Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Aug 26;11:17271. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-96347-y

A prospective cohort study on longitudinal trajectories of cognitive function after stroke

Dongni Buvarp 1,, Lena Rafsten 1,2, Tamar Abzhandadze 1,2, Katharina S Sunnerhagen 1,2
PMCID: PMC8390476  PMID: 34446763

Abstract

The study aimed to determine longitudinal trajectories of cognitive function during the first year after stroke. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used to screen cognitive function at 36–48 h, 3-months, and 12-months post-stroke. Individuals who shared similar trajectories were classified by applying the group-based trajectory models. Data from 94 patients were included in the analysis. Three cognitive functioning groups were identified by the trajectory models: high [14 patients (15%)], medium [58 (62%)] and low [22 (23%)]. For the high and medium groups, cognitive function improved at 12 months, but this did not occur in the low group. After age, sex and education matching to the normative MoCA from the Swedish population, 52 patients (55%) were found to be cognitively impaired at baseline, and few patients had recovered at 12 months. The impact on memory differs between cognitive functioning groups, whereas the impact on activities of daily living was not different. Patients with the poorest cognitive function did not improve at one-year poststroke and were prone to severe memory problems. These findings may help to increase focus on long-term rehabilitation plans for those patients, and more accurately assess their needs and difficulties experienced in daily living.

Subject terms: Stroke, Outcomes research

Introduction

Cognitive impairment is a highly prevalent long-term stroke consequence, with approximately 80% of patients reporting mild cognitive impairments post-stroke14. In addition to motor impairments, cognition decline is one of the key determinants of deterioration in performance of daily activities after stroke5. The ability to early recognize individuals with potential cognitive decline after stroke would make it possible to appropriately assess their actual needs and additional supports, while tailoring interventions towards these individuals, thus improving their quality of life.

Longitudinal changes in cognitive function after stroke were largely diverse. Multiple evolving trends, such as improvement, worsening, or remaining stable, are shown to depend on the use of cognitive instruments, as well as the length of time until follow-up, whether short-term or long-term6. A significant decline in global cognition was demonstrated a few years after stroke (from 3 to 6 years)7,8, whereas significant improvements in cognition were found within 6-months post-stroke9,10. Higher age and post-stroke depression in individuals were associated with a cognitive decline11,12. Mixed findings were reported for other risk factors (e.g. sex, ApoE ε4 status, vascular risk factors)6,13. However, the risk of post-stroke dementia may likely be related to the incidence of stroke rather than to the background vascular risk factors1. To date, it remains unclear how the longitudinal progress of post-stroke cognition varies in patients with different degrees of cognitive functioning in the acute phase. It may be that individuals with baseline severe cognitive deficits are more likely to suffer a cognitive decline. But if so, it is also not clear when this cognitive decline will occur. Moreover, it is also unknown if patients with varying baseline degrees of cognition will share similar trajectories over time. Exploring these longitudinal trajectories is, therefore, warranted to enhance understanding of the recovery of cognitive function post-stroke while deciphering the heterogeneity14. This knowledge will assist in constructing an early individual rehabilitation plan.

The study aimed to identify trajectories of cognition during the first year after stroke, and to examine the impact of different trajectory groups in relation to memory performance and activities of daily living.

Method

Data and material availability

The complete dataset cannot be made publicly available for ethical and legal reasons, according to Swedish regulations (https://etikprovning.se/for-forskare/ansvar/). Researchers can request access to the data by emailing the principal investigator at ks.sunnerhagen@neuro.gu.se.

Study sample and design

This longitudinal study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg (registration number: 426-05 and 042-11) and was conducted in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki. Data was used from the Gothenburg Very Early Supported Discharge clinical trial (URL: http://www.clinicaltrials.gov. Unique identifier: NCT01622205) at the stroke unit of the Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Sweden, from September 2011 to April 201615. The study participants were required to understand the study protocol and written informed consent was provided prior to the clinical trial. The inclusion criteria were age > 18 years, a diagnosis of stroke according to World Health Organization criteria16, a National Institute of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) score from 0 to 16 at Day 2, a Barthel Index (BI) from 50 to 100, and a score of less than 26 in The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) if the BI score was 100. Exclusion criteria were if the participants were living in a nursing facility prior to stroke, lived more than 30 min traveling time to the Sahlgrenska University Hospital, were not able to communicate in Swedish, NIHSS > 16 and life expectancy less than one year (e.g. malign disease condition). Other inclusion and exclusion criteria of the clinical trial were previously reported15.

Clinical assessments

The MoCA was administrated to evaluate cognitive functioning at an interval of 36 to 48 h after stroke (referred to as baseline), 3-months and 12-months after stroke. The MoCA is a valid and reliable instrument for screening of global cognition in patients with mild to moderate stroke17,18. The MoCA consists of subdomains following visuospatial/executive, naming, attention, abstraction, delayed recall or memory and orientation. A maximum score is 30, where lower indicates worse cognition19. One extra point was added to the final score if patients had less than or equal to 12 years of education. Written permission for using the MoCA test was obtained from MoCA© (MoCA—Cognitive Assessment https://www.mocatest.org).

Neurological deficits were evaluated using NIHSS at Day 2 after admission20. Motor-sensory function in the extremities was tested using the Fugl-Meyer Assessment Scale (FMA; lower extremity [-LE] and upper extremity [-UE]), with a lower FMA score indicating a more severe impairment of function21. Clinical measurements were conducted by an experienced physiotherapist and/or occupational therapist.

Self-assessed measures

Psychological distress was assessed using the 14-item Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) self-assessment questionnaire, which comprised a 7-item subscale to assess anxiety and a 7-item subscale to assess depression22. Each item consists of four response levels, scored from 0 to 3. Mild and moderate anxiety or depression is attributed to a score above 7 points on the subscale22.

Memory performance and activities of daily living, two major domains which impact health-related quality of life after stroke, were assessed using the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS, version 3.0)23. The SIS is a 59-item multi-dimensional self-assessed measure of eight domains: strength, memory and thinking, emotion, communication, activities of daily living, mobility, hand function, and participation. Each domain contains a various number of items and each item has five response level to score their self-perceived difficulties after stroke. Memory (8 items) and ADL/IADL (10 items) of SIS-subdomains were used in this study to determine the impact of cognitive function in patients after stroke. Written permission of using the SIS scale version 3.0 was granted in 2006 from MAPI Research Trust, Lyon, France (https://www.mapi-trust.org). All the self-reported assessments were administered on Day 5 to not overburden the patients.

Statistical analysis

Longitudinal trajectories of cognition

Cognition assessed by MoCA was transformed through converting each individual’s MoCA score into proportions, by dividing by the maximal MoCA score (30 points). The proportional MoCA scores are continuous with an interval from 0 to 1, and an upper and lower limited bound of 0.005 and 0.995 were used, respectively, to fit a beta distribution. Patients were considered lost to follow-up if there were more than two missed visits and were further excluded from the longitudinal analysis.

The longitudinal changes in cognition of the study sample were analyzed using a longitudinal beta regression mixed-effect model, while adjusting for age and sex24. Age was dichotomized to < 75 and ≥ 75 years using the median value. Time, age and sex were included as fixed effect, and random intercept was also used. Akaike information criterion, Bayesian information criterion, and pseudo-R2 were used to evaluate the model performance.

A group-based trajectory model was used for clustering, which is a finite mixture modelling designed to classify individuals who share similar longitudinal trajectories into clusters by fitting the developmental course of each individual over time using maximum likelihood estimation25,26. Group-based trajectory models specified for beta distribution were applied in this study to identify the subgroups that followed similar progression of cognition over time after stroke27. The subgroups were stratified through the highest predicted probability assigned to individuals. Model evaluation and selection for developmental trajectories was based on the Bayesian information criterion and average posterior probability.

Cognitive impairments

By identifying patients with cognitive impairments, normative data on MoCA from the Swedish general population was used as a reference by matching age, sex and education28. Cognitive impairment was defined by whether the individuals’ total MoCA score was larger than or equal to 1 standard deviation of normative cognition after age-, sex-, and education-matching, after removal of an extra point for low education28. The number of patients with cognitive impairments in different trajectory groups was explored across the three time points.

Impact of longitudinal cognition on activities of daily living and memory

Both memory and ADL/IADL domains of SIS were transformed to a continuous index, ranging from 0 to 100 (a lower score indicates a higher perceived impact)29. The means scores for SIS-memory performance and SIS-ADL/IADL were compared between different trajectory groups, as well as between different time points.

For group comparison, either Fisher’s exact test, Pearson’s χ2 test, Mann–Whitney U test, independent t tests, Kruskal–Wallis test, and 1-way analysis of variance was used, as appropriate. To compare changes between different time points in SIS and the subdomains of MoCA, either Friedman Test, repeated-measures analysis of variance, paired t test or Wilcoxon signed-rank was used, as appropriate. Significance for multiple comparisons was adjusted using Holm-Bonferroni corrections. A two tailed significance level was defined as p < 0.05. The effect size was also calculated for each applied statistical test to determine the magnitude of difference. Statistical analyses were performed using SAS (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and IBM SPSS statistics 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

One hundred and forty patients were eligible for the study trial and five patients withdrew prior to the baseline assessments. Details of inclusion and exclusion in the longitudinal data analysis are presented in a flow chart (Fig. 1). No statistically significant difference was found between the patients excluded and those included in the study in terms of age, sex and neurological deficits. Data from 94 patients were included in the longitudinal analysis [median age 76 years, range 37–96, 40 females (43%); Table 1].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow chart summarizing study sample recruitment and inclusion and exclusion for longitudinal data analysis. a94 patients who had two or more longitudinal assessments of MoCA were included into the analysis.

Table 1.

Clinical assessments and self-reported measures between the identified trajectory groups.

Characteristic All (N = 94) High cognitive functioning-Cluster I (N = 14) Medium cognitive functioning-Cluster II (N = 58) Low cognitive functioning-Cluster III (N = 22) p-valuea
Age [N (%)] 0.04

 < 75 years

 ≥ 75 years

41 (44)

53 (56)

9 (64)

5 (36)

27 (47)

31 (53)

5 (23)

17 (77)

Sex [N (%)] 0.69

Male

Female

53 (56)

41 (44)

7 (50)

7 (50)

32 (55)

26 (45)

14 (64)

8 (36)

Education [N (%)] 0.12

Max 12 years

 > 12 years

34 (36)

60 (64)

2 (14)

12 (86)

25 (43)

33 (57)

7 (32)

15 (68)

Stroke type [N (%)] 0.22

Ischemic infarct

Intracerebral hemorrhage

88 (94)

6 (6)

14 (100)

0 (0)

55 (95)

3 (5)

19 (86)

3 (14)

Hemisphere of lesion [N (%)] 0.35

Left

Right

Bilateral

Cerebellum

Brain stem

Unclear

20 (21)

27 (29)

4 (4)

8 (9)

2 (2)

33 (35)

1 (7)

5 (36)

0 (0)

3 (21)

1 (7)

4 (29)

14 (24)

18 (31)

2 (3)

4 (7)

1 (2)

19 (33)

5 (23)

4 (18)

2 (9)

1 (5)

0 (0)

10 (45)

OCSP classification [N (%)]b 0.31

TACS

PACS

LACS

POCS

Uncertain

3 (3)

14 (15)

34 (36)

30 (32)

7 (7)

0 (0)

3 (21)

4 (29)

6 (43)

1 (7)

2 (3)

6 (10)

23 (40)

21 (36)

3 (5)

1 (5)

5 (23)

7 (32)

3 (14)

3 (14)

NIHSS 2 (0–4) 2 (1–7) 2 (0–4) 3 (2–5) 0.18
FMA-UE motor function 63 (57.8–65) 65 (63–66) 63 (57–64) 64 (58–65) 0.24
FMA-LE motor function 33 (28.5–34) 34 (29–34) 33 (29–34) 33 (30–34) 0.86
HADS-A 4 (1–7) 1 (0–3) 5 (1–6) 4 (0–9) 0.08
HADS-D 2.5 (1–6) 1 (0–1) 3 (1–6) 6 (2–7) 0.002
MoCA, mean (SD)/median (IQR)
Baseline 21.9 (5)/22 (19–26) 26.8 (2)/27 (27–28) 22.5 (4)/23 (21–25) 16.6 (3)/16.5 (15–19)  < 0.001
3 months 23.4 (4)/24 (21–26) 28.4 (2)/28.5 (28–30) 24.5 (2)/25 (23–26) 16.8 (3)/16.5 (14.5–19)  < 0.001
12 months 23.8 (4)/25 (22–27) 28.5 (1)/29 (28–29) 24.8 (2)/25 (23–26) 17.2 (3)/18 (14–20)  < 0.001
p-valuec  < 0.001 0.02 0.002 0.37
Changes from baseline to 3 months 1.8 (4)/1 (0–3) 1.5 (2)/2 (0–3) 2.3 (4)/1 (0–4) 0.86 (3)/1 (0–3)
Adjusted p-valued  < 0.001 0.07 0.002 0.84
Changes from 3 to 12 months 0.3 (2)/0 (−1 to 2) −0.17 (2)/0 (−1.8 to 1) 0.6 (2)/0.5 (−1 to 2) −0.6 (2)/−0.5 (−2.3 to 1.3)
Adjusted p-valued 0.72 1.0 0.42 1.0

Data are given as median (IQR) unless otherwise noted.

IQR interquartile range, SD standard deviation, NIHSS National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (baseline N = 75), MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment (baseline N = 76), FMA Fugl-Meyer Assessment, UE upper extremity, LE lower extremity (baseline N = 93), HADS Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (baseline n = 134), OCSP Oxford Community Stroke Project classification, TACS total anterior circulation stroke, PACS partial anterior circulation stroke, LACS lacunar stroke, POCS posterior circulation stroke.

aPearson’s χ2, Kruskal–Wallis test or 1-way analysis of variance were used as appropriate.

bNot applicable in 6 patients with intracerebral hemorrhage.

cFriedman test was used to explore changes in MoCA from baseline to 12 months after stroke.

dWilcoxon signed-rank test was used, and multiple comparisons were adjusted using Holm-Bonferroni corrections.

Longitudinal trajectories of cognitive function during the first year after stroke

Compared to baseline, there was a significant increase in MoCA scores of the whole study sample at 3 months [improved 2 scores; standardized β, 0.36 (95% CI, 0.2–0.5), adjusted p < 0.001], as well as at 12-months post-stroke [improved 3 scores; standardised β, 0.37 (95% CI, 0.22–0.52), adjusted p < 0.001], after adjustment for age and sex. No significant changes in cognitive scores were observed between 3- and 12-months post-stroke. Patients with age ≥ 75 years had poorer cognition over time [odds ratio (OR), 0.66 (95% CI, 0.46–0.93), p = 0.02]. Females showed a trend towards better cognition over time, however, this was not statistically significant [OR, 1.25 (95% CI 0.88–1.78), p = 0.2].

The cluster analysis using a group-based trajectory model identified three distinct groups: cluster I [14 out of 94 patients (15%)], cluster II [58 patients (62%)], and cluster III [22 patients (23%)]. The clinical characteristics of each clusters are shown in Table 1. Cluster I was characterised by high cognitive functioning, with the highest mean MoCA scores across the three time points. Cluster II was termed the medium cognitive functioning group, and had overall mean MoCA scores lower than cluster I, but higher than cluster III. The low cognitive functioning group (defined as cluster III) showed the lowest mean MoCA scores. There were significant differences between the three trajectory groups in total MoCA scores at baseline, 3-months, and 12-months post-stroke (Table 1).

For longitudinal changes in cognitive scores for each group, the high and medium cognitive functioning group showed significant improvements from baseline to 12 months (improved 2 median MoCA scores; p = 0.02, effect size Kendall’s W [W] = 0.32 for the high group; and p = 0.002, W = 0.17 for the medium group, Table 1). From baseline to 3-months, only the medium group showed significantly improved cognitive function (improved 1 median MoCA score, adjusted p = 0.002, effect size r = 0.54). No significant changes in cognition neither from baseline to 3 months, nor from 3- to 12-months post-stroke, were noted for the other two groups. Individual changes in cognitive functioning over time by the cognitive functioning groups are shown in Fig. 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(A) Individual changes in total MoCA score over time in the different cognitive functioning groups (high, medium and low) and mean total MoCA scores of each cognitive functioning group are shown. (B) Percent of patients with cognitive impairment within each cognitive functioning group after age, sex and education-matching with the Swedish population. The light brown line represents the mean total MoCA of patients with cognitive impairment for each group. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, CI confidence interval, mos months.

The mean scores of MoCA subdomains by cognitive functioning groups over time are shown in Fig. 3. Most subdomains of MoCA showed statistically significant differences between cognitive functioning groups, except for the subdomain of naming at baseline.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Mean scores of MoCA subdomains by different cognitive functioning groups over time. The black dash lines represent the maximum score of each subdomain. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, CI confidence interval, mos months.

For the high cognitive functioning group, a significant improvement in the memory domain from baseline to 12-months post-stroke was found (mean difference = 1.01, p = 0.049, W = 0.23). For the medium group, three subdomains were shown to be improved: attention (mean difference = 0.59, p = 0.001, W = 0.18), abstraction (mean difference = 0.21, p = 0.02, W = 0.1), and memory (mean difference = 0.92, p = 0.01, W = 0.12). For the low group, a significant decrease was found in language domain from baseline to 12 months (mean difference = − 0.11, p = 0.047, W = 0.26). No significant changes from baseline to 12 months were noted in any other subdomains of MoCA in the cognitive functioning groups.

Cognitive impairments among different cognitive functioning groups

After matching for age, sex and education with the normative MoCA from the Swedish general population, 52 patients (55%) who were identified to have cognitive impairment at baseline, with 50 (53%), and 46 patients (49%) exhibiting impairment at 3 and 12 months, respectively. Among the 52 patients who had cognitive impairment at baseline, 32 (62%) remained cognitively impaired at 12-months post-stroke, while 13 patients (25%) recovered.

Among the cognitive functioning groups, there were significant differences in the number of patients with cognitive impairments at baseline, 3-months, and 12-months post-stroke (adjusted p < 0.001). The percentage of patients with cognitive impairments across the three time points in each group is presented in Fig. 1.

Impact on memory performance and activities of daily living in different cognitive functioning groups

Among the cognitive functioning groups, there were significant differences between groups in memory performance, assessed by SIS, at baseline (adjusted p = 0.006, effect size ER2 = 0.14), 3-months (adjusted p = 0.003, ER2 = 0.18), and 12-months post-stroke (adjusted p = 0.01, ER2 = 0.13, Fig. 4). No significant differences between groups were found at any time points in the ADL/IADL domain assessed by SIS.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Impact on memory and activities of daily living by different cognitive functioning groups across 3 time points. Data available for SIS-memory and thinking in 93 patients at baseline, 87 patients at 3 and 12 months. Data available for SIS-ADL/IADL in 88 patients at baseline, 87 at 3 and 12 months. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. SIS stroke impact scale, CI confidence interval, ADL activities of daily living, IADL instrumental activities of daily living.

There was a significant change for the medium group in memory performance from baseline to 12-months post-stroke (mean difference = − 2.8, p = 0.01, W = 0.09). For the impact on ADL/IDL over time between groups, significant improvements were seen both for the medium and high group from baseline to 12-months post-stroke (mean difference = 14.6, p = 0.009, W = 0.36 for the high group; mean difference = 17.3, p < 0.001, W = 0.5 for the medium group). There were no significant improvements in SIS-ADL/IADL noted in the low group.

Discussion

This longitudinal study demonstrated that the trajectories of cognition in stroke patients improved during the first three months, but subsequently remained unchanged until 12 months, after stroke. The group based-trajectory model stratified three levels of cognitive functioning: low, medium and high. After being age, sex, and education-matched with normative MoCA of the Swedish population, half of the study sample (55%) was found to have cognitive impairment at baseline, and 25% of these patients recovered after 12 months. The impact on memory performance significantly differed between cognitive functioning groups, whereas the impact of ALD/IADL was not different between groups.

Consistent with the general pattern of stroke recovery30, improvements in cognition were found solely within the first three months and tended to decline after that for the low cognitive functioning group in the present study. The improved cognition for the medium and high cognitive functioning groups was higher than the minimal clinically important difference of MoCA which with an estimate of 1.22 was previously suggested31. However, there were diverse patterns for changes in cognition shown after 3-months post-stroke or a longer time of follow-up, depending on the cognitive instruments used10. This may imply that the evolution of cognition is rather heterogenous and the rate of recovery differs significantly among stroke patients. The study findings based on the trajectory-model could contribute to deciphering heterogeneity by stratifying patients with similar developmental patterns over the course of time and identifying potential subgroups that may likely be susceptible to cognitive decline or poor recovery.

Patients with the poorest cognitive function, whose cognition did not improve during the first year after stroke, were older and had more frequent depressive symptom. This is in line with factors responsible for cognitive decline from earlier findings11,12. Furthermore, more severe initial cognitive impairment may reflect a decrease in neurological reserve and neuroplastic potential, which might lead to a poor recovery32,33. The low cognitive functioning group showed a substantially poor performance in the memory/delay recall domain over time and a significant decrease in language performance at 12-months post-stroke. The association between deficit of language expression and a greater risk of progressive cognitive decline was shown in a previous study34. Language deficits in stroke patients commonly involves left-side lesions, but this cannot be determined in our study sample due to the small sample size. However, various findings of the hemisphere of the lesion as a predictor for stroke recovery have previously been reported3537.

Half of the study sample exhibited cognitive impairment at baseline. The prevalence in our study sample was similar in comparison with other studies that demonstrated a range from 30 to 74% depending on instruments used3,4,38. The patients who recovered at 12-months post-stroke were from the medium or high group, and none of the patients with cognitive impairment in the low group recovered. This further suggests that the patients with baseline severe cognitive deficits may show difficulty in recovering over time after stroke, and the loss of cognition may not be reversible.

Patients with lower cognitive functioning perceived a large impact on their memory performance as well as activities of daily living, compared to those with higher cognitive function. Perceived difficulties in these two SIS domains have been frequently reported in patients with cognitive impairment39. However, the perceived impact on memory performance did not change during the first year after stroke, and although there was a slight decrease in the mean score of the medium group, the effect size was considered to be very small. This could be due to a lack of awareness of cognitive problems. In contrast, the impact on activities of daily living was noted to be clinically meaningful, with changes from the baseline to 12-months post-stroke for the medium and the high group, as a clinically important difference of 5.9 was suggested40. The underlying reason maybe that the improved cognition has a positive impact on their activities of daily living, as was also previously shown41. Furthermore, as there was no group difference in ADL/IADL across any time points, this finding suggested that the patients with the poorest cognitive functioning may have difficulty to perceive their daily problems. This highlights the importance of recognising these patients early at baseline and the need to appropriately assess their potential difficulties along with a long-term follow-up plan.

The strength of the study is that the cognitive function assessed by MoCA was conducted in the very early stages of stroke and patients were followed up to 1 year. This captures a relatively complete picture of dynamic changes of cognition from the acute to chronic phase. The advance in the group-based trajectory model was able to identify patients with similar progress patterns, while deciphering certain heterogeneity from the disease group. Moreover, cognitive impairment was defined by age, sex and education-matching with the Swedish general population, which provides a more appropriate assessment of cognitive impairment in comparison to the use of a single cut-off of MoCA.

There were limitations that need to be addressed in this study. As one of the limitations, the included patients, in general, had mild to moderate neurological deficits; therefore, the cognitive function may be overestimated and thus have limited generalisability. However, it needs to be emphasised that a high score in ADL assessment does not necessarily suggest that patients do not suffer from cognitive impairment. With a mild cognitive impairment, patients may be able to perform basic activities of daily living, but there may be a more substantial impact on more complex activities that involve planning and executive function. This may explain why no group difference was found in SIS-ADL/IADL at any time point, as this domain was dominated by basic activities of living items. In addition, patients with severe cognitive deficits may have difficulties in realizing their limitations with basic ADL. The study findings may facilitate the improvement of recognition of cognitive impairments in those patients who score well in ADL assessments.

One more limitation is that the effect of some factors (such as localisation and initial clinical symptoms) on cognitive decline were not possible to explore due to the small sample size. Although substantial effort has been made to assess neuroimaging findings, precise lesions and volume were difficult to determine and were not within the scope of the study. This study has a primary interest in longitudinal trajectories in cognition assessed using MoCA. Furthermore, data on pre-morbid cognition as well as comorbidities were not available, and is also considered as one of the limitations. Including these variables into analyses, might offer a better understanding on longitudinal trajectories of cognition after stroke. The MoCA has similar limitations as in other cognitive instruments; for example, the ceiling effects on MoCA may be limited in its ability to detect potential improvement in the high cognitive functioning group42. The MoCA as a global measure of cognition may not capture subtle cognitive changes. There is a need for assessing executive function and subjective cognitive complaints, and tools for that are desirable. Also, the MoCA assessments could not be conducted for patients with aphasia or hemiplegia, which could result in an overestimation of the functioning. However, MoCA has previously been shown to be a feasible and sensitive tool for use in a very early stage of stroke to screen cognitive function and with excellent accuracy for detecting cognitive impairments among the available cognitive instruments17,18.

Conclusion

Cognition after stroke, in general, improved during the first three months, but no significant improvement was found at any time point for patients with the poorest cognitive function who were older, more frequently suffering from depression, and prone to severe memory problems. This study may help to focus on rehabilitation plans for those patients and to more accurately assess their needs and difficulties in daily living.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge all patients who participated in this study and thank all health professionals who helped collect data. This study was funded in part by grants from the Swedish Science Council (VR2012-3523 and VR2017-00946), the Health & Medical Care Committee of the Regional Executive Board of the Region Västra Götaland, King Gustaf V’s and Queen Victoria’s Freemasons’ Foundation, the Swedish National Stroke Association, Agneta Prytz-Folke’s and Gösta Folke’s Foundation, the Swedish Heart and Lung Foundation, the Swedish Brain Foundation, Promobilia Foundation and Insamlingsstiftelsen för Neurologisk Forskning. The study was financed by grants from the Swedish state under an agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF agreement (ALFGBG-718711 and ALFGBG-877961).

Author contributions

D.B. contributed to drafting the manuscript, conducting the data analysis, and interpreting of the results. L.R., T.A., and K.S.S. contributed to interpreting the results, and critically reviewing the manuscript for intellectual content. L.R., T.A., and K.S.S. substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, and L.R. was mainly responsible for data acquisition.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Gothenburg.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Pendlebury ST, Rothwell PM. Prevalence, incidence, and factors associated with pre-stroke and post-stroke dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:1006–1018. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70236-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Leys D, Hénon H, Mackowiak-Cordoliani M-A, Pasquier F. Poststroke dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:752–759. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70221-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sun J-H, Tan L, Yu J-T. Post-stroke cognitive impairment: Epidemiology, mechanisms and management. Ann. Transl. Med. 2014;2:80–80. doi: 10.3978/j.issn.2305-5839.2014.08.05. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Barbay M, Diouf M, Roussel M, Godefroy O. Systematic review and meta-analysis of prevalence in post-stroke neurocognitive disorders in hospital-based studies. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2018;46:322–334. doi: 10.1159/000492920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Patel MD, Coshall C, Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA. Cognitive impairment after stroke: Clinical determinants and its associations with long-term stroke outcomes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2002;50:700–706. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50165.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tang EY, et al. Longitudinal effect of stroke on cognition: A systematic review. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2018 doi: 10.1161/jaha.117.006443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Toole JF, Bhadelia R, Williamson JD, Veltkamp R. Progressive cognitive impairment after stroke. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2004;13:99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Comijs HC, et al. Somatic chronic diseases and 6-year change in cognitive functioning among older persons. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2009;48:191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Suzuki M, et al. Predicting recovery of cognitive function soon after stroke: Differential modeling of logarithmic and linear regression. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e53488. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wagle J, et al. Cognitive impairment and the role of the ApoE ε4-allele after stroke—A 13 months follow-up study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2010;25:833–842. doi: 10.1002/gps.2425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ghosal MK, et al. Correlates of functional outcome among stroke survivors in a developing country–A prospective community-based study from India. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2014;23:2614–2621. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2014.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tene O, et al. Depressive symptoms following stroke and transient ischemic attack: Is it time for a more intensive treatment approach? Results from the TABASCO cohort study. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2016;77:673–680. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lo JW, et al. Profile of and risk factors for poststroke cognitive impairment in diverse ethnoregional groups. Neurology. 2019;93:e2257. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Saa JP, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of cognition after stroke – A systematic scoping review. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0221735. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sunnerhagen KS, et al. Gothenburg very early supported discharge study (GOTVED) NCT01622205: A block randomized trial with superiority design of very early supported discharge for patients with stroke. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stroke 1989 Recommendations on stroke prevention, diagnosis, and therapy: Report of the WHO Task Force on stroke and other cerebrovascular disorders. Stroke. 1989;20(1407–1431):1989. doi: 10.1161/01.str.20.10.1407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Blackburn DJ, Bafadhel L, Randall M, Harkness KA. Cognitive screening in the acute stroke setting. Age Ageing. 2013;42:113–116. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chiti G, Pantoni L. Use of Montreal cognitive assessment in patients with stroke. Stroke. 2014;45:3135–3140. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.004590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nasreddine ZS, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brott T, et al. Measurements of acute cerebral infarction: A clinical examination scale. Stroke. 1989;20:864–870. doi: 10.1161/01.str.20.7.864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fugl-Meyer AR, Jaasko L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S. The post-stroke hemiplegic patient. 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 1975;7:13–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983;67:361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Duncan PW, Bode RK, Lai SM, Perera S. Rasch analysis of a new stroke-specific outcome scale: The Stroke Impact Scale. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2003;84:950–963. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(03)00035-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hunger M, Döring A, Holle R. Longitudinal beta regression models for analyzing health-related quality of life scores over time. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2012;12:144. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nagin DS. Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric, group-based approach. Psychol. Methods. 1999;4:139. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Nagin DS, Nagin D. Group-Based Modeling of Development. Harvard University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Elmer J, Jones BL, Nagin DS. Using the beta distribution in group-based trajectory models. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018;18:1–5. doi: 10.1186/s12874-018-0620-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Borland E, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment: Normative data from a large Swedish population-based cohort. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59:893–901. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Duncan PW, et al. The stroke impact scale version 2.0: Evaluation of reliability, validity, and sensitivity to change. Stroke. 1999;30:2131–2140. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.30.10.2131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jorgensen HS, et al. Outcome and time course of recovery in stroke. Part II: Time course of recovery. The Copenhagen Stroke Study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1995;76:406–412. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(95)80568-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wu C-Y, et al. Responsiveness, minimal clinically important difference, and validity of the MoCA in stroke rehabilitation. Occup. Ther. Int. 2019;2517658–2517658:2019. doi: 10.1155/2019/2517658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Umarova RM. Adapting the concepts of brain and cognitive reserve to post-stroke cognitive deficits: Implications for understanding neglect. Cortex. 2017;97:327–338. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jaywant A, Toglia J, Gunning FM, O’Dell MW. Subgroups defined by the Montreal cognitive assessment differ in functional gain during acute inpatient stroke rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2020;101:220–226. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2019.08.474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ballard C, Rowan E, Stephens S, Kalaria R, Kenny RA. Prospective follow-up study between 3 and 15 months after stroke. Stroke. 2003;34:2440–2444. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000089923.29724.CE. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Barker-Collo S, et al. Neuropsychological profiles of 5-year ischemic stroke survivors by Oxfordshire stroke classification and hemisphere of lesion. Stroke. 2012;43:50–55. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.627182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Paolucci S, et al. Functional outcome in stroke inpatient rehabilitation: Predicting no, low and high response patients. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 1998;8:228–234. doi: 10.1159/000015856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Fink JN, Frampton CM, Lyden P, Lees KR. Does hemispheric lateralization influence functional and cardiovascular outcomes after stroke? An analysis of placebo-treated patients from prospective acute stroke trials. Stroke. 2008;39:3335–3340. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.523365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sexton E, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence of cognitive impairment no dementia in the first year post-stroke. Eur. Stroke J. 2019;4:160–171. doi: 10.1177/2396987318825484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Almalki O, et al. Can the stroke impact scale 3.0 detect cognitive impairments in patients with a recent stroke? J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 2019;31:563–568. doi: 10.1589/jpts.31.563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lin K-C, et al. Minimal detectable change and clinically important difference of the stroke impact scale in stroke patients. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2010;24:486–492. doi: 10.1177/1545968309356295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Nakling AE, et al. Cognitive deficits in chronic stroke patients: Neuropsychological assessment, depression, and self-reports. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra. 2017;7:283–296. doi: 10.1159/000478851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Toglia J, Fitzgerald KA, O'Dell MW, Mastrogiovanni AR, Lin CD. The mini-mental state examination and Montreal cognitive assessment in persons with mild subacute stroke: Relationship to functional outcome. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2011;92:792–798. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES