone. Interested women signed an informed consent document approved by the Institutional Review Board at each site. Participants completed a baseline survey and were randomly assigned to CCI, SC, or UC. Participants were paid $15 for completing the baseline survey and a total of $215 for attending all psychotherapy sessions. Sessions were video and/or audio recorded.
In the RCT, 1147 women were approached to participate and 352 (30.7%) consented, completed a baseline survey, and were randomized. Further recruitment information is found in the publication (Manne et al., 2017). This sub-study utilized baseline survey and psychotherapy session data from 246 RCT participants who were randomized to CCI (n = 121) or SC (n = 125).
Measures
Session data.
Psychotherapy session data were coded from video/audio recordings. Data were obtained from the first session, the sixth session, and a middle session, which was selected between the second and fifth sessions. The first session was chosen to gather baseline data. The sixth session was chosen to capture emotion data toward the end of treatment, consistent with prior emotional process studies, which suggested that the penultimate session better captures end of treatment processing than the last session (Pos, et al., 2003; Missirlian, Toukmanian, Warwar, & Greenberg, 2005). The middle session was chosen by selecting the session with the highest patient-rated emotional arousal using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), consistent with prior research (Missirlian et al., 2005).
Emotion episodes.
We utilized the Emotion Episode (EE) coding system (Greenberg & Korman, 1993; Korman, 1998) to select in-session segments when patients expressed emotion. EEs were utilized as a unit of coding the level of emotion processing in numerous psychotherapy research studies (Missirlian et al., 2005). Four trained research assistants completed coding. Criterion coders rated one-third of all cases for fidelity. Agreement was 90.6%. See prior work (Myers Virtue et al., 2015) for further details about coding. EEs were transcribed for further coding.
Emotional awareness.
The POES (Leaf & Barchard, 2010) assessed emotional awareness during EEs as described above. The POES scanned transcripts for emotion words. EEs were given scores based on complexity of emotion words. Scores range from 1–5, with higher scores indicating greater emotional awareness. An average session score was calculated. Training in using the system was provided prior to and during the study as needed.
Emotion processing.
The Experiencing Scale (EXP) (Klein, et al., 1969) assessed emotional processing during EEs. This scale is widely considered the “gold standard” of experiential processing and remains one of the most extensively studied and validated measures. The scale provides a mode (average across segment) and peak (highest in segment) rating on a seven-point scale. Higher ratings indicate higher processing. Seven trained research assistants completed coding. Coders had to reach an 80% agreement on master recordings to be considered reliable and ready to code. Fidelity was monitored by coding one-third of all cases. Agreement was acceptable (κmode = .813; κpeak = .784). Further detail on coding can be found in prior work (Manne et al., 2017).
Pre-intervention predictors
Demographic and medical information.
Baseline surveys assessed age, race, marital status, income, and education level. Medical chart review assessed primary cancer diagnosis, cancer stage, time from diagnosis, and type of treatment. The 26-item Cancer Rehabilitation Evaluation System (CARES) (Schag, Ganz, & Heinrich, 1991) assessed functional impairment. Items included physical symptoms associated with cancer. Reliability was acceptable (αCCI = .94, αSC = .92).
Cancer specific distress.
The 15-item Impact of Events Scale (IES) (Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez, 1979) assessed intrusive thoughts and avoidant behaviors associated with distress specific to cancer. Sample items include, “Other things kept making me think about it (the cancer).” Scores range from 0–75 with greater scores indicating more distress. Reliability was acceptable (αCCI = .91, αSC = .9).
Dispositional emotional expressivity.
The Emotional Expressiveness Questionnaire (EEQ) (King & Emmons, 1990) assessed patients’ tendency to express positive and negative emotions. Nine items assessed positive emotional expression (e.g., “I often tell people that I love them.”). Seven items assessed negative emotional expression (e.g., “When I am angry, people around me usually know.”). Higher scores indicate greater tendency toward emotional expression. Reliability was acceptable for positive emotional expression (αCCI = .79, αSC = .73) and not ideal for negative emotional expression (αCCI = .61, αSC = .6).
Physical well-being.
The physical well-being subscale of the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT-G) (Cella et al., 1993) assessed physical well-being on a 5-point scale with higher scores indicating greater well-being. Reliability was acceptable (α CCI = .9, α SC = .89).
Data analysis
To address the first aim, we calculated average emotional awareness scores (sum of all emotional awareness scores in the session/total number of EEs in the session) for the first, middle, and sixth session. We utilized independent-samples t-tests to examine mean differences in emotional awareness scores at each session between conditions (SC and CCI). We modeled change continuously, using the time since the middle and sixth session surveys were completed after the first session. To address the second aim, correspondence between emotional awareness scores and EXP ratings was evaluated with Pearson correlations.
To address the third aim, categorical demographic and medical characteristics (race, marital status, education, cancer stage) were entered into one-way ANOVAs predicting the first, middle and sixth session emotional awareness scores. Bivariate relationships between continuous demographic and medical characteristics (age, income, time since diagnosis, functional impairment) and baseline factors (cancer-specific distress, positive emotional expressivity, negative emotional expressivity, physical well-being) and the first, middle, and sixth session scores were examined with Pearson correlations. Baseline characteristics and factors that were significantly associated with emotional awareness scores (p < .05) were simultaneously entered into a multiple regression model to determine independent predictors of emotional awareness scores. Categorical demographic and medical characteristics were dummy-coded for entry into multiple regression models.
Results
Demographic and medical characteristics
Most women were Caucasian (81.6%SC, 76%CCI), married (65.6%SC, 65.3%CCI), completed college (63.2%SC, 62.8%CCI), and were approximately 55-years-old (M = 55.60SC, SD = 9.91SC; M = 53.77CCI, SD = 10.99CCI) (see Table 1). Most were diagnosed Stage III (50.4%SC, 43%CCI) almost four months prior to study participation (M = 3.57SC, SD = 1.58SC; M = 3.92CCI, SD = 1.86CCI).
Table 1.
Demographic and medical characteristics of participants
SCa | CCIb | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Variable | n(%) | M(SD) | n(%) | M(SD) |
Age (years) | 55.60(9.91) | 53.77(10.99) | ||
Racial background | ||||
Caucasian | 102(81.6) | 92(76) | ||
Other | 22(17.6) | 29(24) | ||
Missing | 1(.8) | 0(0) | ||
Marital status | ||||
Married | 82(65.6) | 79(65.3) | ||
Not married | 43(34.4) | 42(34.7) | ||
Education level | ||||
Some college or less | 45(36) | 45(37.2) | ||
College graduate | 27(21.6) | 23(19) | ||
At least some graduate school | 52(41.6) | 53(43.8) | ||
Missing | 1(.8) | |||
Annual income ($) | 120938(96884) | 92621(62796) | ||
Cancer stage | ||||
Stage I | 19(15.2) | 26(21.5) | ||
Stage II | 15(12) | 17(14) | ||
Stage III | 63(50.4) | 52(43) | ||
Stage IV | 24(19.2) | 24(19.8) | ||
More than one stage | 2(1.6) | 0(0) | ||
Missing | 2(1.6) | 2(1.7) | ||
Time since diagnosis (months) | 3.57(1.58) | 3.92(1.86) | ||
Functional impairment | 1.36(.73) | 1.29(.80) | ||
Emotional Awareness | ||||
First Session | 3.73(.28) | 3.59(.34) | ||
Middle session | 3.80(.24) | 3.75(.32) | ||
Sixth Session | 3.79(.25) | 3.65(.36) | ||
Mode EXP | ||||
First Session | 2.25(.28) | 2.35(.31) | ||
Middle session | 2.19(.21) | 2.19(.26) | ||
Sixth Session | 2.29(.43) | 2.25(.35) | ||
Peak EXP | ||||
First Session | 2.93(.33) | 2.86(.29) | ||
Middle session | 2.89(.29) | 2.80(.31) | ||
Sixth Session | 3.01(.41) | 2.87(.35) |
Note.
Supportive care treatment condition.
Coping and Communication Intervention treatment condition.
Changes in emotional awareness and group differences
The average emotional awareness scores across all participants for the first, middle, and sixth sessions were 3.66 (SD = .32), 3.77 (SD = .28), and 3.72 (SD = .32), respectively, indicating moderate awareness. Scores were significantly higher in the first (t (207.6) = 3.37, p < .001) and sixth session (t(200) = 3.25, p < .002) in SC compared to CCI (Table 1). Change in emotional awareness across sessions was modeled continuously for each condition, using the time since the middle and sixth surveys were completed after the first session. There was not a significant increase in emotional awareness across sessions in either condition.
Correspondence between emotional awareness and emotional processing
Pearson correlations between emotional awareness and EXP for the first, middle and sixth sessions can be found in Table 2. Mode EXP was not significantly associated with emotional awareness scores in any session in either treatment condition (ps > .20). However, peak EXP was significantly associated with emotional awareness scores in the first (r = .25, p = .006) and sixth (r = .24, p = .02) session in SC and all sessions in CCI (rs = .29 - .31, ps < .002).
Table 2.
Correspondence between emotional awareness scores and EXP mode and peak ratings
Mode EXP | Peak EXP | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|||||
r | p | r | p | az(p) | |
bSC | |||||
Emotional awareness first session | −.002 | .98 | .25 | .006 | 1.96(.05) |
Emotional awareness middle session | −.06 | .55 | .11 | .27 | - |
Emotional awareness sixth session | .06 | .55 | .24 | .02 | 1.31(.19) |
cCCI | |||||
Emotional awareness first session | .04 | .65 | .31 | .001 | 2.02(.04) |
Emotional awareness middle session | .03 | .74 | .30 | .003 | 1.94(.05) |
Emotional awareness sixth session | .13 | .20 | .29 | .005 | 1.16(.25) |
Note. Significance at p < .05 level is signified in bold.
Significance test of the difference between Pearson correlations for mode and peak EXP ratings.
Supportive care treatment condition.
Coping and Communication Intervention treatment condition.
Pre-intervention predictors of emotional awareness
First, we examined univariate demographic, medical, and baseline predictors of emotional awareness scores in the first, middle, and sixth session. Predictors were examined within each condition (CCI and SC), separately (Table 3). No demographic, medical, or baseline predictors were significantly associated with emotional awareness scores in the first and middle session within either treatment condition ps > .05. In SC, married women had significantly lower emotional awareness scores in the sixth session (M = 3.74, SD = .26) compared to unmarried women (M = 3.88, SD = .21), t (102) = −2.71, p < .008. Also in SC, greater baseline negative emotional expression was associated with greater emotional awareness scores in the sixth session, r = .24, p < .02. In CCI, greater cancer-specific distress was associated with greater emotional awareness scores in the sixth session, r = .25, p < .02. No other baseline variables were significantly associated with emotional awareness in the sixth session in either condition.
Table 3.
Univariate correlations between baseline predictors and Emotional awareness scores
Emotional awareness session 1 | Emotional awareness middle session | Emotional awareness session 6 | |
---|---|---|---|
r(p) | r(p) | r(p) | |
aSC | |||
Cancer-specific distress | .07(.47) | −.10(.31) | .12(.24) |
Positive emotional expression | .03(.78) | .03(.74) | .004(.97) |
Negative emotional expression | .06(.50) | .10(.29) | .24(.02) |
Physical well-being | .11(.24) | −.12(.23) | −.03(.76) |
bCCI | |||
Cancer-specific distress | .02(.88) | −.06(.59) | .25(.01) |
Positive emotional expression | .05(.60) | .13(.20) | .13(.21) |
Negative emotional expression | .18(.07)c | −.002(.99) | −.07(.51) |
Physical well-being | .17(.09)c | .04(.73) | −.06(.56) |
Note. Significant correlations (p < .05) are depicted in bold.
Supportive care treatment condition.
Coping and Communication Intervention treatment condition.
Marginally significant correlation (p < .10).
Second, we entered significant demographic and psychological predictors of emotional awareness scores in the sixth session into multiple regression models to determine salient baseline predictors of emotional awareness for each condition, separately (Table 4). In SC, women who reported greater negative emotional expressivity at baseline (β = .19) and those who were unmarried (β = −.25) had greater emotional awareness scores (ps < .05) in the sixth session, while controlling for the first (β = .17, p = .07) and middle (β = .10, p = .32) session emotional awareness scores. In CCI, women who reported greater cancer-specific distress at baseline had greater emotional awareness scores (β = .26, p < .01) while controlling for the first (β = .13, p = .20) and middle (β = .07, p = .51) session emotional awareness scores.
Table 4.
Multiple regression models predicting sixth session emotional awareness scores for SC and CCI condition
F(df) | R2 | p | β | t(p) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SCa | |||||
DV = Emotional awareness sixth session | 4.19(4,98) | .15 | < .005 | ||
bMarital status | −.25 | −2.68(.01) | |||
Negative emotional expressivity | .19 | 2.06(.04) | |||
Emotional awareness first session 1 | .17 | 1.82(.07) | |||
Emotional awareness middle session | .10 | 1.00(.32) | |||
CCIc | |||||
DV = Emotional awareness sixth session | 3.05(3,93) | .09 | < .04 | ||
Cancer-specific distress | .26 | 2.63(.01) | |||
Emotional awareness first session | .13 | 1.28(.20) | |||
Emotional awareness middle session | .07 | .67(.51) |
Note. Significant predictors of sixth session emotional awareness scores are depicted in bold.
Supportive care treatment condition.
Marital status is dummy-coded such that unmarried women are coded as 0 (reference group).
Coping and Communication Intervention treatment condition.
Discussion
This study explored levels of emotional awareness during therapy sessions delivered to women who were newly-diagnosed with gynecologic cancer. We expanded our prior work (Myers Virtue et al., 2015; Manne et al., 2017) by describing emotional awareness and its correspondence with emotional processing. The study had four key findings. We will discuss each along with their clinical implications. First, the average level of emotional awareness across all sessions suggested that patients identified emotions (e.g., “I feel sad about my diagnosis”) and approached levels where they recognized opposing emotions at the same time (e.g., Level 4: “I feel both angry that I was diagnosed and grateful for the medical care I received.”). Most patients discussed their emotions beyond physical sensations (e.g., Level 2: “I was in pain”).
Second, as hypothesized, there was a significant difference in levels of emotional awareness between patients in SC and CCI. Higher levels of emotional awareness in SC may reflect the techniques aimed at facilitating emotional awareness and processing (e.g., therapist reflecting an expressed feeling). When therapists utilize those techniques during therapy with patients, patients may achieve greater awareness of their feelings. However, differences were small (i.e., less than one level difference). Contrary to hypotheses, emotional awareness did not increase over SC or CCI sessions, possibly because the interventions were short-term. Studies examined emotional processing during 20 or more therapy sessions (Pos et al., 2003). In the RCT, CCI achieved greater symptom reduction compared to SC (Manne et al., 2017). Future research may consider whether longer-term SC sessions lead to greater increases in emotional awareness and whether this leads to comparable outcomes with CCI.
Third, consistent with our predictions and the literature, emotional awareness corresponded with emotional processing. Emotions provide valuable information about whether needs and values are met, and higher emotional awareness should be linked with higher emotional processing (Levenson, 1994). Increasing conscious awareness of feelings may guide decisions and create meaning. We did not assess emotional awareness prior to beginning therapy. Thus, we cannot make conclusions about whether baseline emotional awareness predicts in-session emotional processing. Level of emotional awareness was associated with peak EXP but not mode EXP. Peak EXP scores represent the highest level of processing that patients were able to achieve within an EE. Mode EXP score can be considered the ‘average’ level of processing. While higher levels of emotional awareness may not translate to consistently processing emotions at a higher level, patients with higher levels of emotional awareness tended to reach higher levels of emotional processing during psychotherapy. Emotional awareness was not associated with emotional processing in the middle session in SC. Sometimes, patients might display high awareness of their emotional experience but are not deeply processing that information. Future research may examine therapeutic factors that affect the link between emotional awareness and processing.
Fourth, no baseline demographic, medical, or psychological factors were associated with emotional awareness in the first and middle sessions, but they were in the sixth session. Among SC participants, the level of emotional awareness in the sixth session was associated with greater baseline negative emotional expressivity and not being married. In sessions with patients who have a greater tendency to express negative emotions, therapists may have more opportunities to ask about emotions, thus leading to opportunities for patients to become aware of emotional experiences. Married patients may process more emotions with their partners, pointing to a greater need for single patients to pursue emotions in therapy. Among CCI participants, greater cancer-specific distress was associated with greater emotional awareness. Researchers suggest that individuals with anxiety disorders may experience more intense emotional experiences, leading to greater awareness (Novick-Kline et al., 2005). Similarly, patients who experience frequent intrusive thoughts and feelings about cancer may be more aware of those emotions.
Study limitations
The first limitation is that the majority of women were Caucasian, well-educated, diagnosed with ovarian cancer, and diagnosed with advanced disease. Patterns of emotional awareness may differ in more diverse populations, especially male cancer patients. Second, patients were part of a RCT. They did not seek out therapy. Third, we examined emotional awareness in the first, sixth, and a middle session. Different patterns might have emerged in different sessions, particularly the seventh session, when therapy progress was reviewed. Fourth, using the POES to code therapy transcripts has not been tested for validity. Further research is needed to determine the validity of using the POES in assessing in-session emotional awareness, and correspondence with the paper-and-pencil version of the LEAS would provide valuable information. Finally, changes in awareness were small in magnitude, and their clinical significance is unclear.
Clinical implications
There are clinical implications from our study. We observed moderate levels of emotional awareness in the first session. Since there is evidence that greater baseline emotional awareness is related to psychotherapeutic outcomes in psychodynamic and CBT treatments (Beutel et al., 2013), clinicians working with this population may foster baseline emotional awareness through various therapeutic techniques, including focusing exercises in emotion-focused therapy, connecting thoughts, feelings, and behaviors in CBT, or emotional exploration. Emotional awareness corresponded with emotional processing. Considering emotional awareness when facilitating emotional processing with patients may be important. Negative emotional expression was related to in-session emotional awareness in SC where patients were encouraged to freely discuss feelings. Patients who tend to express negative emotions may have greater awareness, which can be therapeutically utilized by exploring the meaning behind those feelings. Finally, cancer-specific distress was related to emotional awareness in the CCI intervention. It is not clear whether greater distress led to greater awareness or vice versa, but clinicians may consider this relationship in therapeutic settings.
Finally, to our knowledge, the POES has not been previously used to code transcripts of therapy sessions. This approach to coding in-session emotional awareness is novel but time-consuming and not practical for clinicians. Use of the paper-and-pencil LEAS or an online version of it (www.eLEAStest.net) may be more practical.
Acknowledgements
We wish to acknowledge Sara Frederick, Tina Gadja, Shira Hichenberg, and Kristen Sorice for study management, Jamie Betancourt, Joanna Crincoli, Katie Darabos, Lauren Faust, Rebecca Henderson, Sloan Harrison, Travis Logan, Kellie McWilliams, Marie Plaisme, Danielle Ryan, Caitlin Scally, Arielle Schwerd, Kaitlyn Smith, Nicole Teitelbaum, and Amanda Viner for collection of study data. We thank the oncologists and nurses at all five cancer centers for allowing access to patients. Finally, we thank the study participants and therapists for their time. This work was supported by grant R01CA85566 (PI: Manne).
Footnotes
Disclosures
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
References
- Armstrong DK, Bundy B, Wenzel L, Huang HQ, Baergen R, Lele S, . . . Burger RA (2006). Intraperitoneal Cisplatin and Paclitaxel in Ovarian Cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 354(1), 34–43. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052985 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barchard KA, Bajgar J, Leaf DE, & Lane RD (2010). Computer scoring of the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale. Behavioral Research Methods, 42(2), 586–595. doi: 10.3758/brm.42.2.586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beutel ME, Scheurich V, Knebel A, Michal M, Wiltink J, Graf-Morgenstern M, . . . Subic-Wrana C (2013). Implementing Panic-Focused Psychodynamic Psychotherapy into Clinical Practice. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 58(6), 326–334. doi: 10.1177/070674371305800604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodurka-Bevers D, Basen-Engquist K, Carmack CL, Fitzgerald MA, Wolf JK, de Moor C, & Gershenson DM (2000). Depression, Anxiety, and Quality of Life in Patients with Epithelial Ovarian Cancer. Gynecologic Oncology, 78(3), 302–308. doi: 10.1006/gyno.2000.5908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cella DF, Tulsky DS, Gray G, Sarafian B, Linn E, Bonomi A, . . . et al. (1993). The Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy scale: development and validation of the general measure. Journal of Clinocal Oncology, 11(3), 570–579. doi: 10.1200/jco.1993.11.3.570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chhatwal J, & Lane RD (2016). A Cognitive-Developmental Model of Emotional Awareness and Its Application to the Practice of Psychotherapy. Psychodynamic Psychiatry, 44(2), 305–325. doi: 10.1521/pdps.2016.44.2.305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciarrochi J, Caputi P, & Mayer J (2003). The distinctiveness and utility of a measure of trait emotional awareness. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1477–1490. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00129-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ciere Y, Janse M, Almansa J, Visser A, Sanderman R, Sprangers MAG, ... Fleer J (2017). Distinct Trajectories of Positive and Negative Affect After Colorectal Cancer Diagnosis. Health Psychology, 36(6), 521–528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Classen C, Koopman C, Angell K, & Spiegel D (1996). Coping styles associated with psychological adjustment to advanced breast cancer. Health Psychology, 15(6), 434–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman G, Harley R, Kerrigan M, et al. (2009). Change in emotional processing during a dialectical behavior therapy-based skills group for major depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(4), 316–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitch MI (2010). Gynecologic cancer. In Holland JC & al E. (Eds.), Psycho-Oncology (pp. 167–171). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frewen P, Lane RD, Neufeld RWJ, Densmore M, Stevens T, & Lanius R (2008). Neural Correlates of Levels of Emotional Awareness During Trauma Script-Imagery in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70(1), 27–31. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815f66d4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gendlin ET (1996). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy: A manual of the experiential method. New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Gonçalves V (2010). Long-term quality of life in gynecological cancer survivors. Current Opinion in Obstetrics and Gynecology, 22(1), 30–35. doi: 10.1097/GCO.0b013e328332e626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg LS (2002). Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through their feelings. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg LS, & Korman L (1993). Assimilating emotion into psychotherapy integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 3(3), 249–265. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg LS, & Pascual-Leone A (2006). Emotion in psychotherapy: A practice-friendly research review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(5), 611–630. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg LS & Safran JD (1984). Integrating affect and cognition: A perspective on the process of therapeutic change. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(6), 559–578. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, & Wilson KG (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Hipkins J, Whitworth M, Tarrier N, & Jayson G (2004). Social support, anxiety and depression after chemotherapy for ovarian cancer: A prospective study. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9(4), 569–581. doi: 10.1348/1359107042304542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horowitz M, Wilner N, & Alvarez W (1979). Impact of Event Scale: a measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41(3), 209–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt MG (1998). The only way out is through: Emotional processing and recovery after a depressing life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 361–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang KH, Cho O&Yoo Y (2016). Symptom clusters of ovarian cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy, and their emotional status and quality of life. European Journal of Oncology Nursing, 21, 215–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. (2008). Cancer care for the whole patient: Meeting psychosocial health needs. Washington, D.C.: The National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA, & Emmons RA (1990). Conflict over emotional expression: psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5), 864–877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein MH, Mathieu PL, & Gendlin ET (1969). The Experiencing Scale: A research and training manual. Madison: Wisconsin Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Korman LM (1998). Changes in clients’ emotion episodes in therapy. (59), ProQuest Information & Learning, US. Retrieved from http://www.collectionscanada.gc.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/tape15/PQDD_0019/NQ27300.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Kulpa M, Kosowicz M, Ziętalewicz U, Stypuła-Ciuba B, & Sosnowski R (2017). Psychological adaptation to cancer control emotion and cognitive patients with cancer of the genitourinary system. Medycyna Paliatywna/Palliative Medicine, 9(1), 30–34. [Google Scholar]
- Lane RD (2000). Neural correlates of conscious emotional experience. In Lane R & Nadel L (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 345–370). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lane RD, & Schwartz GE (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(2), 133–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leaf DE, & Barchard KA (2010). Program for Open-Ended Scoring [POES] version 1.4.1. [Unpublished computer program]. Retrieved from Kim Barchard, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, 4505 Maryland Parkway, Las Vegas, NV, 89154–5030, kim.barchard@unlv.edu [Google Scholar]
- Levenson RW (1994). Human emotion: A functional view. In Ekman P & Davidson RJ (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 123–126). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Manne SL, Myers-Virtue S, Darabos K, Ozga M, Heckman C, Kissane D, & Rotter D (2017). Emotional processing during psychotherapy among women newly diagnosed with a gynecological cancer. Palliative & Supportive Care, 15(4), 405–416. doi: 10.1017/s1478951516000845 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manne SL, Virtue SM, Ozga M, Kashy D, Heckman C, Kissane D, . . . Rodriquez L (2017). A comparison of two psychological interventions for newly-diagnosed gynecological cancer patients. Gynecologic Oncology, 144(2), 354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.ygyno.2016.11.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manne SL, Winkel G, Rubin S, Edelson M, Rosenblum N, Bergman C, . . . Rocereto T (2008). Mediators of a coping and communication-enhancing intervention and a supportive counseling intervention among women diagnosed with gynecological cancers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(6), 1034–1045. doi: 10.1037/a0014071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Missirlian TM, Toukmanian SG, Warwar SH, & Greenberg LS (2005). Emotional arousal, client perceptual processing, and the working alliance in experiential psychotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(5), 861–871. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.5.861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers Virtue S, Manne SL, Darabos K, Heckman CJ, Ozga M, Kissane D, . . . Rosenblum N (2015). Emotion episodes during psychotherapy sessions among women newly diagnosed with gynecological cancers. Psycho-Oncology, 24(9), 1189–1196. doi: 10.1002/pon.3737 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norton TR, Manne SL, Rubin S, Carlson J, Hernandez E, Edelson MI, . . . Bergman C (2004). Prevalence and Predictors of Psychological Distress Among Women With Ovarian Cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 22(5), 919–926. doi: 10.1200/jco.2004.07.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Novick-Kline P, Turk CL, Mennin DS, Hoyt EA, & Gallagher CL (2005). Level of emotional awareness as a differentiating variable between individuals with and without generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(5), 557–572. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orlinsky DE & Howard KI (1986). Process and outcome in psychotherapy. In Handbook of psychotherapy and behavioral change: An empirical analysis, 3rd ed. Garfield S & Gergin A (eds.), pp. 311–385. New York: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Pos AE, Greenberg LS, Goldman RN, & Korman LM (2003). Emotional processing during experiential treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(6), 1007–1016. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.6.1007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roemer L, & Orsillo SM (2005). An Acceptance-Based Behavior Therapy for Generalized Anxiety Disorder. In Orsillo SM & Roemer L (Eds.), Acceptance and Mindfulness-Based Approaches to Anxiety: Conceptualization and Treatment (pp. 213–240). Boston, MA: Springer US. [Google Scholar]
- Schag CA, Ganz PA, & Heinrich RL (1991). CAncer Rehabilitation Evaluation System--short form (CARES-SF). A cancer specific rehabilitation and quality of life instrument. Cancer, 68(6), 1406–1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S, Cameron CL, Bishop M, Collins CA, Kirk SB, & Sworowski L (2000). Emotionally expressive coping predicts psychological and physical adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(5), 875–882. doi: 10.1037//0022006X.68.5.875 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subic-Wrana C, Beutel ME, Knebel A, & Lane RD (2010). Theory of Mind and Emotional Awareness Deficits in Patients With Somatoform Disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(4), 404–411. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d35e83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson JC & Bedard DL (2006). Clients’ emotional processing in psychotherapy: A comparison between cognitive behavioral and process-experiential therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(1), 152–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D, Clark LA, & Tellegen A (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts S, Prescott P,Mason J, McLeod N, Lewith G (2015). Depression and anxiety in ovarian cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prevalence rates, BMJ Open 5 e007618 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-007618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]