Abstract
In this perspective paper, we focus on the study of synchronization abilities across the animal kingdom. We propose an ecological approach to studying nonhuman animal synchronization that begins from observations about when, how and why an animal might synchronize spontaneously with natural environmental rhythms. We discuss what we consider to be the most important, but thus far largely understudied, temporal, physical, perceptual and motivational constraints that must be taken into account when designing experiments to test synchronization in nonhuman animals. First and foremost, different species are likely to be sensitive to and therefore capable of synchronizing at different timescales. We also argue that it is fruitful to consider the latent flexibility of animal synchronization. Finally, we discuss the importance of an animal's motivational state for showcasing synchronization abilities. We demonstrate that the likelihood that an animal can successfully synchronize with an environmental rhythm is context-dependent and suggest that the list of species capable of synchronization is likely to grow when tested with ecologically honest, species-tuned experiments.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Synchrony and rhythm interaction: from the brain to behavioural ecology’.
Keywords: synchronization, timing, flexibility, motivation, ecological design
1. Introduction
Humans synchronize flexibly with each other and with environmental rhythms, and enjoy doing so: we share music and dance together, and bounce our babies. These synchronized behaviours contribute to social bonding and group coherence [1,2]. Many nonhuman animal species also engage in synchronized displays. Hundreds of fireflies in Southeast Asia flash in unison [3], creating a ‘beaconing’ effect to attract mates [4]. Groups of up to approximately 10 orthopterans and frogs coordinate their calls [5–8], creating choruses that are louder than any single organism signalling alone [9], improving the probability of reproductive success and serving an anti-predatory function [10,11]. Synchronization between conspecifics has also been observed in bioluminescent fish and marine crustaceans [12,13] as well as claw-waving crabs [14]. Despite its ubiquity across the animal kingdom, rhythmic synchronization is accomplished via different mechanisms, serves different functions and is observed in different contexts for different species.
We propose an ecologically honest approach to studying synchronization abilities in nonhuman animals in the milliseconds-to-seconds range; we are not concerned with synchronization on circadian or seasonal timescales. Our approach involves mapping the possibility space that describes when and why synchrony might be possible in the wild, given the temporal, sensory, motor and motivational constraints on a species' natural repertoire. Taken together with a survey of the latent flexibility of an animal's synchronization behaviours, these considerations will inform us about the range of natural and in-laboratory conditions in which we can experimentally test the capabilities and limitations of animal synchronization, respecting the full range of potential forms synchronization can take on.
We must first consider what it means for any animal (human or nonhuman) to be capable of synchronization. Nonlinear dynamics prescribes a set of preconditions for synchronization, from which we derive behavioural and theoretical consequences [15]. Notably, this definition of synchronization is a general one and can be applied as easily to a pendulum as it can to animal synchronization.
First, the synchronizer must generate its own rhythm. For any animal to be capable of synchronization, it must first be capable of auto-generating a rhythmic behaviour that can then become synchronized to an environmental rhythm. Critically, this distinguishes synchronization from repetitive reactions to repetitive stimuli, which may occur independent of the presence of regular rhythmic structure in a stimulus. Although these two means to temporal coordination are not necessarily separable based on data analysis alone, one way to empirically distinguish between synchronization and repetitive reaction is to focus on anticipation of the stimulus by the response [16]. Human finger taps occur slightly before stimulus events, a phenomenon termed negative mean asynchrony [17]. By contrast, macaque monkeys' movements lag behind repetitive stimulus events [18], though this time lag is shorter than standard reaction times, ruling out a completely reactive strategy. In interactions between katydids of the genus Mecopoda, one male often consistently leads another [19,20], though this phenomenon might simply reflect one male being able to call faster than the other. Nonetheless, independent of the precise underlying mechanism, many species demonstrate signatures of anticipatory synchronization behaviour that are distinguishable from serial reactions to repetitive stimulation.
Second, a synchronizer adjusts its own rhythm during interaction with an environmental rhythm. That is, a synchronizer adopts a common frequency as, and therefore a fixed phase relationship with, a stimulus rhythm. Notably, a fixed phase relationship does not imply temporal coincidence, as anti-phase relationships are also possible between synchronized systems [15]; natural anti-phase synchronization is exhibited by, for example, harbour seals (Phoca vitulina: [21]), bottlenose dolphins (genus Tursiops: [22]), frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus: [23]) and katydids (Ephippiger ephippiger: [6]). Arguably, the most critical implication of this precondition is tempo-flexibility [24]: an organism lacking tempo-flexibility could only synchronize with an environmental rhythm at precisely its own intrinsic rate, and it is obvious that such a tempo-restricted form of synchronization would be of limited practicality.
Third, adjustments of a synchronizer's rhythm occur in a limited range of mismatch with the environmental rhythm (detuning). Synchronization is accomplished most easily when the rate of the organism's own rhythm closely matches the rate of the stimulus rhythm. No organism possesses unlimited flexibility; thus, as the difference between the intrinsic and environmental rates increases, synchronization is less successful. The natural consequence of this precondition is a restricted range of tempi around one's own preferred rate with which an organism can accomplish 1 : 1 synchronization. When an environmental rhythm becomes too extreme, katydids switch to a 1 : 2 or 2 : 1 synchronization mode [25] or synchronize with unstable, constantly changing phase relationships [19].
Thus, empirically, synchronization can be identified based on the presence of anticipation and tempo-flexibility within a restricted range. This definition of synchronization is mechanistically, motivationally and cognitively agnostic. This is important, as there is no privileged mechanism for synchrony across the animal kingdom. In fact, the mechanisms supporting synchronization differ within orthopterans [6,19,20,26,27] and within fireflies [3,28], and humans can achieve synchrony by engaging different neural mechanisms depending on the disease state of an individual [29]. Both anticipation and tempo-flexibility have been recognized in the comparative literature as critical features of synchrony [16,24]. However, this is the first acknowledgement we are aware of that tempo-flexibility must necessarily be rate-restricted. This definition has clear and actionable consequences for empirical assessments of synchrony in different species that necessitate the adoption of an ecological approach: we must establish the natural range of behavioural periods species and individuals produce to fairly assess their synchronization capabilities in the laboratory.
We discuss what we consider to be the most important temporal, physical, sensory and motivational constraints that must be considered together when designing experiments to test synchronization abilities. First, we examine the range of timescales that different species act within and are likely to be sensitive to (§2). Second, we argue that it is fruitful to consider the latent flexibility of synchronization across a number of dimensions (§3). Finally, we discuss the importance of an animal's motivational state for demonstrating synchronization abilities (§4). We review findings from both field and laboratory studies as, for many species, only laboratory data are available. However, we argue that these studies underestimate synchronization abilities across the animal kingdom, and we lay out an alternative approach that starts from an animal's natural repertoire.
2. Time-scale constraints on synchronization
Most animals producing rhythmic behaviours do so across a range of possible rates that depends on context and motor system. For example, typical human gait ranges between 0.7 and 1.2 Hz (42 and 72 beats per minute, BPM) [30], while the mean movement frequency across all daily behaviours is closer to 2.0 Hz (120 BPM) [31]. Interestingly, it is this latter number that best matches human preferences for tempi in popular music [32]. Comparative work exploring nonhuman animal synchronization often bases stimulus design on these human rate preferences. However, to select appropriate stimulus rates for assessing synchrony, we must target tempi within an animal's natural range for any selected behaviour. What sets the limits of these ranges, and how might we chart them for different species?
Skeletal and motor systems set inherent limits on the movement frequencies of animals. Movement rates may be ‘soft assembled’, that is, a byproduct of specific effector properties acting on and reacting to the environment. Thus, animal effector systems will naturally ‘prefer’ certain movement rates in certain contexts. An empirical assessment of stepping frequency in animals of different sizes has found that total body mass, effector size and jointing account for much of the variability across species and animals of different overall sizes ([33]; table 1). Such innate tendencies will vary with age (figure 1) and sex, both of which can affect joint elasticity and body size. Gait can be easily measured across species and may be a good starting point for establishing rate capabilities across species, but will not directly predict movement period across all motor systems and environments. For example, movement patterns may also vary between media: California sea lion vocalization (bark) rates underwater are slower than in air, possibly because of the increased energetic requirements of sound production underwater (table 1 and figure 1).
Table 1.
species | behaviour | cycles/second (Hz) | context | social? |
---|---|---|---|---|
bobwhite [30] | gait | 1.5–4.5 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
guineafowl [30] | gait | 0.7–3 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
turkey [30] | gait | 1.2–2.2 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
emu [30] | gait | 1–1.5 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
rhea [30] | gait | 0.8–1.5 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
ostrich [30] | gait | 0.8–1.3 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
human [30] | gait | 0.7–1.2 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
painted quail [30] | gait | 2–7 | naturally occurring | maybe(a) |
macaque [34] | teeth chattering | 5.7 mean | naturally occurring | yes |
C. sea lion [35] | in air non-directed barking | 2.1 mean | naturally occurring | yes |
C. sea lion [35] | in air directed barking | 2.5 mean | naturally occurring | yes |
C. sea lion [35] | underwater non-directed barking | 1 | naturally occurring | yes |
C. sea lion [35] | underwater directed barking | 1.4 | naturally occurring | yes |
bonobo [36] | contest hooting | 3–4 | naturally occurring | yes |
Thailand fireflies [3] | flashing | 1.8 | naturally occurring | yes |
ostracode crustaceans [37] | flashing | 0.3–2 | naturally occurring | yes |
ponyfish [12] | flashing | 1.8 | naturally occurring | yes |
cicada [38] | ticking | 16–25 | naturally occurring | yes |
cicada [38] | buzzing | 3.6–6.7 | naturally occurring | yes |
katydid [39] | chirps (stridulation) | 1.8–5.0 | naturally occurring | yes |
katydid [39] | forewing strid | 14–250 | naturally occurring | yes |
snowy tree cricket [40] | chirps (stridulation) | 2.2–2.6 | naturally occurring | yes |
Japanese tree frog [41] | vocal chorusing | 3.5 | naturally occurring | yes |
fiddler crab [14] | claw waving | 1.7 | naturally occurring | yes |
dolphin [42] | surfacing while travelling | 0.003–0.009 | naturally occurring | yes |
dolphin [43] | breathing interval | 0.01–0.2 | naturally occurring | yes |
right whale dolphin [44] | burst pulses | 4–11.3 | naturally occurring | maybe(b) |
bottlenose dolphin [22] | whistles | 1–6 | naturally occurring | yes |
gelada [45] | synch calls | 2–3.5 | naturally occurring | yes |
male mouse [46] | ultrasonic vocalizations | 1.3–5.4 | naturally occurring | yes |
bat [47] | echolocation | 5–200 | naturally occurring | maybe(b) |
human [31] | daily movement | 2 mean | naturally occuring | maybe(b) |
human [48] | music tempo | 2 mean | averaged from popular songs | maybe(b) |
Snowball (cockatoo) [49] | bobbing | 1.7–2.2 | untrained to music, experimental | maybe(c) |
bonobo [50] | one-hand drumming | 4.2–4.8 | untrained, experimental | maybe(c) |
giraffe [33] | gait | 1.3 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
buffalo [33] | gait | 2.2 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
eland [33] | gait | 2.7 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
zebra [33] | gait | 3 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
wildebeest [33] | gait | 2.9 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
hartebeest [33] | gait | 2.3 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
topi [33] | gait | 2.5 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
warthog [33] | gait | 3.5 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
impala [33] | gait | 2.3 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
Thomson's gazelle [33] | gait | 3 | chased to run, field | maybe(a) |
budgerigars [51] | pecking | 0.6–2.2 | trained, experimental | maybe(b) |
macaque [18] | tapping | 1–2.2 | trained, experimental | maybe(c) |
chimpanzee [52] | tapping | 2.2–2.8 | trained, experimental | maybe(c) |
Ronan (C. sea lion) [53] | bobbing | 1.2–2.3 | trained, experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 4–5 [54] | tapping | 2.4–5 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 6–7 [54] | tapping | 2.5–5.7 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 8–9 [54] | tapping | 1.4–3.1 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 10–12 [54] | tapping | 1.4–3.3 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 18–38 [54] | tapping | 1.1–2.9 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 39–59 [54] | tapping | 1.3–4.4 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 60–74 [54] | tapping | 1.2–2.9 | experimental | maybe(c) |
human age 75+ [54] | tapping | 1.2–2.5 | experimental | maybe(c) |
While effectors may move at a range of tempi, synchronization will also be limited by an animal's perception. How do we know what rates might be well-perceived by an animal? Perception of rate and regularity can be assessed in controlled laboratory settings [55], but we may also infer the perceptual importance of specific rates by analysing the meaningful rate-varying stimuli that an animal encounters in its natural environment. Wild sea lions produce reliably isochronous barks at a mean rate of 2.1 Hz (126 BPM) [35]. Ronan the California sea lion, one of the most accomplished nonhuman experimental synchronizers, has shown top performance synchronizing with stimuli slightly faster than 2 Hz (120 BPM) [53]. Although Ronan's trained head-bobbing behaviour is likely not naturally occurring, the overlap of the tempi of wild sea lion barks and Ronan's beat synchronization performance is notable. When important stimuli in an animal's environment have reliable rates, and changes in those rates signal important information, as is true for sea lion vocalizations (figure 1), it is a safe bet that the animal is perceptually sensitive to stimulus rates in that range.
A preliminary assessment of prior literature on movement rates across species (table 1) indicates a large variability of rates and ranges within and across species, many far removed from the typical human-preferred rates used in many comparative experiments. Naturally occurring rates in nonhuman animal behaviour should thus anchor empirical assessment of animal synchronization. Animal bodies, perceptual systems and the behavioural ecology they support are not incidental to the question of animal rhythms, but rather absolutely central.
3. Latent flexibility of animal synchronization
(a) . Over what range of timescales might synchronization be observed?
Even the most flexible synchronizers we know of—humans—are constrained [56–59], with many tempi out of our range. Humans cannot synchronize with auditory stimuli at rates faster than approximately 7–10 Hz (420–600 BPM) [58,60,61] or slower than approximately 0.5 Hz (30 BPM) [58]. This range shrinks for (static) visual stimuli, with which we fail to synchronize at rates faster than approximately 2.5 Hz (150 BPM) [61]. Nonhuman animals similarly demonstrate flexibility around their preferred rates (figure 1 and table 1). Macaque monkeys synchronize hand movements or saccades successfully within the range of approximately 1–2.2 Hz (60–132 BPM) [18,62,63], and budgerigars synchronize key pecks within the tested range of approximately 0.6–2.2 Hz (36–132 BPM) [51]. A bonobo drumming together with an experimenter synchronized in bouts within a narrow range of relatively fast rates between 4.2 and 4.8 Hz (252 and 288 BPM) [50]. Snowball, a sulfur-crested cockatoo, demonstrated bouts of synchrony at rates spanning approximately 1.7 and 2.2 Hz, but not outside this range [49]. Finally, Ronan showed anticipatory synchronization with rates in the window of approximately 1.2–2.4 Hz (72–144 BPM), but not faster [53,64]. Mecopooda elongata, a katydid species that chirps at a rate of approximately 0.5 Hz (30 BPM), synchronizes with external rhythms with periods as fast as 0.6 Hz (36 BPM), but not faster [19]. Treefrogs (Eleutherodactylus coqui), who call at rates around 0.4 Hz (24 BPM), demonstrate 1 : 1 phase locking for rates as fast as 0.6 Hz (36 BPM) [65], but switch to 1 : 2 phase locking to every second beat at faster intervals. Thus, all species in which synchronized behaviour has been observed demonstrate some degree of tempo-flexibility. However, the degree of flexibility varies across species and is likely an important clue to how easily a species demonstrates sensorimotor synchronization in the laboratory.
(b) . With which behavioural outputs might a species synchronize?
Humans can synchronize an array of behaviours to external (auditory) stimuli. Often, participants finger tap in time with a stimulus [58,59]. However, humans can perform the same task by drumming with a stick [66], tapping their toes [67], walking [68,69], dancing [70] or speaking [71,72]. Thus, humans are motorically flexible, maybe uniquely so [73–75]; but not all behavioural outputs are equally easy to synchronize to a stimulus. One hurdle to testing nonhuman animal synchronization in the laboratory is knowing a priori what behavioural output with which to ‘ask’ an animal to synchronize. For example, macaques can take years (up to 25 months; [18]) to learn to synchronize hand movements with a stimulus rhythm; then, once they have reached ‘criterion’ performance, their movements continue to lag behind the stimulus. Thus, an animal might fail at an in-laboratory synchronization task because we reinforce a suboptimal behavioural output, when another behavioural output, saccades in the case of macaques [76], might better showcase the flexibility they are capable of. Moreover, we cannot straightforwardly apply this lesson directly to other related species, as chimpanzees and bonobos for example spontaneously synchronize hand movements with rhythmic stimuli without any training [50,52,77]. We argue that we will better identify candidates for suitable behavioural outputs if we focus on the spontaneous behaviours of nonhuman animals in their own natural environments, for example, the timing of steps in horses or the swinging of arms in primates or trunks in elephants.
(c) . Can synchronization be accomplished across multiple modalities?
Humans can synchronize motor outputs with auditory [59], visual [78] and vibrotactile stimulus rhythms [79]. Synchronization is best when stimuli are auditory. Static visual rhythms elicit the poorest synchronization performance, which can critically be improved by adding spatial information to visual rhythms [80–85]. This is important, because an animal may appear to be incapable of synchronizing with an inappropriate stimulus modality, or when experimenters have failed to capture a critical dimension in stimulus design. Consider flocking or swarming animals (see §4), who might synchronize with complex optic flow information for which we might not even fully know how to design appropriate stimuli so that we could test their capabilities [86].
Human synchronization abilities are proposed to be unique in that the behavioural response (clapping, singing, etc.) occurs in a different modality than the stimulus [16]. That is, humans tap their fingers or feet to a sound, while fireflies synchronize their flashes with other flashes. Synchronization of ‘like with like’ is proposed to be a more rigid form of synchronization than that of humans, Snowball and Ronan. However, this criticism sells short the complexity of sensorimotor synchronization in like-with-like synchronizers while simultaneously overselling the complexity of cross-modal synchrony. In katydids, the acoustic stimulus is processed by the auditory nervous system, but the acoustic response is produced in the motor system by a neural oscillator that drives the striking together of specialized structures on the katydids' wings [25,87,88]. Thus, any kind of synchronized animal behaviour is sensorimotor synchronization, even if the response is in the same modality as the stimulus.
4. The importance of motivational state for testing synchronization abilities
Whether in the wild or laboratory, animals will not act without motivation. It is self-evident but often overlooked when reporting negative results that an animal may fail to perform due to lack of capability or willingness. An animal's motivational state is modulated by intrinsic and extrinsic factors and the interaction between them. Intrinsic factors relate to the satisfaction of physiological needs, and extrinsic factors to stimuli present in an animal's environment [89]. The relative importance of these factors will differ based on context, for example, whether an animal is captive or wild. We provide specific examples from three types of overlapping and by no means comprehensive motivational contexts: motivation in mating, locomotive and social-coordination contexts. We illustrate each context with examples from the animal kingdom and demonstrate the importance of considering motivational states in designing species- and context-specific behavioural studies. Importantly, our use of the term motivation is intended to capture the drive to act in a particular way without assuming an animal's recognition of the reason to do so.
(a) . Motivation in mating contexts
Synchrony is often observed in collective mating-display behaviours that have evolved due to selective pressures on mating and reproduction [10,90]. In fiddler crabs, males form small groups and wave their enlarged major claws up and down in near-perfect synchrony [91]. In several species of katydids and grasshoppers, males signal in synchrony with their neighbours [10]. However, synchronous displays arise out of different motivations, although on the surface they might appear very similar. For example, behavioural displays characterized by adaptive synchrony, which serves a cooperative function, attract a higher number of females [9,26,92,93] while simultaneously helping individual species members avoid detection by predators [11]. In other species, however, including katydids, incidental synchrony arises as a consequence of males competing with one another to emit the leading signal [87,88], which is preferred by females [91,94]. Motivations in mating contexts are unlikely to be equally strong at all times and may only be present in a mating season, limiting the available time window for observing mating-motivated rhythmic behaviours as well as our ability to test animals outside of these conditions.
(b) . Motivation in locomotion contexts
Most animals move around within their natural environments to use resources and avoid predators. In doing so, bipedal and quadrupedal animals move with symmetrical gaits, a natural source of rhythmic behaviour (see §2; table 1). However, rhythmic locomotive behaviour is not limited to gaits. For example, head-bobbing is a stabilizing reflex and helps facilitate walking in some birds, such as quails, by synchronizing head movements with the pitch of the trunk [95]. Animals in locomotive contexts may also move in groups, and remarkable temporal coordination has been observed in bird flocks, fish schools and dolphin pods. Coordinated group movement appears to provide an energetic advantage relative to solo locomotion [96,97]. Flying in formation may provide birds with an aerodynamic advantage relative to solo flight, but has also been proposed to serve a social function [96]. Swimming in schools serves an anti-predatory purpose [98], and improves efficient foraging activity [97]. In rough-toothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis), synchronized swimming is thought to be an energetic travelling adaptation that also facilitates eavesdropping [99]. In long-finned pilot whales, coordinated swimming provides an anti-predatory benefit [100] and shows affiliation, demonstrating that a particular behaviour may simultaneously belong to different motivational categories.
Designing experiments to test synchronization of locomotive behaviours must consider the necessary space an animal needs to execute these behaviours, especially in laboratory conditions. For example, dolphins in captivity do not have large pools to swim in, so their natural behavioural repertoire in captivity will be limited relative to natural conditions. Moreover, many individual behaviours only become apparent when the individual becomes part of a group. Thus, studying locomotive behaviours of flocking and schooling species on the individual level may miss complex capacities for synchronization.
(c) . Motivation in social-coordination contexts
There are many benefits to rhythmic social interaction. Interpersonal synchrony between human adults increases affiliation [101], and children that make music together engage in more prosocial behaviour [102]. In the wild, bonobos and some birds engage in asynchronous calling to avoid overlapping their calls with conspecifics with which they have close social ties [103]. The degree of signal coordination in bird duets represents coalition quality [104], and behavioural coordination in zebra finches is enhanced by familiarity [105]. Many species seem capable of adapting their call timing relative to that of a conspecific and are motivated to do so to increase efficient communication or to strengthen social bonds. A socially motivated rhythmic behaviour may not be elicited in a nonsocial context. Thus, in addition to charting the range of rates produced by animals in their natural environment, we may also benefit from assessing the context in which the behaviour occurs (table 1 and figure 1).
5. Conclusion and future directions
We have introduced an ecologically honest approach to studying nonhuman animal synchronization abilities and attempted to illustrate how this approach can be applied across the animal kingdom by surveying the temporal, sensory, motor and motivational constraints that influence naturally produced rhythmic behaviour. Although by no means exhaustive, we provide representative examples from species belonging to a number of different animal clades. Some of these species are known to display synchronized behaviour, and some, to our knowledge, have not been considered in the context of studying synchronization (table 1). Our goal was to demonstrate that animal rhythmic behaviour is context-dependent. We reviewed data from fieldwork, laboratory studies and hybrid designs. For many species, the only existing data points come from the laboratory. We suggest that the relative dearth of data on natural rhythmic behaviour for many species simultaneously handicaps our assessments of synchronization abilities and demands the ecological approach we propose here. We show that ecologically valid experiments studying rhythmic behaviours will benefit from species-specific designs [106] that consider the temporal niche, frequency of occurrence and function of natural behaviours. The ecological approach is a broadly encompassing method to study synchronization, which can and should be applied to humans and nonhumans. It bears emphasis that our admittedly cursory consideration of the comparative data on rate ranges, flexibility in rhythm generation and motivational context-dependence yields a striking degree of multidimensional variability between species, well beyond what has been carefully assessed in comparative laboratory experiments.
Our approach necessitates a close interplay between experimental and fieldwork and is thus by its very nature interdisciplinary. Generating a map of the behavioural possibility space for any species necessitates fieldwork to first identify why, when and how members of that species produce rhythmic and/or synchronized behaviours in their natural environments. In turn, we gain the capacity to design well-informed species-specific experiments to test synchronization abilities. Work on katydids (Mecopoda 'Chirper', Neoconocephalus spiza, Ephippiger ephippiger), grasshoppers (Ligurotettix planum), fireflies (Pteroptyx malaccae, Pteroptyx valida) and frogs (Hyla cinerea, Pseudacris streckceri, Hyperolius marmoratus) can be considered a gold standard. Fieldwork might be buttressed by sophisticated computational approaches, which are becoming more accessible and more user friendly. In particular, algorithmic approaches [107] might help us to identify the timescales, contexts and effectors that characterize the production of rhythmic behaviours, even when those rhythms exist outside of humans' temporal niche. Moreover, a broader experimental approach will integrate converging evidence from traditional sensorimotor synchronization experiments with implicit paradigms where synchronization is spontaneous and so does not need to be trained [77] and neuroscientific paradigms where the neural machinery that supports synchronization abilities or their precursors might be better understood [108].
To conclude, constraining our search for experimental parameters based on wild behaviour and ecologically relevant factors provides a meaningful approach to gaining a more complete understanding of which animal species are capable of synchrony.
Acknowledgements
M.J.H. is supported by a Max Planck Research Group from the Max Planck Society. V.N. is supported by a BBSRC David Phillips fellowship (BB/S009760/1). We thank Andrea Ravignani for his insightful comments on the paper.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
All authors participated in conceptualization and writing, and all authors approved of the final version of the manuscript.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
We declare we received no funding for this study.
References
- 1.Cirelli LK. 2018. How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early prosocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 35-39. ( 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mogan R, Fischer R, Bulbulia JA. 2017. To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 13-20. ( 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Buck J, Buck E. 1968. Mechanism of rhythmic synchronous flashing of fireflies. Science 159, 1319-1327. ( 10.1126/science.159.3821.1319) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Buck J, Buck E. 1966. Biology of synchronous flashing of fireflies. Nature 211, 562-564. ( 10.1038/211562a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Nityananda V, Balakrishnan R. 2008. Leaders and followers in katydid choruses in the field: call intensity, spacing and consistency. Anim. Behav. 76, 723-735. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Greenfield MD, Snedden WA. 2003. Selective attention and the spatio-temporal structure of orthopteran choruses. Behaviour 140, 1-26. ( 10.1163/156853903763999863) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jones DL, Jones RL, Ratnam R. 2014. Calling dynamics and call synchronization in a local group of unison bout callers. J. Comp. Physiol. A 200, 93-107. ( 10.1007/s00359-013-0867-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Grafe TU. 1996. The function of call alternation in the African reed frog (Hyperolius marmoratus): precise call timing prevents auditory masking. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38, 149-158. ( 10.1007/s002650050227) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nityananda V, Balakrishnan R. 2009. Modeling the role of competition and cooperation in the evolution of katydid acoustic synchrony. Behav. Ecol. 20, 484-489. ( 10.1093/beheco/arp022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Greenfield MD. 1994. Synchronous and alternating choruses in insects and anurans: common mechanisms and diverse functions. Am. Zool. 34, 605-615. ( 10.1093/icb/34.6.605) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Legett HD, Page RA, Bernal XE.. 2019. Synchronized mating signals in a communication network: the challenge of avoiding predators while attracting mates. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191067. ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1067) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Woodland DJ, Cabanban AS, Taylor VM, Taylor RJ. 2002. A synchronized rhythmic flashing light display by schooling Leiognathus splendens (Leiognathidae: Perciformes). Mar. Freshw. Res. 53, 159-162. ( 10.1071/MF01157) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Morin JG. 1986. Firefleas of the sea: luminescent signaling in marine ostracode crustaceans. Fla. Entomol. 69, 105. ( 10.2307/3494749) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Backwell P, Jennions M, Passmore N, Christy J. 1998. Synchronized courtship in fiddler crabs. Nature 391, 31-32. ( 10.1038/34076) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pikovsky AS, Rosenblum M, Kurths J. 2001. Synchronization—a unified approach to nonlinear science. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Patel AD. 2014. The evolutionary biology of musical rhythm: was Darwin wrong? PLoS Biol. 12, e1001821. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001821) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aschersleben G. 2002. Temporal control of movements in sensorimotor synchronization. Brain Cogn. 48, 66-79. ( 10.1006/brcg.2001.1304) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zarco W, Merchant H, Prado L, Mendez JC. 2009. Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 3191-3202. ( 10.1152/jn.00066.2009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hartbauer M, Kratzer S, Steiner K, Römer H. 2005. Mechanisms for synchrony and alternation in song interactions of the bushcricket Mecopoda elongata (Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera). J. Comp. Physiol. A 191, 175-188. ( 10.1007/s00359-004-0586-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Nityananda V, Balakrishnan R. 2007. Synchrony during acoustic interactions in the bushcricket Mecopoda ‘Chirper’ (Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera) is generated by a combination of chirp-by-chirp resetting and change in intrinsic chirp rate. J. Comp. Physiol. A 193, 51-65. ( 10.1007/s00359-006-0170-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ravignani A. 2019. Timing of antisynchronous calling: a case study in a harbor seal pup (Phoca vitulina). J. Comp. Psychol. 133, 272-277. ( 10.1037/com0000160) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nakahara F, Miyazaki N. 2011. Vocal exchanges of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J. Ethol. 29, 309-320. ( 10.1007/s10164-010-0259-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Greenfield MD, Rand AS. 2000. Frogs have rules: selective attention algorithms regulate chorusing in Physalaemus pustulosus (Leptodactylidae). Ethology 106, 331-347. ( 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00525.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Merchant H, Honing H. 2014. Are non-human primates capable of rhythmic entrainment? Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis. Front. Neurosci. 7, 274. ( 10.3389/fnins.2013.00274) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sismondo E. 1990. Synchronous, alternating, and phase-locked stridulation by a tropical katydid. Science 249, 55-58. ( 10.1126/science.249.4964.55) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Murphy MA, Thompson NL, Schul J. 2016. Keeping up with the neighbor: a novel mechanism of call synchrony in Neoconocephalus ensiger katydids. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202, 225-234. ( 10.1007/s00359-016-1068-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nityananda V, Balakrishnan R. 2020. Synchrony of non-isochronous signals in an acoustically communicating katydid. bioRxiv, 419101. ( 10.1101/2020.12.10.419101) [DOI]
- 28.Buck J, Buck E, Case JF, Hanson FE. 1981. Control of flashing in fireflies. J. Comp. Physiol. 144, 287-298. ( 10.1007/BF00612560) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kotz SA, Schwartze M. 2010. Cortical speech processing unplugged: a timely subcortico-cortical framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 392-399. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gatesy SM, Biewener AA. 1991. Bipedal locomotion: effects of speed, size and limb posture in birds and humans. J. Zool. 224, 127-147. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1991.tb04794.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.MacDougall HG, Moore ST. 2005. Marching to the beat of the same drummer: the spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. J. Appl. Physiol. 99, 1164-1173. ( 10.1152/japplphysiol.00138.2005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Savage M. 2020 Pop music is getting faster (and happier). BBC News, 9 July.
- 33.Alexander RMcN, Langman VA, Jayes AS. 2009. Fast locomotion of some African ungulates. J. Zool. 183, 291-300. ( 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1977.tb04188.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Toyoda A, Maruhashi T, Malaivijitnond S, Koda H. 2017. Speech-like orofacial oscillations in stump-tailed macaque (Macaca arctoides) facial and vocal signals. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 164, 435-439. ( 10.1002/ajpa.23276) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schusterman R. 1978. Vocal communication in pinnipeds. In Behavior of captive wild animals (eds Markowitz H, Stevens V), pp. 247-308. Chicago, IL: Nelson Hall. [Google Scholar]
- 36.De Waal FM. 1988. The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan Paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour 106, 183-251. ( 10.1163/156853988X00269) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rivers TJ, Morin JG. 2008. Complex sexual courtship displays by luminescent male marine ostracods. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 2252-2262. ( 10.1242/jeb.011130) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Alexander RD, Moore TE. 1958. Studies on the acoustical behavior of seventeen-year cicadas (Homoptera: Cicadidae: Magicicada). Ohio J. Sci. 58, 107-127. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Greenfield MD, Schul J. 2008. Mechanisms and evolution of synchronous chorusing: emergent properties and adaptive functions in Neoconocephalus katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 289-297. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.122.3.289) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Walker TJ. 1969. Acoustic synchrony: two mechanisms in the snowy tree cricket. Science 166, 891-894. ( 10.1126/science.166.3907.891) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ota K, Aihara I, Aoyagi T. 2020. Interaction mechanisms quantified from dynamical features of frog choruses. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 191693. ( 10.1098/rsos.191693) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Connor RC, Smolker R, Bejder L. 2006. Synchrony, social behaviour and alliance affiliation in Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops aduncus. Anim. Behav. 72, 1371-1378. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.014) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Peddemors V. 1990. Respiratory development in a captive-born bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus calf. Afr. Zool. 25, 178-184. ( 10.1080/02541858.1990.11448208) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rankin S, Oswald J, Barlow J, Lammers M. 2007. Patterned burst-pulse vocalizations of the northern right whale dolphin, Lissodelphis borealis. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 1213-1218. ( 10.1121/1.2404919) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Richman B. 1978. The synchronization of voices by gelada monkeys. Primates 19, 569-581. ( 10.1007/BF02373317) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Castellucci GA, Calbick D, McCormick D. 2019. The temporal organization of mouse ultrasonic vocalizations. PLoS ONE 13, e0199929. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0199929) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schnitzler HU, Kalko E, Miller L, Surlykke A. 1987. The echolocation and hunting behavior of the bat, Pipistrellus kuhli. J. Comp. Physiol. A 161, 267-274. ( 10.1007/BF00615246) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Moelants D. 2002. Preferred tempo reconsidered. In Proc. of the 7th Int. Conf. on Music Perception and Cognition, pp. 1-4. Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. 2009. Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr. Biol. 19, 827-830. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Large EW, Gray PM. 2015. Spontaneous tempo and rhythmic entrainment in a bonobo (Pan paniscus). J. Comp. Psychol. 129, 317-328. ( 10.1037/com0000011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hasegawa A, Okanoya K, Hasegawa T, Seki Y. 2011. Rhythmic synchronization tapping to an audio-visual metronome in budgerigars. Sci. Rep. 1, 1-8. ( 10.1038/srep00120) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T. 2015. Distractor effect of auditory rhythms on self-paced tapping in chimpanzees and humans. PLoS ONE 10, e0130682. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0130682) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cook PF, Rouse AA, Wilson M, Reichmuth C. 2013. A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non vocal mimic. J. Comp. Psychol. 127, 412-427. ( 10.1037/a0032345) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.McAuley JD, Jones MR, Holub S, Johnston HM, Miller NS. 2006. The time of our lives: life span development of timing and event tracking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 348-367. ( 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Moelants D, Mckinney MF.. 2004. Tempo perception and musical content: what makes a piece fast, slow or temporally ambiguous. In Proc. of the 8th Int. Conf. on Music Perception and Cognition, pp. 558-562. Sydney, Australia: Causal Productions. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Woodrow H. 1951. Time perception. In Handbook of experimental psychology (ed. Stevens S), pp. 1224-1236. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fraisse P. 1963. The psychology of time. New York, NY: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Repp BH. 2005. Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 969-992. ( 10.3758/BF03206433) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Repp BH, Su YH. 2013. Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 403-452. ( 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Keele SW, Pokorny RA, Corcos DM, Ivry R. 1985. Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correlational analysis. Acta Psychol. 60, 173-191. ( 10.1016/0001-6918(85)90054-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Repp BH. 2003. Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: the synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. J. Mot. Behav. 35, 355-370. ( 10.1080/00222890309603156) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Takeya R, Patel AD, Tanaka M. 2018. Temporal generalization of synchronized saccades beyond the trained range in monkeys. Front. Psychol. 9, 2172. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02172) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Konoike N, Mikami A, Miyachi S. 2012. The influence of tempo upon the rhythmic motor control in macaque monkeys. Neurosci. Res. 74, 64-67. ( 10.1016/j.neures.2012.06.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rouse AA, Cook PF, Large EW, Reichmuth C. 2016. Beat keeping in a sea lion as coupled oscillation: implications for comparative understanding of human rhythm. Front. Neurosci. 10, 257. ( 10.3389/fnins.2016.00257) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zelick R, Narins PM. 1985. Characterization of the advertisement call oscillator in the frog Eleutherodactylus coqui. J. Comp. Physiol. A 156, 223-229. ( 10.1007/bf00610865) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Manning FC, Schutz M. 2016. Trained to keep a beat: movement-related enhancements to timing perception in percussionists and non-percussionists. Psychol. Res. 80, 532-542. ( 10.1007/s00426-015-0678-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rose D, Delevoye-Turrell Y, Ott L, Annett LE, Lovatt PJ. 2019. Music and metronomes differentially impact motor timing in people with and without Parkinson's Disease: effects of slow, medium, and fast tempi on entrainment and synchronization performances in finger tapping, toe tapping, and stepping on the spot tasks. Park. Disease 2019, 1-18. ( 10.1155/2019/6530838) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hove MJ, Suzuki K, Uchitomi H, Orimo S, Miyake Y. 2012. Interactive rhythmic auditory stimulation reinstates natural 1/f timing in gait of Parkinson's patients. PLoS ONE 7, e32600. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0032600) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Ready EA, McGarry LM, Rinchon C, Holmes JD, Grahn JA. 2019. Beat perception ability and instructions to synchronize influence gait when walking to music-based auditory cues. Gait Posture 68, 555-561. ( 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.12.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Burger B, London J, Thompson MR, Toiviainen P. 2018. Synchronization to metrical levels in music depends on low-frequency spectral components and tempo. Psychol. Res. 82, 1195-1211. ( 10.1007/s00426-017-0894-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Assaneo M, Ripollés P, Orpella J, Lin W, de Diego-Balaguer R, Poeppel D.. 2019. Spontaneous synchronization to speech reveals neural mechanisms facilitating language learning. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 627-632. ( 10.1038/s41593-019-0353-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Cummins F, Port R. 1998. Rhythmic constraints on stress timing in English. J. Phon. 26, 145-171. ( 10.1006/jpho.1998.0070) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Heffner R, Masterton B. 1975. Variation in form of the pyramidal tract and its relationship to digital dexterity. Brain. Behav. Evol. 12, 188-200. ( 10.1159/000124403) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Heffner RS, Masterton RB. 1983. The role of the corticospinal tract in the evolution of human digital dexterity. Brain. Behav. Evol. 23, 165-183. ( 10.1159/000121494) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM, Whishaw IQ. 1999. Is digital dexterity really related to corticospinal projections?: a re-analysis of the Heffner and Masterton data set using modern comparative statistics. Behav. Brain Res. 101, 173-187. ( 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00151-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Takeya R, Kameda M, Patel A, Tanaka M. 2017. Predictive and tempo-flexible synchronization to a visual metronome in monkeys. Sci. Rep. 7, 1-12. ( 10.1038/s41598-017-06417-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T. 2013. Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci. Rep. 3, 1566. ( 10.1038/srep01566) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Comstock DC, Hove MJ, Balasubramaniam R. 2018. Sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual modalities: behavioral and neural differences. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12, 53. ( 10.3389/fncom.2018.00053) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ammirante P, Patel AD, Russo FA. 2016. Synchronizing to auditory and tactile metronomes: a test of the auditory-motor enhancement hypothesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1882-1890. ( 10.3758/s13423-016-1067-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Gan L, Huang Y, Zhou L, Qian C, Wu X. 2015. Synchronization to a bouncing ball with a realistic motion trajectory. Sci. Rep. 5, 1-9. ( 10.1038/srep11974) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Iversen JR, Patel AD, Nicodemus B, Emmorey K. 2015. Synchronization to auditory and visual rhythms in hearing and deaf individuals. Cognition 134, 232-244. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Zhou H, Davidson M, Kok P, McCurdy LY, de Lange FP, Lau H, Sandberg K.. 2020. Spatiotemporal dynamics of brightness coding in human visual cortex revealed by the temporal context effect. Neuroimage 205, 116277. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116277) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Hove MJ, Fairhurst MT, Kotz SA, Keller PE. 2013. Synchronizing with auditory and visual rhythms: an fMRI assessment of modality differences and modality appropriateness. Neuroimage 67, 313-321. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.032) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Su Y-H. 2016. Visual enhancement of illusory phenomenal accents in non-isochronous auditory rhythms. PLoS ONE 11, e0166880. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0166880) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Su Y-H. 2016. Sensorimotor synchronization with different metrical levels of point-light dance movements. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 1-15. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00186) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Couzin ID. 2018. Collective animal migration. Curr. Biol. 28, R976-R980. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.044) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Greenfield MD, Roizen I. 1993. Katydid synchronous chorusing is an evolutionary stable outcome of female choice. Nature 364, 618-620. ( 10.1038/364618a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Greenfield MD, Tourtellot MK, Snedden WA.. 1997. Precedence effects and the evolution of chorusing. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1355-1361. ( 10.1098/rspb.1997.0188) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Jensen P, Toates FM. 1993. Who needs ‘behavioural needs’? Motivational aspects of the needs of animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 37, 161-181. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(93)90108-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Buck J. 1988. Synchronous rhythmic flashing of fireflies. II. Q. Rev. Biol. 63, 265-289. ( 10.1086/415929) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Backwell PRY. 2019. Synchronous waving in fiddler crabs: a review. Curr. Zool. 65, 83-88. ( 10.1093/cz/zoy053) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Moiseff A, Copeland J. 2010. Firefly synchrony: a behavioral strategy to minimize visual clutter. Science 329, 181. ( 10.1126/science.1190421) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Buck J, Buck E. 1978. Toward a functional interpretation of synchronous flashing by fireflies. Am. Nat. 112, 471-492. ( 10.1086/283291) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Bailey WJ. 2003. Insect duets: underlying mechanisms and their evolution. Physiol. Entomol. 28, 157-174. ( 10.1046/j.1365-3032.2003.00337.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Abourachid A, Hackert R, Herbin M, Libourel PA, Lambert F, Gioanni H, Provini P, Blazevic P, Hugel V. 2011. Bird terrestrial locomotion as revealed by 3D kinematics. Zoology 114, 360-368. ( 10.1016/j.zool.2011.07.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Bajec IL, Heppner FH. 2009. Organized flight in birds. Anim. Behav. 78, 777-789. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Grünbaum D. 1998. Schooling as a strategy for taxis in a noisy environment. Evol. Ecol. 12, 503-522. ( 10.1023/A:1006574607845) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Handegard NO, Boswell KM, Ioannou CC, Leblanc SP, Tjostheim DB, Couzin ID. 2012. The dynamics of coordinated group hunting and collective information transfer among schooling prey. Curr. Biol. 22, 1213-1217. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.050) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Götz T, Verfuß UK, Schnitzler H-U. 2006. ‘Eavesdropping’ in wild rough-toothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis)? Biol. Lett. 2, 5-7. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0407) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Senigaglia V, de Stephanis R, Verborgh P, Lusseau D. 2012. The role of synchronized swimming as affiliative and anti-predatory behavior in long-finned pilot whales. Behav. Process. 91, 8-14. ( 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Hove MJ, Risen JL. 2009. It's all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949-960. ( 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kirschner Sebastian S, Tomasello M. 2010. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354-364. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Levréro F, Touitou S, Frédet J, Nairaud B, Guéry JP, Lemasson A. 2019. Social bonding drives vocal exchanges in bonobos. Sci. Rep. 9, 1-11. ( 10.1038/s41598-018-36024-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Hall ML, Magrath RD. 2007. Temporal coordination signals coalition quality. Curr. Biol. 17, R406-R407. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Prior NH, Smith E, Dooling RJ, Ball GF. 2020. Familiarity enhances moment-to-moment behavioral coordination in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) dyads. J. Comp. Psychol. 134, 135-148. ( 10.1037/com0000201) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Hoeschele M, Merchant H, Kikuchi Y, Hattori Y, ten Cate C. 2015. Searching for the origins of musicality across species. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20140094. ( 10.1098/rstb.2014.0094) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Chen C, Chen T.2010. Deriving visual rhythm from video signals. Patent. https://patents.google.com/patent/US8654250B2/en .
- 108.Winkler I, Denham SL, Nelken I. 2009. Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regularity representations and perceptual objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 532-540. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.