Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Aug 20.
Published in final edited form as: Science. 2021 Jul 9;373(6551):181–186. doi: 10.1126/science.aba5483

Gut microbiome heritability is near-universal but environmentally contingent

Laura Grieneisen 1, Mauna Dasari 2, Trevor J Gould 1, Johannes R Björk 2, Jean-Christophe Grenier 3,4, Vania Yotova 3, David Jansen 2, Neil Gottel 5, Jacob B Gordon 6, Niki H Learn 7, Laurence R Gesquiere 6, Tim L Wango 8,9, Raphael S Mututua 8, J Kinyua Warutere 8, Long’ida Siodi 8, Jack A Gilbert 5, Luis B Barreiro 3, Susan C Alberts 6,10,11, Jenny Tung 6,10,11,12,*, Elizabeth A Archie 2,*, Ran Blekhman 1,13,*
PMCID: PMC8377764  NIHMSID: NIHMS1733229  PMID: 34244407

Abstract

Relatives have more similar gut microbiomes than non-relatives, but the degree to which this similarity results from shared genotypes versus shared environments has been controversial. Here, we leverage 16,234 gut microbiome profiles, collected over 14 years from 585 wild baboons, to reveal that host genetic effects on the gut microbiome are near-universal. Controlling for diet, age, and socioecological variation, 97% of microbiome phenotypes are significantly heritable, including several reported as heritable in humans. Heritability is typically low (mean=0.068), but is systematically greater in the dry season, with low diet diversity, and in older hosts. We show that longitudinal profiles and large sample sizes are crucial to quantify microbiome heritability and indicate scope for selection on microbiome characteristics as a host phenotype.

One Sentence Summary:

The presence and abundance of most taxa in the baboon gut microbiome are heritable, but heritability estimates change over time and depend on host environment and age.


An important goal of microbiome research is to determine the heritability of gut microbiome traits (18). Linking microbiome variation to host genetic variation can reveal which aspects of the microbiome are capable of responding to selection on the host, suggest which microbiome traits are under host control, and connect microbial abundance to host pathways and disease states (1, 7). However, current research suggests that heritable gut microbiome taxa are uncommon. In humans, only 3 to 13% of gut microbes have non-zero heritability, and one study estimated that overall microbiome heritability may be as low as 0.019 (1, 2, 4, 6, 7). Furthermore, the few heritable microbiome phenotypes in humans, such as the abundance of the family Christensenellaceae, exhibit widely varying heritability estimates across studies (h2=0.31 – 0.64) (1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 9).

There are challenges in accurately estimating narrow-sense heritability (h2, the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by additive genetic variance) for the human microbiome. First, relatives—especially twins and other first-degree relatives, which are the basis for most microbiome heritability studies—often share diets, behaviors, and built environments, which can lead heritability to be overestimated (10). Controlling for gene-environment correlations requires fine-grained, individual-based environmental and behavioral data, which have not been available in previous studies (1, 2, 46). Second, all current estimates of microbiome heritability in humans rely on cross-sectional microbiome sampling, even though microbial abundances are dynamic and difficult to accurately phenotype from one-time measures (1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 11). Further, h2 can change over a host’s lifetime due to shifting environmental conditions and host attributes (e.g., h2 for body mass index decreases with age, as dietary and behavioral effects increase relative to the effects of genotype: (12, 13)). To date, no studies of gut microbiome heritability have fully accounted for this temporal variability or its dependence on the environment.

Estimating microbiome heritability in a natural primate population

To overcome these challenges, we estimated h2 for gut microbiome traits in 585 wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus, the yellow baboon, with some admixture from anubis baboons, Papio anubis; Fig. 1A). To do so, we used 16,234 16S rRNA gene sequencing-based microbiome profiles from fecal samples collected longitudinally over 14 years. These samples were collected from the Amboseli baboon population (Fig. 1B), which has been the subject of individual-based research since 1971 (14). Each study subject had on average 28 samples collected across 4.5 years (range=1 to 177 samples per baboon; median days between samples=28; Fig. 1A).

Fig. 1. Time-series data used to estimate microbiome heritability.

Fig. 1.

(A) The data set consisted of 16,234 microbiome samples collected from 585 individually-recognized baboons. Each point represents a sample; the y-axis is ordered by baboon age at first sample collection. (B) Map of the 90% kernel density estimate (KDE) home ranges and active dates for the 10 baboon social groups sampled over the study period, based on 71,645 GPS points collected during group monitoring. (C) For each microbiome sample, we had data on the diet consumed by members of the corresponding social group in the 30 days prior to sample collection. Each vertical bar represents one sample, ordered by collection date. Colors represent diet components (see table S1). (D) The relative abundances of microbial phyla in the current study are similar to prior primate studies (1821). (E) The relative abundance of microbial phyla in all 16,234 samples ordered by collection date. ‘Rare’ taxa: <0.5% mean relative abundance per sample. In (C) and (E), the x-axis starts in the year 2000. The same legend applies for (D) and (E).

Baboons lead shorter lives than humans, so these time series often span a substantial fraction of the baboon lifespan (female life expectancy at birth is 10 years; females and males achieve sexual maturity at 4.5 and 5.7 years respectively; (15)). Each microbiome sample is accompanied by detailed information on the pedigree relationships of its donor (fig. S1), as well as fine-grained data on environmental conditions, social behavior, demography, and group-level diet composition at the time of sampling (Fig. 1C; table S1, S2). These complementary data allowed us to achieve precise estimates of heritability and quantify the impact of shifting environmental and social conditions on heritability. They also break apart gene-environment correlations: baboon social groups contain a wide range of maternal, paternal, and non-relatives (median within-group relatedness in a given year=0.055, SD=0.11), yet all group members travel in a coordinated fashion across the landscape and feed on the same seasonally available foods (14). Additionally, groups exhibit substantial home range overlap (Fig. 1B; (16)). Here, we studied ten social groups that varied in size from 17 to 118 members (mean=58).

Each 16S gut microbiome profile was generated from a fecal sample collected from an individually recognized baboon and processed following (17) (figs. S2S4; table S3). Similar to other primates (Fig. 1D; (1821)), the most common gut microbial phyla were Firmicutes, Bacteroidetes, and Actinobacteria (Fig. 1E). Both the abundances of these phyla and the composition of baboon diets show cyclic fluctuations (Fig. 1C, 1E), which reflect Amboseli’s wet-dry seasonal dynamics (14).

Using these microbiome profiles, we estimated the narrow-sense heritability (h2) of 1,034 gut microbiome phenotypes. These included 7 community phenotypes, or measures of microbiome community composition (amplicon sequence variant (ASV) richness, ASV Shannon’s H index, and the first five principal coordinates (PCs) of a Bray-Curtis dissimilarity matrix), and 283 single-taxon phenotypes representing the relative abundance of individual microbiome taxa, from ASVs through phyla, found in >50% of samples (figs. S5S6; (16, 17, 22)). We also estimated h2 for 744 presence/absence phenotypes, which reflect whether a taxon is present or absent in a sample (limited to taxa found in 10% to 90% of samples).

We estimated h2 separately for each phenotype using the animal model, implemented in ASReml-R v3 (tables S4S5; (23)). This mixed-effects model estimates each individual’s additive genetic value as a random effect, based on the expected covariance in additive genetic effects between relatives in a pedigree (2426). It also partitions phenotypic variance across additive genetic variance and other random effects, after conditioning on fixed effects. Following the typical approach in human genetics and plant and animal breeding, we estimated total phenotypic variance (the denominator of h2) after correcting for fixed effects (12, 24, 27). This allowed us to exclude the effects of environmentally variable traits like diet and rainfall, technical effects, and demographic variables like sex and age (figs. S7, S8).

Genetic effects on the gut microbiome are near-universal

We found that 97% of single-taxon and community phenotypes were significantly heritable, including all 7 community phenotypes and 93% (273/283) of single-taxon phenotypes (likelihood ratio test; FDR threshold=0.1; Figs. 2A, 2B, S9, S10; table S6). Heritability was not limited to prevalent taxa, as 95% of the 744 presence/absence phenotypes were also significantly heritable, some of which were found in only 10% of samples (Fig. 2B; fig. S9A, S9B, S9C; table S7). However, more prevalent taxa tended to have higher h2 (Pearson’s R=0.28, p=2.3×10−15; fig. S9D). Importantly, the proportion of significantly heritable single-taxon phenotypes was robust across phylogenetically- and compositionally-aware data transformations (PhILR transformation=96% heritable; CLR transformation=99% heritable; FDR threshold = 0.1; Fig. 2B; tables S8S9). Heritability estimates were correlated between single-taxon phenotypes and CLR transformed single-taxon phenotypes (Pearson’s R=0.82, p=2.3×10−69), and between single-taxon phenotypes and presence/absence phenotypes (Pearson’s R=0.68, p=3.2×10−29; fig. S9E, S9F).

Fig. 2. Most microbiome phenotypes are heritable.

Fig. 2.

(A) Heritability estimates for the 40 most heritable single-taxon phenotypes and all 7 community phenotypes. Red text indicates taxa that are also heritable in humans (1, 2, 46). (B) Heritability estimates were robust across data transformations. Dark purple bars show significantly heritable phenotypes; thin yellow bars indicate mean heritability. (C) Additive genetic variance explained significantly more variance in microbiome phenotypes than host identity or maternal effects. The y-axis is ordered by taxonomic level and h2, as given in table S7. (D) For the 32 microbial taxa heritable in our study (x-axis) and at least one human study (y-axis; (1, 2, 46, 32, 33)), h2 was correlated between baboons and humans (Pearson’s R = 0.52, p = 0.002).

The most heritable phenotype among the single-taxon and community phenotypes was the first principal coordinate of a PCoA of Bray-Curtis dissimilarities, which captures a global summary of variation in the baboon gut microbiome (h2=0.21; p=5.7×10−15; Fig. 2A; Bray-Curtis PC1 explained 19% of the variance in microbiome composition overall (17)). More closely related ASVs tended to have similar h2 (Moran’s I=0.0996, p=0.001; and Pagel’s lambda=0.73, p=0.001), especially ASVs belonging to the families Prevotellaceae, Lachnospiraceae, and Ruminococcaceae (local Moran’s I p<0.05; fig. S11), suggesting a phylogenetic signal in microbe heritability.

While h2 for single-taxon and community phenotypes tended to be low to modest (mean h2 among the 280 significant phenotypes=0.068; range=0.008 – 0.21; Fig. 2C), heritability values for presence/absence traits were significantly higher (paired t-test p=2.2×10−27; mean h2=0.077, maximum h2=0.26; Fig. 2B; fig. S9A, S9B, S9C, S9F), as are heritability estimates from compositionally-aware abundance transformations (paired t-test p=2.7×10−27; mean h2=0.084, maximum h2=0.20; Fig. 2B; fig. S9E). Overall, these values are similar to the heritability of social behavioral traits in nonhuman primates (fig. S12; table S10) and traits with strong social components in humans (28, 29), but exceed most available estimates for fitness in animal populations (30).

Across traits, host genotype explained more variance than host identity (paired t-test p=2.4×10−27) or maternal effects (paired t-test p=5.5×10−86). These results suggest that host genotype is more important in creating familial similarity in baboon microbiome composition than matrilines, even though matrilines form the core kinship units in baboon societies (Fig. 2C; fig. S9B, S9C). Further, we found no evidence that microbial transmission between relatives or assortative mating inflates h2. Parent pairs did not have more similar microbiome composition than non-parent female-male pairs, as would be expected under assortative mating by microbiome composition (Mantel test r=0.004, p=0.22; fig. S7B). In addition, accounting for grooming-based social interaction networks (in the subset of models where such networks could be robustly estimated; n=500) decreased h2 by only 0.0051 on average, and did not significantly improve any models (fig. S7C; table S11). We note that the weak effects of social networks on microbiome similarity are likely due to the longitudinal nature of this data set. In our population, social effects on microbiome composition are strongest between samples collected in the same month, and samples from social partners separated by long time periods are not especially similar (18, 31).

Humans and baboons share heritable taxa

We next investigated whether similar gut microbiome taxa are influenced by host genotype across baboons and humans, which would suggest that trait heritability in the microbiome is conserved. Heritability estimates were correlated for the 32 microbiome taxa found to be heritable in both our study and in at least one of 7 human data sets from 5 studies (n=3,511 aggregate sample size in total; (1, 2, 46, 17, 32, 33)), despite substantial methodological variation in data collection and methods for h2 estimation (Pearson’s R=0.52, p=0.002; results are consistent using a linear mixed model that controls for study: b=0.91, p=0.014; Fig. 2D; table S12). Shared, heritable taxa include the family Christensenellaceae, one of the most consistently heritable phenotypes in humans (baboon: single-taxon h2=0.12; presence/absence h2= 0.20; humans: 0.31–0.64); Fig. 2A; fig. S9A; (1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 9, 34)). In contrast to a previous study in humans, heritable taxa did not co-occur more frequently than expected within hosts (1). However, more heritable taxa did exhibit higher connectivity in taxon co-occurrence networks (Pearson’s R = 0.58, p = 0.006; fig. S13).

Year, season, and host age modify heritability estimates

To understand why microbiome h2 estimates often vary across studies (1, 2, 46), we then investigated social and environmental factors that systematically influence trait heritability. Here, we focused on a refined set of 100 collapsed phenotypes, including the 7 community phenotypes and 93 single-taxon phenotypes in which we collapsed phylogenetically nested taxa to the lowest taxonomic level (following (1, 2, 6); fig. S14; table S13). We found that host traits and environmental conditions had substantial effects on h2. Across years, h2 calculated for a single year can differ by up to 0.24 compared to h2 calculated using all years (n=15 most heritable collapsed phenotypes, evaluated in years with at least 150 individuals and 1000 samples; table S11; Fig. 3A). For example, although h2 for Christensenellaceae R-7 group (the collapsed phenotype for Christensenellaceae)=0.12 across all years, its annual h2 estimates range from 0.06 (in 2002) to 0.18 (in 2007).

Fig. 3. Heritability estimates are affected by year, season, and host age.

Fig. 3.

(A) Heritability estimates varied across years. Panels show h2 for the 15 most heritable collapsed phenotypes in years with sufficient sample size (>150 baboons and >1000 total samples). (B) Heritability estimates for all 100 collapsed phenotypes were highly correlated between seasons (black line; R = 0.83, p = 4.7×10−27). Dashed line is x = y. (C) Heritability estimates for collapsed phenotypes were higher in the dry season than in the wet season (n = 89 taxa heritable in both seasons; paired t-test p = 4.4×10−12). (D) Dietary diversity was highest in the wet season (paired t-test; p = 4.2×10−5). (E) Heritability increased with age for 29/100 collapsed phenotypes). Each density plot represents the observed h2 for these 29 collapsed phenotypes across 3-year sliding age classes. The yellow line indicates mean h2 across all age classes. (F) Heritability estimates per age window for the 10 collapsed phenotypes with the steepest increase in h2 with host age.

Within years, we also observed systematic effects of wet/dry seasonal dynamics on microbiome heritability. On the basis of the 89 collapsed phenotypes that were heritable in both dry and wet season samples (estimated separately; red points in Fig. 3B), we found that h2 was, on average, 48% higher in the dry season than in the wet season (paired t-test p=4.4×10−12; Fig. 3C), even though h2 estimates were strongly correlated between seasons (Pearson’s R=0.81, p=3.5×10−22; Fig. 3B). These seasonal differences in h2 may be explained by seasonal changes in phenotypic variance (Vp): weather in Amboseli is highly variable during the 7-month wet season, with periods of intense rain followed by several weeks with little or no rain, compared to the near invariant dry season. In support, Vp for microbiome phenotypes was higher in wet versus dry seasons (paired t-test p=4.2×10−5; fig. S15A). Baboons also consume a greater diversity and evenness of food types in the wet season compared to the dry season (linear mixed model; b=0.15, p=5.9×10−114; Fig. 3D). Although diet composition and rainfall per se are included in our models, individuals who eat diverse diets may also experience season-dependent environmental variation that our model does not capture. To test this hypothesis, we stratified the data by dietary diversity, and found that heritability estimates were higher in the low diet diversity data set (paired t-test p=1.0×10−11; fig. S15B, S15C; 72% of samples in the high diet diversity data set were collected in the wet season).

Host characteristics such as age can also modify trait heritability (13, 35). Indeed, we found that, for the majority of microbiome phenotypes, h2 increased with host age. When we stratified the 100 collapsed phenotypes into overlapping 3-year age classes of similar sample size (table S14), we found that h2 changed significantly with age for 32% of phenotypes, and 91% of these phenotypes (29 of 32) resulted in higher h2 in older animals (linear models p<0.05; Fig. 3E), with a total increase in h2 of up to 0.24 (Fig. 3F). This observation is driven by both increasing genetic contributions to gut microbiome variation with host age (i.e., increased VA; linear mixed model, b=1.7×10−5, p=0.0085) and decreasing contributions from residual environmental variance (i.e., decreased VR; linear mixed model, b=−2.9 ×10−5, p=1.5 ×10−4). Older baboons ate less diverse diets than younger baboons, regardless of season (linear mixed model; effect of age on diet diversity in the wet season: b=−1.6 ×10−2, p=1.6×10−24; effect of age on diet diversity in the dry season: b=−1.2 ×10−2, p=2.0×10−11; fig. S16A, S16B). In addition, females exhibited reduced social partner diversity with age (linear mixed model; b=−0.35, p=1.4×10−19; fig. S16C, S16D). Moreover, microbiome diversity (Shannon’s H) also decreased slightly with age (linear mixed model; b=−0.0063, p=0.024; fig. S16E) and its h2 exhibited the sixth strongest increase with age (linear model; b=0.013, p=2.5×10−5; fig. 3F). A possible explanation for this pattern is behavioral canalization that is not fully captured by the diet composition effects in our models, whereby older baboons increase in behavioral conservatism with age.

Longitudinal sampling affects heritability estimation

Together, our results qualitatively differ from similar research on humans: instead of a very small number of heritable microbiome phenotypes, we find near-universal heritability (1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 9). Further, we explain systematic variation in h2 on the basis of temporal, environmental, and individual characteristics. These findings suggest that deep, longitudinal sampling is required to accurately characterize microbiome heritability and account for potentially extensive temporal variation (Fig. 4A; note that trait heritability was not correlated with its variability across the life course [coefficient of variation in abundance]: R=−0.14, p=0.15 for n=357 individuals with >10 samples; fig. S17).

Fig. 4. Microbiome phenotypes are dynamic and sampling design affects heritability estimates.

Fig. 4.

(A) Highly heritable microbiome phenotypes fluctuate in abundance (y-axis) in individual hosts over time (x-axis), as shown by Bray-Curtis PC1 and Christensenellaceae. Each row represents a baboon with >100 samples. (B) Longitudinal sampling improves the detection of heritable phenotypes. Purple circles indicate the percent of significantly heritable taxa in our data set when subset from 1 to 20 samples per individual. Yellow circles are the percent of significantly heritable microbiome phenotypes in 7 human data sets from 5 studies ((1, 2, 46, 32, 33); note that the plotted points from (33) and (32) show near-perfect overlap). (C) Heritability varies widely at lower sampling depths, even for highly heritable phenotypes (x-axis). The range of h2 from 100 random subsets at each sampling depth is shown on the y-axis. (D) The percentage of significantly heritable traits rises with increasing sample size. Plot shows the percentage of models (out of 100 subsamples) that were improved by adding pedigree information. Each line represents one of the 100 collapsed phenotypes.

In support, we found that sample size and longitudinal sampling affected both our ability to detect heritable microbiome phenotypes and the heritability estimates themselves. Specifically, if we simulated cross-sectional data by randomly subsetting our collapsed phenotype data set to one sample per individual (n=585 samples; repeated 100 times), we found that <5% of phenotypes were significantly heritable, on average (mean = 4.6%; 95% CI = 3.1–6.1%; Fig. 4B; table S12). This proportion is comparable to that described in most human studies, but increases with more longitudinal samples per individual (Fig. 4B). Further, when we randomly subset our collapsed phenotype data set to 1,000 samples (included repeated samples for some individuals), h2 estimates fell outside their standard error in the full data set in an average of 74% of cases (across 100 random subsamples; Figs. 4C, S18; table S15). Increasing the subset size to 10,000 samples dropped this percentage to 11% (Figs. 4C, S18) and increased the number of significantly heritable phenotypes. With 1,000 samples, heritable microbiome phenotypes detected in the full data set were significantly heritable in only 38% of 100 subsamples, on average (Fig. 4D), but at 10,000 samples, this concordance rose to 98%.

Conclusions

Nearly all gut microbiome taxa are heritable in baboons, including both prevalent and rare taxa. Although the magnitude of these heritability estimates is typically small, some traits exhibit h2 greater than 0.15 (n = 59/744 presence/absence phenotypes; 6/283 single-taxon phenotypes; 1/7 community phenotypes). The universal role played by host genetic variation in our data set contrasts with previous work in humans, which finds few heritable taxa (1, 2, 4, 6, 7). These data sets may have had limited power because all human studies to date have been cross-sectional, and may have lacked data on key environmental variables that mask or modify heritability levels (1, 2, 4, 6, 7). Further, h2 for traits detected in both baboons and humans are correlated (Fig. 2D), suggesting that traits with low h2 in baboons may also be heritable—but have gone undetected—in humans.

Our findings do, however, agree with the observation that environmental effects on gut microbiome variation are larger than additive genetic effects (7). Future work will help refine our understanding of these environmental influences, including whether they mediate and/or interact with the effects of host genotype. Additionally, as 16S rRNA sequencing data have limited resolution, large-scale metagenomic data will be important for understanding whether individual microbial strains or gene content are also heritable, and perhaps more interestingly, whether microbial genotype affects host heritability. Together, our work argues for a qualitative change in perspective: from a microbial landscape largely unaffected by host genotype to one in which host genetics plays a consistent and sometimes appreciable role. These qualities imply that microbiome traits are therefore visible to natural selection on the host genome.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary text and figs
Supplementary tables

Acknowledgments:

We thank J. Altmann for her stewardship of the Amboseli Baboon Research Project (ABRP) and for collecting the fecal samples used in this manuscript (see complete ABRP acknowledgments at https://amboselibaboons.nd.edu/acknowledgements/). We thank the Kenya Wildlife Service, the National Council for Science, Technology, and Innovation, and the National Environment Management Authority for permission to conduct research and collect biological samples in Kenya; the research in this study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUC) at Duke University, Princeton University, and University and Notre Dame and adhered to the laws and guidelines of the Kenyan government. We also thank the University of Nairobi, Institute of Primate Research, National Museums of Kenya, the Amboseli-Longido pastoralist communities, the Enduimet Wildlife Management Area, Ker & Downey Safaris, Air Kenya, and Safarilink for support in Kenya; K. Pinc for ABRP database design; T. Voyles, A. Dumaine, Y. Zhang, M. Rao, T. Vilgalys, A. Lea, N. Snyder-Mackler, P. Durst, J. Zussman, G. Chavez, S. Mukherjee, and R. Debray for fecal sample processing; and three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments.

Funding:

This work was directly supported by NSF DEB 1840223 (EAA, SM, JAG), NIH R21 AG055777 (EAA, RB), NIH R01 AG053330 (EAA), and NIGMS R35 GM128716 (RB). We also acknowledge support from the University of Minnesota Grand Challenges in Biology Postdoctoral Fellowship (LG), the Duke University Population Research Institute P2CHD065563 (pilot award to JT), and Notre Dame’s Eck Institute for Global Health (EAA) and Environmental Change Initiative (EAA). Since 2000, ABRP has been supported by NSF and NIH, including IOS 1456832 (SCA), IOS 1053461 (EAA), DEB 1405308 (JT), IOS 0919200 (SCA), DEB 0846286 (SCA), DEB 0846532 (SCA), IBN 0322781 (SCA), IBN 0322613 (SCA), BCS 0323553 (SCA), BCS 0323596 (SCA), P01AG031719 (SCA), R21AG049936 (JT, SCA), R03AG045459 (JT, SCA), R01AG034513 (SCA), R01HD088558 (JT), and P30AG024361 (SCA). We also thank Princeton University, the Chicago Zoological Society, the Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research, the L.S.B. Leakey Foundation, and the National Geographic Society.

Footnotes

Competing interests: The authors have no competing interests.

Supplementary Materials:

Materials and Methods

Figures S1–S18

Tables S1–S15

References (38–131)

Data and materials availability:

Our data and code are publicly available, but the original biological and DNA samples cannot be shared due to restrictions on third-party sharing for CITES-regulated samples exported from Kenya. 16S rRNA gene sequences are deposited on EBI-ENA (project ERP119849) and Qiita (study 12949; (36)). Analyzed data and code are available on Zenodo (DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.4662081; (37)).

References and Notes:

  • 1.Goodrich JK et al. , Human Genetics Shape the Gut Microbiome. Cell. 159, 789–799 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Goodrich JK et al. , Genetic Determinants of the Gut Microbiome in UK Twins. Cell Host Microbe. 19, 731–743 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blekhman R et al. , Host genetic variation impacts microbiome composition across human body sites. Genome Biol. 16, 191 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Davenport ER et al. , Genome-Wide Association Studies of the Human Gut Microbiota. PLoS One. 10, e0140301 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lim MY et al. , The effect of heritability and host genetics on the gut microbiota and metabolic syndrome. Gut. 66, 1031–1038 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Turpin W et al. , Association of host genome with intestinal microbial composition in a large healthy cohort. Nat. Genet 48, 1413–1417 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Rothschild D et al. , Environment dominates over host genetics in shaping human gut microbiota. Nature (2018), doi: 10.1038/nature25973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kurilshikov A et al. , Genetics of human gut microbiome composition (2020), p. 2020.06.26.173724.
  • 9.Goodrich JK, Davenport ER, Clark AG, Ley RE, The Relationship Between the Human Genome and Microbiome Comes into View. Annu. Rev. Genet 51, 413–433 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lax S et al. , Longitudinal analysis of microbial interaction between humans and the indoor environment. Science. 345, 1048–1052 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schlomann BH, Parthasarathy R, Timescales of gut microbiome dynamics. Curr. Opin. Microbiol 50, 56–63 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Visscher PM, Hill WG, Wray NR, Heritability in the genomics era—concepts and misconceptions. Nat. Rev. Genet 9, 255 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ge T, Chen C-Y, Neale BM, Sabuncu MR, Smoller JW, Phenome-wide heritability analysis of the UK Biobank. PLoS Genet. 13, e1006711 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Alberts SC, Altmann J, in Long-Term Field Studies of Primates, Kappeler PM, Watts DP, Eds. (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2012), pp. 261–287. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bronikowski AM et al. , Female and male life tables for seven wild primate species. Sci Data. 3, 160006 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Markham AC, Guttal V, Alberts S, Altmann J, When good neighbors don’t need fences: temporal landscape partitioning among baboon social groups. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 67, 875–884 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Materials and methods are available as supplementary materials at the Science website.
  • 18.Tung J et al. , Social networks predict gut microbiome composition in wild baboons. Elife. 4 (2015), doi: 10.7554/eLife.05224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grieneisen LE, Livermore J, Alberts S, Tung J, Archie EA, Group Living and Male Dispersal Predict the Core Gut Microbiome in Wild Baboons. Integr. Comp. Biol 57, 770–785 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Berer K et al. , Gut microbiota from multiple sclerosis patients enables spontaneous autoimmune encephalomyelitis in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 114, 10719–10724 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mann AE et al. , Biodiversity of protists and nematodes in the wild nonhuman primate gut. ISME J. 14, 609–622 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Walters WA et al. , Large-scale replicated field study of maize rhizosphere identifies heritable microbes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115, 7368–7373 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gilmour AR, in Proceedings of the Association for the Advancement of Animal Breeding and Genetics (1997), vol. 12, pp. 386–390. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kruuk LEB, Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the “animal model.” Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci 359, 873–890 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wilson AJ et al. , An ecologist’s guide to the animal model. J. Anim. Ecol 79, 13–26 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wilson AJ, Why h2 does not always equal V A/V P? J. Evol. Biol 21, 647–650 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kruuk LEB, Hadfield JD, How to separate genetic and environmental causes of similarity between relatives. J. Evol. Biol 20, 1890–1903 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barban N et al. , Genome-wide analysis identifies 12 loci influencing human reproductive behavior. Nat. Genet 48, 1462–1472 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cesarini D, Visscher PM, Genetics and educational attainment. NPJ Sci Learn. 2, 4 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hendry AP, Schoen DJ, Wolak ME, Reid JM, The Contemporary Evolution of Fitness. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst (2018), doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110617-062358. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ren T, Grieneisen LE, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Wu M, Development, diet and dynamism: longitudinal and cross‐sectional predictors of gut microbial communities in wild baboons. Environ. Microbiol (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yatsunenko T et al. , Human gut microbiome viewed across age and geography. Nature. 486, 222–227 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Turnbaugh PJ et al. , A core gut microbiome in obese and lean twins. Nature. 457, 480–484 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Waters JL, Ley RE, The human gut bacteria Christensenellaceae are widespread, heritable, and associated with health. BMC Biol. 17, 83 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Plomin R, Deary IJ, Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 98–108 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gonzalez A et al. , Qiita: rapid, web-enabled microbiome meta-analysis. Nat. Methods 15, 796–798 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Grieneisen L et al. , blekhmanlab/universal_microbiome_h2: v1.0. Zenodo (2021), doi: 10.5281/zenodo.4662081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Alberts SC, Altmann J, Archie EA, Tung J, Monitoring guide for the Amboseli baboon research project: protocols for long-term monitoring and data collection. Published online at http://princeton.edu/baboon/monitoring_guide.html (2011).
  • 39.Grieneisen LE et al. , Genes, geology and germs: gut microbiota across a primate hybrid zone are explained by site soil properties, not host species. Proc. Biol. Sci 286, 20190431 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Alberts SC et al. , in Primate Seasonality: Implications for Human Evolution, Van Schaik DKBACP, Ed. (2005), pp. 157–196.
  • 41.Ren T, Grieneisen LE, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Wu M, Development, diet and dynamism: longitudinal and cross-sectional predictors of gut microbial communities in wild baboons. Environ. Microbiol 18, 1312–1325 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Alberts SC, Altmann J, Balancing Costs and Opportunities: Dispersal in Male Baboons. Am. Nat 145, 279–306 (1995). [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Alberts SC, Altmann J, in Long-Term Field Studies of Primates, Kappeler PM, Watts DP, Eds. (Springer; Berlin Heidelberg, 2012), 12, pp. 261–287. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Buchan JC, Alberts SC, Silk JB, Altmann J, True paternal care in a multi-male primate society. Nature. 425, 179–181 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Alberts SC, Buchan JC, Altmann J, Sexual selection in wild baboons: from mating opportunities to paternity success. Anim. Behav 72, 1177–1196 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Morrissey MB, Wilson AJ, pedantics: an r package for pedigree-based genetic simulation and pedigree manipulation, characterization and viewing. Mol. Ecol. Resour 10, 711–719 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Altmann J, Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 49, 227–267 (1974). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Silk JB, Activities and feeding behavior of free-ranging pregnant baboons. Int. J. Primatol 8, 593–613 (1987). [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Altmann SA, Fallback foods, eclectic omnivores, and the packaging problem. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 140, 615–629 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Altmann SA, Foraging for survival: yearling baboons in Africa (University of Chicago Press, 1998). [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bronikowski AM, Altmann J, Foraging in a Variable Environment: Weather Patterns and the Behavioral Ecology of Baboons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 39, 11–25 (1996). [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Muruthi P, Altmann J, Altmann S, Resource base, parity, and reproductive condition affect females’ feeding time and nutrient intake within and between groups of a baboon population. Oecologia. 87, 467–472 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Shopland JM, Food quality, spatial deployment, and the intensity of feeding interference in yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 21, 149–156 (1987). [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Gesquiere Laurence, Beehner Jacinta, Khan Memuna, Lynch Jessica, Fox Elizabeth, Altmann Jeanne, “AMBOSELI BABOON RESEARCH PROJECT - LABORATORY PROCEDURES FOR FECAL HORMONES” (Princeton University, 2017), (available at https://amboselibaboons.nd.edu/assets/243149/abrp_lab_procedures_for_fecal_hormones_updated_21_jul_2017.pdf). [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Puberty and dispersal in a wild primate population. Horm. Behav 64, 240–249 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gesquiere LR et al. , Life at the top: rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science. 333, 357–360 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Beehner JC et al. , Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas. Horm. Behav 56, 472–480 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gesquiere LR, Onyango PO, Alberts SC, Altmann J, Endocrinology of year-round reproduction in a highly seasonal habitat: environmental variability in testosterone and glucocorticoids in baboon males. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 144, 169–176 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gesquiere LR et al. , Coming of age: steroid hormones of wild immature baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Am. J. Primatol 67, 83–100 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Testosterone positively associated with both male mating effort and paternal behavior in Savanna baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Horm. Behav 63, 430–436 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Blekhman R et al. , Common methods for fecal sample storage in field studies yield consistent signatures of individual identity in microbiome sequencing data. Sci. Rep 6, 31519 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.MoBio, “PowerSoil DNA Isolation Kit,” (available at https://mobio.com/media/wysiwyg/pdfs/protocols/12888.pdf).
  • 63.QIAGEN, “MO BIO’s PowerSoil-htp 96 Well Soil DNA Kit,” (available at https://www.qiagen.com/us/resources/resourcedetail?id=fd3fa52e-3a66-4d55-a9cd-7ed20ea046d9&lang=en).
  • 64.Gilbert JA, Jansson JK, Knight R, The Earth Microbiome project: successes and aspirations. BMC Biol. 12, 69 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Caporaso JG et al. , Global patterns of 16S rRNA diversity at a depth of millions of sequences per sample. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 108Suppl 1, 4516–4522 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Caporaso JG et al. , Ultra-high-throughput microbial community analysis on the Illumina HiSeq and MiSeq platforms. ISME J. 6, 1621–1624 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Callahan BJ et al. , DADA2: High-resolution sample inference from Illumina amplicon data. Nat. Methods 13, 581–583 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Illumina, “bcl2fastq2 Conversion Software,” (available at https://emea.support.illumina.com/content/dam/illumina-support/documents/documentation/software_documentation/bcl2fastq/bcl2fastq2-v2-20-software-guide-15051736-03.pdf).
  • 69.Martin M, Cutadapt removes adapter sequences from high-throughput sequencing reads. EMBnet. journal 17, 10–12 (2011). [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Callahan B, “A DADA2 workflow for Big Data,” (available at https://benjjneb.github.io/dada2/bigdata.html).
  • 71.Wright ES, Yilmaz LS, Noguera DR, DECIPHER, a search-based approach to chimera identification for 16S rRNA sequences. Appl. Environ. Microbiol 78, 717–725 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Quast C et al. , The SILVA ribosomal RNA gene database project: improved data processing and web-based tools. Nucleic Acids Res. 41, D590–6 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Goh WWB, Wang W, Wong L, Why Batch Effects Matter in Omics Data, and How to Avoid Them. Trends Biotechnol. 35, 498–507 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Leek JT et al. , Tackling the widespread and critical impact of batch effects in high-throughput data. Nat. Rev. Genet 11, 733–739 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Schloss PD, Gevers D, Westcott SL, Reducing the effects of PCR amplification and sequencing artifacts on 16S rRNA-based studies. PLoS One. 6, e27310 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Oksanen J et al. , The vegan package. Community ecology package. 10, 631–637 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Paulson JN, Pop M, Bravo HC, metagenomeSeq: Statistical analysis for sparse high-throughput sequencing. Bioconductor package. 1, 63 (2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Roberts DW, Others, labdsv: Ordination and multivariate analysis for ecology. R package version. 1 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Gloor GB, Macklaim JM, Pawlowsky-Glahn V, Egozcue JJ, Microbiome Datasets Are Compositional: And This Is Not Optional. Front. Microbiol 8 (2017), doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2017.02224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Silverman JD, Washburne AD, Mukherjee S, David LA, A phylogenetic transform enhances analysis of compositional microbiota data. Elife. 6 (2017), doi: 10.7554/eLife.21887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.van den Boogaart KG, Tolosana-Delgado R, “compositions”: A unified R package to analyze compositional data. Comput. Geosci 34, 320–338 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Harrell FE Jr, Dupont MC, The Hmisc Package. R package version. 3, 3 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Pico BA, Neser FWC, Van Wyk JB, Genetic parameters for growth traits in South African Brahman cattle. S. Afr. J. Anim. Sci 34, 44–46 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Neira R et al. , Genetic improvement in coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). II: Selection response for early spawning date. Aquaculture. 257, 1–8 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ellen ED, Ducrocq V, Ducro BJ, Veerkamp RF, Bijma P, Genetic parameters for social effects on survival in cannibalistic layers: combining survival analysis and a linear animal model. Genet. Sel. Evol 42, 27 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Crump RE, Haley CS, Thompson R, Mercer J, Individual animal model estimates of genetic parameters for reproduction traits of Landrace pigs performance tested in a commercial nucleus herd. Anim. Sci 65, 285–290 (1997). [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Safari E, Fogarty NM, Gilmour AR, A review of genetic parameter estimates for wool, growth, meat and reproduction traits in sheep. Livestock Production Science. 92, 271–289 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Brommer JE, Rattiste K, Wilson AJ, Exploring plasticity in the wild: laying date-temperature reaction norms in the common gull Larus canus. Proc. Biol. Sci 275, 687–693 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Teplitsky C, Mills JA, Alho JS, Yarrall JW, Merilä J, Bergmann’s rule and climate change revisited: disentangling environmental and genetic responses in a wild bird population. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 105, 13492–13496 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Foerster K et al. , Sexually antagonistic genetic variation for fitness in red deer. Nature. 447, 1107–1110 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Charmantier A, Perrins C, McCleery RH, Sheldon BC, Quantitative genetics of age at reproduction in wild swans: support for antagonistic pleiotropy models of senescence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 103, 6587–6592 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Hicks AL et al. , Gut microbiomes of wild great apes fluctuate seasonally in response to diet. Nat. Commun 9, 1786 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.David LA et al. , Diet rapidly and reproducibly alters the human gut microbiome. Nature. 505, 559–563 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Bennett G et al. , Host age, social group, and habitat type influence the gut microbiota of wild ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Am. J. Primatol 78, 883–892 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Markle JGM et al. , Sex differences in the gut microbiome drive hormone-dependent regulation of autoimmunity. Science. 339, 1084–1088 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Maurice CF et al. , Marked seasonal variation in the wild mouse gut microbiota. ISME J. 9, 2423–2434 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Yang J et al. , Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat. Genet 42, 565–569 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Jelenkovic A et al. , Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994. Elife. 5 (2016), doi: 10.7554/eLife.20320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y, Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Stat. Methodol 57, 289–300 (1995). [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Butler DG, Cullis BR, Gilmour AR, Gogel BJ, Thompson R, ASReml-R reference manual (version 3). The State of Queensland, Department of Primary Industries and Fisheries: Brisbane, Qld (2009) (available at https://asreml.kb.vsni.co.uk/wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2018/02/ASReml-R-Reference-Manual-4.pdf). [Google Scholar]
  • 101.de Villemereuil P, Quantitative genetic methods depending on the nature of the phenotypic trait. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1422, 29–47 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Schliep KP, phangorn: phylogenetic analysis in R. Bioinformatics. 27, 592–593 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K, APE: Analyses of Phylogenetics and Evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 20, 289–290 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Keck F, Rimet F, Bouchez A, Franc A, phylosignal: an R package to measure, test, and explore the phylogenetic signal. Ecol. Evol 6, 2774–2780 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kamilar JM, Cooper N, Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci 368, 20120341 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Friedman J, Alm EJ, Inferring correlation networks from genomic survey data. PLoS Comput. Biol 8, e1002687 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Brent LJN et al. , Personality Traits in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Are Heritable but Do Not Predict Reproductive Output. Int. J. Primatol 35, 188–209 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Madlon-Kay S et al. , Weak effects of common genetic variation in oxytocin and vasopressin receptor genes on rhesus macaque social behavior. Am. J. Primatol 80, e22873 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Blomquist GE, Brent LJN, Applying Quantitative Genetic Methods to Primate Social Behavior. Int. J. Primatol 35, 108–128 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Brent LJN et al. , Genetic origins of social networks in rhesus macaques. Sci. Rep 3, 1042 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Staes N et al. , Bonobo personality traits are heritable and associated with vasopressin receptor gene 1a variation. Sci. Rep 6, 38193 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Havill LM et al. , Heritability of lumbar trabecular bone mechanical properties in baboons. Bone. 46, 835–840 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Joganic JL et al. , Additive genetic variation in the craniofacial skeleton of baboons (genus Papio) and its relationship to body and cranial size. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 165, 269–285 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Martin LJ et al. , Lifespan in captive baboons is heritable. Mech. Ageing Dev 123, 1461–1467 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Williams-Blangero S, Blangero J, Heritability of age at first birth in captive olive baboons. Am. J. Primatol 37, 233–239 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Hlusko LJ, Weiss KM, Mahaney MC, Statistical genetic comparison of two techniques for assessing molar crown size in pedigreed baboons. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 117, 182–189 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Miley DD et al. , Heritability of alveolar bone loss from periodontal disease in a baboon population: a pilot study. J. Periodontol 82, 575–580 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Hansen HL, Bredbenner TL, Nicolella DP, Mahaney MC, Havill LM, Cross-sectional geometry of the femoral midshaft in baboons is heritable. Bone. 45, 892–897 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Voruganti VS et al. , Characterization of ghrelin in pedigreed baboons: evidence for heritability and pleiotropy. Obesity. 16, 804–810 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Rogers J et al. , Genetics of monoamine metabolites in baboons: overlapping sets of genes influence levels of 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid, 3-hydroxy-4-methoxyphenylglycol, and homovanillic acid. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 739–744 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Cole SA et al. , Genetics of leptin expression in baboons. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord 27, 778–783 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Kammerer CM, Effects of sex, age, weight, and heredity on blood pressure in baboons. Am. J. Hum. Biol 7, 149–158 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Johnson Z et al. , Genetic influences on response to novel objects and dimensions of personality in Papio baboons. Behav. Genet 45, 215–227 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Comuzzie AG et al. , The baboon as a nonhuman primate model for the study of the genetics of obesity. Obes. Res 11, 75–80 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Jaquish CE et al. , Genetics of adult body mass and maintenance of adult body mass in captive baboons (Papio hamadryas subspecies). Am. J. Primatol 42, 281–288 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Archie EA, Tung J, Clark M, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Social affiliation matters: both same-sex and opposite-sex relationships predict survival in wild female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences. 281, 20141261 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Hijmans RJ et al. , Package “raster.” R package (2015) (available at ftp://slartibardfast.gtlib.gatech.edu/pub/CRAN/web/packages/raster/raster.pdf). [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Ruby JG et al. , Estimates of the Heritability of Human Longevity Are Substantially Inflated due to Assortative Mating. Genetics. 210, 1109–1124 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Tung J, Charpentier MJ, Mukherjee S, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Genetic effects on mating success and partner choice in a social mammal. Am. Nat 180, 113–129 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Amato KR et al. , Patterns in Gut Microbiota Similarity Associated with Degree of Sociality among Sex Classes of a Neotropical Primate. Microb. Ecol 74, 250–258 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Thomson CE, Winney IS, Salles OC, Pujol B, A guide to using a multiple-matrix animal model to disentangle genetic and nongenetic causes of phenotypic variance. PLoS One. 13, e0197720 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary text and figs
Supplementary tables

Data Availability Statement

Our data and code are publicly available, but the original biological and DNA samples cannot be shared due to restrictions on third-party sharing for CITES-regulated samples exported from Kenya. 16S rRNA gene sequences are deposited on EBI-ENA (project ERP119849) and Qiita (study 12949; (36)). Analyzed data and code are available on Zenodo (DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.4662081; (37)).

RESOURCES