Abstract
Recent work has highlighted that people who have had TIA may have abnormal motor and cognitive function. We aimed to quantify deficits in a cohort of individuals who had TIA and measured changes in their abilities to perform behavioural tasks over 1 year of follow-up using the Kinarm Exoskeleton robot. We additionally considered performance and change over time in an active control cohort of migraineurs. Individuals who had TIA or migraine completed 8 behavioural tasks that assessed cognition as well as motor and sensory functionality in the arm. Participants in the TIA cohort were assessed at 2, 6, 12, and 52 weeks after symptom resolution. Migraineurs were assessed at 2 and 52 weeks after symptom resolution. We measured overall performance on each task using an aggregate metric called Task Score and quantified any significant change in performance including the potential influence of learning. We recruited 48 individuals to the TIA cohort and 28 individuals to the migraine cohort. Individuals in both groups displayed impairments on robotic tasks within 2 weeks of symptom cessation and also at approximately 1 year after symptom cessation, most commonly in tests of cognitive-motor integration. Up to 51.3% of people in the TIA cohort demonstrated an impairment on a given task within 2-weeks of symptom resolution, and up to 27.3% had an impairment after 1 year. In the migraine group, these numbers were 37.5% and 31.6%, respectively. We identified that up to 18% of participants in the TIA group, and up to 10% in the migraine group, displayed impairments that persisted for up to 1 year after symptom resolution. Finally, we determined that a subset of both cohorts (25–30%) experienced statistically significant deteriorations in performance after 1 year. People who have experienced transient neurological symptoms, such as those that arise from TIA or migraine, may continue to experience lasting neurological impairments. Most individuals had relatively stable task performance over time, with some impairments persisting for up to 1 year. However, some individuals demonstrated substantial changes in performance, which highlights the heterogeneity of these neurological disorders. These findings demonstrate the need to consider factors that contribute to lasting neurological impairment, approaches that could be developed to alleviate the lasting effects of TIA or migraine, and the need to consider individual neurological status, even following transient neurological symptoms.
Subject terms: Cerebrovascular disorders, Migraine, Human behaviour, Diseases of the nervous system
Introduction
Transient ischaemic attack (TIA) is defined as transient neurological symptoms of suspected vascular origin that resolve on their own without leaving clear evidence of brain tissue injury1. The self-recovery aspect, for decades, made researchers and clinicians alike think that TIA was associated with no lasting brain dysfunction for the individual compared to stroke, which is pathophysiologically related. We now appreciate that TIA, as with stroke, can lead to cognitive and motor deficits in some individuals2–6. However, what is not clear is whether motor and cognitive impairment in this clinical population change over time.
Traditional clinical measures have been used to quantify changes primarily in cognition after TIA or minor stroke, but less attention is paid to motor impairments or recovery. It is well-known that people who have had TIA or minor strokes experience cognitive impairments, mood decline, and reduced quality of life after symptom resolution4, 5, 7–9. Some individuals will improve between 7 days and 1-month post-event10; however, by 3- to 6-months, some individuals may still have impairments in cognitive domains such as information processing11. Impairments in verbal memory could persist for as much as 3 years post-event in some people12. However, the same detailed characterization does not exist for possible changes to motor-related ability after TIA. Potentially, an approach that considers cognitive and motor aspects of behaviour could shed light on how performance in these behavioural domains changes over time in people who have had TIA.
We have previously demonstrated that people who have had TIA can have deficits in cognitive and motor tasks in an objective robotic assessment paradigm for up to 2 weeks after symptom resolution2. However, it is presently unclear whether or not these individuals will have persistence of both cognitive and motor deficits for months after their events. At this point, persistence of cognitive deficits has been suggested to occur in a subset of individuals on traditional cognitive assessments3, 10, 13. Our objective in this study was to quantify changes in both motor and cognitive functionality over time in a cohort of individuals who had TIA and were seen initially within 2 weeks of symptom resolution. We previously reported on part of this cohort2, and in this work we report on an expansion of it that utilizes the Kinarm robotic assessment platform and its associated Kinarm Standard Tests™ (KSTs) to perform behavioural assessments. Furthermore, we investigated an active control group of individuals who experienced transient neurological symptoms but were diagnosed with migraine. These individuals were included because migraine is frequently confused with TIA14, even though they are pathophysiologically distinct. Thus, migraineurs make a good comparison group to assess whether any changes over time observed in the TIA group were specific to that group.
Results
Participants and demographics
We recruited 48 people who had TIA and 28 people who had migraine. The median [IQR] age of the participants at the first assessment was 69.4 [15.3] years in the TIA cohort and 60.0 [14.5] years in the migraine cohort. See Table 1 for a brief demographic summary of the participants in the study. We quantified the NIHSS in people who had TIA and people who had migraine. In the TIA cohort, two individuals had NIHSS scores > 0. One individual had a score of 2, because of left leg drift and left facial droop, however their diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (DWI) scan was negative for a corresponding lesion. Another individual had an NIHSS of 4 because of diabetic lumbosacral radiculoplexus neuropathy, which pre-existed their TIA. In both instances, the cause of the increased NIHSS score was not related to the TIA in question (no MRI evidence of corresponding lesions) and therefore we allowed these participants to be included in the final analysis. In the migraine cohort, one individual had an NIHSS = 1. In this case, the individual had right side face and arm numbness and dizziness; their CT and MRI scans were both normal.
Table 1.
TIA | Migraine | |
---|---|---|
N | 48 | 28 |
Age median [IQR]a | 69.4 [15.3] | 60.0 [14.5] |
% Right-handeda | 93 | 94 |
% Femalea | 46 | 54 |
NIHSS (n > 0, [scores]) | 2, [2, 4]† | 1, [1]b |
aZ-scores used in classification analyses are adjusted for age, sex, and handedness, and so statistical differences in age or proportion of sex or handedness were not tested for.
bNIHSS scores > 0 were permitted in these instances because the scores were not related to the event in question.
Group-level statistical tests of Z-Task Scores in TIA and migraine
We tested the hypotheses that TIA- and migraine cohorts’ Task Scores were significantly different from 0, and the hypothesis that the difference between first- and last assessments was significantly different from 0, using 1-sample Student’s t-tests. See Table 2 for a summary of p-values and test statistics for these tests. Furthermore, Fig. 1 displays the cumulative sums of Z-Task Scores for three tasks at first and last assessments for each group; these provide a depiction of the location and scale of these distributions to provide further context to our statistical tests.
Table 2.
TIA | Migraine | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 weeks | 1 year | Difference | 2 weeks | 1 year | Difference | |||||||
p | t | p | t | p | t | p | t | p | t | p | t | |
VGR-D/UA | 2.7e−5† | 4.65 | 0.18 | 1.37 | 0.01 | 2.70 | 0.01 | 2.74 | 0.91 | 0.11 | 0.07 | 1.94 |
VGR-ND/A | 2.9e−5† | 4.63 | 0.05 | 2.08 | 0.08 | 1.81 | 0.07 | 1.88 | 0.76 | 0.31 | 0.63 | 0.48 |
BOB | 5.4e−6† | 5.31 | 0.02 | 2.49 | 0.03 | 2.30 | 0.05 | 2.05 | 0.23 | 1.25 | 0.24 | 1.22 |
OH | 0.43 | 0.79 | 0.35 | 0.95 | 0.12 | − 1.61 | 0.93 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 1.86 | 0.38 | − 0.89 |
OHA | 0.08 | 1.81 | 0.03 | 2.36 | 0.15 | − 1.50 | 0.55 | 0.60 | 0.44 | 0.79 | 0.59 | − 0.54 |
RVGR-D/UA | 9.7e−7† | 5.83 | 0.01 | 2.83 | 4.3e-4† | 4.17 | 2.0e-4† | 4.30 | 0.02 | 2.59 | 0.01 | 2.83 |
RVGR-ND/A | 4.8e−7† | 6.09 | 0.01 | 2.84 | 7.9e-3 | 2.94 | 4.3e-3† | 3.17 | 0.07 | 1.91 | 0.15 | 1.49 |
TM | 0.01 | 2.65 | 0.35 | 0.95 | 0.88 | 0.15 | 0.80 | − 0.25 | 0.91 | 0.12 | 0.63 | − 0.48 |
SPS | 3.9e−3† | 3.08 | 0.54 | 0.63 | 0.39 | 0.89 | 0.42 | 0.83 | 0.87 | 0.13 | 0.77 | 0.30 |
APM-ND/A | 0.04 | 2.08 | 0.98 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 2.41 | 0.02 | − 2.51 | 0.17 | − 1.48 | 0.84 | 0.21 |
ND non-dominant arm (migraine only), D dominant arm (migraine only), UA unaffected arm (TIA only), A affected arm (TIA only), t t-statistic.
Small values are represented using scientific notation for compact formatting.
†p < (0.05/60).
At the group level, tasks performed by the TIA cohort captured frequent differences from 0 (standard normal mean) at 2-weeks, although such differences were uncommon at 1-year, and the change over time often did not differ significantly from 0. See Fig. 1 and Table 2 or graphical and numerical summaries of these results. Consider the example of RVGR-A: a significant difference from 0 was observed at 2-weeks (p = 4.8e−7; significant after multiple comparison correction), but there was no significant difference from 0 by 1-year, and the group-level difference between 2-weeks and 1-year was not significantly different from 0. Similar observations were made in other tasks, with the only exception being RVGR-UA. In this task, a significant difference from 0 was observed at 2-weeks but not at 1-year; the difference between assessments was significantly different from 0.
In the migraine group, fewer significant differences and changes were observed than in the TIA group. Only RVGR-D had a group mean that was significantly different from 0 at 2-weeks, and no tasks had significant differences from 0 at 1-year. Furthermore, none of the differences between assessments was significant.
Impairment rates in TIA and migraine
We collected KST data at 2 weeks and 1 year after symptom resolution in both TIA and migraine groups, as well as at 6-weeks and 3-months in the TIA group. We identified that tasks that were used for testing the integrated cognitive-motor ability identified the greatest number of impairments in both cohorts and that, at a group level, the overall impairment rate appeared to decline in most tasks. See Table 3 for a summary of these results. Individuals in the TIA cohort demonstrated frequent impairments on multiple behavioural tasks spanning motor and cognitive domains. RVGR-UA identified 51.3% of people in the TIA cohort as impaired within 2-weeks, which reduced to 27.3% at 1 year.
Table 3.
Impaired/total (percent) | TIA | Migraine | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 weeks | 6 weeks | 3 months | 1 year | 2 weeks | 1 year | |
VGR-D/UA | 15/48 (31.2) | 8/44 (18.2) | 7/39 (17.9) | 4/28 (14.3) | 3/29 (10.3) | 1/21 (4.8) |
VGR-ND/A | 14/48 (29.2) | 9/43 (20.9) | 9/39 (23.1) | 6/28 (21.4) | 2/29 (6.9) | 3/21 (14.3) |
BOB | 8/38 (21.1) | 4/34 (11.8) | 2/30 (6.7) | 1/20 (5.0) | 3/23 (13.0) | 3/16 (18.8) |
OH | 1/48 (2.1) | 2/44 (4.5) | 3/39 (7.7) | 2/28 (7.1) | 2/29 (6.9) | 3/21 (14.3) |
OHA | 4/48 (8.3) | 3/44 (6.8) | 4/39 (10.3) | 4/28 (14.3) | 0/29 (0.0) | 2/21 (9.5) |
RVGR-D/UA | 21/39 (51.3) | 13/36 (36.1) | 8/31 (25.8) | 6/22 (27.3) | 9/24 (37.5) | 6/19 (31.6) |
RVGR-ND/A | 16/38 (42.1) | 10/35 (28.6) | 7/31 (22.6) | 5/22 (22.7) | 8/24 (33.3) | 3/19 (15.8) |
TM | 10/48 (20.8) | 3/44 (6.8) | 6/39 (15.4) | 4/28 (14.3) | 0/29 (0.0) | 0/21 (0.0) |
SPS | 7/38 (18.4) | 2/34 (5.9) | 2/29 (6.9) | 0/19 (0.0) | 0/23 (0.0) | 0/18 (0.0) |
APM-ND/A | 6/47 (12.8) | 2/43 (4.7) | 2/36 (5.6) | 1/27 (3.7) | 0/18 (0.0) | 0/12 (0.0) |
ND non-dominant arm, D dominant arm, UA unaffected arm, A affected arm.
Tasks testing cognition and motor behaviour separately also captured frequent impairments. In TM (a cognitive task), over 20% of individuals in the TIA cohort were impaired at the first assessment, which was approximately 15% at the end of 1 year of follow-up. In VGR (a motor task), over 30% of the group was impaired within 2-weeks on this purely sensorimotor task, which reduced to ~ 14% by the end of 1 year of follow-up.
Individuals in the migraine group were most-often impaired on tasks testing cognitive-motor integration. For example, 37.5% of individuals in the migraine cohort had impairments in RVGR-D. This reduced to 31.6% at 1 year. At the first assessment, two other tasks that tested motor skill specifically had impairment rates > 10%: BOB (3/23; 13.0%) and VGR-D (3/29; 10.3%). At the second assessment, the impairment rate on BOB was 3/16 (18.0%) whereas the impairment rate on VGR-D was 1/21 (4.8%).
Individual longitudinal changes
Next, we considered changes in score between first and last assessments at an individual level. See Fig. 2 for scatterplots of first- and last-Task Score plots, with p-values for individual changes labelled when changes between assessments were statistically significant.
Figure 2a–c depicts scatterplots for the TIA group. At an individual level, most individuals did not change significantly between assessments, reflecting the results of the group-level analyses above. However, there were a few individuals who changed significantly, demonstrating the heterogeneity of this cohort, and which are not reflective of the group-level results. Furthermore, changes were bidirectional; deterioration and improvement both occurred. In VGR-A (Fig. 2a) seven individuals were impaired at 2-weeks and of those participants, three remained impaired at 1-year (in the upper-right quadrant of the scatterplot). Therefore, 10.7% of individuals remained impaired in this task at both timepoints. Only two individuals demonstrated statistically significant changes across assessments in VGR-A: one individual worsened and the other improved. In the TM task (Fig. 2c), 3 individuals had impairments at 2-weeks and those individuals stayed impaired at 1-year (10.7%). Furthermore, 1 individual deteriorated and 0 improved in TM. Finally, in RVGR-A, 4/22 participants (18.2%) had impairments at both timepoints and 3 improved significantly (none deteriorated significantly). Note that in all three example tasks mentioned here, group-level difference between assessments was not significantly different from 0, see Table 2.
In the migraine group (Fig. 2d–f), few individuals demonstrated persistence of impairments and, as a whole, the group generally stayed within the limits of significant change. Furthermore, as in the TIA cohort, we observed a small number of individuals who changed significantly between assessments, even if the corresponding group effects were not significantly different from 0. For example, there were no instances of someone in the migraine group being impaired at both first- and last assessment in either VGR-ND or TM. There were three instances of this in RVGR-ND (3/21, i.e., 14.3%), one of which had impairments very close to the threshold (Task Scores of ~ 2.0). Very few individuals in this group showed any significant change; 1 in VGR-ND, 2 in RVGR-ND, and 0 in TM.
Patterns of significant change across tasks in TIA and migraine
We next investigated the number of significant changes over time across tasks at an individual level. These results are summarized in Fig. 3. For the TIA group, a total of 10 assessments were considered for 28 individuals who completed first and last assessments, i.e., 28 × 10 = 280 assessments were performed. Of these, 23 significant changes were observed, a rate of approximately 1/12 assessments. Furthermore, 16/28 (57%) individuals changed significantly on at least 1 behavioural task, and 6/28 (21%) changed significantly on at least 2 behavioural tasks. Of these, 6/28 (21%) significantly deteriorated on at least 1 task, whereas 13/28 (46%) significantly improved on 1 task. These changes were heterogeneous. For example, one individual in the TIA cohort significantly improved on both BOB and RVGR-A. Another significantly improved in VGR-UA and RVGR-UA, but significantly deteriorated on TM.
In the migraine group, 21/210 total assessments demonstrated significant change between first and last test (approximately 1/10 individuals). Furthermore, 12/21 (57%) changed significantly on at least 1 behavioural task, and 7/21 (33%) changed significantly on at least 2 behavioural tasks. Of these, 8/21 (38%) significantly deteriorated on at least 1 behavioural task, and 7/21 (33%) significantly improved on at least 1 task. As in the TIA cohort, these changes were heterogeneous. For example, one individual got better on RVGR-D, but deteriorated on OHA. Another significantly deteriorated on BOB and VGR-ND.
Individual change over multiple assessments in TIA
We determined the frequency of Task Score changes between all assessments for the TIA group, accounting for the large number of statistical tests. A subset of these results is depicted in Fig. 4. We generally found that individuals in the TIA group did not change significantly between individual assessments, and sometimes an individual remained impaired throughout follow up (grey lines in Fig. 4 that were higher than the 1.96 Task Score impairment threshold). For example, 2 individuals in RVGR-UA had consistently impaired scores throughout follow-up (see grey lines towards the top of the RVGR-UA panel in Fig. 4).
After accounting for the false discovery rate (FDR), only a relatively small number of Task Score changes between time points in the TIA group were considered statistically significant. Some significant changes had a very high magnitude, such as one individual in BOB that changed > 4 Task Score units between 6 weeks and 3 months. Other significant changes were smaller, but still considered significant when starting values were closer to 0. This reflects the nonlinearity of the Task Score change as a function of the magnitude of the Task Score itself; the further from 0 a value is, the more it has to change in order to be considered significant. This effect is evident from the curvature of the significant change thresholds depicted in Fig. 2.
Discussion
We performed a longitudinal robotic assessment of people who had TIAs as well as people who had migraines, and found that up to 18% of individuals in the TIA cohort had impairments on behavioural tasks that lasted for 1 year. A persistent impairment rate of up to 10% was observed in the migraine (active control) group. Furthermore, 25–30% of individuals from each group had significantly poorer performance after 1 year on at least 1 task. We observed few group-level significant changes over time (i.e., changes in score that were significantly different from 0), as a consequence of the heterogeneity of individual-level changes. Finally, we observed diverse patterns of change across multiple tasks, which demonstrates the heterogeneous behavioural presentations of TIA and migraine.
Cognitive deficits are known to be associated with TIA and migraine3, 15–19, but persistence of impairments in motor tasks and changes in the ability to perform them are less well-characterized. Here, we identified that primarily the TIA group displayed impairments in motor-domain tasks that lasted for 1 year. For example, VGR, a task that straightforwardly tests the ability to make reaching movements with veridical visual feedback, captured more impairments in the TIA cohort than might be expected based on the clinical definition of a TIA. In their affected arms, 3/28 individuals in the TIA group had impairments on VGR that persisted for 1 year. Furthermore, 3/28 individuals transitioned from being unimpaired at 2 weeks to impaired at 1 year. The migraineurs appeared to have less prevalent impairments across all tasks, including from the motor domain, and they also generally did not demonstrate persistence of impairments over the 1 year of follow-up (RVGR-ND was a notable exception to this rule, see Fig. 2e).
We observed persistence of impairment on cognitive- and cognitive-motor tasks in both the TIA and migraine groups that would not be expected based on the clinical definitions of these conditions and the performance of healthy individuals. It is now acknowledged that both stroke and TIA leads to persistent cognitive decline for several years afterwards10, 20–22, which can have heterogeneous trajectories23 and which are potentially related to post-ischemic immune response24 and existing vascular risk factors25. Previous work identified that people who have TIAs experience increased rates of whole-brain atrophy beyond that expected for healthy aging26, 27. In the present study, 18% of the TIA group had impairments that lasted for more than 1 year on RVGR-A (see Fig. 2b). RVGR previously demonstrated strong learning effects in healthy individuals28, and so it is clear that not only do some people in the TIA cohort display deficits in this task, but some fail to learn or improve in the task even after 4 repeated trials. RVGR requires participants to override or inhibit prepotent motor plans to reach towards a visual target, and instead move in the opposite direction to attain a spatial goal, and retain this reversal rule across trials. Healthy adults have been shown to retain motor skills for periods ranging from 1 week29 to 1 year30–32. Neurological disease, such as stroke, can lead to impaired learning of motor skills such as visuomotor rotations33 as well as specific movement features (e.g., initial movement and final accuracy), in a manner dependent upon lesion location33, 34. Potentially, white matter injury can influence the rate of visuomotor adaptation in healthy individuals35, and can impair executive functioning36, 37. In addition to findings in the TIA group, we observed several cases of impairments and persistence of deficits in the migraineurs, with potential causes including disruptions of thalamic sensory relays and impaired thalamocortical connectivity38, 39 that could disrupt cortical information processing loops.
Deficits in almost all tasks were less prevalent in the migraine group than the TIA group, which may reflect patterns of post-TIA effects on the brain. For example, in the migraine group at the first assessment, 4/10 tasks did not identify anyone as impaired whereas in the TIA group every task highlighted at least a few impairments. After 1 year, 3/10 tasks failed to capture any impairments in the migraineurs, and these tasks had also not identified deficits at 2 weeks. In contrast, individuals from the TIA group had some impairments at almost every task at both time points (except for SPS at the 1-year assessment). These results are even more remarkable when considering that the TIA cohort was assessed 4 times over the 1 year of follow up, whereas the migraineurs were only assessed twice. Thus, people in the TIA cohort demonstrated poorer group-level performance despite having the potential to benefit from additional practice, even in tasks such as RVGR and TM that have learning effects28. The breadth of motor and cognitive-motor impairments in the TIA group could reflect distributed brain pathology. Possible culprits of motor impairments in the TIA group are elevated white matter lesion load40, 41, impaired connectivity between motor regions42, and damage to the corticospinal tract (or, more broadly, internal capsule)43–47, which can affect unimanual and bimanual motor skill. White matter injury can also impair attention48, which could impact performance on cognitive-motor tasks such as RVGR.
Some of the most important general observations from this study were that it was not sufficient to consider only group effects when dealing with conditions as heterogeneous as TIA and migraine, and that longitudinal assessment lent additional strength to our investigation of impairments in these groups. We observed very few significant group-level changes over time, i.e., group-averaged changes of score that were significantly different from 0, in both TIA and migraine groups. Yet, we observed that unexpectedly large proportions of individuals from both groups (more so with the TIA group) had significant changes at an individual level. Fifty-seven percent of individuals in both groups significantly changed on at least one behavioural task, and approximately one-third did so on at least two tasks. It is clear from inspecting the data that the disparity between group-level and individual results arose from the bidirectionality of changes. There was little in the way of an average effect across the groups, because the average effect was obfuscated by the presence of both improvements and deteriorations at an individual level. Furthermore, we observed that considering only group-level impairment rates did not convey the true persistence of impairments within individuals. This reflects the value added by considering longitudinal change within individuals instead of only changes in cross-sectional statistics49, 50. For example, we observed that 21/39 (51.3%) of individuals in the TIA group were impaired in RVGR-UA at the first assessment, and 6/22 (27.3%) were impaired in this task at the final assessment. However, of the 6 individuals who were impaired at 1-year, 4 had been impaired since the beginning of the study. A similar observation was made in the TMT task; the group impairment rate from 2-weeks to 1-year decreased from 20.8 to 14.3% (a 50% relative decrease), but of the 4 individuals that had impairments at the 1-year assessment, 3 had been impaired at the 2-week assessment, demonstrating persistence of impairment at an individual level. It is important to address this dichotomy between the apparent group-level decrease in impairment and the persistence of impairments at an individual level. In the present study, considering only impairment rates at a single time point, this valuable information is lost: these individuals had long-term impairments in cognitive-motor integration for up to 1 year after TIA. Future studies with larger samples, particularly at longitudinal follow-ups, will be important to grasp the extent of cognitive and/or motor impairment persistence after TIA, and any factors that contribute to it.
Limitations
Our study has some limitations to address. First, although we discuss significant change, our previous work did not consider inter-test intervals longer than approximately 2 weeks. Thus, it remains unclear how much change is permissible over longer time scales and if, indeed, those results generalize. We did observe subjective differences between TIA and migraine cohorts after 1 year of follow-up on some tasks such as VGR and TM, although the sample sizes at 1 year were quite small. The sample size reduced over 1 year largely because participants lost interest, or moved away and so were unable to participate any further. Further work with larger sample sizes will be required to directly compare these two clinical populations, particularly using longitudinal data. It remains possible that because the people in the TIA cohort were seen more frequently than the migraineurs, that they could have had extra practice on tasks that predisposed them to improve more than the migraineurs, especially on tasks with strong learning effects like RVGR. However, we did not identify that individuals in the TIA group improved frequently in the other tasks that also displayed strong learning effects (TM and SPS). Indeed, approximately 10–15% of individuals displayed persistence of impairments despite having 4 repeated tests. Furthermore, it is unclear how consistent learning effects are across multiple repeated assessments. Our previous work did not quantify learning effects beyond two assessments. This will require further study, possibly considering RVGR performance changes within-assessment.
Conclusions
We performed a novel longitudinal assessment of people who had TIA, considering changes over a total of 4 assessments. We also assessed an active control group of migraineurs at 2-weeks and 1-year. We identified first that our TIA cohort had prevalent impairments in motor and cognitive tasks, some of which persisted for up to a year after symptom resolution. We identified substantial heterogeneity across individuals in both cohorts, in terms of impairments as well as in terms of change over time. This study lays the groundwork for future studies that will seek to understand the relationship between behavioural change and neuroanatomical change in the brains of individuals who have experienced clinically transient cerebrovascular injuries.
Methods
Participants and clinical assessment
We recruited people who had TIAs or migraines from the Kingston Health Sciences Centre (Kingston, ON, Canada) stroke prevention clinic. Inclusion criteria that were common for both clinical groups were (1) the ability to understand the robotic task instructions, and (2) a lack of any upper-limb or other neurological injury that could impact performance on the robotic tests. Inclusion criteria specific for the TIA group were (1) a diagnosis of TIA, defined according to modern criteria1, and (2) a score of either (2a) zero or (2b) non-zero, but not pertaining to the relevant clinical event, on the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS). The only specific inclusion criterion for the migraine group was a diagnosis of migraine following ICHD-3 criteria.
In addition to their initial assessments, individuals in the TIA group were invited to return at 6 weeks, 3 months, and 1 year after symptom resolution. People in the migraine group were invited back after 1 year only. All participants in this study provided written informed consent prior to taking part. This study was reviewed and approved by the Queen’s University Research Ethics Board, and was performed in accordance with the guidelines laid out by the Declaration of Helsinki.
Robotic assessment
Individuals in both the TIA and migraine groups completed a series of 8 behavioural tasks that tested motor, cognitive, and sensory domains using the Kinarm Exoskeleton Lab (Kinarm, Kingston, ON, Canada; see Fig. 5). This collection of behavioural tasks is referred to as the Kinarm Standard Tests™ (KSTs). See Supplemental Table S1 for detailed descriptions of the robotic tasks. Briefly, the Kinarm Exoskeleton required participants to be seated and to place their arms in troughs attached to the robotic linkage that also supported their weight against gravity. Participants moved their arms in the horizontal plane underneath a screen providing a virtual reality system aligned with this horizontal workspace. Visual feedback of the limbs was provided via the screen, usually with a white cursor dot (radius = 0.5 cm), sometimes with other shapes, or sometimes no feedback was provided.
There were 4 tasks to test primarily motor skills. Visually guided reaching (VGR) tested the participants’ ability to make quick and accurate reaches out and back to a series of either 8 or 4 peripheral targets51. The number of targets reduced during collection because the assessment time was substantially reduced, even though much of the same information is represented in the abbreviated version of the task52. Ball on bar (BOB) tested bimanual coordination by requiring participants to use both hands simultaneously to control a virtual ball balanced on a bar53. Object hit (OH) tested rapid motor planning by having individuals hit objects that were falling towards them on the screen, with the objective being to hit as many as possible before the end of the task54. Object hit and avoid (OHA) was almost the same as OH, except participants had to hit as many target objects away from them as possible while avoiding distractors55.
Three tasks placed additional emphasis on cognitive skills. Reverse visually guided reaching (RVGR) was similar to VGR, except that the hand had to be moved in the opposite direction to bring the cursor to the target, thus testing the ability to inhibit an automatic motor action56. Trail making (TM) was a robotic implementation of the classic neuropsychological assessment, the trail-making test, which assesses information processing speed and set-switching57–59. Spatial span (SPS) was similarly a robotic implementation of the Corsi block-tapping task, primarily testing spatial working memory60.
Finally, we tested sensory skill via proprioception. This was achieved using the arm position matching (APM) task, in which the robot moved one arm and the participant had to mirror-match the position using their other arm61.
Statistical analysis
Performance in each robotic task was quantified using 10–20 spatial and temporal features of performance using a variety of disparate units (e.g., m/s, s, cm). These measures were transformed into standardized Z-scores (i.e., mean of 0, standard deviation of 1), referred to as “task parameters”, so that they could be compared and combined as necessary (See KST Summary, www.kinarm.com). Z-score transforms for each task parameter were generated based on performance of a large cohort of healthy controls and includes the influence of age, sex, handedness, and the type of robotic platform.
In addition to parameter Z-scores, we also calculated an aggregate metric referred to as “Task Score”, which is a single value that describes overall performance on a given task. Details of the process for going from multiple parameters’ Z-scores to a single Task Score are described in previous work2, 28 (see also KST Summary, www.kinarm.com). Briefly, Z-scores for each task parameter were all converted such that best performance was at zero and poorer performance was large positive. The root-sum square (RSS) of all task parameters was calculated for a large cohort healthy controls and this distribution was renormalized to a standard normal distribution (Z-Task Score). We then compressed the cumulative distribution function (CDF) of the Z-Task Scores to the right such that negative Z-Task Scores became close to zero but positive. Thus, the Task Score is an exclusively positive value with values close to 0 representing excellent performance and increasing values reflecting poorer overall performance on a task. The quantiles of the CDF of the Task Score distribution approximate those of the standard Normal distribution i.e., 68.3% of the area under the curve lies to the left of a Task Score of 1 and 95.4% lies to the left of a Task Score of 2.0. We define impairment as performance outside the envelope of 95% of healthy controls, corresponding to a Task Score of 1.96 or greater.
We previously quantified thresholds for significant change (SC) and expected learning effects (LE) for all task parameters and Z-Task Scores28. In order to calculate the boundaries for SC for the one-sided Task Scores, we performed a transform of the Z-Task Score to the Task Score as usual2, but with the addition or subtraction of the SC amount, creating a symmetric and curved SC boundary. LE were accounted for by calculating second assessment minus first assessment values, and identifying significant differences using a paired sample t-test. LE that were previously deemed significant were used in the present study. Our previous study identified that four tasks had significant LE: RVGR-D (− 0.72 expected change in Z-Task Score between first and second assessments), RVGR-ND (− 0.76), SPS (− 0.43), and TM (− 0.50). These were incorporated by subtracting the LE amount from Z-Task Score differences, and then performing the one-sided Task Score transform on the modified values, analogous to the approach with the SC values. Note that this makes the SC boundary asymmetrical. See Supplemental Fig. S1 for a graphical depiction of the consequences of this transformation.
We performed statistical analyses comparing Z-Task Scores in TIA and migraine groups at first and final assessments to the null hypothesis of a mean of 0, i.e., what is expected for the healthy control individuals (see previous sections on calculation of Z-scores for more details). We also tested the hypothesis of a difference in means of 0 between first and last assessments, to test for the significance of any observed changes over time. These comparisons were all performed by way of a 1-sample Student’s t-test on the differences between assessments. In order to correct for the 60 comparisons (10 total scores, 3 statistical tests, 2 timepoints), we set a significant p-value for these tests at ≈ 0.0077 using the false discovery rate (FDR) correction of Benjamini and Hochberg62.
We quantified the significance of changes between first- and last assessments at the individual level. Individual observation p-values were calculated by first fitting a Normal distribution using the mean and standard deviations (SDs) derived from healthy control Z-Task Score difference scores (final score minus initial score) that we reported on previously47, and then deriving the cumulative density function (CDF) of these distributions. The area under the curve was derived for each TIA or migraine Z-Task Score observation. For values with p > 0.5, we reported 1-CDF so that the reported probabilities were small values and not large (this has the same interpretation but is easier to plot). For example, if the control data difference scores for a given task had a mean of 0 and an SD of 1, then an observation at − 1.96 would have a p-value of ~ 0.025, whereas an observation at + 1.96 would have a p-value of ~ 0.975, which after subtracting from 1 would correspond to a p-value of ~ 0.025 as well.
We additionally used this individualized significant change approach to identify the p-values of changes between all assessments in the TIA cohort, in which 4 total assessments were performed. We simulated 1000 individual p-values for each transition between 1st and 2nd assessment, 2nd to 3rd, and 3rd to 4th, respectively, for all participants across all tasks. We used the FDR correction and determined that a p-value of approximately 1.3 × 10–5 was the threshold for significance. All statistical analyses were performed using SciPy v1.4.0 implemented in Python 3.7.1. Analysis code is publicly available on Github (https://github.com/8lers/TIA-kinarm-longitudinal).
Reuse of data
Some of the TIA participant data in the present manuscript are reused from a previous paper2. We confirm that all 4 authors of the previous paper have provided written and informed consent for the data from the previous study to be used in the present paper.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We would like to sincerely thank Simone Appaqaq, Helen Bretzke, Mary-Jo Demers, Kimberly Moore, and Justin Peterson for their immense help with participant assessment, database management, and technical support.
Author contributions
L.E.R.S. analyzed data, drafted the manuscript, and prepared figures. A.Y.J. and S.H.S. supervised the project. A.Y.J. recruited for TIA and migraine cohorts. All authors contributed to editing the manuscript.
Funding
S.H.S. is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health operating Grant MOP-106662, an Ontario Research Fund Grant ORF-RE-09-112, a GSK Chair in Neuroscience, and an NSERC Discovery Grant.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
S.H.S. is the cofounder and Chief Scientific Officer of Kinarm, the company that commercializes the robotic technology used in this study. L.E.R.S. and A.Y.J. declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
The original online version of this Article was revised: The original version of this Article contained errors, where the authors found several values used for calculating the significant change thresholds and learning effects to be incorrect. Full information regarding the correction made can be found in the correction for this Article.
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Change history
3/2/2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1038/s41598-023-30417-1
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-96177-y.
References
- 1.Sacco RL, Kasner SE, Broderick JP, Caplan LR, Connors JJB, Culebras A, et al. AHA/ASA expert consensus document an updated definition of stroke for the 21st Century. Stroke. 2013;44:2064–2089. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e318296aeca. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Simmatis L, Krett J, Scott SH, Jin AY. Robotic exoskeleton assessment of transient ischemic attack. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:1–13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Van Rooij FG, Schaapsmeerders P, Maaijwee NAM, Van Duijnhoven DAHJ, De Leeuw FE, Kessels RPC, et al. Persistent cognitive impairment after transient ischemic attack. Stroke. 2014;45:2270–2274. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.005205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Coutts SB, Modi J, Patel SK, Aram H, Demchuk AM, Goyal M, et al. What causes disability after transient ischemic attack and minor stroke?: Results from the CT and MRI in the triage of TIA and minor cerebrovascular events to identify high risk patients (CATCH) study. Stroke. 2012;43:3018–3022. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.665141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sangha RS, Caprio FZ, Askew R, Corado C, Bernstein R, Curran Y, et al. Quality of life in patients with TIA and minor ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2015;85:1957–1963. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lodha N, Harrell J, Eisenschenk S, Christou EA. Motor impairments in transient ischemic attack increase the odds of a subsequent stroke: A meta-analysis. Front. Neurol. 2017;8:243. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sörös P, Harnadek M, Blake T, Hachinski V, Chan R. Executive dysfunction in patients with transient ischemic attack and minor stroke. J. Neurol. Sci. 2015;354:17–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Verbraak ME, Hoeksma AF, Lindeboom R, Kwa VIH. Subtle problems in activities of daily living after a transient ischemic attack or an apparently fully recovered non-disabling stroke. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2012;21:124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2010.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Muus I, Petzold M, Ringsberg KC. Health-related quality of life among Danish patients 3 and 12 months after TIA or mild stroke. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 2010;24:211–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2009.00705.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pendlebury ST, Wadling S, Silver LE, Mehta Z, Rothwell PM. Transient cognitive impairment in TIA and minor stroke. Stroke. 2011;42:3116–3121. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.621490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sachdev P, Brodaty H, Valenzuela M, Lorentz L, Looi J, Wen W, et al. The neuropsychological profile of vascular cognitive impairment in stroke and TIA patients. Neurology. 2004;62:912–919. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000115108.65264.4B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Crawford JD, Wen W, Brodaty H. Progression of cognitive impairment in stroke/TIA patients over 3 years. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2014;85:1324–1330. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.van Rooij FG, Kessels RPC, Richard E, De Leeuw F-E, van Dijk EJ. Cognitive impairment in transient ischemic attack patients: A systematic review. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2016;42:1–9. doi: 10.1159/000444282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nadarajan V, Perry RJ, Johnson J, Werring DJ. Transient ischaemic attacks: Mimics and chameleons. Pract. Neurol. 2014;14:23–31. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2013-000782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pendlebury ST, Markwick A, de Jager CA, Zamboni G, Wilcock GK, Rothwell PM. Differences in cognitive profile between TIA, stroke and elderly memory research subjects: A comparison of the MMSE and MoCA. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2012;34:48–54. doi: 10.1159/000338905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mulder EJCM, Linssen WHJP, Passchier J, Orlebeke JF, De Geus EJC. Interictal and postictal cognitive changes in migraine. Cephalalgia. 1999;19:557–565. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-2982.1999.019006557.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pearson AJ, Chronicle EP, Maylor EA, Bruce LAM. Cognitive function is not impaired in people with a long history of migraine: A blinded study. Cephalalgia. 2006;26:74–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2005.01001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jelicic M, Van Boxtel MPJ, Houx PJ, Jolles J. Does migraine headache affect cognitive function in the elderly? Report from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS) Headache. 2000;40:715–719. doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4610.2000.00124.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Moran GM, Calvert M, Feltham MG, Ryan R, Marshall T. A retrospective cohort study to investigate fatigue, psychological or cognitive impairment after TIA: Protocol paper. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e008149. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sivakumar L, Kate M, Jeerakathil T, Camicioli R, Buck B, Butcher K. Serial montreal cognitive assessments demonstrate reversible cognitive impairment in patients with acute transient ischemic attack and minor stroke. Stroke. 2014;45:1709–1715. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.004726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Levine DA, Galecki AT, Langa KM, Unverzagt FW, Kabeto MU, Giordani B, et al. Trajectory of cognitive decline after incident stroke. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2015;314:41–51. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.6968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Schaapsmeerders P, Maaijwee NAM, Van Dijk EJ, Rutten-Jacobs LCA, Arntz RM, Schoonderwaldt HC, et al. Long-term cognitive impairment after first-ever ischemic stroke in young adults. Stroke. 2013;44:1621–1628. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.000792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rasquin SMC, Verhey FRJ, Lousberg R, Winkens I, Lodder J. Vascular cognitive disorders: Memory, mental speed and cognitive flexibility after stroke. J. Neurol. Sci. 2002;203–204:115–119. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(02)00264-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Doyle KP, Quach LN, Solé M, Axtell RC, Nguyen TVV, Soler-Llavina GJ, et al. B-lymphocyte-mediated delayed cognitive impairment following stroke. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:2133–2145. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4098-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Douiri A, McKevitt C, Emmett ES, Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA. Long-term effects of secondary prevention on cognitive function in stroke patients. Circulation. 2013;128:1341–1348. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.002236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Munir M, Ursenbach J, Reid M, Sah RG, Wang M, Sitaram A, et al. Longitudinal brain atrophy rates in transient ischemic attack and minor ischemic stroke patients and cognitive profiles. Front. Neurol. 2019;10:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Walters RJL, Fox NC, Schott JM, Crum WR, Stevens JM, Rossor MN, et al. Transient ischaemic attacks are associated with increased rates of global cerebral atrophy. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2003;74:213–216. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.2.213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Simmatis LER, Early S, Moore KD, Appaqaq S, Scott SH. Statistical measures of motor, sensory and cognitive performance across repeated robot-based testing. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2020;2:1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12984-020-00713-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Krakauer JW, Ghez C, Ghilardi MF. Adaptation to visuomotor transformations: Consolidation, interference, and forgetting. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:473–478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4218-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Landi SM, Baguear F, Della-Maggiore V. One week of motor adaptation induces structural changes in primary motor cortex that predict long-term memory one year later. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11808–11813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2253-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Yamamoto K, Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Rapid and long-lasting plasticity of input-output mapping. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:2797–2801. doi: 10.1152/jn.00209.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Taubert M, Draganski B, Anwander A, Müller K, Horstmann A, Villringer A, et al. Dynamic properties of human brain structure: Learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:11670–11677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY. Left parietal regions are critical for adaptive visuomotor control. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:6972–6981. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6432-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke. Brain Res. 2009;1298:78–91. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Della-Maggiore V, Scholz J, Johansen-Berg H, Paus T. The rate of visuomotor adaptation correlates with cerebellar white-matter microstructure. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:4048–4053. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Burton E, Ballard C, Stephens S, Kenny RA, Kalaria R, Barber R, et al. Hyperintensities and fronto-subcortical atrophy on MRI are substrates of mild cognitive deficits after stroke. Dement Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2003;16:113–118. doi: 10.1159/000070684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Williamson J, Nyenhuis D, Stebbins GT, Lamb D, Simkus V, Sripathirathan K, et al. Regional differences in relationships between apparent white matter integrity, cognition and mood in patients with ischemic stroke. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2010;32:673–681. doi: 10.1080/13803390903427406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Maleki N, Becerra L, Brawn J, Bigal M, Burstein R, Borsook D. Concurrent functional and structural cortical alterations in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2012;32:607–620. doi: 10.1177/0333102412445622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Coppola G, Di Renzo A, Tinelli E, Lepre C, Di Lorenzo C, Di Lorenzo G, et al. Thalamo-cortical network activity between migraine attacks: Insights from MRI-based microstructural and functional resting-state network correlation analysis. J. Headache Pain. 2016 doi: 10.1186/s10194-016-0693-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Liu G, Dang C, Chen X, Xing S, Dani K, Xie C, et al. Structural remodeling of white matter in the contralesional hemisphere is correlated with early motor recovery in patients with subcortical infarction. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2015;33:309–319. doi: 10.3233/RNN-140442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Zamboni G, Griffanti L, Mazzucco S, Pendlebury ST, Rothwell PM. Age-dependent association of white matter abnormality with cognition after TIA or minor stroke. Neurology. 2019;93:E272–E282. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grefkes C, Nowak DA, Eickhoff SB, Dafotakis M, Küst J, Karbe H, et al. Cortical connectivity after subcortical stroke assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Ann. Neurol. 2008;63:236–246. doi: 10.1002/ana.21228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.de Shelton FN, Reding MJ. Effect of lesion location on upper limb motor recovery after stroke. Stroke. 2001;32:107–112. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.32.1.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang LE, Tittgemeyer M, Imperati D, Diekhoff S, Ameli M, Fink GR, et al. Degeneration of corpus callosum and recovery of motor function after stroke: A multimodal magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2941–2956. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Borich MR, Mang C, Boyd LA. Both projection and commissural pathways are disrupted in individuals with chronic stroke: Investigating microstructural white matter correlates of motor recovery. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13:1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pendlebury S, Blaire A, Lee M, Styles P, Matthews PM. Axonal injury in the internal capsule correlates with motor impairment after stroke. Stroke. 1999;30:956–962. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.30.5.956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kuczynski AM, Dukelow SP, Hodge JA, Carlson HL, Lebel C, Semrau JA, et al. Corticospinal tract diffusion properties and robotic visually guided reaching in children with hemiparetic cerebral palsy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39:1130–1144. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kynast J, Lampe L, Luck T, Frisch S, Arelin K, Hoffmann KT, et al. White matter hyperintensities associated with small vessel disease impair social cognition beside attention and memory. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2018;38:996–1009. doi: 10.1177/0271678X17719380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cudeck R. Mixed-effects models in the study of individual differences with repeated measures data. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1996;31:371–403. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3103_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Edwards LJ. Modern statistical techniques for the analysis of longitudinal data in biomedical research. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2000;30:330–344. doi: 10.1002/1099-0496(200010)30:4<330::AID-PPUL10>3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Coderre AM, Zeid AA, Dukelow SP, Demmer MJ, Moore KD, Demers MJ, et al. Assessment of upper-limb sensorimotor function of subacute stroke patients using visually guided reaching. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2010;24:528–541. doi: 10.1177/1545968309356091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mostafavi SM, Dukelow SP, Glasgow JI, Scott SH, Mousavi P. Reduction of stroke assessment time for visually guided reaching task on KINARM exoskeleton robot. Annu Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2014;2014:5296–5299. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2014.6944821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lowrey CR, Jackson CPT, Bagg SD, Dukelow SP, Scott SH. A novel robotic task for assessing impairments in bimanual coordination post-stroke. Int. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2014 doi: 10.4172/2329-9096.S3-002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Tyryshkin K, Coderre AM, Glasgow JI, Herter TM, Bagg SD, Dukelow SP, et al. A robotic object hitting task to quantify sensorimotor impairments in participants with stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2014;11:47. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bourke TC, Lowrey CR, Dukelow SP, Bagg SD, Norman KE, Scott SH. A robot-based behavioural task to quantify impairments in rapid motor decisions and actions after stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2016;13:91. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0201-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hawkins KM, Sergio LE. Visuomotor impairments in older adults at increased Alzheimer’s disease risk. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014;42:607–621. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Little CE, Emery C, Black A, Scott SH, Meeuwisse W, Nettel-Aguirre A, et al. Test-retest reliability of KINARM robot sensorimotor and cognitive assessment: In pediatric ice hockey players. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2015;12:78. doi: 10.1186/s12984-015-0070-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2000;22:518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Tombaugh TN. Trail making test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Corsi, P. Memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Doctoral Thesis in Philosophy, McGill University, Montreal (1972).
- 61.Dukelow SP, Herter TM, Moore KD, Demers MJ, Glasgow JI, Bagg SD, et al. Quantitative assessment of limb postion sense following stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2010;24:178–187. doi: 10.1177/1545968309345267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.