Abstract
(1) The effects of intensive mental training based on meditation on the functional and structural organization of the human brain have been addressed by several neuroscientific studies. However, how large-scale connectivity patterns are affected by long-term practice of the main forms of meditation, Focused Attention (FA) and Open Monitoring (OM), as well as by aging, has not yet been elucidated. (2) Using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and multivariate pattern analysis, we investigated the impact of meditation expertise and age on functional connectivity patterns in large-scale brain networks during different meditation styles in long-term meditators. (3) The results show that fMRI connectivity patterns in multiple key brain networks can differentially predict the meditation expertise and age of long-term meditators. Expertise-predictive patterns are differently affected by FA and OM, while age-predictive patterns are not influenced by the meditation form. The FA meditation connectivity pattern modulated by expertise included nodes and connections implicated in focusing, sustaining and monitoring attention, while OM patterns included nodes associated with cognitive control and emotion regulation. (4) The study highlights a long-term effect of meditation practice on multivariate patterns of functional brain connectivity and suggests that meditation expertise is associated with specific neuroplastic changes in connectivity patterns within and between multiple brain networks.
Keywords: functional connectivity, meditation, MVPA, brain networks, fMRI, neuroplasticity, machine learning
1. Introduction
Meditation can be characterized as a set of mind–body practices involving the regulation of attention, awareness and mental states [1,2]. It can be usefully classified into two main styles—focused attention (FA) and open monitoring (OM) meditation—depending on how attentional processes and cognitive monitoring are set [1,3]. In the FA meditation style, attention is focused on a given meditation object in a sustained manner, with the implication of attention regulation processes preventing distraction (e.g., mind wandering) and refocusing on the meditation object [4]. Open Monitoring is based on non-reactive and non-judgmental monitoring of the contents of experiences and mind-body processes, primarily as a mean to become aware of emotional and cognitive patterns and to increase cognitive and emotional flexibility [5,6]. Meditation practices are increasingly relevant in society and have important clinical, educational and workplace applications [7].
The executive, attentional, and emotional regulatory processes involved in these two main forms of meditation may have a deep impact on the brain, on cognition, and on behavior [1,2,6,8]. Several studies have suggested that FA and OM meditation styles involve different brain regions and processes, in association with their different phenomenological, attentional, and cognitive features [1,3,9,10,11]. However, other studies have highlighted common neural processes involved in these meditation forms, plausibly in association with the common executive and meta-awareness functions of FA and OM meditation [2,6,12].
In the past years, it has been shown that brain processes cannot be functionally characterized using isolated brain activations, but rather in terms of large-scale networks that enable an effective information exchange between regions [13,14,15]. The characterization of large-scale brain networks appears crucial for the study of consciousness and meditation [6,9,16]. Moreover, FA and OM meditation forms may differentially modulate the activity of large-scale brain networks [8,9].
It can furthermore be hypothesized that FA and OM meditation expertise differentially affects neuroplasticity in terms of functional connectivity (FC) patterns in the brain, in association with the attentional, monitoring, and regulation processes involved in FA and OM meditation [1,9]. In particular, the recently proposed Brain Theory of Meditation [8] suggests that FA meditation leads to a sharpening of activations and coupling between Salience, Central Executive, and Default Mode networks, whereas OM meditation is characterized by a more distributed pattern of activations and higher coupling, in particular in the left hemisphere.
Another relevant subject of study involves the relationships between meditation expertise, age and neuroplasticity related to FA and OM meditation states and traits, since aging leads to decreased executive control, monitoring, and attentional performances [17,18], whereas meditation practices and expertise lead to enhanced and more efficient executive control, monitoring, and attentional processes [1,2,3,19].
The effects on connectivity have been addressed for both expertise [10,19,20] and age [21,22]; however, these investigations focused only on a subset of brain regions and networks and compared different groups of participants without directly assessing the specific modulation of brain FC by meditation expertise and age in FA and OM meditation forms, including patterns of brain networks related to the regulation of attention, emotion, and self [5].
A promising tool to investigate these effects is the study of patterns of FC through multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA). The interest in MVPA in neuroimaging [23,24] has grown rapidly due to the ability to detect fine-grained differences in neurophysiological patterns as compared with univariate methods [25]. Most MVPA studies have focused on cognitive and perceptual aspects of brain functions [26,27,28,29]. Recently, multivariate methods have been successfully used with FC, as a promising tool for the prediction of individual subject traits [30,31,32,33,34]. MVPA has also been used in meditation studies, using structural MRI to predict the age of a group of meditators [35] and discriminate between meditators and non-meditators [36] as well as to classify the effects on FC of a body–mind training course [37].
Here we studied the impact of meditation expertise and age on patterns of FC within and between large-scale brain networks in FA and OM meditation states. We sought to disentangle the modulation of meditation expertise and age on these networks by using pattern regression techniques. In particular, we assessed whether patterns of functional connectivity in FA and OM meditation states are predictive of the number of years of meditation practice (meditation expertise) and age in a group of long-term Theravada Buddhist monks, who extensively practice both forms of meditation.
Although meditation expertise and age are necessarily correlated, given their opposite effects on cognitive performance, it is likely that they affect functional connections in different ways. Moreover, given the different nature of FA and OM meditation practices, these should affect functional connections differently as well.
Specifically, we hypothesized that (i) the patterns of FC within and between functionally relevant brain networks in the two forms of meditation can be used to predict years of meditation practice (expertise) as well as age; (ii) age-modulated patterns are independent from the type of meditation; (iii) expertise-modulated patterns substantially differ between FA and OM meditation forms. In particular, we hypothesized that the connectivity pattern modulated by meditation expertise in FA meditation includes nodes and connections implicated in focusing, sustaining, and monitoring attention, while modulated FC patterns in OM meditation include nodes and connections associated with interoception, cognitive and affective monitoring, and regulation.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Twelve Theravada Buddhist monks (males, mean age 37.9 years, SD 9.4 years, range 25–60) from Santacittarama Buddhist Monastery in Central Italy, following the Thai Forest Tradition, participated in our study. Participants practiced FA (Samatha) and OM (Vipassana) meditation forms in a balanced way in this tradition, as suggested by the abbot of the monastery. Meditation expertise was measured in years of practice (mean = 16.41 years, SD = 7.69 years), with each year assumed to correspond to 1200 h of meditation practice. The experiment was conducted with the subjects’ written informed consent, according to the Declaration of Helsinki and with the approval of the Ethics Committee of “G. d’Annunzio” University of Chieti-Pescara.
2.2. Experimental Design
The experimental design consisted of three blocks of the following sequence: 6 min of FA and 6 min of OM meditation blocks intermixed with a 3 min non-meditative rest block (Figure 1A) and cued by vocal instructions. The total duration of the experiment was 57 min. Instructions on the meditation type or rest were provided verbally before the beginning of each block. Retrospective self-reports of the meditators suggested that they adequately performed both FA and OM forms during the experiment.
2.3. Data Acquisition and Preprocessing
Images were acquired on a 1.5 T Siemens Magnetom Vision Scanner; BOLD signal images were obtained using T2*-weighted echo planar (EPI) sequence (TR = 4.087 s, 28 slices, voxel size 4 × 4 × 4 mm3, 860 volumes).
A high-resolution T1-weighted whole-brain image was acquired with a 3D-MPRAGE sequence (sagittal matrix = 256 × 256, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, flip angle = 12°, TR/TE = 9.7/4.0 ms).
Preprocessing of raw images was performed using Brain Voyager QX 1.7 software (Brain Innovation, Netherlands). The first five scans were discarded to reach T1 saturation. Preprocessed functional volumes were co-registered with the corresponding structural image. Temporal filtering included linear and non-linear (high-pass filter of two cycles per time course) trend removal. No spatial or temporal smoothing was applied, to avoid the increase of similarity of between-subject connectivity [38].
2.4. Functional Connectivity Analysis
We used the functional ROI parcellation proposed in Shirer et al. (2012) [39], which defines 90 ROIs from 14 large-scale brain networks. The selected ROI and network lists are presented in Table S1.
We segmented anatomical images into grey matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) using the FSL FAST algorithm [40]. White-matter and CSF average signals were regressed out from the fMRI signal, together with movement parameters. Then, the residual signal was filtered (0.009–0.08 Hz) and finally scrubbed by removing the volumes with a Framewise Displacement (FD) index greater than 0.5 [41].
2.5. Registration and ROI Definition
We applied a nonlinear transformation to change the ROI coordinate space (MNI) into the subject space. This procedure is necessary since ROI coordinates were given in the MNI space; therefore a non-linear co-registration from the individual brain space to the MNI template was performed using the FSL FNIRT tool [42]. Then, the inverse of the obtained non-linear transformation was applied to the defined ROIs in order to extract the signal from the subject space. Once the inverse transformation was applied, we performed the intersection, in the individual space, between the ROI mask and the grey matter mask to obtain the ROI time courses. We selected only the ROIs that had an intersection with grey matter in all participants (7 ROIs discarded).
The average ROI time course was extracted, and the pairwise Pearson correlation coefficient was computed between all the ROIs, independently from the meditation form, and for each experimental block. Then, Fisher z-transformation was applied.
2.6. Multivariate Pattern Regression
The values from the upper triangle of the correlation matrix were used as features (Figure 1B). We computed a correlation matrix for each condition, block, and participant. Therefore, the dataset for pattern regression analysis was composed of six correlation matrices per participant (3 for FA and 3 for OM), resulting in 3403 features for 36 samples for each meditation form.
To reduce the difference between the number of features and the number of samples, we adopted a feature selection procedure: we computed Pearson correlation between each feature and meditation expertise or age, then we selected the first 70 connections with the highest absolute correlation as input to the regression algorithm [30]. The dataset was split into two parts: 75% of subjects were used as a training set and the remaining 25% as a testing set. To ensure the independence of data, we used in the training dataset only data from a set of subjects, and in the testing dataset, only data from the remaining set of subjects. This procedure was repeated 50 times, by randomly selecting subjects for the training set to provide a good estimator of the prediction error [43]. Feature selection was calculated in the training set and then was applied to the testing set [44].
We used the linear Support Vector Regression, with C = 1, and then we computed both the mean squared error (MSE) and the Pearson correlation (COR) between predicted and real values as metrics to estimate the prediction error.
The statistical significance was assessed through permutation tests [45] (n = 1000) to obtain the null distribution for MSE and COR. Bonferroni correction was used to assess for multiple comparisons.
The analyses were carried out using scikit-learn [46] and scipy/numpy [47] packages.
2.7. Relevant Feature Analysis
We extracted the weights of each connection tuned during the training phase. Since we selected only important features before training the prediction model, we discarded noisy features that could bias the interpretation of the model feature weights [48].
The weights were normalized using the z-score and averaged across cross-validation to obtain a single matrix with the contribution of each connection.
2.8. Control Analysis for Confound Effects
The two variables (age and expertise) could represent a reciprocal potential confound for the prediction task. The main approaches to control confounds are (i) using the confound as a feature to check its predictive power, but a good classification performance could be misinterpreted due to noise of the confound [48], and (ii) regressing out the confound from the data, but it may also remove some variance related to the variable of interest [49]. We performed a control analysis by regressing out the confound from the data (expertise for age prediction and vice versa).
Since we trained two separate predictive models, for age and expertise, we could also control for confounding effects by comparing the features selected by the two models [50]. If feature sets overlapped in the models, then the prediction was biased, while if the relevant feature sets were disjointed, the prediction was not affected by the confound variable. The advantage of this approach is that the target variable was directly predicted from the connectivity matrix, without removing the signal from a correlated confound as in regression, in order to better interpret the features used for prediction.
2.9. Control Analyses in a Novice Group
We performed an analysis by using the same model trained on the meditator group to predict the age of a novice group of subjects, aiming at understanding whether the trained model was generalizable or was specific for meditators.
The control group was composed of 10 novice meditators (males, mean age 33.0 years, SD 4.0 years, range 22–36), with no prior meditation expertise, recruited from the local community. They practiced FA and OM meditation for 10 days (30 min a day for each form of meditation) before taking part in the experiment. The specificity of the predictive model for meditators could strengthen our hypothesis about the long-term effects of meditation in FC modulation.
A separate analysis to understand whether age can be predicted by functional connectivity in FA and OM meditation forms in the control group was also performed.
Finally, we contrasted, by using a two-sided t-test, the average connectivities of meditators and novices, in FA and OM, to further validate our hypotheses.
3. Results
3.1. Prediction of Years of Meditation Expertise
The prediction performance was calculated using the mean squared error (MSE) and the correlation coefficient (COR). We predicted the years of meditation expertise with an MSE of 0.497 (p < 0.01) and a COR of 0.857 (p < 0.001) in FA meditation. During OM meditation we predicted the expertise with a MSE of 0.520 (p < 0.01) and a COR of 0.834 (p < 0.001). The average error of the model was about 0.71 years.
3.2. Prediction of Age
The age of the meditators could be predicted during the FA meditation (MSE = 0.186, p < 0.005; COR = 0.863, p < 0.001) and during OM meditation (MSE = 0.201, p < 0.005; COR = 0.791, p < 0.001). The average error was about 0.45 years for FA and OM.
3.3. Control Analyses for the Confounding Effect
Control analysis for the confounding effect was performed, since both variables were correlated (r = 0.68).
The results confirmed that we can predict expertise and age by regressing out the confounding variable (age and expertise, respectively). Specifically, the expertise can be predicted after regressing out the age effect in FA, with MSE = 0.531 (p < 0.01) and COR = 0.847 (p < 0.001), and in OM, with MSE = 0.563 (p < 0.01) and COR = 0.811.
The age could be predicted after regressing out the expertise effect as well, with MSE = 0.175 (p < 0.005) and COR = 0.875 (p < 0.001) during FA, while in OM we found MSE = 0.190 (p < 0.005) and COR = 0.813 (p < 0.001).
These results confirmed that prediction is not affected by confounding variables. The prediction error is generally worse in the expertise prediction than in the age prediction, due to some variance lost after regressing out the confounding variable.
3.4. Control Analyses in the Control Group
We used the models trained using meditator data to test the control group to understand whether the age predictive model can be generalized to a different group of subjects. The models did not generalize to the novice group in FA (MSE = 2.54, n.s.; COR = 0.04, n.s.) or in OM (MSE = 4.01, n.s.; COR = 0.2, n.s.) meditation.
The prediction of age using a model built on the control group gave no statistically significant results, neither in FA (MSE = 1.877, n.s.; COR = 0.14, n.s.) nor in OM (MSE = 3.59, n.s.; COR = −0.2, n.s.).
In addition, to add evidence that long-term practice affects functional connectivity, we contrasted the connectomes of meditators and control groups in both conditions. We used a two-sided t-test for the null hypothesis that connectivities of meditators and the control group have identical average values (see Figure S1). As mentioned above, the specificity of the predictive model for meditators and the differences in the connectivities between the two groups supported our hypothesis about the long-term effects of meditation in FC modulation, despite the fact that differences between the two groups can be ascribed to other factors (e.g., diet, educational level), which are difficult to control but are common in the vast majority of meditation studies [2].
3.5. Feature Selection Frequency
We plotted, in Figure 2 and Figure 3, the number of times a connection was selected by the feature selection algorithm for each cross-validation fold.
Figure 2 shows the sets of connections used to predict age and meditation expertise in FA (panel a) and in OM meditation (panel b). The figure suggests that the selection frequency of the features was uncorrelated for the two prediction tasks (FA: r = −0.05, p > 0.05; OM: r = −0.04, p > 0.05), since the dots lie mostly far from the diagonal. These results demonstrate that the connections that were involved in age prediction were different from those involved in predicting the meditation expertise.
In Figure 3 we plotted the frequency of the selected features for the prediction of meditation expertise (panel a) and age (panel b). Specifically, when features were used to predict the meditation expertise (panel a), feature selection frequencies appeared to largely differ in the two forms of meditation. In other words, the two meditation styles recruited different connections; this was confirmed by the low value (r = 0.14, p < 0.05) of the frequency correlation across features. Instead, when features were used to predict the age of the meditators (Figure 3b), the algorithm selected the same features in the two meditation styles, and the frequencies were highly correlated (r = 0.97, p < 0.001). This demonstrates that the connections affected by age are insensitive to the meditation style and are distinct from those involved in meditation.
We also observed the same effect when the confound variable was regressed out (r= 0.917, p < 0.001) (Figure S2).
3.6. Prediction-Relevant Feature Weight Analysis
Next, we analyzed the model weights to evaluate which connections and nodes were more predictive of meditation expertise and age in FA and OM meditation and to what extent. We then restricted the analysis to those connections that were selected at least in 95% of the trained models. For each connection, the weights were averaged across cross-validation folds and z-scored.
We averaged and normalized the connection weights within and between networks to have an integrated view of the contributions of each large-scale network, as shown in Figure 4, for the prediction of expertise in FA (panel a) and OM (panel b) meditation and for the prediction of age (panel c).
We further plotted, in Figure 5, the normalized absolute weights of the ten most important nodes for the prediction of meditation expertise (Figure 5a,b) and for the prediction of age in both meditation forms (Figure 5c,d).
In Figure 6 we plotted the contribution of the connections in the prediction of meditation expertise in FA (panel a) and OM (panel b) meditation.
In Figure 7 we show the connections that were used to predict expertise (panel a) and age (panel b) and those shared in both meditation styles; we chose those that were selected 95% of the time in the regression models.
4. Discussion
In this study, we investigated the modulation by meditation expertise and age on patterns of functional brain connectivity in brain networks during FA and OM meditation states.
We used pattern regression techniques to disentangle the influences of meditation expertise and age. We want to point out that the primary goal of this study was to leverage machine learning to investigate these effects and not to build a predictive model for meditation expertise and age.
The results show that meditation expertise and age can be predicted with significant accuracy from fMRI functional connectivity (FC) data recorded during meditation. The results also highlight a long-term effect of the extensive practice of meditation on FC in brain networks and shed light on the different contributions of meditation expertise and age on brain FC in meditation.
We found that expertise-based patterns differed between FA and OM meditation forms, while age-specific patterns were independent of the type of meditation. Specifically, the stability of the connectivity patterns modulated by age, across different meditation styles, suggests that the performed meditation moderately reorganizes age-specific connections, providing further evidence of the stability of brain networks [51]. The meditation-specific modulation of brain networks due to expertise demonstrates that long-term histories of co-activation among meditation-specific regions strongly impact the organization of brain networks [51], supporting the idea that connectivity profiles could predict cognitive behavior [31].
4.1. Prediction of Meditation Expertise in FA
In our investigation of the relevant features for prediction of expertise in FA meditation, we found an increase in connectivity associated with meditation expertise between the left intraparietal sulcus (IPS)–angular gyrus (AngG) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), two brain regions that are involved in top-down attention and attentional control. In particular, the ACC is crucial for monitoring conflicts [52,53], such as the conflict between focus on the meditation object and distractions, while the IPS and AngG are directly involved in top-down attentional selection and sustained attention [4,54], two other important facets of FA meditation.
In addition, the white matter fractional anisotropy of ACC has been shown to increase in short-term meditators, suggesting an increased communication efficiency and an improvement of attention monitoring skills [55]. The increase of the correlation of the ACC with the IPS/AngG with an increase in expertise may reflect an efficient communication process for detecting distractions and reorienting attention, as in the proposed FA model by Hasenkamp et al. (2012) [4]. We also observed that FA meditation is associated with connections with a negative correlation with meditation expertise, including the connection between the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the left occipital gyrus, which can be explained as a reduced occurrence of spontaneous thoughts and mental images during sustained attention in FA [11,56] and as the well-established involvement of PCC in mind wandering and stimulus-independent thought within the default mode network (DMN) [57,58].
We also found a positive correlation of expertise with the connectivity of nodes in the higher visual network, which is in line with studies that showed an increased involvement of higher visual areas in expert meditators [37,59,60,61]. The reduced expertise-modulated connectivity in nodes within the posterior salience network suggests that salient stimuli may be dropped in favor of a narrow focus of awareness on a single object [62].
4.2. Prediction of Meditation Expertise in OM
In the model proposed by Vago and Zeidan (2016) [62], the fronto-parietal network (FPN) and the ventral attention network play a key role in coordinating awareness and monitoring of experience in the present moment during OM meditation. Results showed that the dorsomedial PFC, which is a core region of the FPN, has a high correlation with the left Crus, a node implicated in emotional processing [63].
Our finding highlights the specific involvement of right caudate (RCau) in the OM facet of meditation (Figure 3b), which is characterized by a wider conscious access to the fields of experience. In addition, it has been demonstrated that expert meditators have greater widespread FC of the RCau with several brain regions [64]. Here we found an increased expertise-modulated connection of RCau with nodes in the Language Network, the left angular gyrus (LAG), and left middle temporal gyrus, which can be associated with an enhanced hub function of the caudate in multiple cognitive abilities [65].
Here we also found a negative connection weight between the right superior frontal gyrus and the right premotor cortex, which can be related to the reduced enactment (plausibly associated with the preparation of movement) of mental states (and in particular negative emotions) in OM meditation with increased meditation expertise [1].
Finally, a high correlation of connectivity between the left and right hippocampus with meditation expertise was shown, in line with the evidence of hippocampal involvement in mindfulness meditation [2,5]. Moreover, the hippocampus is a key convergence zone in the brain, integrating episodic information [66] and related emotional contents [1] as well as enhancing processes of interoception [67].
Our results highlighted a set of connections used to predict expertise in both OM and FA (Figure 7a). This shared pattern of connectivities may be related to common aspects of these two styles, since both forms involve a narrow focus on the present-moment experience and a detachment from reactivity and judgment patterns [62]; moreover, OM meditation is often practiced after FA meditation [1].
In our analysis, the model revealed a high implication of the posterior insula, which is involved in awareness of breath sensations [68,69] and is characterized by decreased connections with the right thalamus and the LAG, which may be associated with the prevention of broadcasting of thalamo-cortico-limbic signals associated with emotional reactivity, with a potential interference during FA and OM forms [16].
We also found an increased connectivity between the LAG and the middle frontal gyrus, which may be associated with the enhanced executive inhibition skills of meditators and plausibly related to changes in fronto-parietal FC [70,71,72].
Interestingly, our analyses showed the importance of regions in the left hemisphere in the prediction of expertise (Figure 6 and Figure 7). This finding can be associated with the hypothesis of leftward dominance of the brain in long-term meditators, which helps the top-down regulation of brain processes and states [8]. This hypothesis is supported by other findings by our group in the same set of subjects [10,11], in which the left brain hemisphere was involved in terms of activation and neural couplings in both forms of meditation. In addition, a meta-analysis and review highlighted the relevance of this hemisphere in long-term meditators [73]. Finally, studies in long-term meditators showed the prevalence of left regions for emotion processing [74,75] as well as structural changes related to attentive [35] and executive regions [76] in the left hemisphere.
4.3. Prediction of Age
The analysis revealed that the caudate-thalamus connections within the basal ganglia network have the highest importance for age prediction, which may be related with the importance of these regions in practitioners of yoga and insight meditation [64].
Our findings also revealed that thalamic connections and insular regions are central for the prediction of age from FA and OM meditation. This result appears consistent with the aging effects on the white matter changes observed in long-term meditators in these regions [77].
Further investigations should be performed to elucidate whether the age-specific patterns of FC are related to the reduction of neurocognitive decline with aging due to extensive meditation practice [78,79].
4.4. Functional Connectivity Patterns at the Brain Network Level
As shown in Figure 4, the analysis at the brain network level reveals interesting differential FC patterns related to expertise and age in FA and OM meditation forms.
In line with the analysis at the level of brain regions within and between networks, fewer expertise-modulated connections were found in FA meditation as compared to OM meditation.
We showed that functional connectivity in the posterior Salience Network (SN) was strongly modulated by meditation expertise in both FA and OM meditation forms, in terms of a reduced coupling, and by age (in both forms of meditation), in terms of an increased coupling. The posterior SN areas, such as the posterior insula, may play the role of key hubs in the regulation or modulation of sensory and feeling experiences, including pain feeling [80], plausibly in terms of down-regulation of activation and its coupling in meditative states [8,10,81]. The increased coupling within the posterior SN with age may reflect a reduced selectivity of neural responses related to sensory and feeling experiences of relevance in both FA and OM meditation forms. In OM meditation we also found an increased expertise-related connectivity between the posterior SN and the visuospatial network, which may be related to an enhanced conscious access to a range of sensory and feeling experiences during OM meditation [1,16].
In FA meditation, results demonstrated an increased expertise-related connectivity within the high visual network, as observed at the level of brain regions. This finding can be associated with a spontaneous focused visualization (with closed eyes) called nimitta in the Buddhist meditation tradition, which may arise during intense meditation [82].
The expertise-modulated connectivity pattern within and between brain networks in OM meditation also includes an increased coupling within the RECN as well as between this network and the language network. These enhanced functional connections may be related to a controlled conscious (metacognitive or mindful) access to sensory and thought contents arising during OM meditation, preventing judgments with affective valence as well as mind wandering [1,2,6,83].
Expertise-related modulation of functional connectivity in OM meditation also included the sensorimotor network, in terms of enhanced within-network connectivity, and reduced connectivity with the dorsal DMN. The first effect may be related to an increased conscious access to top-down regulation of perception and action during this form of meditation [1,16], while the second may be related to the functional segregation between (conscious) extrinsic sensorimotor processing and intrinsic mentation involving the dorsal DMN [4].
Finally, age-related modulation in both FA and OM forms implied a decreased coupling within the anterior SN, which may be explained by an age-related reduced efficiency in this network [84]. The increased FC between the basal ganglia and the auditory network may be explained by the effect of higher interference of auditory experiences, related to the fMRI-acoustic noise in our experiment. An increase in the striatal connectivity with the auditory cortex has also been found in tinnitus [85].
A limitation of our study is the small sample size used [86]. The vast majority of meditation studies involving long-term meditators used tens of subjects [2]; however, recently it has been found that a limited sample size can reveal the general principle of brain functioning more productively [87]. Several studies have used a comparable sample size for predictive analyses [37,88,89,90,91], also using highly non-linear models [92]. In addition, the robustness of the statistical analysis and the cross-validation schema [43] suggests that the conclusions are not over-inflated.
5. Conclusions
Our study demonstrated that fMRI connectivity patterns within and between brain networks can differentially predict meditation expertise and age of long-term meditators, with different patterns in FA and OM meditation forms. Using machine learning techniques, we showed that the connectivity patterns that predicted meditation expertise depended on the meditation style, while age-predictive patterns were the same for both meditation styles. In conclusion, these findings suggest that connectivity patterns in brain networks are differentially shaped by meditation expertise and age, with differential influences of focused attention and open monitoring meditation forms, confirming the specificity of the mental (cognitive and emotion regulation) processes involved in these main forms of meditation [12].
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the monks of Santacittarama Monastery for their outstanding participation in our study. We would also like to thank Carlo Sestieri, Simone Di Plinio, Antea D’Andrea, and Annalisa Tosoni for useful discussions.
Supplementary Materials
The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci11081086/s1, Table S1: List of selected ROIs, Figure S1: Differential connectivities between meditators and control group, Figure S2: Feature selection frequency for expertise and age prediction in control analysis.
Author Contributions
R.G., C.D.G., G.L.R., and A.R. designed the research; R.G., C.D.G., and M.G.P. performed the research; R.G. and M.G.P. analyzed the data; R.G., C.D.G., G.L.R., and A.R. wrote the paper. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
Antonino Raffone was supported by a grant from the BIAL Foundation (Portugal) on the project “Aware Mind-Brain: bridging insights on the mechanisms and neural substrates of human awareness and meditation”. This work was supported by the “Departments of Excellence 2018–2022” initiative of the Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research for the Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences (DNISC) of the University of Chieti-Pescara.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The experiment was conducted with the subjects’ written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki and with the approval of the Ethics Committee of “G. d’Annunzio” University of Chieti-Pescara.
Informed Consent Statement
The experiment was conducted with the subjects’ written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Lutz A., Slagter H., Dunne J.D., Davidson R.J. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008;12:163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tang Y.-Y., Hölzel B.K., Posner M.I. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cahn B.R., Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 2006;132:180–211. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hasenkamp W., Wilson-Mendenhall C.D., Duncan E., Barsalou L.W. Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage. 2012;59:750–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hölzel B.K., Carmody J., Vangel M., Congleton C., Yerramsetti S.M., Gard T., Lazar S. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2011;191:36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Malinowski P. Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:8. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Goleman D., Davidson R.J. The Science of Meditation: How to Change Your Brain, Mind and Body. 2nd ed. . Penguin Life; London, UK: 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Raffone A., Marzetti L., Del Gratta C., Perrucci M.G., Romani G.L., Pizzella V. Toward a brain theory of meditation. Prog. Brain Res. 2019;244:207–232. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lutz A., Jha A.P., Dunne J.D., Saron C.D. Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. Am. Psychol. 2015;70:632–658. doi: 10.1037/a0039585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Manna A., Raffone A., Perrucci M.G., Nardo D., Ferretti A., Tartaro A., Londei A., Del Gratta C., Belardinelli M.O., Romani G.L. Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation. Brain Res. Bull. 2010;82:46–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Marzetti L., Di Lanzo C., Zappasodi F., Chella F., Raffone A., Pizzella V. Magnetoencephalographic alpha band connectivity reveals differential default mode network interactions during focused attention and open monitoring meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:832. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yordanova J., Kolev V., Mauro F., Nicolardi V., Simione L., Calabrese L., Malinowski P., Raffone A. Common and distinct lateralised patterns of neural coupling during focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:7430. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-64324-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fox M.D., Snyder A.Z., Vincent J.L., Corbetta M., Van Essen D.C., Raichle M.E. From The Cover: The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Heuvel M.V.D., Stam C.J., Kahn R.S., Pol H.H. Efficiency of Functional Brain Networks and Intellectual Performance. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:7619–7624. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bressler S.L., Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Raffone A., Srinivasan N. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier BV; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2009. An adaptive workspace hypothesis about the neural correlates of consciousness: Insights from neuroscience and meditation studies; pp. 161–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zanto T.P., Gazzaley A. Attention and Ageing. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Reuter-Lorenz P.A., Festini S.B., Jantz T.K. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 8th ed. Elsevier Inc.; Amsterdam, The Netherlands: 2015. Executive Functions and Neurocognitive Aging; p. 245. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Brefczynski-Lewis J.A., Lutz A., Schaefer H.S., Levinson D.B., Davidson R. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:11483–11488. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606552104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tomasino B., Fregona S., Skrap M., Fabbro F. Meditation-related activations are modulated by the practices needed to obtain it and by the expertise: An ALE meta-analysis study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;6:346. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kurth F., Cherbuin N., Luders E. Promising Links between Meditation and Reduced (Brain) Aging: An Attempt to Bridge Some Gaps between the Alleged Fountain of Youth and the Youth of the Field. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:860. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Malinowski P., Shalamanova L. Meditation and Cognitive Ageing: The Role of Mindfulness Meditation in Building Cognitive Reserve. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2017;1:96–106. doi: 10.1007/s41465-017-0022-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Haxby J.V. Multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI: The early beginnings. NeuroImage. 2012;62:852–855. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Haynes J.-D. A Primer on Pattern-Based Approaches to fMRI: Principles, Pitfalls, and Perspectives. Neuron. 2015;87:257–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.O’Toole A.J., Jiang F., Abdi H., Pénard N., Dunlop J.P., Parent M.A. Theoretical, Statistical, and Practical Perspectives on Pattern-based Classification Approaches to the Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Data. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007;19:1735–1752. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Knops A., Thirion B., Hubbard E.M., Michel V., Dehaene S. Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic. Science. 2009;324:1583–1585. doi: 10.1126/science.1171599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tosoni A., Guidotti R., Del Gratta C., Committeri G., Sestieri C. Preferential coding of eye/hand motor actions in the human ventral occipito-temporal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:116–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Guidotti R., Tosoni A., Perrucci M.G., Sestieri C. Choice-predictive activity in parietal cortex during source memory decisions. NeuroImage. 2019;189:589–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Guidotti R., Tosoni A., Sestieri C. Properties and temporal dynamics of choice- and action-predictive signals during item recognition decisions. Brain Struct. Funct. 2020;225:2271–2286. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02124-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dosenbach N.U.F., Nardos B., Cohen A., Fair D.A., Power J.D., Church J., Nelson S.M., Wig G.S., Vogel A.C., Lessov-Schlaggar C.N., et al. Prediction of Individual Brain Maturity Using fMRI. Science. 2010;329:1358–1361. doi: 10.1126/science.1194144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Finn E.S., Shen X., Scheinost D., Rosenberg M., Huang J., Chun M., Papademetris X., Constable R. Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1664–1671. doi: 10.1038/nn.4135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cole J.H., Franke K. Predicting Age Using Neuroimaging: Innovative Brain Ageing Biomarkers. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40:681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Woo C.-W., Chang L., A Lindquist M., Wager T.D. Building better biomarkers: Brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:365–377. doi: 10.1038/nn.4478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Syrjälä J.J., Basti A., Guidotti R., Marzetti L., Pizzella V. Decoding working memory task condition using magnetoencephalography source level long-range phase coupling patterns. J. Neural Eng. 2021;18:016027. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/abcefe. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Luders E., Cherbuin N., Gaser C. Estimating brain age using high-resolution pattern recognition: Younger brains in long-term meditation practitioners. NeuroImage. 2016;134:508–513. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sato J.R., Kozasa E.H., Russell T.A., Radvany J., Mello L.E.A.M., Lacerda S.S., Amaro E. Brain Imaging Analysis Can Identify Participants under Regular Mental Training. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e39832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Tang Y.-Y., Tang Y., Tang R., Lewis-Peacock J.A. Brief Mental Training Reorganizes Large-Scale Brain Networks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2017;11:6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Alakörkkö T., Saarimäki H., Glerean E., Saramäki J., Korhonen O. Effects of spatial smoothing on functional brain networks. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017;46:2471–2480. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shirer W.R., Ryali S., Rykhlevskaia E., Menon V., Greicius M.D. Decoding Subject-Driven Cognitive States with Whole-Brain Connectivity Patterns. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;22:158–165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Zhang Y., Brady M., Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med Imaging. 2001;20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Power J.D., Barnes K.A., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Andersson J., Jenkinson M., Smith S. High resolution nonlinear registration with simultaneous modelling of intensities. bioRxiv. 2019:646802. doi: 10.1101/646802. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Varoquaux G., Raamana P.R., Engemann D.A., Hoyos-Idrobo A., Schwartz Y., Thirion B. Assessing and tuning brain decoders: Cross-validation, caveats, and guidelines. NeuroImage. 2017;145:166–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pereira F., Mitchell T., Botvinick M. Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: A tutorial overview. NeuroImage. 2009;45:S199–S209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nichols T.E., Holmes A.P. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pedregosa F., Varoquaux G., Gramfort A., Michel V., Thirion B., Grisel O., Blondel M., Prettenhofer P., Weiss R., Dubourg V., et al. Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in python. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2011;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Virtanen P., Gommers R., Oliphant T.E., Haberland M., Reddy T., Cournapeau D., Burovski E., Peterson P., Weckesser W., Bright J., et al. SciPy 1.0: Fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat. Methods. 2020;17:261–272. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haufe S., Meinecke F., Görgen K., Dähne S., Haynes J.-D., Blankertz B., Bießmann F. On the interpretation of weight vectors of linear models in multivariate neuroimaging. NeuroImage. 2014;87:96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Snoek L., Miletić S., Scholte H. How to control for confounds in decoding analyses of neuroimaging data. NeuroImage. 2019;184:741–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bron E.E., Smits M., van der Flier W., Vrenken H., Barkhof F., Scheltens P., Papma J.M., Steketee R., Orellana C.M., Meijboom R., et al. Standardized evaluation of algorithms for computer-aided diagnosis of dementia based on structural MRI: The CADDementia challenge. NeuroImage. 2015;111:562–579. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gratton C., Laumann T.O., Nielsen A.N., Greene D., Gordon E., Gilmore A.W., Nelson S.M., Coalson R.S., Snyder A.Z., Schlaggar B.L., et al. Functional Brain Networks Are Dominated by Stable Group and Individual Factors, Not Cognitive or Daily Variation. Neuron. 2018;98:439–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Botvinick M.M., Braver T.S., Barch D.M., Carter C.S., Cohen J.D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 2001;108:624. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Petersen S.E., Posner M.I. The Attention System of the Human Brain: 20 Years After. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;35:73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Thakral P.P., Slotnick S.D. The role of parietal cortex during sustained visual spatial attention. Brain Res. 2009;1302:157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lee T.M.C., Leung M.-K., Hou W.-K., Tang J.C.Y., Yin J., So K.-F., Lee C.-F., Chan C.C.H. Distinct Neural Activity Associated with Focused-Attention Meditation and Loving-Kindness Meditation. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e40054. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brewer J.A., Garrison K.A. The posterior cingulate cortex as a plausible mechanistic target of meditation: Findings from neuroimaging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013;1307:19–27. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Greicius M.D., Krasnow B., Reiss A.L., Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Pagnoni G. Dynamical Properties of BOLD Activity from the Ventral Posteromedial Cortex Associated with Meditation and Attentional Skills. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:5242–5249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4135-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hasenkamp W., Barsalou L.W. Effects of Meditation Experience on Functional Connectivity of Distributed Brain Networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:38. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Xu J., Vik A., Groote I.R., Lagopoulos J., Holen A., Ellingsen Ø., Håberg A.K., Davanger S. Nondirective meditation activates default mode network and areas associated with memory retrieval and emotional processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:86. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Berkovich-Ohana A., Harel M., Hahamy A., Arieli A., Malach R. Alterations in task-induced activity and resting-state fluctuations in visual and DMN areas revealed in long-term meditators. NeuroImage. 2016;135:125–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Vago D.R., Zeidan F. The brain on silent: Mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2016;1373:96–113. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stoodley C.J. The Cerebellum and Cognition: Evidence from Functional Imaging Studies. Cerebellum. 2012;11:352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Gard T., Taquet M., Dixit R., Hã¶lzel B.K., Dickerson B.C., Lazar S.W., Hölzel B.K. Greater widespread functional connectivity of the caudate in older adults who practice kripalu yoga and vipassana meditation than in controls. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:137. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Graff-Radford J., Williams L., Jones D.T., Benarroch E.E. Caudate nucleus as a component of networks controlling behavior. Neurology. 2017;89:2192–2197. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Eichenbaum H. Hippocampus. Neuron. 2004;44:109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Dudley L., Stevenson R.J. Interoceptive awareness and its relationship to hippocampal dependent processes. Brain Cogn. 2016;109:26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Marchand W.R. Neural mechanisms of mindfulness and meditation: Evidence from neuroimaging studies. World J. Radiol. 2014;6:471–479. doi: 10.4329/wjr.v6.i7.471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kuehn E., Mueller K., Lohmann G., Schuetz-Bosbach S. Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221:1555–1571. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-0989-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Seghier M.L. The Angular Gyrus: Multiple Functions and Multiple Subdivisions. Neuroscientist. 2012;19:43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.D’Andrea A., Chella F., Marshall T.R., Pizzella V., Romani G.L., Jensen O., Marzetti L. Alpha and alpha-beta phase synchronization mediate the recruitment of the visuospatial attention network through the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus. NeuroImage. 2019;188:722–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Marshall T.R., Bergmann T.O., Jensen O. Frontoparietal Structural Connectivity Mediates the Top-Down Control of Neuronal Synchronization Associated with Selective Attention. PLoS Biol. 2015;13:e1002272. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Fox K.C., Dixon M.L., Nijeboer S., Girn M., Floman J.L., Lifshitz M., Ellamil M., Sedlmeier P., Christoff K. Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;65:208–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Davidson R.J. Emotion and Affective Style: Hemispheric Substrates. Psychol. Sci. 1992;3:39–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00254.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Davidson R.J., Kabat-Zinn J., Schumacher J., Rosenkranz M., Muller D., Santorelli S.F., Urbanowski F., Harrington A., Bonus K., Sheridan J. Alterations in Brain and Immune Function Produced by Mindfulness Meditation. Psychosom. Med. 2003;65:564–570. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kurth F., MacKenzie-Graham A., Toga A.W., Luders E. Shifting brain asymmetry: The link between meditation and structural lateralization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015;10:55–61. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Elaneri D., Eschuster V., Edietsche B., Ejansen A., Eott U., Esommer J. Effects of Long-Term Mindfulness Meditation on Brain’s White Matter Microstructure and its Aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016;7:254. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Farb N.A.S., Segal Z.V., Anderson A.K. Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;8:15–26. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Pickut B.A., Van Hecke W., Kerckhofs E., Mariën P., Vanneste S., Cras P., Parizel P.M. Mindfulness based intervention in Parkinson’s disease leads to structural brain changes on MRI. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 2013;115:2419–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2013.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Segerdahl A.R., Mezue M., Okell T., Farrar J.T., Tracey I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:499–500. doi: 10.1038/nn.3969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Lehmann D., Faber P.L., Tei S., Pascual-Marqui R.D., Milz P., Kochi K. Reduced functional connectivity between cortical sources in five meditation traditions detected with lagged coherence using EEG tomography. NeuroImage. 2012;60:1574–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Polak G. Reexamining Jhāna: Towards a Critical Reconstruction of Early Buddhist Soteriology. 1st ed. UMCS; Lublin, Poland: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Giannandrea A., Simione L., Pescatori B., Ferrell K., Belardinelli M.O., Hickman S.D., Raffone A. Effects of the Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction Program on Mind Wandering and Dispositional Mindfulness Facets. Mindfulness. 2018;10:185–195. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1070-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Touroutoglou A., Zhang J., Andreano J.M., Dickerson B.C., Barrett L.F. Dissociable Effects of Aging on Salience Subnetwork Connectivity Mediate Age-Related Changes in Executive Function and Affect. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:410. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Hinkley L.B., Mizuiri D., Hong O., Nagarajan S.S., Cheung S.W. Increased striatal functional connectivity with auditory cortex in tinnitus. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:568. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cui Z., Gong G. The effect of machine learning regression algorithms and sample size on individualized behavioral prediction with functional connectivity features. NeuroImage. 2018;178:622–637. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Naselaris T., Allen E., Kay K. Extensive sampling for complete models of individual brains. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;40:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Baldassarre A., Lewis C., Committeri G., Snyder A.Z., Romani G.L., Corbetta M. Individual variability in functional connectivity predicts performance of a perceptual task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:3516–3521. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113148109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Rosenberg M.D., Finn E.S., Scheinost D., Papademetris X., Shen X., Constable R., Chun M. A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:165–171. doi: 10.1038/nn.4179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Salice S., Esposito R., Ciavardelli D., Pizzi S.D., Di Bastiano R., Tartaro A. Combined 3 Tesla MRI Biomarkers Improve the Differentiation between Benign vs Malignant Single Ring Enhancing Brain Masses. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0159047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Yamashita M., Yoshihara Y., Hashimoto R., Yahata N., Ichikawa N., Sakai Y., Yamada T., Matsukawa N., Okada G., Tanaka S.C., et al. A prediction model of working memory across health and psychiatric disease using whole-brain functional connectivity. eLife. 2018;7:e38844. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Chiarelli A.M., Bianco F., Perpetuini D., Bucciarelli V., Filippini C., Cardone D., Zappasodi F., Gallina S., Merla A. Data-driven assessment of cardiovascular ageing through multisite photoplethysmography and electrocardiography. Med Eng. Phys. 2019;73:39–50. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2019.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.